Abstract
To accurately estimate the state of the body, the nervous system needs to account for delays between signals from different sensory modalities. To investigate how such delays may be represented in the sensorimotor system, we asked human participants to play a virtual pong game in which the movement of the virtual paddle was delayed with respect to their hand movement. We tested the representation of this new mapping between the hand and the delayed paddle by examining transfer of adaptation to blind reaching and blind tracking tasks. These blind tasks enabled to capture the representation in feedforward mechanisms of movement control. A Time Representation of the delay is an estimation of the actual time lag between hand and paddle movements. A State Representation is a representation of delay using current state variables: the distance between the paddle and the ball originating from the delay may be considered as a spatial shift; the low sensitivity in the response of the paddle may be interpreted as a minifying gain; and the lag may be attributed to a mechanical resistance that influences paddle’s movement. We found that the effects of prolonged exposure to the delayed feedback transferred to blind reaching and tracking tasks and caused participants to exhibit hypermetric movements. These results, together with simulations of our representation models, suggest that delay is not represented based on time, but rather as a spatial gain change in visuomotor mapping.
Keywords: Delay, reaching, representation, tracking, transfer
Significance Statement
It is known that the brain copes with sensory feedback delays to control movements, but it is unclear whether it does so using a representation of the actual time lag. We addressed this question by exposing participants to a visuomotor delay during a dynamic game of pong. After the game, participants exhibited hypermetric reaching and tracking movements indicating that delay is represented as a visuomotor gain rather than as a temporal shift.
Introduction
It is unclear whether the brain represents time explicitly (Karniel, 2011) using “neural clocks” (Ivry, 1996; Spencer et al. 2003; Ivry and Schlerf, 2008). Evidence suggests that no such clock is involved in the control of movement: humans can adapt to force perturbations that depend on the state of the arm (position, velocity, etc.), but not to forces that are explicit functions of time (Karniel and Mussa-Ivaldi, 2003); also, time-dependent forces are sometimes treated as state-dependent (Conditt and Mussa-Ivaldi, 1999). Instead, for the timing of movements, the sensorimotor system may use the temporal dynamics of state variables that are associated with the performance of actions.
Time representation is important for sensory integration, movement planning, and execution. Sensory signals are characterized by different transmission delays (Murray and Wallace, 2011), and movement planning and execution require additional processing time. Therefore, to enable an organism’s survival, the sensorimotor system must account for these delays. The current literature is equivocal on how delays are represented. Humans can adapt to visuomotor delays (Miall and Jackson, 2006; Botzer and Karniel, 2013) and delayed force feedback (Witney et al. 1999; Levy et al. 2010; Leib et al. 2015; Avraham et al. 2017). However, delayed feedback biases perception of impedance (Pressman et al. 2007; Nisky et al. 2008, 2010; Di Luca et al. 2011; Kuling et al. 2015; Takamuku and Gomi, 2015; Leib et al. 2016), suggesting that the sensorimotor system has limited capability to realign the signals for accurate estimations of the environment (Ionta et al. 2014).
To understand how the sensorimotor system controls movements with nonsynchronized feedback, we examined the representation of visuomotor delay in an ecological interception task. Participants played a pong game and controlled a paddle to hit a moving ball. The paddle movement was either coincident or delayed with respect to hand movement (Fig. 1). Because the delay influences the distance between the hand and the paddle, its representation can be Time-based or State-based. In Time Representation, the player represents the actual time lag, whereas in State Representation, she uses current state variables and may attribute the distance between the hand and the paddle to a spatial shift, a minifying gain, or a mechanical resistance. Using Time Representation, the player would precede the movement of the hand by the appropriate time so that the paddle would hit the ball at the planned location. Instead, using State Representation, she would aim her hand to a more distant location.
Coping with delayed feedback is critical for forming internal representations in feedforward control. A thorough understanding of this process requires identifying delay effects on feedforward mechanisms of movement coordination. Such mechanisms can be isolated only in the absence of visual feedback. Previous studies suggested the Time-based (Rohde et al. 2014; Farshchiansadegh et al. 2015) and the State-based spatial shift (Smith and Bowen, 1980) and mechanical system (Sarlegna et al. 2010; Takamuku and Gomi, 2015; Leib et al. 2017) as candidate representation models for visuomotor delay (Rohde and Ernst, 2016). They probed delay effects on perception, or by observing action during adaptation and its aftereffects, but always with visual feedback. Also, these studies evaluated the representation using a single task. Another common approach to characterizing changes in internal representations is to examine transfer of adaptation (Shadmehr and Mussa-Ivaldi, 1994; Krakauer et al. 2006). Although various terminologies are used in different fields, we define transfer as a change in performance in one task after experiencing another task. We explored visuomotor delay representation in the pong game by investigating its transfer to blind reaching and tracking tasks. Transfer to these well-understood movements allowed for comparing our experimental observations to simulations of the four representation models. By omitting the visual feedback, we could examine performance when participants had to rely solely on feedforward control and proprioceptive feedback. Thus, our transfer tasks enabled capturing visuomotor delay representation in feedforward mechanisms.
An abrupt or gradual perturbation schedule was shown to affect transfer of adaptation. Specifically, stronger transfers were reported after gradual presentations (Kluzik et al. 2008; Torres-Oviedo and Bastian, 2012), possibly owing to the influences of awareness (Kluzik et al. 2008) and credit-assignment (Berniker and Kording, 2008). We hypothesized that a gradual rather than an abrupt increase in the delay during the game would enhance the behavioral effects in our transfer tasks.
Our simulations and experimental results suggest a state-based visuomotor delay representation that is not influenced by perturbation schedule. Particularly, performance changes in both pong and transfer tasks favor a delay representation as a gain change in visuomotor mapping.
Methods
Notations
We use lowercase letters for scalars, lowercase bold letters for vectors, and uppercase bold letters for matrices. x is the Cartesian space position vector, with x and y the position coordinates (for the right-left/frontal and forward-backward/sagittal planes, respectively). f is the force vector, with fx and fy force coordinates. n indicates the number of participants in a group. Superscript lowercase letters refer to Table 1.
Table 1.
Location | Data structure | Type of test | Power/confidence interval |
---|---|---|---|
a | Normal distribution | Two-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 1.000 |
b | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [–0.136 0.062] |
c | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [–0.103 0.096] |
d | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [–0.026 0.093] |
e | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [0.153 0.340] |
f | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [–0.120 –0.08] |
g | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [0.088 0.276] |
h | Normal distribution | Unpaired-sample t test | [–0.384 –0.089] |
i | Normal distribution | Three-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.529 |
j | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [0.470 2.925] |
k | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [–1.427 1.176] |
l | Normal distribution | Three-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 1.000 |
m | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [1.940 3.936] |
n | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [2.048 3.400] |
o | Normal distribution | Three-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.977 |
p | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [–2.917 –0.549] |
q | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [–1.163 0.921] |
r | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [–1.243 0.236] |
s | Normal distribution | Three-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.093 |
t | Normal distribution | Two-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 1.000 |
u | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [–0.185 –0.029] |
v | Normal distribution | Unpaired-sample t test | [0.200 0.395] |
w | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [0.060 0.206] |
x | Normal distribution | Three-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.988 |
y | Normal distribution | Three-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.214 |
z | Normal distribution | Three-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.463 |
aa | Normal distribution | Three-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.216 |
ab | Normal distribution | Three-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.080 |
ac | Normal distribution | Two-way mixed effect ANOVA | 0.096 |
ad | Normal distribution | Two-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.366 |
ae | Normal distribution | Two-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.467 |
af | Normal distribution | Two-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.395 |
ag | Normal distribution | Two-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.872 |
ah | Normal distribution | Two-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.846 |
ai | Normal distribution | Two-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.829 |
aj | Normal distribution | Two-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 1.000 |
ak | Normal distribution | Two-way mixed-effect ANOVA | 0.071 |
al | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [0.006 0.027] |
am | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [0.045 0.227] |
an | Normal distribution | Paired-sample t test | [–3.078 –0.971] |
Experiments
Participants and experimental setup
Seventy-seven healthy volunteers (aged 19–41 years, 41 females) participated in four experiments: 17 in experiment 1, 20 in experiment 2, 20 in experiment 3, and 20 in experiment 4. All experiments were conducted after the participants signed an informed consent form approved by the Human Subjects Research Committee of Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Be'er-Sheva, Israel (Experiments 1, 3 and 4) or the Institutional Review Board of Northwestern University, Chicago, USA (Experiment 2).
The experiments were administered in a virtual reality environment in which the participants controlled the handle of a robotic device, a six-degrees-of-freedom Phantom Premium 1.5 haptic device (Geomagic; Experiments 1 and 4), a two-degrees-of-freedom MIT Manipulandum (experiment 2), or a six-degrees-of-freedom Phantom Premium 3.0 haptic device (Geomagic; experiment 3). Fig. 1a illustrates the experimental setup. Seated participants held the handle of the device with their right hand while looking at a screen that was placed horizontally above their hand, at a distance of ∼10 cm below their chin. They were instructed to move in a horizontal (transverse) plane. In Experiments 1, 3, and 4, hand position was maintained in this plane by forces generated by the device that resisted any vertical movement. The update rate of the control loop was 1000 Hz. Because the manipulandum is planar, this was not required in Experiment 2. In Experiments 1, 2, and 4, a projector that was suspended from the ceiling projected the scene onto a horizontal white screen placed above the participant’s arm. In experiment 3, a flat LED television was suspended ∼20 cm above a reflective screen, placing the visual scene ∼20 cm below the screen, on the horizontal plane in which the hand was moving. The hand was hidden from sight by the screen, and a dark sheet covered the upper body of the participants to remove all visual cues about the arm configuration. When visual feedback of the hand location was provided, the movement of the device was mapped to the movement of a cursor; when it was not perturbed by the delay, the cursor movement was consistent with the hand movement, with a delay of 5 (experiment 2) or 10 (Experiments 1, 3, and 4) ms because of the refresh rate of the display. The experimentally manipulated delay in the delay condition was added on top of this delay.
Tasks
Each experiment consisted of two tasks: a pong game task and another “blind” task. During the latter, no visual feedback about the hand location was provided. In Experiments 1 and 2, the blind task was a reaching task, and in Experiments 3 and 4, it was a tracking task. The purposes of the blind tasks were to examine transfer and capture the participants’ representation of the hand–cursor dynamics after exposure to either the nondelayed or delayed pong game.
Pong game
In the pong game, participants observed the scene illustrated in Fig. 1b. The rectangle delineated by the black walls (Experiments 1 and 3, [sagittal × frontal dimensions] 16 × 24 cm; Experiment 2, 17 × 34 cm; Experiment 4, 18 × 26 cm) indicates the pong arena. The red horizontal bar marks the location of the paddle and corresponds to the hand location. As described below (see Protocol), each experiment consisted of two Pong game sessions. We termed the first Pong session Pong No Delay, and the second Pong session Pong Delay. In the Pong No Delay session, the paddle moved synchronously with the hand. In the Pong Delay session, the paddle movement was delayed with respect to the hand movement: , where and are the positions of the paddle and the hand, respectively, and τ is the applied delay (note that for the Control group in Experiment 1 alone, the delay in the Pong Delay session was equal to zero, and hence, the dynamics between the hand and the paddle in this session was equivalent to the dynamics during the Pong No Delay session). To apply the delay, we saved the location of the hand in a buffer that was updated with the update rate of the control loop and displayed the paddle at the location of the hand τ time before it. τ was set to values between 0 and 0.1 s, depending on the protocol and the stage within the session. The green dot indicates a ball that bounces off the walls and the paddle as it hits them. The duration of each Pong trial was tTrial = 60 s. Information about the elapsed time from the beginning of the trial was provided to the participants by a magenta-colored timer bar. Feedback on performance in each trial was also provided using a blue hit bar that incremented according to the recorded paddle-ball hits from trial initiation onward. In Experiments 1, 3, and 4, during each trial, we updated the hit bar on every hit. The total amount of hits required to fill the bar completely was set to 80 in Experiment 1 and 60 in Experiments 3 and 4, and it remained constant throughout the entire experiment. In experiment 2, during each trial, we updated the hit bar every time the participants reached 5% of . During the Pong No Delay session, we set . After the last trial of the Pong No Delay session, we calculated each participant’s average hitting rate on that trial, , where nhit is the number of hits on the last trial of the Pong No Delay session. On the first trial of the second Pong Delay session, we matched the progression rate of the hit bar for each participant according to performance at the end of the Pong No Delay session, such that . Then, to encourage participants to improve, we decreased the progression rate of the hit bar by 5% on each successive trial.
The ball was not displayed between trials. The initiation of a trial was associated with the appearance of the ball in the arena. In Experiments 1 and 2, a trial was initiated when participants moved the paddle to the “restart zone,” a green rectangle (Experiment 1, 1 × 4 cm; Experiment 2, 2 × 10 cm) that was placed 3 cm below the bottom (proximal) border of the arena. Throughout the entire experiment, including the Pong Delay session, the paddle was never delayed between trials. Because the displayed paddle movement between trials was always instantaneous with hand movement, we were concerned that the effect of delay on state representation could be attenuated by a recalibration of the estimated hand location according to the nondelayed paddle. Thus, in Experiments 3 and 4, we did not display the paddle between trials, and participants were instructed to initiate a trial by moving the handle of the robotic device backward (toward the body). When the invisible paddle crossed a distance of 3 cm from the bottom border of the arena, the trial was initiated. In Experiments 1, 3, and 4, the initial velocity of the ball in the first Pong trial was 20 cm/s, and in every other Pong trial, it was the same as the velocity at the end of the previous trial. In experiment 2, the initial velocity of the ball in each Pong trial was 28 cm/s.
The participants were instructed to hit the ball toward the upper (distal) wall as many times as possible. When the ball hit a wall, its movement direction was changed to the reflected arrival direction, keeping the same absolute velocity (consistent with the laws of elastic collision). To encourage the participants to explore the whole arena and to eliminate a drift to stationary strategies, the reflection of the upper wall (and not the other walls) included some random jitter. Introducing the jitter effectively corresponded to creating a compromise between playing against a wall and playing against an opponent. This was done by adding the jitter component j to the horizontal component of the ball velocity before the collision with the upper wall , such that
(1) |
where is the horizontal component of ball’s velocity after the collision with the upper wall. In Experiments 1, 3, and 4, , where is the vertical component of ball’s velocity before the collision with the upper wall, and . μ and σ2 denote the mean and variance of the normal distribution N, respectively. In Experiment 2, , where U is the uniform distribution between its two arguments.
