Abstract
We elucidated whether verticality misperception is associated with the generation of Pisa syndrome (PS) in patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD). To examine the heterogenous influence of verticality perception, we also identified the characteristics distinguishing between PD patients with PS who tilted toward the deviation of perceived verticality and those who did not. Subjective visual vertical (SVV) testing was performed in 54 PD patients with PS and 36 without PS to measure verticality perception. Other potential risk factors for PS were evaluated by assessing the asymmetry of motor symptoms, EMG activities of paraspinal muscles, bithermal caloric tests, back pain history, and Berg Balance Scale. Abnormal SVV (odds ratio (OR) 18.40, p = 0.006), postural imbalance (OR 0.71, p = 0.046), and unilateral EMG hyperactivity of paraspinal muscles (OR 39.62, p = 0.027) were independent contributors to PS. In subgroup analysis, EMG hyperactivity of paraspinal muscles contralateral to the leaning side and postural imbalance were associated with PD patients with PS who tilted toward the SVV deviation, whereas back pain was more frequent in those who did not. Verticality misperception is a potent risk factor for PS in PD and contributes differentially to PS depending on the congruence between its direction and PS direction, indicating distinct pathogenic roles.
Introduction
Pisa syndrome (PS), characterized by a lateral tilt of the trunk that can be alleviated by passive movement or recumbent positioning, is a disabling postural deformity in patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD)1–3. Several hypotheses, including asymmetric basal ganglia output, impaired sensorimotor integration, musculoskeletal problems, and dystonia, have been proposed to explain the occurrence of PS, reflecting its complex pathophysiology1–8.
PD patients with PS also show abnormal perception of verticality in the roll plane7,8. To maintain an upright posture, sensory information from visual, vestibular, and somatosensory systems is centrally integrated to provide information to align the internal representation of the body axis with the earth-vertical9. Accordingly, damage to this integrative procedure may alter verticality perception, leading to postural misalignment. Indeed, impaired verticality perception, measured with subjective visual vertical (SVV) tests, is associated with lateral body tilts in several neurological conditions, including pusher syndrome and lateral medullary infarctions10,11. In these patients, body tilts are observed in the same direction of pathological perceived verticality, suggesting a primary pathogenic role of verticality misperception10,11. However, perceived verticality in PD patients with PS has been reported to deviate either towards or away from the side of body tilts7,8. Furthermore, other potential risk factors for PS have not been considered in previous reports7,8. Thus, the pathophysiological implications of verticality perception in PD patients with PS remain unclear, making appropriate management of PS more challenging.
In this study, we aimed to determine the pathogenic role of verticality perception to the generation of PS. Given the various involvement of verticality perception in PD patients with PS, we hypothesized that its role in pathogenesis of PS might be different depending on the congruence between the direction of perceived verticality and that of PS. To address this issue, we also examined the characteristics discriminating PD patients with PS who tilted to the same direction of perceived verticality from those who did not.
Methods
Study participants
Through the Movement Disorders Clinic of Samsung Medical Center, we recruited 90 patients fulfilling the UK PD Society’s Brain Bank criteria for idiopathic PD12. All included patients received at least one dopaminergic medication at stable and optimal doses over the preceding 4 weeks. Subjects who met these criteria were excluded: (1) postural deformities other than PS, including camptocormia, anterocollis, and retrocollis; (2) dyskinesia hindering adequate evaluation; (3) exposure to medications of potential relevance to PS, including antiemetics, neuroleptics, antidepressants, and central cholinergic inhibitors; (4) ocular misalignment or visual loss; (5) history or clinical signs of vestibular disorders, including spontaneous nystagmus or corrective saccades during head impulse testing; (6) signs of somatosensory disturbances, including decreased response of deep tendon reflex; (7) history of spinal surgery or major orthopedic problems disclosed by spine X-ray; (8) dementia; (9) neurosurgical intervention; (10) comorbid neurological disorders possibly affecting posture, such as stroke. All assessments were performed during the on-medication state.
Motor disability was scored using the Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale motor score (UPDRS-III). The dominant side and phenotype of motor symptoms at disease onset were recorded. The asymmetry of motor symptoms was defined as the sum of differences between each lateralized score of 20–26 items from UPDRS-III at the time of examinations13,14. Clinical postural stability was estimated using Berg Balance Scale (BBS), a 56-point scale in which lower scores indicate worse balance15. Global cognitive functions were tested using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). We calculated levodopa equivalent daily dose (LEDD)16 and classified treatment regimens according to the use of levodopa or dopamine agonists. The presence of back pain was documented when it lasted >12 months with a severity of ≥5 on a visual analogue scale and disturbed the patient’s activities of daily living.