The velocity of the ball was also influenced by the paddle velocity at the time of a hit. We determined the relationship between the velocity of the ball after a paddle hit according to the velocity of the ball before the hit and the velocity of the paddle when contacting the ball . For the frontal dimension, the ball velocity after bouncing off the paddle was computed as
(2) |
For the sagittal dimension, we let a hit occur only when the paddle was moving upward and the ball was moving downward. In all other cases, the ball passed through the paddle as if it was moving over different planes. The rationale for allowing hits to occur only in the upward direction was to differentiate between the effects of the Time and State – Spatial Shift representation models. In our design, we assumed that a change in representation would occur primarily during meaningful events in the pong game; i.e., paddle–ball hits. Hence, allowing hits to occur in both the upward and downward directions could have cancelled the State Representation – Spatial Shift effect and would have restricted our ability to distinguish it from the Time Representation model. In this dimension, after a hit occurred, the ball’s movement direction was always reversed, and its velocity was computed as
(3) |
In our setup, the forward movement direction had a positive velocity, and the backward direction was negative. Note that because a hit occurred only when was negative and was positive, the resulting was always positive. This way, the ball movement direction after the hit was reversed, and it moved toward the upper wall. A possible strategy to cope with the delay was to slow down, and thus, for the delay to be effective, we encouraged participants to maintain their movement velocity as much as possible during the game despite the change in delay. Therefore, we set the coefficients’ absolute values of and (Eq. 3) to be between 0 and 1, such that they would reduce the effect of these velocities on the velocity of the ball after the hit. Thus, to maintain the ball speed after the hit as it was before the hit or to make it faster, needed to be at least . In addition to the constraint on the paddle to move upward, participants were informed that they should control the paddle to move fast enough at the moment of a hit, otherwise the ball would slow down, reducing the number of opportunities to hit it.
Once participants hit the ball with the paddle, a haptic pulse was delivered by the device simultaneously with the displayed collision; that is, when the paddle was delayed, the pulse was delayed. This design was thought to strengthen the delay effect during the hit. The pulse was applied according to
(4) |
where mb is the ball’s mass and Δt is the duration of the applied force. The specific parameters of the magnitude and durations of the haptic pulses were tuned for each of the devices that were used in the different experiments such that a relatively similar haptic stimulation was applied despite the differences in the specifications of the devices. In Experiments 1, 3, and 4, mb = 0.15 kg, and we calculated as the maximum applied force according to a time interval of Δt = 0.025 s. However, the haptic pulse was applied for 0.05 s, in which it gradually and linearly increased from zero to for the first 0.025 s (because the update rate of the control loop in this setup was 1000 Hz, this is equivalent to 25 sample intervals) and then decreased back to zero in a similar manner for the remaining 0.025 s. In experiment 2, mb = 0.05 kg, and the force was applied during a single sample interval of Δt = 0.005 s.
Reaching
At the beginning of a reaching trial, the entire display was turned off, and the device applied a spring-like force that brought the hand to a start location, which was at the center of the bottom wall of the pong arena (that was displayed only during the pong trials) and 1 cm (Experiment 1) or 3 cm (Experiment 2) below it. A trial began when a target (a hollow square, 1.5 × 1.5 cm inner area) appeared in one of three locations in the plane, which were 10 cm (Experiment 1) or 12 cm (Experiment 2) from the start location in the forward direction, and separated from each other by 45° (Figs. 2a,b, 4a, and 6a). Throughout a reaching session, each of the three targets appeared 15 times in a random and predetermined order. The appearance of the target was the cue for the participants to reach fast and to stop at the target. During each reaching trial in the experiment, we defined movement initiation as the time when the hand was 3 cm from the start location (Experiment 1) or when the sagittal component of the hand velocity () exceeded 25 cm/s (Experiment 2). Movement stop was defined at 0.5 s after went below 10 cm/s (Experiment 1) or 0.2 s after it went below 15 cm/s (Experiment 2). After identifying that a reaching movement had been initiated and completed, the device returned the hand to the start location in preparation for the next target to appear. We used three types of reaching sessions that differed from each other in terms of the visual feedback provided to the participants (Fig. 2a,b). During the Reach – Training session, participants received full visual feedback on the hand location using a cursor (filled square, 1.5 × 1.5 cm) on the screen throughout the entire movement. They were instructed to put the cursor inside the hollow target. During the Blind Reach – Training session, the cursor was not presented during the movement, and participants were requested to imagine there was a cursor, and to stop when the invisible cursor was within the target. When they stopped, we displayed the cursor, providing the participants with feedback about their movement endpoint with respect to the location of the target. During the Blind Reach sessions that were presented after each of the Pong sessions, participants did not receive any visual feedback about their performance during or after the trial.
Tracking: figure-eight
At the beginning of a tracking trial, the entire display was turned off, and the device applied a spring-like force that brought the hand to a start location, which was at the center of the bottom wall of the Pong arena and 2 cm below it. During each trial, participants were asked to track a target (a hollow square, 1.5 × 1.5 cm inner area) that moved along an invisible figure-eight path (Fig. 2c). This path was constructed as a combination of the following cyclic trajectories in the 2D plane:
(5) |
where A = 8 cm is the path amplitude, and T = 5 s is the cycle time. The center of the figure-eight path was located 15 cm ahead of the location of the hand at trial initiation (start location). A trial began when a target appeared in one of five locations in the plane: in the center of the figure-eight path (15 cm ahead of the start location) or in each of the four sagittal extrema (∼9 and ∼24 cm ahead). Throughout a tracking session, the five targets appeared equally often and in a random and predetermined order. The appearance of the target was the cue for the participants to reach fast and stop at the target. Reaching initiation was defined as the time when either the frontal () or sagittal () components of hand velocity exceeded 10 cm/s. Reaching stop was defined as 0.5 s after both and went below 5 cm/s. When the reaching movement stopped, the target started moving along the figure-eight path until it returned to its initial location. The targets moved in the same direction along the path (as illustrated by the dotted arrow in Fig. 2c), regardless of their initial location. A trial was completed after the device returned the hand to the start location in preparation for the next target to appear. Each experiment included two types of tracking sessions that differed from each other by the visual feedback that was provided to the participants (Fig. 2c). During the Track – Training session, participants received full visual feedback on the hand location using a cursor (filled square, 1.5 × 1.5 cm) on the screen throughout the entire movement. They were instructed to keep the cursor inside the hollow target. During the Blind Track session, the cursor was not visible during the trial, and participants were requested to imagine there was a cursor, and to keep the imagined cursor within the moving target.
Tracking: mixture of sinusoids
At the beginning of the tracking trial, the entire display was turned off, and the device applied a spring-like force that brought the hand to a start location, which was 2 cm above the center of the Pong arena. This was followed by the appearance of a target (a hollow square, 1.5 × 1.5 cm inner area) above the start location. A trial began 2 s later with the movement initiation of the target along an invisible 1D path (Fig. 2d). This path was constructed as a mixture of five cyclic trajectories, all of which had the same amplitude (A = 2 cm), but each trajectory consisted of a different frequency () and phase ():
(6) |
In each trial, participants were asked to track the movement of the target. The duration of each trial was 2 min. The tracking path was the same across trials. Each experiment included two types of tracking sessions that were different from each other in terms of the visual feedback that was provided to the participants (Fig. 2d). During a Track – Training session, participants received full visual feedback of their hand location using a cursor (filled square, 1.5 × 1.5 cm) on the screen throughout the entire movement. They were instructed to keep the cursor inside the hollow target. During the Blind Track sessions, the cursor was not presented, and participants were requested to imagine there was a cursor and keep the imagined cursor within the moving target.
Protocol
Experiment 1
In each experiment, sessions alternated a pong game and a reaching task (Fig. 2a). Each Reach session consisted of 45 trials (15 for each target). An experiment started with a Reach – Training session. The purpose of this session was to familiarize participants with the reaching task. After training, participants were presented with the Pong No Delay session for ∼10 min. This was followed by a Blind Reach session (Post No Delay). Next, participants experienced the Pong Delay session for ∼30 min. In the Delay group (n = 9), we introduced a delay of τ = 0.1 s between hand and paddle movements on the first trial of the Pong Delay session, which remained constant throughout the entire session. In the Control group (n = 8), no delay was applied in the Pong Delay session. The Pong Delay session was followed by another Blind Reach session (Post Delay).
Experiment 2
In each experiment, sessions alternated a pong game and a reaching task (Fig. 2b). An experiment started with a Reach – Training session that consisted of six trials (two for each target) and familiarized participants with the reaching task. The next session was a Blind Reach – Training session that consisted of 45 trials (15 for each target). By providing visual feedback only after the movement ended, we aimed in this session to train participants to reach accurately to the targets when they did not have any visual indication of their hand location throughout the movement and to improve their baseline performance. After training, participants were presented with a Pong No Delay session consisting of 10 trials. This was followed by a Blind Reach session (Post No Delay) with 45 trials. Next, participants experienced a Pong Delay session consisting of 30 trials. In the Abrupt group (n = 10), we introduced a delay of τ = 0.1 s between hand and paddle movements on the first trial of the Pong Delay session that remained constant throughout the entire session. In the Gradual group (n = 10), we introduced a delay of τ = 0.004 s on the first trial of the Pong Delay session and gradually increased it by 0.004 s on every trial until the 25th trial of the session, when it reached to τ = 0.1 s; then, the delay was kept constant for the remaining five trials in the session. The experiment ended with another Blind Reach session (Post Delay) of 45 trials.
Experiment 3
In each experiment, sessions alternated a pong game and a tracking task (Fig. 2c). An experiment started with a Track – Training session that consisted of 30 trials (six for each target). The purpose of this session was to familiarize participants with the tracking task and train them on the predictable figure-eight path. After training, participants were presented with a Pong No Delay session consisting of 10 trials. This was followed by a Blind Track session (Post No Delay) that consisted of 15 trials (three for each target). Next, participants experienced a Pong Delay session consisting of 30 trials. The time course of change in delay throughout the Pong Delay session in the Abrupt (n = 10) and Gradual (n = 10) groups was the same as in Experiment 2. The experiment ended with another Blind Track session (Post Delay) of 45 trials.
Experiment 4
In each experiment, sessions alternated a Pong game and a tracking task (Fig. 2d). Each tracking session consisted of a single trial. An experiment started with a Track – Training session, followed by a Blind Track – Training session. The purpose of these sessions was to familiarize participants with the task. After training, participants were presented with a Pong No Delay session consisting of 10 trials. This was followed by a Blind Track session (Post No Delay). Next, participants experienced a Pong Delay session. The time course of change in delay throughout the Pong Delay session was the same as that of the Gradual groups in Experiments 2 and 3 for all participants (n = 20). The experiment ended with another Blind Track session (Post Delay).
Simulations of the representation models
To control movements, it is commonly accepted that the brain performs state estimation of the body using sensory feedback (Wolpert et al. 1998; Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008). Thus, to computationally formalize predictions of delay representation in the pong game, we assumed that the participants updated an estimate of the relationship between the hand location and the state of the visual feedback, the displayed paddle . For the Pong No Delay session, we assumed that participants estimated the hand movement as being aligned with the movement of the paddle, and thus
(7) |
For the Pong Delay session, the hand moved according to the hand–paddle relationship that was predicted by each of the representation models. A Time-based Representation of the delay would lead to an estimate of hand location that explicitly included the actual time lag (τ) between hand and paddle movements:
(8) |
where is the location of the paddle at estimated τ () time ahead (Fig. 1c, left panel). A State-based Representation of the delay may follow one of three alternative models (Fig. 1c, right panel): participants may represent the current location of the hand according to the current location of the paddle spatially shifted by as a result of the delay (Spatial Shift – the paddle is constantly behind the hand):
(9) |
Alternatively, participants may attribute the distance between the hand and the delayed paddle to an altered proportional mapping () between the movement amplitudes of the hand and the paddle (Gain – the paddle moves in a smaller amplitude with respect to the amplitude of the hand):
(10) |
Another State-based alternative is to use a Mechanical System equivalent. A possible representation is that the paddle is a damped () mass () that is connected to the estimated representation of the hand position with a spring ():
(11) |
Such an approximation is based solely on the current state, i.e., the position, velocity and acceleration of the paddle. One possible choice of parameters in this representation can be calculated by considering a Taylor’s series approximation of the expression in Eq. 8 around the position of the delayed paddle:
(12) |
Rewriting Eq. 11 as
(13) |
reveals that by choosing and Eqs. 12 and 13 are equivalent.
We constructed predictions about the way each of the delay representation models affects the performance during the blind transfer tasks by simulating the predicted movements of each type of blind transfer task. Because during the transfer tasks participants were requested to imagine that there is a cursor, we assumed that they performed the task in the visual space, estimating the location of the imagined cursor [] and attempting to place it in the target (which was either stationary during reaching or moving during tracking). Also, we assumed that the participants estimated based on the position of the displayed paddle [] during the former pong session such that . Thus, in our simulations, the hand moved according to the estimated relationship between the hand and the paddle. For the Post No Delay session, the simulated hand movement was based on a complete alignment between the hand and the paddle movements (Eq. 7). For the Post Delay session, the hand moved according to the hand–paddle relationship that was predicted by each of the representation models (Eqs. 8–10, 12).
Reaching
Reaching movements were simulated according to the minimum jerk trajectory (Flash and Hogan, 1985):
(14) |
where tf = 0.3 s was the movement duration; xh0 = yh0 = 0 cm, and xhf, yhf were the initial and final hand positions coordinates of the simulated reaching movement, respectively. For the Post No Delay session, we set xhf and yhf at the location of the targets in Experiment 1 such that the simulated movement amplitude was 10 cm. For the Post Delay, we simulated the predicted hand trajectories according to each of the representation models such that the imagined cursor/paddle would reach the target (Eqs. 8–10, 12). We chose the parameters that, when possible, produced the effects that were similar in magnitude to the effects that were observed in the experiment. For Time Representation of the delay, we presented the simulation results for (Eq. 8). For the State Representation models, the reaching movements for the Spatial Shift model were generated with the free parameter (Eq. 9) equal to 1.5 cm; the results for the Gain model were generated with the free parameter (Eq. 10) equal to 1.2; and the results for the Mechanical System model were generated with a free parameter of the Taylor’s series approximation (Eq. 12) equal to 0.1 s.
To present the simulation results (Fig. 4c) in a consistent manner with the presentation of the experimental results, we added noise to the endpoint of each simulated movement. The noise was drawn from a normal distribution with zero mean and 1 cm standard deviation. This noise is thought to correspond to the noise present in various stages of sensorimotor control (Franklin and Wolpert, 2011).
Tracking: figure-eight
Tracking movements were simulated for a complete single cycle of the target movement along the sagittal dimension of the figure-eight path (Fig. 7). Thus, each hand trajectory was simulated as a single sine cycle. Because accurate performance during such a task is very rare, participants may exhibit various tracking errors even during baseline. However, the predicted relative effects of the delay are valid regardless of baseline accuracy. Thus, for illustration purposes, we assume that during the Post No Delay session, the hand lagged behind the movement of the target (Rohde et al. 2014) by 0.2 s. For the Post Delay session, we simulated the effect of each delay representation model on the resulting hand movement. For Time Representation, we presented the simulation results for (Eq. 8; 0.7 s relative to baseline). For the State Representation models, the tracking movements for the Spatial Shift model were generated with (Eq. 9); those for the Gain model were generated with (Eq. 10); and the results for the Mechanical System model were generated with the values of , , and that fulfill and (Eq. 13). Note that we did not draw any conclusions from the magnitudes of the parameters in the representations; we chose parameters that resulted in an observable change in the hand trajectory due to the delay and that could illustrate the effects qualitatively.