Patients were assigned to the PD with PS (PD-PS) group if they exhibited a lateral tilt of the trunk ≥10° that was reversed while supine or passively mobilized1,2. We determined the degree of a lateral trunk tilt while the subject was standing by measuring the angle between a vertical line on the wall and an imaginary line passing through markers placed at C7 and L4 (Figs 1 and 2). A body tilt was documented using a digital camera positioned 2 m behind the subject at a height of 1 m. PD-PS and PD patients without PS (PD-noPS) were matched for age and on Hoehn and Yahr stage ranging from 2 to 3.
Standard protocol approvals, registration, and patient consents
Written informed consent for both study participants and publication of identifying images/information in an online open-access publication was obtained from each participant prior to the study. This study protocol was in accordance with Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Samsung Medical Center.
Subjective visual vertical testing
The ability to perceive verticality was assessed using SVV tests17. Patients sat in a dark room viewing a rod (8.4-cm long, 0.4-cm wide) on a liquid-crystal display monitor positioned 1 m from their eyes. The rod was viewed through a hole (15 cm in diameter) in a black panel positioned in front of the monitor to prevent visual inference of verticality from the surroundings. Patients were instructed to sit upright, keeping their head upright. The rod was randomly presented at various angles from the vertical. Patients were asked to verbally instruct the examiner to move the rod into a vertical position. The examiner, who was unaware of the rod’s position, rotated the rod using a computer mouse until the patients said the rod was exactly vertical. The SVV value was determined by averaging the results from 10 trials under binocular viewing conditions. SVV was considered abnormal when it exceeded the normal range (mean ± 2 SD = −3.0 to 3.0°) obtained from 40 healthy control (20 men; mean age, 57.2 years [range, 45–72]). An abnormal SVV in the same direction of lateral trunk flexion was designated as ipsiversive SVV.
EMG analysis
We performed multi-channel EMG recordings using a conventional EMG machine (Viking IV, Nicolet Biomedicals, Madison, USA). EMG activity from bilateral paraspinal thoracolumbar (T12-L1)5 muscles were recorded with silver-silver chloride surface electrodes in the static position while patients stood with a relaxed posture. EMG signals were amplified and bandpass-filtered between 100 and 2000 Hz. EMG recordings were sampled for 60 seconds in 15 consecutive 4-second sweeps. EMG hyperactivity was defined as the presence of tonic EMG discharges, indicating sustained muscle contraction.
Bithermal caloric tests
Caloric stimuli were provided by irrigating each ear with cold (30.5 °C) and warm (43.5 °C) air. Asymmetries of vestibular response between ears were calculated using Jongkee’s formula and response asymmetries >25% were considered as canal paresis (CP)18. Unilateral CP on the same side of lateral trunk flexion was designated as ipsilateral CP.
Statistical analysis
SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC) and R 3.3.2 (Vienna, Austria; http://www.R-project.org/) were used for statistical analysis. The significance level was set at 0.05. To assess the association between potentially relevant factors and the presence of PS, we conducted univariable and multivariable logistic regression analyses using Firth’s penalized maximum likelihood estimation method. For multivariable analysis, we used variables with p < 0.2 in univariable analysis. Variables with a high level of multi-collinearity (variance inflation factors ≥4) were excluded from the multivariable model. To investigate the different influence of SVV on PS depending on the congruence between the direction of SVV and that of PS, we further divided PD-PS patient into two groups, PD-PS with ipsiversive SVV and PD-PS without ipsiversive SVV, then repeated the univariable and multivariable analyses.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to patient confidentiality but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Results
Association between abnormal SVV and the presence of PS
Demographics and clinical measurements are presented in Table 1. In our PD-PS, the lateral flexion of the trunk ranged from 12 to 22 degrees and increased with the degree of SVV tilt (r = 0.485, p < 0.001). The onset of postural deformity in PD-PS patients was insidious, lasting more than 3 months. Forty-five of PD-PS patients (83.3%) exhibited abnormal SVV (Table 1) and 34 of them (63%) deviated to the leaning side (Figs 1 and 3), whereas only one PD-noPS patient had an abnormal SVV. Unilateral EMG hyperactivity of paraspinal muscles was only observed in PD-PS patients (Table 1), which was uniformly contralateral to the leaning side (Fig. 3). The frequency of unilateral CP did not differ between PD-PS and PD-noPS patients (Table 1), and always ipsilateral when present in PD-PS patients (n = 8, 14.8%; Figs 2 and 3). The frequency of asymmetry of motor symptoms did not differ between PD-PS and PD-noPS patients, but 32 of PD-PS patients (59.3%) tilted toward the less affected hemibody (Fig. 3). Chronic back pain was more common and postural imbalance more severe in PD-PS patients compared with PD-noPS patients (Table 1). There were no differences in age, gender, disease duration or severity, motor subtype, dominant side of motor symptoms, LEDD, treatment regimens, body mass index (BMI), or MMSE between groups. Multivariable analysis revealed that abnormal SVV, postural imbalance, and unilateral EMG hyperactivity of paraspinal muscles were independently associated with PS.