Tracking: mixture of sinusoids
We simulated frequency responses for tracking movements in different frequencies to illustrate the predicted effect of delay representation as Gain and Mechanical System on frequency-dependent increase in movement amplitude (Fig. 9). The simulation was conducted for a target movement that had an amplitude of At = 2 cm for all movement frequencies (fr) within the range of . For an accurate baseline (Post No Delay) tracking performance, we simulated hand amplitude (Ah) that was equivalent to the target amplitude at all movement frequencies: Ah = At. For the Post Delay session, we calculated for the Gain model the predicted hand amplitude in all frequencies with . For the Mechanical System model, we used the Fourier transform of Eq. 12 (for the sagittal plane, which was the only dimension in which the target was moving) and calculated the transfer function of the hand-paddle relationship:
(15) |
Thus, the predicted hand amplitude for a target moving at an amplitude of At with a Mechanical System representation is
(16) |
The simulation results for this model were generated with . We presented the predicted frequency responses for both the amplitude in metric scale (Ah, in cm) and the decibel amplitude (DAh, in dB). We calculated the latter as DAh = 10 · log10(pow), where is the power associated with each movement frequency.
To illustrate the effect of baseline accuracy on the predicted amplitude for each model, we also simulated the frequency responses for the case of an increase in the baseline movement amplitude with an increase in the movement frequency (Foulkes and Miall, 2000). We presented the simulation for the function , which exhibits an increase in the examined range of ω.
Pong
We simulated frequency responses for the movements during the pong game to illustrate the predicted effect of all the representation models on changes in movement amplitudes due to the delay (Fig. 11c). To conduct the simulation, we averaged the frequency response profiles of the last four trials of the Pong No Delay session from a representative participant (Fig. 11b, black; see Data analysis: metrics); we used this mean profile as an example for a baseline frequency response in the pong game. For the Delay session, we used the same frequency response mean profile for both the Time and Spatial Shift models, as they are not associated with any change in the movement amplitude. For the Gain model, we calculated the predicted hand amplitude in all frequencies with . For the Mechanical System model, we used the same transfer function of the hand–paddle relationship as with the tracking – mixture of sinusoid simulation (Eq. 15) and calculated the predicted hand amplitude using Eq. 16 (this time, At in Eq. 16 is the baseline frequency response profile). The simulation results for this model were generated with . We presented the predicted frequency responses for the amplitude in metric scale and the frequency response difference profile between the Delay and No Delay sessions in decibel units.
Code accessibility
Matlab codes for the above simulations can be found as Extended Data and in the following GitHub repository: https://github.com/guyavr/StateBasedDelayRepresentation.git.
Data analysis
Metrics
Device position, velocity, and the forces applied were recorded throughout the experiments at 200 Hz. They were analyzed offline using custom-written Matlab code (MathWorks, RRID: SCR_001622).
Pong: hit rate
To examine performance in the Pong game, we analyzed the change in the paddle-ball hit rate throughout the experiment. As mentioned above, the ball changed its movement direction from down to up when it either hit the bottom wall of the arena or during a hit. Thus, we identified the number of hits offline by extracting the number of times the ball movement direction changed upward and its sagittal location was not at the bottom wall at the time of the change. Because the duration of each of the Pong sessions in Experiment 1 varied across participants, to analyze the changes in the average hit rate of all participants in each group, for each participant, we pooled the data of a session and divided it into bins of equal duration. The Pong No Delay session was divided into five bins, and the Pong Delay session was divided into 20 bins. Hit rate was calculated as , where nhit was the number of hits in a bin. In experiment 2, the duration of the Pong sessions was equal between participants and consisted of the same number of trials, each with a duration of tTrial = 60 s. Thus, in this experiment, hit rate was calculated as , where nhit was the number of hits in a trial.
Reaching: amplitude
For the purpose of data analysis, we defined movement onset at the first time the velocity exceeded 2% of its maximum value. Movement end time was set at 0.1 s after the velocity dropped below 5% of its maximum value; the reaching endpoint was thus defined as the hand location () at that time point. Reaching amplitude was calculated as the Euclidean distance between at movement onset and movement endpoint.
Tracking figure-eight: target-hand delay, slope, and intercept (Experiment 3)
As mentioned above, during each figure-eight tracking trial, the tracking task began immediately after the participant reached toward a target within the figure-eight path and stopped. Thus, we segregated the tracking movement from the reaching movement by defining tracking onset as the first sampled time point in which the target started moving.
To evaluate tracking accuracy, we calculated an R2 value for each tracking trial according to (Nagengast et al. 2009)
(17) |
where var is the variance of the expression in parentheses. In 12% of the individual Blind Track trials, the R2 was <0.6, and they were omitted from further analyses.
Because the pong game was two dimensional, we analyzed the effect of the game on both the and components of the hand movement that tracked the 2D target path (Eq. 5). To measure Target–Hand Delay, for each dimension, we calculated the cross-correlation between target and hand positions [ and for the frontal dimension, and and for the sagittal dimension] on each trial and found the lag for which the cross-correlation was maximal. Positive values of Target–Hand Delay indicate that the hand movement preceded the movement of the target. The purpose of this measure was to examine whether participants used a Time-based Representation to cope with the delay. If they did, the predicted effect would be an increase in the Target-Hand Delay from the Post No Delay to the Post Delay tracking session.
As illustrated in Figs. 7 and 8, we examined the relationship between the target and the hand during tracking by projecting the sampled position of each in a target-hand position space. Then, we fitted an ellipse to the data points with the following form (Fitzgibbon et al. 1999; Chernov, 2009):
(18) |
where xt and xh are the Euclidean space coordinates of the target and hand frontal movement direction in a single trial, respectively. The same was done also for yt and yh, the Euclidean space coordinates of the target and hand sagittal movement direction. Note that the figure-eight is constructed from a single frontal sine cycle and two sagittal cycles, but for each dimension we fitted a single ellipse for all the data points. Then, we extracted the slope and the intercept of the ellipse’s major line. To do this, we derived the coordinates of the center of the ellipse (ot,oh) according to
(19) |
The counterclockwise angle of rotation (θ) between the xt or the yt axis and the ellipse’s major line is
(20) |
The ellipse’s major line slope (smaj) and intercept (imaj) were calculated according to
(21) |
(22) |
The slope and intercept measures were used to assess how the State Representation of the delay takes place; an increase in the intercept suggests a representation of delay in the form of a Spatial Shift, whereas an increase in the slope is consistent with a delay representation as a Gain or a Mechanical System.
Tracking mixture of sinusoids: frequency response (Experiment 4)
To measure the hand amplitude for each of the main frequencies in the tracking movement, we calculated the periodogram power estimate for each hand trajectory using the Matlab function periodogram() and with a Hanning window [Matlab’s hann() function]. To obtain accurate estimates of the amplitudes in the sharp peaks of the discrete Fourier transform in our experiment, each hand trajectory vector (∼24,000 samples length) was padded with zeros to a vector length of 600,000 samples. Then, we extracted the five peak power estimates associated with each of the five frequencies in the target trajectory. The amplitude (Ah, in cm; Fig. 10b) was calculated from the power (pow) as . To examine the effect of delay, we calculated the decibel amplitude (DAh, in dB units; Fig. 10c) from the power as . Finally, we calculated the difference in DA between the Post Delay and the Post No Delay sessions (Fig. 10d).
Pong: frequency response (Experiment 4)
To measure the change in hand amplitude due to the delay during the Pong game, we calculated the fast Fourier transform (FFT) for each hand trajectory from the last four trials of each of the Pong No Delay and the Pong Delay sessions using the Matlab function fft(). Because the design of our pong game encouraged participants to repetitively hit the ball toward the upper wall of the arena, we focused our analysis on the sagittal component of the hand movement. Before the FFT calculation, each hand trajectory vector (∼12,000 samples length) was padded with zeros to a vector length of 300,000 samples. For each trajectory, we calculated the amplitude as where L is the length of the original hand trajectory vector (before the zero padding), and the decibel amplitude as for all movement frequencies. Then, for each participant, we averaged the frequency responses of the four trials in each stage. Visual examination of the responses revealed that participants were mainly moving within the [0.5 1.5] Hz frequency range, and therefore, we focused on the responses within this range [we also observed a low-frequency (<0.2 Hz) peak that is due to pauses during the game and is less interesting in terms of dynamic delay perturbation]. For each of the mean DAh frequency responses, we filtered the mean responses by calculating the centered moving average with a window size of 101 samples and found the maximum decibel amplitude and its corresponding frequency.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using custom-written Matlab functions, Matlab Statistics Toolbox, and IBM SPSS (RRID: SCR_002865). The raw data and custom software will be made available upon request from the corresponding author.
We used the Lilliefors test to determine whether our measurements were normally distributed (Lilliefors, 1967). For ANOVA models that included a within-participant independent factor with more than two levels, we used Mauchly’s test to examine whether the assumption of sphericity was met. When it was not, F-test degrees of freedom were corrected using the Greenhouse–Geisser adjustment for violation of sphericity. We denote the p-values that were calculated using these adjusted degrees of freedom as . For the factors that were statistically significant, we performed planned comparisons, and corrected for family-wise error using a Bonferroni correction. We denote the Bonferroni-corrected p values as .
In Experiment 1, to analyze the change in hit rate throughout the experiment for each of the Delay and Control groups, for each participant we calculated the mean hit rate of the last four bins in the Pong No Delay session (Late No Delay), and the first (Early Delay) and last (Late Delay) four bins in the Pong Delay session. Then, we fitted a two-way mixed-effect ANOVA model, with the mean hit rate as the dependent variable, one between-participants independent factor (group: two levels, Delay and Control), and one within-participant independent factor (stage: three levels, Late No Delay, Early Delay, and Late Delay).
In Experiment 2, to analyze the change in hit rate throughout the Pong Delay session and to compare the Abrupt and Gradual groups, for each participant we calculated the mean hit rate of the last five trials in the Pong No Delay session (Late No Delay), and the first (Early Delay) and last (Late Delay) five trials in the Pong Delay session. Then, we fitted a two-way mixed-effect ANOVA model, with the mean hit rate as the dependent variable, one between-participants independent factor (group: two levels, Abrupt and Gradual), and one within-participant independent factor (stage: three levels, Late No Delay, Early Delay, and Late Delay).
To analyze the effect of the delayed pong on reaching amplitude, for each participant we evaluated the mean reaching amplitude during the Post No Delay and Post Delay sessions. We fitted a three-way mixed-effects ANOVA model, with the mean reaching amplitude as the dependent variable, one between-participants independent factor (group: two levels, Experiment 1: Delay and Control, Experiment 2: Abrupt and Gradual), and two within-participant independent factor (session: two levels, Post No Delay and Post Delay; target: three levels, Right, Middle, and Left). Mauchly’s test indicated a violation of the assumption of sphericity for the main effect of Target on the reaching amplitude in experiment 2 [,] and for the Session and Target interaction effect [, ]. Thus, we applied the Greenhouse–Geisser correction factor to the Target factor’s degrees of freedom in the former () and to the Session-Target and the Session-Target-Group interactions’ degrees of freedom in the latter ().
To analyze the effect of the delayed Pong on the figure-eight tracking performance in Experiment 3, for each participant we evaluated the mean Target-Hand Delay, slope, and intercept measures for each movement dimension during the Post No Delay and Post Delay sessions. For each measure, we fitted a two-way mixed-effect ANOVA model, with the measure as the dependent variable, one between-participants independent factor (group: two levels, Abrupt and Gradual), and one within-participant independent factor (session: two levels, Post No Delay and Post Delay).
To analyze the effect of the delayed Pong on the mixture of sinusoids tracking performance in Experiment 4, for each participant we evaluated the decibel amplitude of the main five movement frequencies during the Post No Delay and Post Delay sessions. We fitted a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA model, with the decibel amplitude as the dependent variable, and two within-participant independent factors (session: two levels, Post No Delay and Post Delay; frequency: five levels). Mauchly’s test indicated a violation of the assumption of sphericity for the main effect of Frequency on the tracking amplitude [, ], and for the Session–Frequency interaction effect [, ].
We used a two-tailed paired-sample t test to examine the effect of the Post Delay on the Target-Hand delay in the tracking task of Experiment 4, and on the maximum decibel movement amplitude and its corresponding frequency (dominant frequency) in the Pong game.
Throughout this paper, statistical significance was set at the threshold.
Results
Experiments 1 and 2
Transfer of hypermetria after a delayed pong game to a blind reaching task suggests State rather than Time Representation of the delay
In Experiment 1, the Delay (n = 9) and the Control (n = 8) groups played two Pong sessions (Fig. 2a). To evaluate performance in the Pong game, we calculated the paddle-ball hit rate and analyzed its change throughout the experiment in both the Delay and Control groups (Fig. 3). The change in hit rate throughout the stages of the experiment was different between the groups (Stage–Group interaction effect: , a). The hit rate of the Control group, who did not experience a delay in both Pong sessions, remained the same throughout the experiment (Late No Delay – Early Delay: b; Late No Delay – Late Delay: c; Early Delay – Late Delay: d). However, as a result of the sudden presentation of the delay, the hit rate of the Delay group decreased drastically (e), and then increased with continued exposure to delay (f). Yet, they did not reach the same hit rate as during Late No Delay (g) or the Control group at the corresponding Late Delay stage (h). Thus, participants from the Delay group were able to improve their performance during exposure to the delay, but this improvement was mild, suggesting a difficulty in adapting to the perturbation.
Both the Delay and the Control groups performed sessions of a blind reaching task after the two Pong sessions (Fig. 2a, blue and orange frames). This enabled us to capture the representation of hand–paddle dynamics after exposure to either the nondelayed or the delayed Pong game where participants had to rely solely on a feedforward mechanism and proprioceptive feedback. Analysis of participants’ performance in the blind reaching task revealed that participants from the Delay group, but not the Control group, made longer (hypermetric) reaching movements after the delayed pong game. Fig. 4a presents the reaching endpoints—the locations of movement terminations—during the Post No Delay and Post Delay blind reaching sessions from a representative participant in each group. Whereas for the participant in the Delay group, Post Delay movement endpoints reached farther from the start location than the Post No Delay movements’ endpoints (Fig. 4a, left), for the participant in the Control group, the blind reaching movements from the Post No Delay and Post Delay sessions ended at around the same location (Fig. 4a, right).
We extracted the reaching amplitude from all movements in each session (Fig. 4b). Playing Pong in the presence of a delay affected reaching amplitudes [Session–Group interaction effect: , i]. For participants in the Delay group, the reaching amplitude significantly increased from the Post No Delay to the Post Delay session (Post Delay – Post No Delay: [mean difference, 95% CI], , j; Fig. 4b, left). A similar increase was not found in the Control group (, k; Fig. 4b, right). Overall, these statistical analyses suggest that the specific experience with the delayed pong caused the participants to perform larger blind reaching movements.
Analysis showed that participants made larger movements toward the right target than they did toward the other targets [main effect of Target: , l]. For both Delay and Control groups, the reaching amplitudes to the right target were larger than to the left (m) and to the middle (n) targets. In addition, for the right target alone [Target–Session interaction effect: , o], there was a statistically significant increase in movement amplitude between the Post No Delay and the Post Delay blind reaching sessions (p). No such differences were found for the left (q) and middle (r) targets. Importantly, these differences in reaching amplitudes between the targets did not stem from the applied delay [Group–Target–Session interaction effect: , s]. Thus, we reasoned that they stemmed from biomechanical differences in reaching toward different directions (Mussa-Ivaldi et al. 1985; Carey et al. 1996), the difficulty of reaching to visual targets without visual feedback of the hand, and potentially, insufficient training on this task. Therefore, in Experiment 2, we added an additional session at the beginning of the experiment to train the participants on the blind reaching task.
To understand which of the representation models depicted in Fig. 1c best accounted for the observed results, we simulated reaching movements toward targets for the Post No Delay and Post Delay conditions of the Delay group based on four models: Time Representation, State Representation – Spatial Shift, State Representation – Gain, and State Representation – Mechanical System.