Table 1.
PD-PS (n = 54) | PD-noPS (n = 36) | Univariable analysis* | Multivariable analysis† | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95% CI | p value | OR | 95% CI | p value | |||
Age (years) | 66.4 ± 6.4 | 66.9 ± 5.7 | 0.99 | 0.92 to1.06 | 0.761 | — | — | — |
Male gender (%) | 33 (61.1) | 18 (50) | 1.56 | 0.67 to 3.65 | 0.307 | — | — | — |
Disease duration (years) | 7.9 ± 3.4 | 8.0 ± 3.5 | 1.00 | 0.88 to1.13 | 0.959 | — | — | — |
Hoehn and Yahr stage | 2.2 ± 0.3 | 2.3 ± 0.3 | 0.70 | 0.20 to 2.53 | 0.589 | — | — | — |
UPDRS-III | 22.8 ± 5.8 | 22.9 ± 5.5 | 1.00 | 0.93 to 1.08 | 0.954 | — | — | — |
PD motor subtype (PIGD/TD) | 23/31 | 18/18 | 0.75 | 0.32 to1.74 | 0.498 | — | — | — |
Abnormal SVV (%) | 45 (83.3) | 1 (2.8) | 113.36 | 18.80 to 683.38 | <0.001 | 18.40 | 2.33 to 145.00 | 0.006 |
Asymmetry of motor symptoms | 5.5 ± 2.6 | 4.8 ± 1.3 | 1.17 | 0.95 to 1.43 | 0.137 | — | — | — |
Dominant side of motor symptoms (R/L) | 32/22 | 18/18 | 1.44 | 0.62 to 3.37 | 0.396 | — | — | — |
Canal paresis (%) | ||||||||
Unilateral | 8 (14.8) | 4 (11.1) | 1.32 | 0.37 to 4.71 | 0.669 | — | — | — |
No | 46 (85.2) | 17 (85.0) | — | — | — | — | — | — |
EMG patterns of paraspinal muscles (%) | ||||||||
Unilateral hyperactivity | 38 (70.4) | 0 | 170.34 | 9.50 to 3054.86 | 0.001 | 39.62 | 1.52 to 1035.36 | 0.027 |
Bilateral hyperactivity | 16 (29.6) | 36 (100.0) | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Back pain (%) | 25 (46.3) | 7 (19.4) | 3.40 | 1.28 to 9.00 | 0.014 | 3.36 | 0.60 to 18.71 | 0.167 |
BBS | 49.4±3.1 | 53.6 ± 1.5 | 0.48 | 0.35 to 0.66 | <0.001 | 0.71 | 0.50 to 0.99 | 0.046 |
LEDD (mg) | 593.7±218.0 | 604.0 ± 222.0 | 1.00 | 0.99 to 1.00 | 0.826 | — | — | — |
Treatment regimen (%) | ||||||||
Levodopa + dopamine agonist‡ | 44 (81.5) | 27 (75.0) | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Levodopa | 4 (7.4) | 6 (16.7) | 0.43 | 0.09 to 2.00 | 0.434 | — | — | — |
Dopamine agonist | 6 (11.1) | 3 (8.3) | 1.15 | 0.22 to 6.03 | 1.000 | — | — | — |
BMI (kg/m2) | 24.1 ± 2.6 | 23.4 ± 3.2 | 1.10 | 0.94 to 1.28 | 0.239 | — | — | — |
MMSE | 28.0 ± 1.9 | 27.9 ± 1.9 | 1.03 | 0.82 to 1.29 | 0.795 | — | — | — |
Education (years) | 11.6 ± 3.6 | 11.6 ± 3.9 | 1.00 | 0.90 to 1.12 | 0.962 | — | — | — |
*p values and 95% CI were corrected using Bonferroni’s correction for multiple tests. †Variables with p < 0.2 in univariable analysis were included in multivariable analysis. ‡Reference. BBS: Berg Balance Scale, BMI: body mass index, LEDD: levodopa equivalent daily dose, MMSE: Mini-Mental State Examination, PIGD: postural instability and gait disturbance, SVV: subjective visual vertical, TD: tremor dominant, UPDRS-III: Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale motor score.
Differences between PD-PS patients with and without ipsiversive SVV
Subgroup analysis showed that EMG hyperactivity of paraspinal muscles contralateral to the leaning side was more frequent in PD-PS patients with ipsiversive SVV than in PD-PS patients without ipsiversive SVV (Table 2 and Fig. 1). PD-PS patients with ipsiversive SVV also had more difficulty maintaining postural balance than those without ipsiversive SVV. Compared with PD-PS patients with ipsiversive SVV, chronic back pain was more common in PD-PS patients without ipsiversive SVV. The two groups did not differ in age, gender, disease duration and severity, motor subtype, degree and direction of PS, degree of SVV, asymmetry and dominant side of motor symptoms, frequency of PS tilting to the less affected hemibody, frequency of ipsilateral CP, LEDD, treatment regimens, BMI, and MMSE. In multivariable analysis, poor postural balance and EMG hyperactivity of paraspinal muscles contralateral to the leaning side were independently associated with PD-PS with ipsiversive SVV, whereas chronic back pain was negatively associated with PD-PS with ipsiversive SVV.
Table 2.
PD-PS with ipsiversive SVV (n=34) | PD-PS without ipsiversive SVV (n=34) | Univariable analysis* | Multivariable analysis† | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95% CI | p value | OR | 95% CI | p value | |||
Age (years) | 66.74 ± 5.4 | 65.95 ± 8.0 | 1.02 | 0.93 to 1.11 | 0.681 | — | — | — |
Male gender (%) | 20 (58.8) | 13 (65.0) | 0.79 | 0.25 to 2.46 | 0.679 | — | — | — |
Disease duration (years) | 7.7 ± 3.0 | 8.4 ± 4.1 | 0.94 | 0.80 to 1.11 | 0.462 | — | — | — |
Hoehn and Yahr stage | 2.2 ± 0.4 | 2.2 ± 0.3 | 1.65 | 0.29 to 9.42 | 0.574 | — | — | — |
UPDRS-III | 22.7 ± 6.4 | 23.1 ± 4.6 | 0.99 | 0.90 to 1.09 | 0.790 | — | — | — |
PD motor subtype (PIGD/TD) | 15/19 | 8/12 | 1.17 | 0.38 to 3.58 | 0.785 | — | — | — |
Degree of PS (°) | 15.4 ± 3.26 | 14.8 ± 2.75 | 1.07 | 0.88 to 1.29 | 0.513 | — | — | — |
Direction of PS (R/L) | 19/15 | 14/6 | 0.56 | 0.18 to 1.81 | 0.335 | — | — | — |
Degree of SVV (°) | 6.5 ± 3.4 | 5.1 ± 4.7 | 1.09 | 0.93 to 1.27 | 0.284 | — | — | — |
Asymmetry of motor symptoms | 5.6 ± 2.8 | 5.3 ± 2.3 | 1.05 | 0.84 to 1.30 | 0.689 | — | — | — |
PS tilting to the less affected hemibody (%) | 21 (61.8) | 11 (55.0) | 1.32 | 0.43 to 4.02 | 0.630 | — | — | — |
Dominant side of motor symptoms (R/L) | 22/12 | 10/10 | 1.80 | 0.59 to 5.52 | 0.304 | — | — | — |
Canal paresis (%) | ||||||||
Ipsilateral | 4 (11.8) | 4 (20.0) | 0.54 | 0.12 to 2.46 | 0.426 | — | — | — |
No | 30 (88.2) | 16 (80.0) | — | — | — | — | — | — |
EMG patterns of paraspinal muscles (%) | ||||||||
Contralateral hyperactivity | 28 (82.4) | 10 (50.0) | 4.38 | 1.27 to 15.11 | 0.019 | 5.76 | 1.04 to 31.95 | 0.045 |
Bilateral hyperactivity | 6 (17.6) | 10 (50.0) | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Back pain (%) | 11 (32.3) | 14 (70.0) | 0.22 | 0.07 to 0.72 | 0.012 | 0.21 | 0.05 to 0.95 | 0.043 |
BBS | 48.1 ± 2.4 | 51.6 ± 2.8 | 0.63 | 0.48 to 0.82 | 0.001 | 0.66 | 0.50 to 0.88 | 0.005 |
LEDD (mg) | 569.3 ± 197.5 | 635.1 ± 248.8 | 0.99 | 0.99 to 1.00 | 0.310 | — | — | — |
Treatment regimen (%) | ||||||||
Levodopa + dopamine agonist | 27 (79.4) | 17 (85.0) | 1.00‡ | — | — | — | — | — |
Levodopa | 3 (8.8) | 3 (15.0) | 0.64 | 0.09 to 4.50 | 1.000 | — | — | — |
Dopamine agonist | 4 (11.8) | 0 | 5.72 | 0.13 to 255.53 | 0.607 | — | — | — |
BMI (kg/m2) | 24.4 ± 2.7 | 23.7 ± 2.6 | 1.10 | 0.89 to 1.36 | 0.371 | — | — | — |
MMSE | 27.6 ± 2.0 | 28.6 ± 1.5 | 0.74 | 0.52 to 1.06 | 0.101 | 0.84 | 0.53 to 1.34 | 0.468 |
Education (years) | 11.4 ± 3.7 | 12 ± 3.5 | 0.95 | 0.82 to 1.11 | 0.546 | — | — | — |