The simulation results are presented in Fig. 4c. The Post No Delay endpoints were closely distributed around the target locations. For Time Representation of the delay, in which an estimate of the actual time delay was available ( in Eq. 8), the Post Delay endpoints were also distributed around the target locations and were not influenced by the value chosen for the estimated delay parameter . Hence, there was no parameter value in the Time Representation model yielding simulation results that were consistent with the reaching overshoot observed in the experimental results. In contrast, for all the State Representation models (the Spatial Shift, the Gain, and the Mechanical System), we identified parameter values that resulted in simulated Post Delay overshoots similar to the experimental observations. Thus, State Representation and not Time Representation appeared to be able to account for the increase in movement amplitude after the delayed pong task.
Hypermetria is comparable in the abrupt and gradual conditions
The group that experienced the delay in Experiment 1, which then exhibited hypermetric movements during transfer to a blind reaching task, was presented with an abrupt delay perturbation. Because adaptation through gradually increasing perturbations was shown to enhance transfer (Kluzik et al. 2008; Torres-Oviedo and Bastian, 2012), we hypothesized that presenting participants with a gradually increasing delay during the Pong Delay session would result in an increase in the reaching movement amplitude during the blind transfer task compared to the abrupt case. To test this hypothesis, we ran a second experiment (Experiment 2) in which we compared between a gradual (Gradual, n = 10) and abrupt (Abrupt, n = 10) presentation of the delay.
The analysis of the paddle-ball hit rate (Fig. 5) revealed that the change in the hit rate throughout the delayed pong session differed between groups [Group–Stage interaction effect: F2,36 = 18.546, t]. Participants in the Abrupt group improved their performance in the presence of the delay (u). In contrast, because the Gradual group did not experience an abrupt change in the delay, the mean hit rate of these participants was higher than that of the Abrupt group at the beginning of the Pong Delay session (pB < 0.001v). As the delay increased, there was a decrease in their performance (w). Altogether, although these results suggest that the Abrupt group adapted to the delay, owing to the increase in the delay in the Gradual protocol, which may conceal a possible tendency toward improvement, we cannot claim the same for the participants in the Gradual group.
Similar to Experiment 1, we examined transfer for each type of schedule of delay presentation to a blind reaching task after each of the Pong sessions (Fig. 2b, blue and orange frames). Analysis of participants’ performance in the blind reaching task revealed that regardless of whether the delay was presented abruptly or gradually, participants made larger reaching movements after the delayed pong game, and the effect size was similar between the two groups. Fig. 6a presents the reaching endpoints during the Post No Delay and Post Delay blind reaching sessions of a representative participant from each group. In both participants, whereas the Post No Delay movement endpoints reached close to the targets, the Post Delay movement endpoints overshot them. We analyzed the changes in reaching amplitude due to the delayed pong and compared the Abrupt and Gradual groups (Fig. 6b). Playing the delayed pong resulted in a significant increase in reaching amplitudes (main effect of the session: , x, [mean difference, 95% CI], [mean difference, 95% CI], 1.638 cm, [0.907 2.369]), and this effect was not different between the groups [Session–Group interaction effect: F(1,18) = 1.507, y; Fig. 6b]. These results suggest that the hypermetric blind reaching movements after the experience with the delayed pong were not influenced by the schedule of the delay presentation.
There was no significant difference in reaching amplitudes between the targets [main effect of Target: F(1.327, 23.887) = 3.228, z]. In addition, there was no difference in the change in reaching amplitudes throughout the experiment between the targets [Target–Session interaction effect: , pε = 0.304aa], and no difference between the Abrupt and Gradual groups [Group–Target–Session: , ab]. Thus, the increase in the blind reaching amplitudes following the delayed pong game was similar across the different targets.
Experiment 3
Transfer of hypermetria to a blind tracking task suggests State Representation as either a Gain or a Mechanical System equivalent rather than a Spatial Shift
Although the comparison between the blind reaching experimental and simulation results suggested that State and not Time variables were used to represent the delayed feedback, the blind reaching task has two limitations: (1) the increase in blind reaching amplitude after the experience with the delay indicated that the delay affected the representation of the state of the hand with respect to the paddle, but it may also have masked some extent of the time representation. Because the reaching task is mainly spatial, if there is a partial representation of the time lag, it cannot be identified on this transfer task. (2) The blind reaching cannot differentiate between the different types of State Representations. All three State Representation models—the Spatial Shift, the Gain and the Mechanical System—predict reaching overshoot after the delayed Pong.
Thus, to determine which model best explains delay representation, we conducted an additional experiment in which we examined transfer to blind tracking after each of the Pong sessions (Fig. 2c, purple and green frames). On each trial, a target moved along a figure-eight path and participants were required to track and maintain the imagined cursor within the target. Importantly, we designed the tracking task so that it would be predictive, and therefore could reveal any temporal components in the representation (for both the Time and Mechanical System Representation models; Rohde et al. 2014). To test whether the transfer is influenced by the schedule of delay presentation, the participants were again assigned to one of two groups: Gradual (n = 10) and Abrupt (n = 10), which were different from each other by the schedule of delay presentation during the Pong Delay session.
Fig. 7 presents the predicted blind tracking performance in a single dimension (e.g. sagittal) and for a complete single cycle of the figure-eight path during Post No Delay and Post Delay sessions for each of the representation models. The figure displays both the predicted target and hand position trajectories (upper panels) and the corresponding target-hand position space plots (lower panels). The latter panels depict the position of the hand as a function of the position of the target for each sample during the movement. We assume that during the Post No Delay session, the hand lagged slightly behind the movement of the target (Rohde et al. 2014). This relationship is equivalent to an ellipse in the target-hand position space that has a major axis with a zero intercept and a slope of 1. For the Post Delay session, if participants coped with the delay using the Time Representation, the movement of their hand would be shifted in time with respect to the movement of the paddle, by preceding the path according to the represented time lag. When viewed in terms of the relationship between hand and target, this would result in a wider ellipse in the target-hand position space, and the major axis of this ellipse was expected to overlap with the Post No Delay target-hand position space line. Alternatively, if participants represented the delay as a Spatial Shift, the entire path of the hand would be shifted farther away from the body of the participant relative to the target. This would result in an upward shift of the target-hand position space ellipse, and thus, a higher ordinate intercept value for its major axis with respect to that of the Post No Delay target-hand position space ellipse, but without any change in its slope. A representation of the delay as either a Gain or a Mechanical System would result in an increase in the hand amplitude from the Post No Delay to the Post Delay tracking session, which is equivalent to an increase in the slope of the target-hand position space line. Note that a representation of the delay as a Mechanical System would also result in a hand trajectory that would precede the target trajectory. Because both the hand lead and hand lag scenarios predict an increase in the width of the target-hand position space ellipse, we examined this temporal relationship using a cross-correlation analysis between the hand and target trajectories rather than based on ellipse fitting.
We analyzed participants’ blind tracking performance by examining the hand and target positional trajectories in both the frontal and sagittal dimensions of the movements. We evaluated the dynamics between hand and target movements by mapping the hand position to the target position for each sample, and by fitting an ellipse to the scatter of each trial. Fig. 8a presents examples of target-hand position space scatters and their corresponding fitted ellipses of a single participant from two blind tracking trials, one from a Post No Delay session (purple) and one from a Post Delay session (green), and for a single cycle in the sagittal dimension. The results demonstrate that the major axis of the Post Delay ellipse had a greater slope than that of the Post No Delay ellipse. This type of change in slope is consistent with both the Gain and Mechanical System representation models, but not with the Time or Spatial Shift representation models.
For a quantitative analysis of the dynamics between the hand and the target in each of the frontal (Fig. 8b) and sagittal (Fig. 8c) dimensions, we extracted three measures from each trial: the delay between the target and the hand (Target-Hand Delay), the intercept of the major axis of the ellipse, and the slope of the major axis. The Target-Hand Delay was evaluated by finding the lag for which the cross-correlation between the movements of the target and the hand was maximal. Positive values of Target-Hand Delay indicate that the hand movement preceded the movement of the target. The delayed Pong did not cause participants to precede their hand movement with respect to the target movement in the blind tracking task. In both the frontal and sagittal dimensions of the task, the mean Target-Hand Delay was not significantly different between the Post No Delay and the Post Delay blind tracking sessions (Table 2, Session main; [mean difference, 95% CI], frontal: ac, sagittal: ad; Fig. 8b,c, left). This suggests that participants did not use a Time Representation of the experienced delay. Furthermore, participants’ hands did not move farther away from the target in a consistent manner as a result of the experience of the delayed pong. There was no significant difference in the mean intercept between the Post No Delay and the Post Delay sessions (Table 2, Session main; frontal: −0.541, [−1.093 0.012]ae, sagittal: 0.608, [−0.070 1.286]af; Fig. 8b,c, middle). This suggests that it is unlikely the State Representation – Spatial Shift model can account for participants’ performance. In contrast, playing the delayed Pong game caused participants to execute longer hand movements during the blind tracking task. We found a significantly higher slope during the Post Delay than during the Post No Delay session (Table 2, Session main; frontal: 0.114, [0.046 0.182]ag, sagittal: 0.162, [0.061 0.262]ah; Fig. 8b,c, right). These results are consistent with both the State Representation – Gain and State Representation – Mechanical System models.
Table 2.
Measure | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Effect | Dimension | Target-hand delay | Intercept | Slope | |||
F(1,18) | p | F(1,18) | p | F(1,18) | p | ||
Session main | Frontal | 0.437 | 0.517 | 3.937 | 0.063 | 10.729 | 0.004 |
Sagittal | 2.919 | 0.105 | 3.195 | 0.091 | 9.924 | 0.006 | |
Group main | Frontal | 0.032 | 0.860 | 0.054 | 0.819 | 2.233 | 0.152 |
Sagittal | 0.693 | 0.416 | 1.152 | 0.297 | 0.487 | 0.494 | |
Session–group interaction | Frontal | 2.949 | 0.103 | 2.322 | 0.145 | 1.110 | 0.306 |
Sagittal | 0.104 | 0.751 | 1.668 | 0.213 | 1.156 | 0.296 |
For each of the target-hand delay, the intercept, and the slope measures, and for each of the frontal and sagittal dimensions of the tracking path, we fitted a two-way mixed-effect ANOVA model, with the measure as the dependent variable, one between-participants independent factor (Group: two levels, Abrupt and Gradual), and one within-participants independent factor (Session: two levels, Post No Delay and Post Delay). The reported values for each measure are the F ratio, with the corresponding factor and residuals degrees of freedom in parentheses (left column), and the corresponding p-value (right column). Bold text indicate significant effects.
We did not find an overall difference between the groups in any of these three measures (Table 2, Group main), and no difference in the influence of the delayed pong between the groups (Table 2, Session–Group interaction). These results suggest that similar to the transfer to reaching case (Experiment 2), the schedule of the delay presentation did not influence tracking performance.
Experiment 4
Transfer of hypermetria to a blind tracking task with different movement frequencies suggests State Representation as a Gain rather than a Mechanical System equivalent
The results of Experiments 1–3 could not dissociate between the Gain and the Mechanical System representation models. Both models could explain the increase in the movement amplitude during reaching and tracking. However, these models provide different predictions in terms of frequency and velocity dependence.
To illustrate the predicted effect of frequency on movement amplitude, we simulated the frequency response according to each of these representation models (Fig. 9). Since the Time and Spatial Shift representation models were not associated with any change in the hand amplitude, we focused our simulations solely on the State Representation models of the Gain and Mechanical System. Consider a task in which after each Pong No Delay and Pong Delay session, the hand blindly tracks a target moving along a sinusoid trajectory that has a specific amplitude of 2 cm, but which varies in its frequency. In the case of an accurate tracking performance in the Post No Delay session, the hand amplitude would be the same as the target amplitude at all movement frequencies (Fig. 9a, upper panel, magenta lines). For the Post Delay session, whereas hypermetria due to a Gain representation does not depend on movement frequency, hypermetria resulting from a Mechanical System representation is predicted to increase with frequency (Fig. 9a, upper panel, cyan lines). Another way of thinking about this prediction is considering both representations at different velocities. Although the Gain representation is not expected to depend on movement velocity, the Mechanical System representation should yield a velocity-dependent response. Higher frequencies for similar amplitudes of target motion should also result in faster movements of the target.
To test these predictions, in Experiment 4 we examined transfer to a blind tracking task in which the target was moving along a trajectory that was generated as a mixture of five sinusoids (Miall, 1996; Miall and Jackson, 2006), all having the same amplitude (2 cm) but each with a different frequency (0.23, 0.31, 0.42, 0.54, and 0.67 Hz) and with a different phase shift (Fig. 2d, magenta and cyan frames). This served to examine the effect of frequency on the delayed-induced hypermetria.
We analyzed participants’ blind tracking performance by examining the hand amplitude for each of the main frequencies in the tracking movements and compared the Post No Delay to the Post Delay sessions. Fig. 10a,b presents the tracking movements of the hand of a representative participant from each session and the frequency responses, respectively. This figure shows an overall increase in the movement amplitude between the Post No Delay and the Post Delay session and in all five main frequencies. Note that this participant exhibited an increase in the baseline (Post No Delay) movement amplitude with an increase in the movement frequency. This effect was also observed in other participants, and in a previous study that examined tracking of a target that moved in frequencies <1 Hz (Foulkes and Miall, 2000). Because of this effect, the Gain model predicts a nonconstant increase in the metric measure of the amplitude due to the delay (Fig. 9b). Although the predictions between models remain different, this makes them statistically and qualitatively less distinguishable. Therefore, we calculated the 10 · log10 of the resulting power (decibel amplitude, dB) for each movement (Fig. 9c,d, upper panels) and examined the difference between the Post Delay and the Post No Delay sessions to control for the baseline modulation in the amplitude that results from the increase in movement frequency (Fig. 9c,d, lower panels).
Playing the delayed Pong game caused an increase in tracking amplitude, where the magnitude of the increase did not depend on the movement frequency. An analysis of the tracking performance of all participants revealed a significant increase in movement amplitude from the Post No Delay to the Post Delay session (main effect of Session: , p = 0.006ai, [mean difference, 95% CI], ; Fig. 10c). Consistent with the results of Experiments 1–3, this effect suggests that the participants did not use either a Time or a Spatial Shift representation of the delay. As was mentioned above, we also found a significant effect of frequency on the tracking amplitude in both the Post No Delay and the Post Delay sessions [main effect of Frequency: , aj]. However, we did not find a dependence of the delay-induced hypermetria on movement frequency [Session–Frequency interaction effect: F(2.423, 46.040) = 0.132, ak; Fig. 10d]. These results are consistent with a representation of the delay as a Gain, rather than as a Mechanical System equivalent.
We also calculated the Target-Hand Delay measure using a cross-correlation analysis between the hand and target trajectories for each participant and during each of the Post No Delay and Post Delay sessions. The Target-Hand Delay is positive when the hand precedes the target. If participants had used Time Representation of the delayed feedback, their hand would have preceded the target movement to a greater extent during the Post Delay tracking session than during the Post No Delay session, and the Target-Hand Delay would have increased, regardless of its baseline level. To a lesser extent, a small increase in this measure would also be predicted by the Mechanical System representation (Fig. 7a). We found a significant decrease in the Target-Hand Delay from the Post No Delay to the Post Delay session [,al]. This result indicates that after the Post Delay tracking session, participants hand lagged farther behind the movement of the target with respect to the Post No Delay session, contrary to the predictions of the Time Representation and the Mechanical System models.
Hypermetria during the delayed pong game is consistent with the Gain Representation model rather than the Time, Spatial Shift, and Mechanical System models
The hypermetria observed in all the blind transfer tasks that we examined and the finding that its magnitude does not depend on movement frequency suggest that the nervous system constructs a feedforward representation of the delay as a Gain. To examine whether the representation is also reflected in the Pong game, we analyzed the frequency response of the sagittal position trajectories during the game and compared it to the frequency responses predicted by each of the representation models (Fig. 11). For a representative participant, both the sagittal position trajectories of the hand from the last Pong trial of each session (Fig. 11a) and the mean profiles of the frequency responses of the last four trials from each session (Fig. 11b) suggest that the participant increased the movement amplitude from the No Delay to the Delay session. The frequency responses show that the participant had a preferred frequency range of movement. Therefore, to illustrate the predicted effect of each representation model, we simulated frequency responses according to each of the models using the frequency response profile of the No Delay session around this frequency range ([0.5 1.5] Hz; Fig. 11c). Consistent with the simulations of the transfer tasks, the simulation results of the Pong game show that the Time and Spatial Shift models did not predict a change in the movement amplitude due to the delay; in contrast, the Gain model predicted a frequency-independent hypermetria, and the Mechanical System model was expected to result in hypermetria that increases with movement frequency. Such a response is expected to have a stronger effect on higher frequencies, and it might cause one of the higher frequencies to become dominant. Because the participant exhibited hypermetria that did not seem to increase with higher frequencies, his performance is consistent with the Gain representation model rather than all the other models that we tested.
All the transfer tasks that we used posed some constraints, such as movements in specific amplitudes or frequencies, which enabled us to examine the effect of the delay by controllably comparing the performance between the Post No Delay and Post Delay sessions. For example, the target movement of the tracking transfer task of Experiment 4 directed participants to move in the same five frequencies in both the Post No Delay and Post Delay sessions, and thus, we could examine the change in amplitude for each of these frequencies. In contrast, because of the less constrained nature of the Pong task, participants were not necessarily moving with the same specific frequencies between the nondelayed and delayed Pong sessions. Specifically, we found a significant decrease in the dominant movement frequency, which was defined as the frequency with which the movement had the highest amplitude [, am; Fig. 11d]. This means that the participants moved more slowly in the presence of delay, possibly to reduce the larger magnitude of the spatial disturbance that results from the online delayed feedback in faster movements. For the dominant movement frequency of each session, the respective movement amplitude (maximum amplitude) significantly increased [, an; Fig. 11e]; this effect is not consistent with both the Time and the Spatial Shift representation models. Moreover, the Mechanical System model predicts hypermetria that increases with movement frequency, and thus, this is the only representation that may result in an increase in the dominant movement frequency from the nondelayed to the delayed Pong session. Hence, the findings that the dominant movement frequency decreased owing to the delay while increasing its amplitude (Fig. 11f) favor the Gain model more than the Mechanical System representation of visuomotor delay.
Discussion
We exposed participants to delayed feedback in an ecological task: a pong game. After prolonged experience with the delay, regardless of whether the delay was introduced gradually or abruptly, their movements became hypermetric during subsequent blind reaching and tracking. Simulations suggest that this hypermetria was an outcome of a delay representation as an altered gain rather than as a temporal lag, a spatial shift, or a mechanical system equivalent.
Delay representation: time-based or state-based?
There is an inherent difficulty in deciphering the representation of delay because it is a temporal perturbation that causes spatial effects. For example, a visuomotor delay was shown to increase driving errors (Cunningham et al. 2001) and the size of drawn letters and shapes (Kalmus et al. 1960; Morikiyo and Matsushima, 1990). The ability to determine the representation of the delay in these ecological tasks is limited because of their complexity. Our experimental setup was not entirely natural—the task scene was in 2D and the manipulated objects were not real—and more natural setups are useful for ecological investigations (Jeannerod et al. 1995; Fourneret and Jeannerod, 1998). Nevertheless, the pong game is more complex and dynamic than the motor tasks that are usually employed to study the sensorimotor system: it is composed of multiple interception movements that start and end at various locations of the workspace, and the movement of the target (the ball) is altered by the paddle hits. To overcome the difficulty of extracting the change in representation from such a task, we examined transfer to simple and well-understood tasks. The hypermetria in the transfer tasks implies that the participants used a state-based representation of the visuomotor delay. This may also help account for the limited transfer of adaptation to delay to timing-related tasks (de la Malla et al. 2014).
Conversely, recent studies have reported evidence for a time-based representation of delay. In a tracking task, participants adapted to a visuomotor delay by time-shifting the motor command (Rohde et al. 2014), even in highly redundant tasks (Farshchiansadegh et al. 2015). Contrary to our ecological pong game, the tracking tasks in these studies were highly predictable. In addition, the reported time-shift was observed during adaptation and after perturbation removal with a single task. If our participants represented time, it only partially contributed to the adaptation and was not transferred to blind reaching and tracking. Similar temporal adjustments were also observed with delayed force feedback (Witney et al. 1999; Levy et al. 2010; Leib et al. 2015; Avraham et al. 2017); such adjustments may be based on the capability of sensory organs that respond to force, such as the Golgi tendon organ (Houk and Simon, 1967) or mechanoreceptors in the skin of the fingers (Zimmerman et al. 2014), to represent delay as a time lag.
Adaptation to delay versus a spatial shift
There is an apparent similarity between a visuomotor delay and a spatial shift. In previous studies of reach movements, both displaced and delayed feedback caused overshoots that were reduced after adaptation, and a surprise removal of the perturbations caused undershoots (Smith and Bowen, 1980; Botzer and Karniel, 2013). In those studies, participants were required to stop at stationary targets, whereas in the interception task of the pong game, movement endpoints were not constrained. Importantly, in Smith and Bowen (1980), the transfer to movements in the opposite direction was different: overshoot in displacement, and undershoot in delay. This is consistent with our claim that delay is not represented as a spatial shift.
Mechanical system representation of delay
A dynamic systems approach to the representation of visuomotor delay, and specifically a spring-damper-mass system, was suggested in previous studies (Sarlegna et al. 2010; Rohde and Ernst, 2016; Leib et al. 2017). Unlike our experiments that integrated blind transfer tasks to capture representational changes in feedforward control, the studies that found evidence for the mechanical system representation did so in contexts that included online visual feedback, which may have influenced the motor response (Botzer and Karniel, 2013; Cluff and Scott, 2013).
In studies of tracking tasks, due to the delay, participants changed their grip force control in accordance with the dynamics of a mechanical system (Leib et al. 2017), but the modulations vanished immediately on delay removal (Sarlegna et al. 2010). Thus, it is unclear whether these effects were the result of a change in an internal representation of hand–cursor dynamics or an online effect that could possibly be tied to perceptual illusions. The discrepancy between the grip force evidence and ours can also be explained by other results showing that anticipatory grip force adjustment is dissociable from trajectory adaptation (Danion et al. 2013).
In terms of kinematics, delay representation as a mechanical system should result in a frequency-dependent increase in hand movement amplitude and a lead of the hand with respect to the target (Rohde and Ernst, 2016). Studies of tracking with visuomotor delay observed an increase in task related movement errors (Tass et al. 1996; Sarlegna et al. 2010; Leib et al. 2017) that were modulated with frequency (Langenberg et al. 1998) and a hand-leading phenomenon (Hefter and Langenberg, 1998; Sarlegna et al. 2010; Leib et al. 2017). Because the errors appeared in the presence of perturbed feedback, they could result from online correction attempts (Botzer and Karniel, 2013), and they highlight the difficulty of the sensorimotor system to interpret the delay as an actual time lag. Alternatively, they could have stemmed from changes in both tracking amplitude and the observed temporal phase shifts. However, these studies did not report changes in the movement amplitude due to the delay. Also, hand-leading was not observed in our blind tracking tasks, suggesting that this anticipatory behavior is not part of the feedforward representation of delay.
Recent studies have reported effects of delayed visual feedback on perception, including increased mass (Honda et al. 2013) or resistance (Takamuku and Gomi, 2015), that are suggestive of a mechanical system representation. The anecdotal verbal responses of our participants that the paddle is “harder to maneuver,” “sluggish,” or “mechanical” are consistent with this view and with previous reports (Smith, 1972; Vercher and Gauthier, 1992). Our findings that the transfer of delay effects is not consistent with explicit reports may stem from the separate processing of visual information for perception and action (Goodale and Milner, 1992).
We considered a representation of a spring-mass-damper that is computationally derived from a Taylor’s series approximation of the delay, and its representational effect is predicted to depend on frequency. In fact, our results of frequency-independent hypermetria are inconsistent with any mechanical system whose gain depends on frequency within the range that we examined; other classes of mechanical systems could yield a frequency-independent response, and these would still be consistent with our gain model. Yet, changes in hypermetria with frequency could appear for larger movement frequencies, for longer delays or after longer experience. Future studies should examine these possibilities.
Representation of delay as an altered gain
Our finding that hypermetria during tracking does not depend on movement frequency is consistent with a delay representation as a gain change in visuomotor mapping. Indeed, gain and delay perturbations have several common features: for both of them, the target and cursor locations at movement onset are unaltered, and the aftereffects are similar (Krakauer et al. 2000; Paz et al. 2005). However, the way the magnitude of the spatial effects depends on the movement is different: the effects of delay depend on velocity, and the effects of gain depend on movement amplitude. Indirect evidence for the relationship between gain and delay comes from interference studies. The interference paradigm shows that both successive (Krakauer et al. 1999; Tong et al. 2002; Caithness et al. 2004) or simultaneous (Tcheang et al. 2007; Sing et al. 2009) presentations of competing tasks disrupt learning and consolidation. Delayed visual feedback disrupts adaptation to visuomotor rotation and displacement (Held et al. 1966; Honda et al. 2012), but gain and rotation were not found to interfere with each other (Prager and Contreras-Vidal, 2003). This comparison suggests that gain and delay are processed and represented separately.
Nevertheless, our study provides direct evidence that gain may be used as a representation of delay. None of the previous studies that linked the reported effects of delayed visual feedback to a mechanical system representation (Sarlegna et al. 2010; Honda et al. 2013; Takamuku and Gomi, 2015; Leib et al. 2017) examined them in the context of different movement frequencies or velocities. Because a mechanical system is essentially a frequency-dependent gain together with a phase shift, evaluating the frequency dependence of the representation is critical for distinguishing between the two representations.
Similar transfer of adaptation between abrupt and gradual schedules
In both reaching and tracking, the strength of transfer did not depend on whether delay was introduced abruptly or gradually. Other studies have reported no difference in the influence of the schedule of perturbation presentation on motor learning of other types of perturbations, in either healthy (Wang et al. 2011; Joiner et al. 2013; Patrick et al. 2014) or impaired (Gibo et al. 2013; Schlerf et al. 2013) participants. In contrast, abruptly introduced perturbations were shown to strengthen interlimb transfer (Malfait and Ostry, 2004). Furthermore, gradually introduced perturbations strengthen aftereffects (Kagerer et al. 1997) and the transfer of adaptation to other contexts (Kluzik et al. 2008; Torres-Oviedo and Bastian, 2012). This was found despite the fact that for the same duration of adaptation and for the same maximum magnitude of the perturbation, participants experienced a smaller integral of the perturbation in the gradual compared to the abrupt protocol. In this sense, by comparing the transfer effects with respect to the overall experienced perturbation, and not with respect to its terminal/maximum value, the influence of the gradual presentation of the perturbation on transfer to another context can be considered stronger than the abrupt presentation.
In any case, differences between abrupt and gradual presentations of perturbations may be attributed to the presence or absence of an awareness of these perturbations (Kluzik et al. 2008). Awareness may affect the assignment of the perturbation to extrinsic rather than intrinsic sources (Berniker and Kording, 2008) and to elicit explicit rather than implicit learning (Mazzoni and Krakauer, 2006; Taylor et al. 2014). It may have been the case here that the delay was assigned to an intrinsic source, and that the adaptation to the delayed feedback was a result of an implicit process. This is likely because the brain naturally deals with intrinsic transmission and processing delays. However, this conjecture should be entertained with caution because we probed the delay representation before and after a prolonged exposure to the delay and, therefore, may have missed differences between the abrupt and gradual groups during adaptation.
The learning rule for adaptation to the delayed Pong
Although we saw an improvement in the hit rate in the groups that experienced an abrupt and constant delay, the effects were not strong. In addition, due to the dynamic nature of the gradual protocol, we did not find adaptation in the gradual groups. However, it is obvious in terms of the change in performance during both transfer tasks that an internal representation was indeed constructed during the participants’ experience with the delayed environment and independently of whether they improved in the game. Importantly, the findings that the participants were unable to regain their baseline performance during the delayed Pong game are likely a direct consequence of the failure to learn the true dynamics of the environment. Although previous studies that examined adaptation to visuomotor delays showed a slight improvement with prolonged training (Foulkes and Miall, 2000), participants could not return to their baseline performance even after 5 days of exposure to the delay (Miall and Jackson, 2006). In our pong game, only a full representation of the actual time lag between the hand and the paddle could have led to complete compensation of the perturbation and recovery of baseline performance.
The gain representation of the delay is reflected in the change of the participants’ performance during the game: with repeated exposure to the delayed Pong, participants increased the movement amplitude. In addition, they exhibited a decrease in the dominant movement frequency. The latter finding can be explained by the influences of the uncontrolled nature of the Pong game and the online visual feedback. The Pong task does not constrain the participants to continuously track a target that moves with specific frequencies, but it requires to estimate the future locations of the ball and the paddle at each interception attempt. Therefore, participants likely wait for the feedback for planning their next movement, thus reducing their movement velocity. This effect is consistent with evidence that humans slow down their movements when the feedback is delayed (Ferrell, 1965; Avraham et al. 2017), which effectively weakens the delayed-state dependent perturbation.
We did not deal here with the learning mechanisms involved in adaptation to the delay. Various measures can be used to examine adaptation in our Pong game (Sternad, 2006; Faisal and Wolpert, 2009; Reichenthal et al. 2016). Because participants were instructed to hit the ball as many times as possible within the time duration of each trial, and were provided with a feedback according to this performance measure, we reported their hit rate throughout the experiments. These hits can be considered as reward signals that influence future interception attempts in a reinforcement learning mechanism (Izawa and Shadmehr, 2011; Wolpert et al. 2011; Shmuelof et al. 2012; Nikooyan and Ahmed, 2015). If the adaptation is error-based (Thoroughman and Shadmehr, 2000; Donchin et al. 2003; Smith et al. 2006; Herzfeld et al. 2014), the candidate error signals need to be identified; for example, the distance between the hand and the paddle at meaningful events during the game such as ball-paddle hits. Further studies are required to understand how the state-based representation of the delay is constructed.
We assumed that the brain uses an estimation of the current position of the hand and updates it according to the delayed visual feedback; thus, for the gain model, it computes a proportional relationship between the hand and the paddle. Another solution that does not require estimation of current hand state is to update a threshold position (Pilon and Feldman, 2006), that is, set a desired position of the hand that is farther away; this would increase the emergent muscle torques that would bring the arm to the distant position. Also, delayed feedback tends to decrease stability (Milner and Cloutier, 1993), which in turn may change the impedance control of the arm (Burdet et al. 2001). However, this would not cause hypermetria, and such a process may occur in parallel to the update of the internal model (Franklin et al. 2003).