*p values and 95% CI were corrected using Bonferroni’s correction for multiple tests. †Variables with p < 0.2 in univariable analysis were included in multivariable analysis. ‡Reference. BBS: Berg Balance Scale, BMI: body mass index, LEDD: levodopa equivalent daily dose, MMSE: Mini-Mental State Examination, PIGD: postural instability and gait disturbance, SVV: subjective visual vertical, TD: tremor dominant, UPDRS-III: Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale motor score.
Discussion
A vast body of literature has proposed numerous hypotheses regarding the pathophysiology of PS1–3. However, few studies systematically assessed perceptual alteration of verticality in PD patients with PS7,8, although defected body schema (i.e., misperception of the body in space) plays a key role in postural control deficits in PD19. Furthermore, other potential risk factors for PS have rarely been considered in previous reports, providing limited evidence for a pathogenic role of verticality misperception in PS. In fact, PD patients with PS have shown variable responses to treatments targeting a single pathophysiology of PS20. Accordingly, defining distinct properties correlated with patterns of verticality perception in PD patients with PS may lead to new therapeutic approaches for PS. This also helps promote the efficacy of those therapies by precisely identifying patients who will benefit from them. Our study shed some lights on these issues by confirming the independent pathogenic role of verticality misperception to PS and delineating the clinical heterogeneity of verticality perception in PD patients with PS.
We found that disruptive verticality perception in the roll plane was an important risk factor for PS. Our SVV adjustment tasks depend primarily on gravitational inputs from vestibular otolithic system and somatosensory graviceptors21,22. Accordingly, abnormal SVV in our study may result from impaired integration of gravitational signals from these two sensory systems. Thus, our finding is comparable to a recent study which demonstrated veering while walking with eyes closed, representing unbalanced processing of vestibular or proprioceptive feedbacks, as an independent risk factor for PS14. Our result is also consistent with a recent hypothesis which emphasizes sensory integration failure and consequent misperception of body schema as key factors leading to PS1,2. Additionally, pathomechanism of perceptual deficits in gravitational vertical can explain the marked improvement of PS in the supine position, in which the influence of gravity lessens1,2. As most of our PD patients did not exhibit sensory loss affecting peripheral apparatus, abnormal SVV may stem from dysfunction in central graviceptive network, in agreement with previous reports23–25. Central graviceptive pathway travels from the vestibular nuclei, either crosses or uncrosses in the brainstem, then reaches higher cortical and subcortical structures, engaging the posterior lateral thalamus, basal ganglia, insular cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, and inferior parietal lobule17,26. Damage to these structures is commonly associated with postural and locomotor deficits in PD patients by interrupting integration of multisensory information for ensuring spatial orientation19,27,28. Accordingly, asymmetric involvement of PD pathology might cause biased signal processing in graviceptive network, resulting in an abnormal deviation of perceived verticality. This can, in turn, align the longitudinal body axis with erroneous perceived verticality, consequently producing PS. Indeed, our PD-PS patients often tilted to the same direction of SVV.