Representation of longer delays
The representation of visuomotor delay in the sensorimotor system may depend on the magnitude of the delay. Typically, delays in visuomotor integration processes range from 150 to 250 ms (Miall and Wolpert, 1995; Kawato, 1999; Franklin and Wolpert, 2011), and numerous results suggest that humans can cope with such internal delays through neural structures that predict the sensory outcomes of a motor command (Miall et al. 1998, 2001; Imamizu, 2010). The delays that were applied between the hand and paddle movements in our experiments did not exceed 100 ms. For the mean movement frequency that the participants exhibited in the game (∼1 Hz), this absolute delay magnitude is equivalent to a relative delay of ∼10% of the movement cycle duration, which was considered relatively easy to cope with in visuomotor tasks (Hefter and Langenberg, 1998; Langenberg et al. 1998). Thus, it was possibly small enough for the sensorimotor system to be able to adopt a current state-based approximation of the delay to moderately improve in the task. However, higher delays are likely to result in new coping strategies that suggest a time-based representation (Diedrichsen et al. 2007), such as using a delayed state (Witney et al. 1999). Another solution is the move-and-wait strategy (Sheridan and Ferrell, 1963; Ferrell, 1965) where participants move in a feedforward manner and stop to wait for the responsive visual feedback; after the delayed object that is being controlled starts to move, they execute an additional corrective movement. In fact, in the presence of longer delays (from 300 ms to 3.2 s), the total task completion time is longer (Sheridan and Ferrell, 1963; Ferrell, 1965). We believe that in the context of our pong game, such high delays would deteriorate performance even further, would break down the causal relationship between the motor command and the visual feedback, and would impede any form of representation.
Implications
Understanding delay representation in the sensorimotor system can be useful for understanding the motor consequences of delay-associated pathologies such as multiple sclerosis (Trapp and Stys, 2009). Also, this study opens a new prospect regarding to the role of temporal information in rehabilitation. Traditionally, rehabilitation tasks focus on spatial accuracy. However, reproducing temporal aspects of sensory feedback may improve rehabilitation and help recovering performance at different phases of movements (planning, preparation, and execution). Furthermore, our results may be useful in developing in-home rehabilitation procedures using virtual games and simple devices such as a computer mouse. The use of delayed visual feedback as a perturbation has several advantages: it encourages participants to exhibit longer movements, it has a strong transfer to different contexts, and it seems to be robust to explicit processes that would enable to maintain an improvement outside the clinic (Taub et al. 1999).
Understanding the relationship between temporal and spatial aspects of visuomotor coordination is important for the development of additional technologies, such as remote teleoperation (Nisky et al. 2013), brain–machine interfaces (Wolpaw et al. 2000), and prosthetics. The interaction with such systems should be improved by artificially reproducing the natural sensory consequences of the motor commands (Perruchoud et al. 2016) or by incorporating the necessary training if the latter is impossible. Because such systems include substantial feedback delays due to information transmission or processing, the development process of these technologies can benefit from accounting for the spatial aspects in the representation of these temporal discrepancies.
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgments: We thank Ali Farshchiansadegh and Dr. Felix Huang for their help in constructing the experimental setups, Matan Halevi for assistance in data collection, and Dr. Tirza Routtenberg for her valuable advice on signal processing.
Synthesis
Reviewing Editor: Christophe Bernard, INSERM & Institut de Neurosciences des Systèmes
Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Julien Lagarde
The reviewers and myself agree that your study is solid and that the topic is important for the understanding of sensorimotor functions. The question of delays is an important one and the literature of the topic is relatively limited. The aim at testing time delay versus spatial (gain), versus mechanical models of adaptation is a good contribution to the topic. We think that no additional experiment is required, but you need to clarify a few issues listed below.
Important remark.
Ethical notice: In case of acceptance, you must provide evidence of approval from Ethical committee, IRB.
Specific questions
THEORY
- Does the CNS need to estimate the hand position at all? Is there another solution?
- Are there state of the art references to approach a delayed dynamics (or control) with a mass spring with damping?
- Other approaches of delay in a dynamical system (or controlled dynamical system)
use delayed differential equations (DDE), hence infinite dimensional state space, not only a single past time of hand state (position and velocity) at tau, but the interval
from (t) to (t-tau), which by ordinary calculus brings an infinite number of past states perturbs the current state. How the framework of the present study fits with this point of view?
- Let us consider the paddle motion as a function of the hand motion:
xh(t+tau) is not the solution of dxp(t)/dt = F(xh(t-tau)), even for F linear
here: dxh(t) = H(xp(t+tau)), which is the future, hence requires a model of xp(t) enabling predictions. What is your opinion about this?
- The haptic feedback on the hand at contact paddle- target is not delayed. Does it resynchronize the visual consequences of hand movement and hand actual position afferences?
METHODS
- A complementary analysis of the behavior in the task could be envisioned; for example of the correlations of fluctuations? What would be the predictions of the 3 models with a stochastic version enabling estimates of power spectrum or autocorrelation function?
- Introduce the model simulations in the methods, develop, as it stands it is too implicit regarding the way Flash Hogan model is implemented here.
- the gradual delay is not very much expected from the introduction, it may be developed earlier why it is useful in the study.
RESULTS
- Figure 4 a: It seems to be an anisotropy in reaching, overshoot are larger to right targets.
This is not central to the study but you have any idea about this?
- Figure 7: Simulations of Gain vs Mechanical: the phasing is of opposite sign, leading for Gain, lagging for Mechanical.
Should the angle of the ellipses change accordingly? This would be expected as it doesn't seem to be changes in amplitude between the two models.
DISCUSSION
It would be important to add a paragraph in the discussion to disclose the possible implementation of the described procedures and obtained results in real life situations. For example, for clinical applications, it has been recently proposed that the improvement of sensorimotor rehabilitation protocols depends on the ability to artificially reproduce the natural sensory consequences of a movement (Perruchoud et al., 2016, JNE). In this vein, the present study could represent an important step forward to the understanding of the qualitative relationship between temporal and spatial aspects of visuomotor coordination. Thus it could provide insights into a number of open questions, including (i) which precise aspects (e.g. temporal vs. spatial) of sensory feedback need to be reproduced to improve rehabilitation, (ii) the relative weight of these aspects along the different phases of a movement (planning, preparation, execution, etc.), (iii) how to ameliorate acceptance and therefore restoration of sensorimotor functions in the context of biomedical engineering (e.g. neuroprosthetics).
780 “prolonged experience”: 2 x 60 seconds?
782 “simulations suggest”: this shows how important are those simulations. cf above, this must be described in more details in Methods
864: the present study didn't compared adaptation to Gain and to delay neither... If a non linear model is used instead of a linear one, does the prediction of phase and amplitude with frequency change?
Finally, can the gain or mechanical models could account for previous visuo-motor tracking studies (which were not adaptation studies then test in tracking tasks, but tracking task with delays), and the bifurcations observed when delay is increased. For example:
Hefter, H., & Langenberg, U. (1998). Sinusoidal forearm tracking with delayed visual feedback II. Dependence of the relative phase on the relative delay. Experimental brain research, 118(2), 171-179.
Tass, P., Kurths, J., Rosenblum, M. G., Guasti, G., & Hefter, H. (1996). Delay-induced transitions in visually guided movements. Physical Review E, 54(3), R2224.
Tass, P., Wunderlin, A., & Schanz, M. (1995). A theoretical model of sinusoidal forearm tracking with delayed visual feedback. Journal of biological physics, 21(2), 83-112.
Langenberg, U., Hefter, H., Kessler, K. R., & Cooke, J. D. (1998). Sinusoidal forearm tracking with delayed visual feedback I. Dependence of the tracking error on the relative delay. Experimental brain research, 118(2), 161-170.
Minor comments
Line 6 and throughout the manuscript - As the present study does not include brain imaging or neurophysiological data, the references to the brain mechanisms should be down-toned, re-edited, and/or removed.
Line 31-32 - If you intended to use a “whether [...], or ” form, the second part of the sentence seems to be missing.
Line 34-35 - Please consider moving the definitions of state- and time-dependent perturbations here. It will help the reader enter immediately in the context.
Line 41 - “the brain must take these delays into consideration”. Only the brain, or also the spinal cord, brain stem, different structures in the brain, etc.? As this is not a brain imaging study, avoid references to neural activity.
Line 47 - “the brain does not realign the signals”. Or maybe the brain has a limit, a threshold, above which it does not realign signals anymore (e.g. Ionta et al., 2014, SCAN)?
Line 53 - “in a more natural environment than was previously studied”. Please consider to explicit the main differences/advances brought by the present setup with respect to similar setups used in previous studies on visuo-motor coordination (e.g. Jeannerod et al., 1995, TIN; Fourneret and Jeannerod, 1997, NSY).
Line 67 - Please consider strengthening the rationale of the study. Why is it important? (the fact that something has not been done does not automatically mean that it is worth doing it).
Author Response
Dear editor,
In the following, we provide a point-by-point response to the reviewers' comments. Unless stated otherwise, line numbers and figures refer to the new version of the manuscript (the file with the tracked changes).
Throughout the text, we use the following font-coding:
• Original comments by the reviewers - regular font
• Our responses - italic font
• Citation from the paper - “bold italic font in quotation marks”
Synthesis of Reviews:
Synthesis Statement for Author (Required):
The reviewers and myself agree that your study is solid and that the topic is important for the understanding of sensorimotor functions. The question of delays is an important one and the literature of the topic is relatively limited. The aim at testing time delay versus spatial (gain), versus mechanical models of adaptation is a good contribution to the topic. We think that no additional experiment is required, but you need to clarify a few issues listed below.
Specific questions:
THEORY
- Does the CNS need to estimate the hand position at all? Is there another solution?
We thank the reviewer for raising this question. In our modeling, we follow the assumption that the CNS controls visually guided movements by updating an internal model of the relationship between a moving effector and the resulting visual feedback. This assumption is based on the literature that suggests that such an updating process enables adaptation to novel environments. To highlight this, we modified the sentences that introduce the computational modelling:
“To control movements, it is commonly accepted that the brain performs state estimation of the body using sensory feedback (Wolpert et al., 1998; Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008). Thus, to computationally formalize predictions of delay representation in the pong game, we assumed that the participants updated an estimate of the relationship between the hand location and the state of the visual feedback, the displayed paddle ...”
Indeed, the study of motor control has generated other theories about the way the brain controls movements that do not require estimate of the hand position. According to the threshold control theory (Feldman, 1966), for every intentional movement, there is a virtual position of the arm that precedes its actual position so that forces emerge in the muscles depending on the differences between the two positions. Also, previous studies suggested that the sensorimotor system may use impedance control that modulates the impedance of the neuromuscular system (Hogan, 1984), specifically when moving in unstable (Burdet et al., 2001) and unpredictable (Takahashi et al., 2001) environments. We added a paragraph that discusses these theories in light of our results to the Discussion section (The learning rule for adaptation to the delayed pong).
- Are there state of the art references to approach a delayed dynamics (or control) with a mass spring with damping?
Yes, there are. We generally mention in the introduction that a mechanical system representation of delay was suggested before: “Previous studies suggested the Time-based (Rohde et al., 2014; Farshchiansadegh et al., 2015) and the State-based spatial shift (Smith and Bowen, 1980) and mechanical system (Sarlegna et al., 2010; Takamuku and Gomi, 2015; Leib et al., 2017) as candidate representation models for visuomotor delay (Rohde and Ernst, 2016).”
Also, we now highlight that studies specifically examined visual delay representation as a mass-spring-damper system, and we extended the discussion on the differences between these and our study that provide possible explanations for the different conclusions. Specifically, and to address another comment below to strengthen the rationale of the study, we now emphasize that we isolated the feedforward representation of visuomotor delay (Discussion - 'Mechanical system representation of delay', and in 'Representation of delay as an altered gain').
- Other approaches of delay in a dynamical system (or controlled dynamical system) use delayed differential equations (DDE), hence infinite dimensional state space, not only a single past time of hand state (position and velocity) at tau, but the interval from (t) to (t-tau), which by ordinary calculus brings an infinite number of past states perturbs the current state. How the framework of the present study fits with this point of view?
Indeed, the delayed differential equations is a possible approach to represent the delay as an actual time lag (the Time Representation model). However, as the reviewer pointed out, it requires access to all the delayed states along the period between now and a certain time beforehand (defined by the magnitude of the actual delay), and thus an infinite number of state space in the continuous scenario, or a large number of states for a discrete case. Also, the iterative approach to solve differential equations might be energy consuming, and thus, it requires high amount of resources. Therefore, it is likely that this approach is less efficient to the nervous system than using a single past time of hand state. Either way, it might be that the complexity of such computations is a reason for our evidence that the system does not use Time Representation of the delay, but rather a Gain Representation that only requires the information about the current state.
- Let us consider the paddle motion as a function of the hand motion: xh(t+tau) is not the solution of dxp(t)/dt = F(xh(t-tau)), even for F linear here: dxh(t) = H(xp(t+tau)), which is the future, hence requires a model of xp(t) enabling predictions. What is your opinion about this?
In the experiment, the paddle motion is a function of the hand motion a certain time beforehand. We now explicitly present the equation that describes this relationship in the methods. To computationally formalize our predictions about each of the representation models, we assumed that the participants updated an estimate of the relationship between the hand location and the state of the visual feedback, the displayed paddle . Since the paddle is displayed according to the position of the hand in the past, a time-based representation is an estimate of the current hand location according to the paddle location in the future (Eq. 8): . Indeed, such a representation requires predictions. Therefore, it is possible that time representation may be used by the sensorimotor system only when the tasks are highly predictable (Rohde et al., 2014). This is why in our Experiment 3, we used a predictable tracking task for testing the representation. It is likely that because of the ecological nature of the pong game, the participants could not form predictions that are accurate enough to improve in the task, and the system utilized another solution (which is current state-based) that does not require estimations of future states.
- The haptic feedback on the hand at contact paddle- target is not delayed. Does it resynchronize the visual consequences of hand movement and hand actual position afferences?
Since the haptic feedback is provided at the time of paddle-ball contact, it is also delayed when the paddle is delayed. Thus, the feedback is provided when the hand is farther away from the paddle. We believe that the discrete events of paddle-ball hits are significant to the construction process of the representation: “In our design, we assumed that a change in representation would occur primarily during meaningful events in the pong game; i.e., paddle-ball hits.” Therefore, providing the haptic feedback together with the delayed visual feedback is thought to strengthen the hypermetria effect. We clarify it now in the Methods:
“Once participants hit the ball with the paddle, a haptic pulse was delivered by the device simultaneously with the displayed collision; that is, when the paddle was delayed, the pulse was delayed. This design was thought to strengthen the delay effect during the hit.”
METHODS
- A complementary analysis of the behavior in the task could be envisioned; for example of the correlations of fluctuations? What would be the predictions of the 3 models with a stochastic version enabling estimates of power spectrum or autocorrelation function?
We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. Following this comment, we added an analysis of the frequency responses of the participants' movements during the pong game and examined the way the movement amplitude was affected by the online delay feedback. Also, we simulated the predicted frequency responses according to each of the representation models and compared them to the experimental results. Both the simulation and the experimental results are presented in a new Figure 11 and are described in the Results section (Hypermetria during the delayed pong game is consistent with the Gain Representation model rather than the Time, Spatial Shift and Mechanical System models).
Note that we highlight now that the purpose of the paper was to extract the representation of the feedforward control of visuomotor delay that is experienced during a dynamic task (the pong game). This is the reason for using simple blind transfer tasks that eliminate any effect of online visual feedback and isolate the feedforward component. Therefore, even though the analysis results of the pong game are more consistent with the gain representation model than the other models that we tested, the decrease in movement frequency is not predicted by any of the representation models. We think that this decrease could be an effect of the task complexity or of a feedback response. We added a paragraph to the Discussion (The learning rule for adaptation to the delayed pong), which discusses the finding of this analysis.
- Introduce the model simulations in the methods, develop, as it stands it is too implicit regarding the way Flash Hogan model is implemented here.
We thank the reviewer for the comment. We edited the methods section and we now provide a detailed description of the simulations in the subsection “Simulations of the representation models”.
- The gradual delay is not very much expected from the introduction, it may be developed earlier why it is useful in the study.
We moved the paragraph that first describes the rationale for using the gradual perturbation paradigm from the methods to the introduction, following some editing.
RESULTS
- Figure 4 a: It seems to be an anisotropy in reaching, overshoot are larger to right targets. This is not central to the study but you have any idea about this?
As the reviewer noticed, the participants in Experiment 1 tended to make larger movements towards the right target than they did towards the other targets. This effect was also revealed in the statistical analysis (main effect of Target: , ). For both Delay and Control groups, the reaching amplitudes to the right target were larger than to the left ( ) and to the middle ( ) targets. In addition, for the right target alone (Target-Session interaction effect: , ), there was a statistically significant increase in movement amplitude between the Post No Delay and the Post Delay blind reaching sessions ( ). No such differences were found for the left ( ) and for the middle targets ( ). Importantly, these differences in reaching amplitudes between the targets did not stem from the applied delay (Group-Target-Session interaction effect: , ). Thus, we reasoned that they stemmed from biomechanical differences in reaching towards different directions (Mussa-Ivaldi et al., 1985; Carey et al., 1996), from the difficulty of reaching to visual targets without visual feedback of the hand, and potentially, insufficient training on this task. Therefore, in Experiment 2, we added an additional session at the beginning of the experiment to train the participants on the blind reaching task. Indeed, in Experiment 2, likely due to the training, there was no significant difference in reaching amplitudes between the targets (main effect of Target: , ). In addition, there was no difference in the change in reaching amplitudes throughout the experiment between the targets (Target-Session interaction effect: , ).
We thank the reviewer for pointing this out and we now explicitly mention the non-delayed specific differences in performance between the targets and the statistical analyses described above in the manuscript (Results section).
- Figure 7: Simulations of Gain vs Mechanical: the phasing is of opposite sign, leading for Gain, lagging for Mechanical. Should the angle of the ellipses change accordingly? This would be expected as it doesn't seem to be changes in amplitude between the two models.
Note that, as described in the paper, “for illustration purposes, we assume that during the Post No Delay session, the hand lagged behind the movement of the target (Rohde et al., 2014).” The Gain model does not predict any change in the phasing, and thus, the hand movement during the Post Delay is predicted to slightly lag the target (with a similar lag to that of the Post No Delay). However, for the Mechanical System model, the hand is predicted to lead with respect to the Post No Delay condition. Small phasing only changes the width of the ellipse and not the slope, as illustrated for the Time Representation ellipses - the Post No Delay hand lag the target and the Post Delay lead it, but both have the same slope. Large enough phasing would affect the slope, and even switch the minor and major axis, resulting in a negative slope; however, such cases were not observed in our data. Importantly, since an analysis of the ellipses does not capture the phasing direction, we used the cross correlation analysis for measuring both the size and the sign of the phasing.
For the range of phasing changes that were observed in our study, a change to the slope magnitude can only occur when there is a change in the amplitude. We chose to simulate similar amplitudes for the two models to highlight that the predictions are qualitative and that we do not draw conclusions from the specific parameters of each model.
DISCUSSION
It would be important to add a paragraph in the discussion to disclose the possible implementation of the described procedures and obtained results in real life situations. For example, for clinical applications, it has been recently proposed that the improvement of sensorimotor rehabilitation protocols depends on the ability to artificially reproduce the natural sensory consequences of a movement (Perruchoud et al., 2016, JNE). In this vein, the present study could represent an important step forward to the understanding of the qualitative relationship between temporal and spatial aspects of visuomotor coordination. Thus it could provide insights into a number of open questions, including (i) which precise aspects (e.g. temporal vs. spatial) of sensory feedback need to be reproduced to improve rehabilitation, (ii) the relative weight of these aspects along the different phases of a movement (planning, preparation, execution, etc.), (iii) how to ameliorate acceptance and therefore restoration of sensorimotor functions in the context of biomedical engineering (e.g. neuroprosthetics).
We thank the reviewer for the suggestions to include a discussion about the implications of our findings and for referring us to the interesting review of Perruchoud et al., 2016. We added an Implication section to the Discussion that describes the importance of understanding delay representation for the understanding of related pathological conditions, as well as possible implementation for rehabilitation and clinical technologies.
780 “prolonged experience”: 2 x 60 seconds?
The duration of each trial of pong was 60 seconds. The No Delay session consisted of 10 trials, whereas the Delay session consisted of ~30 trials. Thus, a “prolonged experience” refers to an experience of ~30 minutes with the delayed pong game.
782 “simulations suggest”: this shows how important are those simulations. cf above, this must be described in more details in Methods
We added a detailed description of the simulations in the Methods section.
864: the present study didn't compare adaptation to Gain and to delay neither... If a non linear model is used instead of a linear one, does the prediction of phase and amplitude with frequency change?
We thank the reviewer for identifying the inconsistency. We rephrased the sentence:
“Nevertheless, our study provides direct evidence that gain may be used as a representation of delay.”
We believe that the gain model is attractive due to its relative simplicity - the brain computes a scaling of the current position of the hand. However, as the reviewer mentioned, it might be that we could find a mechanical system (either linear or non-linear) that would be consistent with the predictions to the gain model. We mention this in the discussion:
“In fact, our results of frequency-independent hypermetria are inconsistent with any mechanical system whose gain depends on frequency within the range that we examined; other classes of mechanical systems could yield frequency-independent response, and these would still be consistent with our gain model.”
Finally, can the gain or mechanical models could account for previous visuo-motor tracking studies (which were not adaptation studies then test in tracking tasks, but tracking task with delays), and the bifurcations observed when delay is increased. For example:
Hefter, H., & Langenberg, U. (1998). Sinusoidal forearm tracking with delayed visual feedback II. Dependence of the relative phase on the relative delay. Experimental brain research, 118(2), 171-179.
Tass, P., Kurths, J., Rosenblum, M. G., Guasti, G., & Hefter, H. (1996). Delay-induced transitions in visually guided movements. Physical Review E, 54(3), R2224.
Tass, P., Wunderlin, A., & Schanz, M. (1995). A theoretical model of sinusoidal forearm tracking with delayed visual feedback. Journal of biological physics, 21(2), 83-112.
Langenberg, U., Hefter, H., Kessler, K. R., & Cooke, J. D. (1998). Sinusoidal forearm tracking with delayed visual feedback I. Dependence of the tracking error on the relative delay. Experimental brain research, 118(2), 161-170.
We thank the reviewer for refereeing us to these studies. We now discuss these findings together with studies that suggested mechanical system representation of delay (Discussion - Mechanical system representation of delay) and when discussing the predicted effects on longer delays (Discussion - Representation of longer delays)
Minor comments:
Line 6 and throughout the manuscript - As the present study does not include brain imaging or neurophysiological data, the references to the brain mechanisms should be down-toned, re-edited, and/or removed.
We replaced the word 'brain' with 'sensorimotor system' in line 6. Also, we made sure that throughout the manuscript we do not provide any decisive claim regarding specific brain mechanisms, but that the representation is attributed to the sensorimotor system in general.
Line 31-32 - If you intended to use a “whether [...], or ” form, the second part of the sentence seems to be missing.
We changed the sentence to the following:
“It is unclear if the brain represents time explicitly (Karniel, 2011) using ”neural clocks“ (Ivry, 1996; Spencer et al., 2003; Ivry and Schlerf, 2008).”
Line 34-35 - Please consider moving the definitions of state- and time-dependent perturbations here. It will help the reader enter immediately in the context.
We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. We rephrased the sentence that first relates to state- and time-dependent perturbations to clarify their meaning:
“humans can adapt to force perturbations that depend on the state of the arm (position, velocity, etc.), but not to forces that are explicit functions of time (Karniel and Mussa-Ivaldi, 2003);”
Note that the definitions provided later for the state- and time- based representations refer to the representation models that we tested for visuomotor delay.
Line 41 - “the brain must take these delays into consideration”. Only the brain, or also the spinal cord, brain stem, different structures in the brain, etc.? As this is not a brain imaging study, avoid references to neural activity.
We replaced the word 'brain' with 'sensorimotor system'.
Line 47 - “the brain does not realign the signals”. Or maybe the brain has a limit, a threshold, above which it does not realign signals anymore (e.g. Ionta et al., 2014, SCAN)?
We rephrased the sentence:
“...suggesting that the sensorimotor system has limited capability to realign the signals for accurate estimations of the environment (Ionta et al., 2014).”.
Line 53 - “in a more natural environment than was previously studied”. Please consider to explicit the main differences/advances brought by the present setup with respect to similar setups used in previous studies on visuo-motor coordination (e.g. Jeannerod et al., 1995, TIN; Fourneret and Jeannerod, 1997, NSY).
We thank the reviewer for referring to these studies. Indeed, the term 'natural environment' may relate to an experimental setup that either permits free hand movements and interactions with actual objects (Jeannerod et al., 1995) or a manipulation of light objects that are commonly used in our everyday life such as a stylus (Fourneret and Jeannerod, 1997). In that sense, our setup is not entirely natural since the participants in our study manipulated arms of robotic devices that have rather complex dynamics. However, using this term, we referred to the pong task itself being more complex and dynamic than the simple reaching and tracking tasks that are usually at the core to motor control studies that examine adaptation and learning of movement kinematics.
To avoid confusion, we chose to remove this sentence from the introduction. We also edited the paragraph that discusses this in the Discussion section (Delay representation - time-based or state-based?).
Line 67 - Please consider strengthening the rationale of the study. Why is it important? (the fact that something has not been done does not automatically mean that it is worth doing it).
We thank the reviewer for highlighting this lack of clarity on the rationale of the study. We modified the following sentence in the Introduction that relates to the gap in knowledge that is mentioned before about the equivocality on how delay is represented in the sensorimotor system:
“To understand how the sensorimotor system controls movements with non-synchronized feedback, we examined the representation of visuomotor delay in an ecological interception task.”
To further strengthen the rationale of our study, we modified the following paragraph in the Introduction. We believe it now highlights the fact that previous studies had focused on perception or on action in the presence of online visual feedback to understand delay representation, while our study examine which representation is reflected in the feedforward control of movement, thus, advancing towards a more complete understanding of visuomotor delay representation:
“Coping with delayed feedback is critical for forming internal representations in feedforward control. A thorough understanding of this process requires identifying delay effects on feedforward mechanisms of movement coordination. Such mechanisms can be isolated only in the absence of visual feedback. Previous studies suggested the Time-based (Rohde et al., 2014; Farshchiansadegh et al., 2015) and the State-based spatial shift (Smith and Bowen, 1980) and mechanical system (Sarlegna et al., 2010; Takamuku and Gomi, 2015; Leib et al., 2017) as candidate representation models for visuomotor delay (Rohde and Ernst, 2016). They probed delay effects on perception, or by observing action during adaptation and its aftereffects, but always with visual feedback. Also, these studies evaluated the representation using a single task. Another common approach to characterizing changes in internal representations is to examine transfer of adaptation (Shadmehr and Mussa-Ivaldi, 1994; Krakauer et al., 2006). While various terminologies are used in different fields, we define transfer as a change in performance in one task after experiencing another task. We explored visuomotor delay representation in the pong game by investigating its transfer to blind reaching and tracking tasks. Transfer to these well-understood movements allowed for comparing our experimental observations to simulations of the four representation models. By omitting the visual feedback, we could examine performance when participants had to rely solely on feedforward control and proprioceptive feedback. Thus, our transfer tasks enabled capturing visuomotor delay representation in feedforward mechanisms.”
We now also mention it in the abstract.
References
- Avraham G, Mawase F, Karniel A, Shmuelof L, Donchin O, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Nisky I (2017) Representing delayed force feedback as a combination of current and delayed states. J Neurophysiol 118:2110–2131. 10.1152/jn.00347.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berniker M, Kording K (2008) Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization. Nat Neurosci 11:1454–1461. 10.1038/nn.2229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botzer L, Karniel A (2013) Feedback and feedforward adaptation to visuomotor delay during reaching and slicing movements. Eur J Neurosci 38:2108–2123. 10.1111/ejn.12211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burdet E, Osu R, Franklin DW, Milner TE, Kawato M (2001) The central nervous system stabilizes unstable dynamics by learning optimal impedance. Nature 414:446–449. 10.1038/35106566 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caithness G, Osu R, Bays P, Chase H, Klassen J, Kawato M, Wolpert DM, Flanagan JR (2004) Failure to consolidate the consolidation theory of learning for sensorimotor adaptation tasks. J Neurosci 24:8662–8671. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2214-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey DP, Hargreaves EL, Goodale MA (1996) Reaching to ipsilateral or contralateral targets: within-hemisphere visuomotor processing cannot explain hemispatial differences in motor control. Exp Brain Res 112:496–504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chernov N (2009) Ellipse Fit (Direct method). http://www.mathworks.com/matlabcentral/fileexchange/22684-ellipse-fit–direct-method-. MATLAB Central File Exchange.