We also identified the characteristics differentiating between PD-PS patients with ipsiversive SVV and those without ipsiversive SVV. PD-PS patients with ipsiversive SVV showed distinct properties, possibly supporting a primary role of verticality perception deficits in generation of PS. Even though postural balance was compromised in our PD-PS, consistent with a previous study29, PD-PS patients with ipsiversive SVV had more difficulty maintaining postural balance compared to those without ipsiversive SVV. This is likely due to primary adoption of altered verticality perception rather than compensation for progressive body inclination. In addition, more frequent sustained activity of paraspinal muscles contralateral to the leaning side might be interpreted as reflexive, but inefficient, muscle contraction to overcome postural instability. In contrast, PD-PS patients without ipsiversive SVV exhibited suggestive features of other underlying causes for PS. For instance, our PD-PS patients without ipsiversive SVV more frequently reported chronic back pain. Back pain may be attributable to postural deformities, by prompting protective posturing from further pain and restricting spinal motion3,14. Notably, this patient group maintained better postural balance than PD-PS patients with ipsiversive SVV, although the degree of SVV and PS were similar between groups. Presumably compensatory adjustment, rather than primary alteration, of verticality reference system might occur to restore postural balance after a body tilt from causative factors other than verticality misperception. Taken together, verticality misperception may be associated with PS in different pathomechanisms by participating in primary alteration or compensatory adjustment in central graviceptive pathway.
It has been also suggested that asymmetric functioning of basal ganglia circuit has a primary role in developing PS1,2. This hypothesis is corroborated by animal and clinical data demonstrating hemiparkinsonian animal models bending to the denervated striatum30, greater motor symptom asymmetry in PS4, and directionality of PS preferentially toward the less affected hemibody5,31. Nevertheless, neither asymmetry nor laterality of motor symptoms was associated with PS in our patients, consistent with a recent large study14. Furthermore, a recent autopsy report showed no pathological evidence of asymmetric involvement in a PD patient with PS32. These findings might reinforce the concept that multiple factors are involved in PS, apart from unbalanced dopaminergic functioning1–3.
Vestibular dysfunction has also been reported to have a critical role in generating PS in PD31,33. In a study evaluating vestibular function by bithermal caloric testing, all PD patients with PS exhibited unilateral vestibular hypofunction, exclusively in the same direction of PS31. In some of our PD patients, vestibular responses to caloric stimuli were decreased unilaterally. However, the frequency of unilateral CP was similar in both PD-PS and PD-noPS patients. These discordant findings may stem from differences in patients characteristics including relatively lesser degree of PS and the absence of spontaneous nystagmus, denoting long-standing compensation, in our patients. Instead, all PD-PS with unilateral CP tilted toward the same side of CP, regardless of patterns of SVV or motor symptoms laterality (see Supplementary Table). Together, it is assumable that unilateral vestibular hypofunction during caloric stimulation may play a part in PS, possibly by determining the direction of PS, toward the ipsilateral side of reduced caloric response. Future investigations addressing neural correlates for PS and unilateral CP in a larger sample of PD patients can further define the precise role of subclinical vestibular hypofunction in PS.
In favor of dystonic etiology, several EMG investigations have reported tonic activation of paraspinal or abdominal muscles ipsilateral to the leaning side in PD patients with PS4–6,20,34. However, this pattern was not observed in our PD-PS. Instead, our PD-PS patients usually presented sustained muscle activity contralateral to the leaning side, consistent with previous studies which interpreted this EMG pattern as muscle contraction compensating for body inclination5,6. These contradictory findings might reflect methodological variability, including muscles explored and EMG recording paradigm, or different patient characteristics, such as disease severity and duration of PS. Indeed, we did not specifically examine EMG activity of the abdominal muscles, such as the external oblique, which are a potential target of lidocaine injection therapy to ameliorate PS34. Nevertheless, there have been arguments against dystonic hypothesis, due to a paucity of clinical features compatible with dystonia in PS, including overflow, twisting or twitching, and aggravation with motion1,2. Furthermore, a recent series demonstrated that only a small number of PD patients with PS improved their postural deformities using sensory tricks that typically ameliorate dystonia14. Nevertheless, some PD patients with PS have reported beneficial effects of botulinum toxin or lidocaine injections, which relieve dystonic contraction of paraspinal or external oblique muscles20,34,35. Further investigations using a unified EMG protocol in a large population of PD patients with PS are required to clarify this issue.