- Cluff T, Scott SH (2013) Rapid feedback responses correlate with reach adaptation and properties of novel upper limb loads. J Neurosci 33:15903–15914. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0263-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conditt MA, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (1999) Central representation of time during motor learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96:11625–11630. 10.1073/pnas.96.20.11625 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham DW, Chatziastros A, Von der Heyde M, Bülthoff HH (2001) Driving in the future: temporal visuomotor adaptation and generalization. J Vis 1:3. 10.1167/1.2.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danion F, Diamond JS, Flanagan JR (2013) Separate contributions of kinematic and kinetic errors to trajectory and grip force adaptation when transporting novel hand-held loads. J Neurosci 33:2229–2236. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3772-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Malla C, López-Moliner J, Brenner E (2014) Dealing with delays does not transfer across sensorimotor tasks. J Vis 14:8. 10.1167/14.12.8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Luca M, Knörlein B, Ernst MO, Harders M (2011) Effects of visual–haptic asynchronies and loading–unloading movements on compliance perception. Brain Res Bull 85:245–259. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diedrichsen J, Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Shadmehr R (2007) Dissociating timing and coordination as functions of the cerebellum. J Neurosci 27:6291–6301. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0061-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donchin O, Francis JT, Shadmehr R (2003) Quantifying generalization from trial-by-trial behavior of adaptive systems that learn with basis functions: theory and experiments in human motor control. J Neurosci 23:9032–9045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faisal AA, Wolpert DM (2009) Near optimal combination of sensory and motor uncertainty in time during a naturalistic perception-action task. J Neurophysiol 101:1901–1912. 10.1152/jn.90974.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farshchiansadegh A, Ranganathan R, Casadio M, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2015) Adaptation to visual feedback delay in a redundant motor task. J Neurophysiol 113:426–433. 10.1152/jn.00249.2014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrell WR (1965) Remote manipulation with transmission delay. IEEE Trans Human Factors Electr 24–32. 10.1109/THFE.1965.6591253 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgibbon A, Pilu M, Fisher RB (1999) Direct least square fitting of ellipses. IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Machine Intell 21:476–480. 10.1109/34.765658 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Flash T, Hogan N (1985) The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model. J Neurosci 5:1688–1703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foulkes AJM, Miall RC (2000) Adaptation to visual feedback delays in a human manual tracking task. Exp Brain Res 131:101–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fourneret P, Jeannerod M (1998) Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 36:1133–1140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franklin DW, Wolpert DM (2011) Computational mechanisms of sensorimotor control. Neuron 72:425–442. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franklin DW, Osu R, Burdet E, Kawato M, Milner TE (2003) Adaptation to stable and unstable dynamics achieved by combined impedance control and inverse dynamics model. J Neurophysiol 90:3270–3282. 10.1152/jn.01112.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibo TL, Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Okamura AM, Bastian AJ (2013) Cerebellar motor learning: are environment dynamics more important than error size? J Neurophysiol 110:322–333. 10.1152/jn.00745.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15:20–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hefter H, Langenberg U (1998) Sinusoidal forearm tracking with delayed visual feedback II. Dependence of the relative phase on the relative delay. Exp Brain Res 118:171–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Held R, Efstathiou A, Greene M (1966) Adaptation to displaced and delayed visual feedback from the hand. J Exp Psychol 72:887. 10.1037/h0023868 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Herzfeld DJ, Vaswani PA, Marko MK, Shadmehr R (2014) A memory of errors in sensorimotor learning. Science 345:1349–1353. 10.1126/science.1253138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honda T, Hirashima M, Nozaki D (2012) Adaptation to visual feedback delay influences visuomotor learning. PLoS one 7:e37900. 10.1371/journal.pone.0037900 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honda T, Hagura N, Yoshioka T, Imamizu H (2013) Imposed visual feedback delay of an action changes mass perception based on the sensory prediction error. Front Psychol 4:760. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00760 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houk J, Simon W (1967) Responses of Golgi tendon organs to forces applied to muscle tendon. J Neurophysiol 30:1466–1481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imamizu H (2010) Prediction of sensorimotor feedback from the efference copy of motor commands: a review of behavioral and functional neuroimaging studies. Jpn Psychol Res 52:107–120. 10.1111/j.1468-5884.2010.00428.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ionta S, Martuzzi R, Salomon R, Blanke O (2014) The brain network reflecting bodily self-consciousness: a functional connectivity study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9:1904–1913. 10.1093/scan/nst185 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivry RB (1996) The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control. Curr Opin Neurobiol 6:851–857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivry RB, Schlerf JE (2008) Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends Cogn Sci 12:273–280. 10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izawa J, Shadmehr R (2011) Learning from sensory and reward prediction errors during motor adaptation. PLoS Comput Biol 7:e1002012. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeannerod M, Arbib MA, Rizzolatti G, Sakata H (1995) Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends Neurosci 18:314–320. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joiner WM, Brayanov JB, Smith MA (2013) The training schedule affects the stability, not the magnitude, of the interlimb transfer of learned dynamics. J Neurophysiol 110:984–998. 10.1152/jn.01072.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kagerer FA, Contreras-Vidal JL, Stelmach GE (1997) Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions. Exp Brain Res 115:557–561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalmus H, Fry D, Denes P (1960) Effects of delayed visual control on writing, drawing and tracing. Language Speech 3:96–108. 10.1177/002383096000300205 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Karniel A (2011) Open questions in computational motor control. J Integr Neurosci 10:385–411. 10.1142/S0219635211002749 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karniel A, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2003) Sequence, time, or state representation: how does the motor control system adapt to variable environments? Biol Cybern 89:10–21. 10.1007/s00422-003-0397-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawato M (1999) Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 9:718–727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kluzik J, Diedrichsen J, Shadmehr R, Bastian AJ (2008) Reach adaptation: what determines whether we learn an internal model of the tool or adapt the model of our arm? J Neurophysiol 100:1455–1464. 10.1152/jn.90334.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krakauer JW, Ghilardi M-F, Ghez C (1999) Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching. Nat Neurosci 2:1026–1031. 10.1038/14826 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krakauer JW, Pine ZM, Ghilardi M-F, Ghez C (2000) Learning of visuomotor transformations for vectorial planning of reaching trajectories. J Neurosci 20:8916–8924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krakauer JW, Mazzoni P, Ghazizadeh A, Ravindran R, Shadmehr R (2006) Generalization of motor learning depends on the history of prior action. PLoS Biol 4:e316. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuling IA, Smeets JB, Lammertse P, Onneweer B, Mugge W (2015) Delays in admittance-controlled haptic devices make simulated masses feel heavier. PloS One 10:e0138023. 10.1371/journal.pone.0138023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langenberg U, Hefter H, Kessler K, Cooke J (1998) Sinusoidal forearm tracking with delayed visual feedback I. Dependence of the tracking error on the relative delay. Exp Brain Res 118:161–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leib R, Karniel A, Nisky I (2015) The effect of force feedback delay on stiffness perception and grip force modulation during tool-mediated interaction with elastic force fields. J Neurophysiol 113:3076–3089. 10.1152/jn.00229.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leib R, Karniel A, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2017) The mechanical representation of temporal delays. Sci Rep 7:article 7669. 10.1038/s41598-017-07289-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leib R, Mawase F, Karniel A, Donchin O, Rothwell J, Nisky I, Davare M (2016) Stimulation of PPC affects the mapping between motion and force signals for stiffness perception but not motion control. J Neurosci 36:10545–10559. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1178-16.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy N, Pressman A, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Karniel A (2010) Adaptation to delayed force perturbations in reaching movements. PloS One 5:e12128. 10.1371/journal.pone.0012128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lilliefors HW (1967) On the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test for normality with mean and variance unknown. J Am Stat Assoc 62:399–402. 10.1080/01621459.1967.10482916 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Malfait N, Ostry DJ (2004) Is interlimb transfer of force-field adaptation a cognitive response to the sudden introduction of load? J Neurosci 24:8084–8089. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1742-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazzoni P, Krakauer JW (2006) An implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy during visuomotor adaptation. J Neurosci 26:3642–3645. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5317-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miall R (1996) Task-dependent changes in visual feedback control: a frequency analysis of human manual tracking. J Motor Behav 28:125–135. 10.1080/00222895.1996.9941739 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miall R, Wolpert D (1995) The cerebellum as a predictive model of the motor system: a Smith Predictor hypothesis In: Neural Control of Movement. New York: Springer, pp. 215–223. [Google Scholar]
- Miall R, Jackson J (2006) Adaptation to visual feedback delays in manual tracking: evidence against the Smith Predictor model of human visually guided action. Exp Brain Res 172:77–84. 10.1007/s00221-005-0306-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miall R, Reckess G, Imamizu H (2001) The cerebellum coordinates eye and hand tracking movements. Nat Neurosci 4:638–644. 10.1038/88465 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miall R, Keating J, Malkmus M, Thach W (1998) Simple spike activity predicts occurrence of complex spikes in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Nat Neurosci 1:13–15. 10.1038/212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milner TE, Cloutier C (1993) Compensation for mechanically unstable loading in voluntary wrist movement. Exp Brain Res 94:522–532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morikiyo Y, Matsushima T (1990) Effects of delayed visual feedback on motor control performance. Percept Motor Skills 70:111–114. 10.2466/pms.1990.70.1.111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray MM, Wallace MT (2011) The neural bases of multisensory processes. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Hogan N, Bizzi E (1985) Neural, mechanical, and geometric factors subserving arm posture in humans. J Neurosci 5:2732–2743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagengast AJ, Braun DA, Wolpert DM (2009) Optimal control predicts human performance on objects with internal degrees of freedom. PLoS Comput Biol 5:e1000419. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000419 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nikooyan AA, Ahmed AA (2015) Reward feedback accelerates motor learning. J Neurophysiol 113:633–646. 10.1152/jn.00032.2014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nisky I, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Karniel A (2008) A regression and boundary-crossing-based model for the perception of delayed stiffness. IEEE Trans Haptics 1:73–82. 10.1109/TOH.2008.17 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nisky I, Baraduc P, Karniel A (2010) Proximodistal gradient in the perception of delayed stiffness. J Neurophysiol 103:3017–3026. 10.1152/jn.00939.2009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nisky I, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Karniel A (2013) Analytical study of perceptual and motor transparency in bilateral teleoperation. IEEE Trans Human-Mach Syst 43:570–582. 10.1109/TSMC.2013.2284487 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Patrick SK, Musselman KE, Tajino J, Ou H-C, Bastian AJ, Yang JF (2014) Prior experience but not size of error improves motor learning on the split-belt treadmill in young children. PloS One 9:e93349. 10.1371/journal.pone.0093349 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paz R, Nathan C, Boraud T, Bergman H, Vaadia E (2005) Acquisition and generalization of visuomotor transformations by nonhuman primates. Exp Brain Res 161:209–219. 10.1007/s00221-004-2061-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perruchoud D, Pisotta I, Carda S, Murray MM, Ionta S (2016) Biomimetic rehabilitation engineering: the importance of somatosensory feedback for brain–machine interfaces. J Neural Eng 13:041001. 10.1088/1741-2560/13/4/041001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pilon J-F, Feldman AG (2006) Threshold control of motor actions prevents destabilizing effects of proprioceptive delays. Exp Brain Res 174:229–239. 10.1007/s00221-006-0445-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prager AD, Contreras-Vidal JL (2003) Adaptation to display rotation and display gain distortions during drawing. Hum Movement Sci 22:173–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pressman A, Welty LJ, Karniel A, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2007) Perception of delayed stiffness. Int J Robot Res 26:1191–1203. 10.1177/0278364907082611 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Reichenthal M, Avraham G, Karniel A, Shmuelof L (2016) Target size matters: target errors contribute to the generalization of implicit visuomotor learning. J Neurophysiol 116:411–424. 10.1152/jn.00830.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rohde M, Ernst MO (2016) Time, agency, and sensory feedback delays during action. Curr Opin Behav Sci 8:193–199. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.029 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rohde M, van Dam LC, Ernst MO (2014) Predictability is necessary for closed-loop visual feedback delay adaptation. J Vis 14:4. 10.1167/14.3.4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarlegna FR, Baud-Bovy G, Danion F (2010) Delayed visual feedback affects both manual tracking and grip force control when transporting a handheld object. J Neurophysiol 104:641–653. 10.1152/jn.00174.2010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlerf JE, Xu J, Klemfuss NM, Griffiths TL, Ivry RB (2013) Individuals with cerebellar degeneration show similar adaptation deficits with large and small visuomotor errors. J Neurophysiol 109:1164–1173. 10.1152/jn.00654.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadmehr R, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (1994) Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task. J Neurosci 14:3208–3224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadmehr R, Krakauer JW (2008) A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Exp Brain Res 185:359–381. 10.1007/s00221-008-1280-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheridan TB, Ferrell WR (1963) Remote manipulative control with transmission delay. IEEE Trans Hum Factors Electron 4:25–29. 10.1109/THFE.1963.231283 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Shmuelof L, Huang VS, Haith AM, Delnicki RJ, Mazzoni P, Krakauer JW (2012) Overcoming motor “forgetting” through reinforcement of learned actions. J Neurosci 32:14617–14621a. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2184-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sing GC, Joiner WM, Nanayakkara T, Brayanov JB, Smith MA (2009) Primitives for motor adaptation reflect correlated neural tuning to position and velocity. Neuron 64:575–589. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith MA, Ghazizadeh A, Shadmehr R (2006) Interacting adaptive processes with different timescales underlie short-term motor learning. PLoS Biol 4:e179. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith WM (1972) Feedback: Real-time delayed vision of one’s own tracking behavior. Science 176:939–940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith WM, Bowen KF (1980) The effects of delayed and displaced visual feedback on motor control. J Motor Behav 12:91–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer RM, Zelaznik HN, Diedrichsen J, Ivry RB (2003) Disrupted timing of discontinuous but not continuous movements by cerebellar lesions. Science 300:1437–1439. 10.1126/science.1083661 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sternad D (2006) Stability and variability in skilled rhythmic action—A dynamical analysis of rhythmic ball bouncing In: Motor Control and Learning. New York: Springer, pp. 55–62. [Google Scholar]
- Takamuku S, Gomi H (2015) What you feel is what you see: inverse dynamics estimation underlies the resistive sensation of a delayed cursor. Proc R Soc B 282:20150864. 10.1098/rspb.2015.0864 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tass P, Kurths J, Rosenblum M, Guasti G, Hefter H (1996) Delay-induced transitions in visually guided movements. Phys Rev E 54:R2224. 10.1103/PhysRevE.54.R2224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taub E, Uswatte G, Pidikiti R (1999) Constraint-induced movement therapy: a new family of techniques with broad application to physical rehabilitation—a clinical review. J Rehab Res Dev 36:237. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor JA, Krakauer JW, Ivry RB (2014) Explicit and implicit contributions to learning in a sensorimotor adaptation task. J Neurosci 34:3023–3032. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3619-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tcheang L, Bays PM, Ingram JN, Wolpert DM (2007) Simultaneous bimanual dynamics are learned without interference. Exp Brain Res 183:17–25. 10.1007/s00221-007-1016-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thoroughman KA, Shadmehr R (2000) Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives. Nature 407:742–747. 10.1038/35037588 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tong C, Wolpert DM, Flanagan JR (2002) Kinematics and dynamics are not represented independently in motor working memory: evidence from an interference study. J Neurosci 22:1108–1113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torres-Oviedo G, Bastian AJ (2012) Natural error patterns enable transfer of motor learning to novel contexts. J Neurophysiol 107:346–356. 10.1152/jn.00570.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trapp BD, Stys PK (2009) Virtual hypoxia and chronic necrosis of demyelinated axons in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol 8:280–291. 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70043-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vercher J-L, Gauthier G (1992) Oculo-manual coordination control: ocular and manual tracking of visual targets with delayed visual feedback of the hand motion. Exp Brain Res 90:599–609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang J, Joshi M, Lei Y (2011) The extent of interlimb transfer following adaptation to a novel visuomotor condition does not depend on awareness of the condition. J Neurophysiol 106:259–264. 10.1152/jn.00254.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witney AG, Goodbody SJ, Wolpert DM (1999) Predictive motor learning of temporal delays. J Neurophysiol 82:2039–2048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpaw JR, Birbaumer N, Heetderks WJ, McFarland DJ, Peckham PH, Schalk G, Donchin E, Quatrano LA, Robinson CJ, Vaughan TM (2000) Brain-computer interface technology: a review of the first international meeting. IEEE Trans Rehab Eng 8:164–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpert DM, Miall RC, Kawato M (1998) Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn Sci 2:338–347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpert DM, Diedrichsen J, Flanagan JR (2011) Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:739–751. 10.1038/nrn3112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman A, Bai L, Ginty DD (2014) The gentle touch receptors of mammalian skin. Science 346:950–954. 10.1126/science.1254229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.