Our study has several limitations. Firstly, the PD-noPS group contained fewer patients than the PD-PS group because patients in the two groups were matched for disease severity, which increased with the presence of PS14. Secondly, although global cognitive function did not differ between patient groups, we did not perform detailed assessments of executive or visuospatial dysfunction, which may also have a role in the development of PS36,37. Thirdly, healthy controls or patients with PS due to other diseases were not included in our study. Fourthly, as the vestibular measurements used in our study assess the angular VOR or utricular pathway, possible involvement of vestibular system other than these two pathways might have been missed in our PD patients with PS. Finally, cross-sectional nature of this study did not allow clarification regarding the causal relationship between abnormal verticality perception and PS in PD patients. For instance, different features between PD-PS patients with ipsiversive SVV and those without may be interpreted as the dynamic evolution of the SVV tilt from reactive to the postural misalignment to compensatory to postural imbalance. Longitudinal monitoring of verticality perception, plus other potential contributors, can help verify whether PS is a primary pathology of PS, compensatory adaptation for postural imbalance, or a reactive change to the postural misalignment elicited by other factors, such as CP.
To conclude, verticality misperception can be a strong risk factor for the generation of PS in PD patients. Especially, our study provides discriminating clinical profiles which can imply the primary alteration of verticality perception in PD patients with PS who tilt toward the perceived verticality or compensatory deviation in those who do not. This result may highlight differing roles of verticality perception in pathogenesis of PS depending on the congruence between the direction of perceived verticality and that of PS. Quantitative assessment of verticality perception deficits in PD patients with PS may increase our understanding of pathomechanisms of PS. Future prospective studies investigating verticality perception deficits in PD patients might provide quantitative measures predicting a high-risk population of PS. Moreover, these will help develop new therapeutic strategies manipulating verticality perception in PD patients with PS and define suitable patients for those therapies for alleviating PS.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
This research is supported by the Basic Science Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea, funded by the Ministry of Education (201600810001).
Author Contributions
Y.E.H. contributed to design and conceptualization of the study, drafting the manuscript, analysis and interpretation of data for intellectual content, obtaining funding. K.K. was involved in acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of data for intellectual content. J.Y. and W.-H.C. contributed to analysis and interpretation of data for intellectual content. S.K. was responsible for statistical analysis, analysis and interpretation of data for intellectual content. J.W.C. contributed to design and conceptualization of the study, drafting and revising the manuscript, analysis and interpretation of data for intellectual content, obtaining funding.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-018-20129-2.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Castrioto A, Piscicelli C, Perennou D, Krack P, Debu B. The pathogenesis of Pisa syndrome in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2014;29:1100–1107. doi: 10.1002/mds.25925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Barone P, Santangelo G, Amboni M, Pellecchia MT, Vitale C. Pisa syndrome in Parkinson’s disease and parkinsonism: clinical features, pathophysiology, and treatment. Lancet Neurol. 2016;15:1063–1074. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(16)30173-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Tinazzi M, et al. Pisa syndrome in Parkinson’s disease: An integrated approach from pathophysiology to management. Mov Disord. 2016;31:1785–1795. doi: 10.1002/mds.26829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Tassorelli C, et al. Pisa syndrome in Parkinson’s disease: clinical, electromyographic, and radiological characterization. Mov Disord. 2012;27:227–235. doi: 10.1002/mds.23930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Di Matteo A, et al. Lateral trunk flexion in Parkinson’s disease: EMG features disclose two different underlying pathophysiological mechanisms. J Neurol. 2011;258:740–745. doi: 10.1007/s00415-010-5822-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tinazzi M, et al. Pisa syndrome in Parkinson’s disease: an electrophysiological and imaging study. J Neurol. 2013;260:2138–2148. doi: 10.1007/s00415-013-6945-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Scocco DH, Wagner JN, Racosta J, Chade A, Gershanik OS. Subjective visual vertical in Pisa syndrome. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2014;20:878–883. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.04.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gandor F, Basta D, Gruber D, Poewe W, Ebersbach G. Subjective Visual Vertical in PD Patients with Lateral Trunk Flexion. Parkinsons Dis. 2016;2016:7489105. doi: 10.1155/2016/7489105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Horak FB. Postural orientation and equilibrium: what do we need to know about neural control of balance to prevent falls? Age Ageing. 2006;35(Suppl 2):ii7–ii11. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Perennou DA, et al. Lateropulsion, pushing and verticality perception in hemisphere stroke: a causal relationship? Brain. 2008;131:2401–2413. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Dieterich M, Brandt T. Wallenberg’s syndrome: lateropulsion, cyclorotation, and subjective visual vertical in thirty-six patients. Ann Neurol. 1992;31:399–408. doi: 10.1002/ana.410310409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55:181–184. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tinazzi M, et al. Imaging of the dopamine transporter predicts pattern of disease progression and response to levodopa in patients with schizophrenia and parkinsonism: a 2-year follow-up multicenter study. Schizophr Res. 2014;152:344–349. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.11.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Tinazzi M, et al. Pisa syndrome in Parkinson disease: An observational multicenter Italian study. Neurology. 2015;85:1769–1779. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Johnson L, et al. Clinical and posturographic correlates of falling in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2013;28:1250–1256. doi: 10.1002/mds.25449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Tomlinson CL, et al. Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2010;25:2649–2653. doi: 10.1002/mds.23429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Yang TH, et al. Topology of brainstem lesions associated with subjective visual vertical tilt. Neurology. 2014;82:1968–1975. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Moon IJ, et al. Predictive value of electrocochleography for determining hearing outcomes in Meniere’s disease. Otol Neurotol. 2012;33:204–210. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0b013e318241b88c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Takakusaki K. Neurophysiology of gait: from the spinal cord to the frontal lobe. Mov Disord. 2013;28:1483–1491. doi: 10.1002/mds.25669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bonanni L, Thomas A, Varanese S, Scorrano V, Onofrj M. Botulinum toxin treatment of lateral axial dystonia in Parkinsonism. Mov Disord. 2007;22:2097–2103. doi: 10.1002/mds.21694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bronstein AM. The interaction of otolith and proprioceptive information in the perception of verticality. The effects of labyrinthine and CNS disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;871:324–333. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09195.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Barra J, et al. Humans use internal models to construct and update a sense of verticality. Brain. 2010;133:3552–3563. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Barnett-Cowan M, et al. Multisensory determinants of orientation perception in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience. 2010;167:1138–1150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.02.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Pereira CB, Kanashiro AK, Maia FM, Barbosa ER. Correlation of impaired subjective visual vertical and postural instability in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci. 2014;346:60–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2014.07.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Danta G, Hilton RC. Judgment of the visual vertical and horizontal in patients with Parkinsonism. Neurology. 1975;25:43–47. doi: 10.1212/WNL.25.1.43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Baier B, Suchan J, Karnath HO, Dieterich M. Neural correlates of disturbed perception of verticality. Neurology. 2012;78:728–735. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318248e544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Shine JM, et al. Exploring the cortical and subcortical functional magnetic resonance imaging changes associated with freezing in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2013;136:1204–1215. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Muller ML, et al. Thalamic cholinergic innervation and postural sensory integration function in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2013;136:3282–3289. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Geroin C, et al. Does the Pisa syndrome affect postural control, balance, and gait in patients with Parkinson’s disease? An observational cross-sectional study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2015;21:736–741. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.04.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Herrera-Marschitz M, Utsumi H, Ungerstedt U. Scoliosis in rats with experimentally-induced hemiparkinsonism: dependence upon striatal dopamine denervation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1990;53:39–43. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.53.1.39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Vitale C, et al. Vestibular impairment and adaptive postural imbalance in parkinsonian patients with lateral trunk flexion. Mov Disord. 2011;26:1458–1463. doi: 10.1002/mds.23657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Solla P, Grau-Rivera O, Gelpi E, Marrosu F, Marti MJ. Pisa syndrome in a patient with pathologically confirmed Parkinson’s disease. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol. 2016;42:654–658. doi: 10.1111/nan.12324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Mamo H, et al. [Transitory latero-pulsion after subthalamic and thalamic coagulations in parkinsonism patients] Rev Neurol (Paris). 1965;112:509–520. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Di Martino S, et al. Efficacy of a combined therapeutic approach in the management of Pisa Syndrome. NeuroRehabilitation. 2017;41:249–253. doi: 10.3233/NRE-171478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Tassorelli C, et al. Botulinum toxin type A potentiates the effect of neuromotor rehabilitation of Pisa syndrome in Parkinson disease: a placebo controlled study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2014;20:1140–1144. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Vitale C, et al. Neuropsychological correlates of Pisa syndrome in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neurol Scand. 2016;134:101–107. doi: 10.1111/ane.12514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ninomiya S, et al. Relationship between Postural Deformities and Frontal Function in Parkinson’s Disease. Parkinsons Dis. 2015;2015:462143. doi: 10.1155/2015/462143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to patient confidentiality but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.