Abstract
Fear appeals are a polarizing issue, with proponents confident in their efficacy and opponents confident that they backfire. We present the results of a comprehensive meta-analysis investigating fear appeals’ effectiveness for influencing attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. We tested predictions from a large number of theories, the majority of which have never been tested meta-analytically until now. Studies were included if they contained a treatment group exposed to a fear appeal, a valid comparison group, a manipulation of depicted fear, a measure of attitudes, intentions, or behaviors concerning the targeted risk or recommended solution, and adequate statistics to calculate effect sizes. The meta-analysis included 127 papers (9% unpublished) yielding 248 independent samples (NTotal = 27,372) collected from diverse populations. Results showed a positive effect of fear appeals on attitudes, intentions, and behaviors, with the average effect on a composite index being random-effects = 0.29. Moderation analyses based on prominent fear appeal theories showed that the effectiveness of fear appeals increased when the message included efficacy statements, depicted high susceptibility and severity, recommended one-time only (vs. repeated) behaviors, and targeted audiences that included a larger percentage of female message recipients. Overall, we conclude that (a) fear appeals are effective at positively influencing attitude, intentions, and behaviors, (b) there are very few circumstances under which they are not effective, and (c) there are no identified circumstances under which they backfire and lead to undesirable outcomes.
Keywords: Fear appeals, risk, health communication, meta-analysis
Fear appeals are persuasive messages that attempt to arouse fear by emphasizing the potential danger and harm that will befall individuals if they do not adopt the messages’ recommendations (Dillard, 1996; Maddux & Rogers, 1983). Although these messages are often used in political, public health, and advertising campaigns in the hopes of reducing risky attitudes, intentions, or behaviors, their use is often a polarizing issue. Whereas some practitioners are confident in the power of fear appeals to persuade audiences (e.g., CDC, 2014; Xu et al., 2015), others are adamant that such messages are counterproductive (e.g., Drug Free Action Alliance, 2013; Ruiter et al., 2014). The fear appeal literature reflects this disagreement, and empirical studies, literature reviews, and meta-analyses conducted over the past six decades have offered a diverse array of perspectives on the topic. Although some meta-analytic examinations have found positive effects of fear appeals on some outcomes (Witte & Allen, 2000), others have found null effects (de Hoog et al., 2007) or even negative effects (Peters et al., 2012). In the current paper, we present the results of a comprehensive meta-analysis of fear appeal research with two goals in mind. Our first goal was to compile the largest available meta-analytic database of fear appeal research and estimate average effects. Our second goal was to test a variety of theoretical predictions, many of which have never been examined meta-analytically, and to organize them within a framework that takes into account characteristics of a fear appeal’s message, recommended behavior, and audience.
A Message-Behavior-Audience Framework of Fear Appeals
Existing theories about fear appeals have focused on either the content of the message, the nature of the behavior recommended by the communication, or the characteristics of the audience receiving the message. However, all three of these aspects (message, behavior, and audience) are important and were considered in the framework that guided this review. This integrative framework gave our meta-analysis a broader scope beyond past analyses of fear appeals. Specifically, each prior meta-analysis has only tested theories relevant to the message portion of our framework, and thus was only able to address a limited set of questions pertaining to fear appeal effectiveness (for a description of prior meta-analyses, see Table 1) (Boster & Mongeau, 1984; de Hoog et al., 2007; Earl & Albarracin, 2007; Floyd et al., 2000; Milne et al., 2000; Peters et al., 2012; Sutton, 1982; Witte & Allen, 2000). By adopting this more holistic view of fear appeals, we connected existing models that are generally treated as separate and examined novel hypotheses about fear appeal effectiveness that have previously gone untested. Further, the current meta-analysis used a substantially larger meta-analytic database than prior analyses, thus providing us with more precision to test relevant hypotheses.
Table 1.
MBA Aspect | Theory | Hypothesis | Current Meta-Analysis | Relevant Prior Meta-Analyses |
---|---|---|---|---|
Message | LM | High depicted fear will lead to better outcomes than moderate depicted fear | Partial support |
Boster & Mongeau (1984) Sutton (1982) Witte & Allen (2000) |
CM | High depicted fear will lead to worse outcomes than moderate depicted fear | Not supported | ||
ES | Strong: Fear appeals that lack efficacy statements will produce negative effects | Not supported |
de Hoog et al. (2007) Earl & Albarracin (2007) Floyd et al. (2000) Milne et al. (2000) Peters et al. (2012) Witte & Allen (2000) |
|
ES | Weak: Fear appeals that lack efficacy statements will produce weaker effects (less positive or null) relative to fear appeals that include efficacy statements | Supported | ||
SM | Fear appeals with high depicted severity (and low depicted susceptibility) will positively influence attitudes but will not influence intentions or behaviors | Partial support |
de Hoog et al. (2007) Floyd et al. (2000) Milne et al. (2000) |
|
SM | Fear appeals with high depicted susceptibility (and low depicted severity) will positively influence intentions and behaviors but will not influence attitudes | Supported | ||
SM | Fear appeals with high depicted severity and high depicted susceptibility will positively influence attitudes, intentions, and behaviors | Supported | ||
Behavior | RSAT | Fear appeals will be more effective for one-time versus repeated behaviors | Supported | None |
PT | Fear appeals will be more effective for detection versus promotion/prevention behaviors | Not supported | ||
TMT | When fear appeals recommend an SEE behavior, fear appeals that mention death should be more effective than fear appeals that do not | Not supported | ||
TMT | When fear appeals recommend an SEH behavior, fear appeals that mention death should be less effective than fear appeals that do not | Not supported | ||
TMT | Fear appeals that mention death (versus not) will be more effective for delayed outcomes | Not supported | ||
Audience | RFT | Fear appeals will be more effective for female versus male audiences | Supported | None |
RFT | Fear appeals will be more effective for collectivist versus individualist audiences | Not supported | ||
TM | Early: Fear appeals will be more effective for people in early TM stages of change | Not supported | ||
TM | Late: Fear appeals will be more effective for people in late TM stages of change | Not supported |
Note: LM = Linear Model. CM = Curvilinear Model. ES = Efficacy Statements. SM = Stage Model. RSAT = Robertson’s Single Action Theory. PT = Prospect Theory. TMT = Terror Management Theory. RFT = Regulatory Focus Theory. TM = Transtheoretical Model. SEE = Self-esteem enhancing. SEH = Self-esteem hindering.
The Content of Fear Appeals
Six prominent theories make predictions about the impact of message characteristics on fear appeal effectiveness1: The linear model of fear appeals (e.g., Witte & Allen, 2000), the curvilinear model of fear appeals (e.g., Hovland et al., 1953), the health belief model (Rosenstock, 1966; Becker, 1974; Becker et al., 1977; Becker et al., 1978; Rosenstock, 1974), the parallel process model (Leventhal, 1970), the extended parallel process model (Witte, 1992; Witte, 1998), and the stage model (de Hoog et al., 2007). These theories concern the level of depicted fear within messages, the use (or omission) of efficacy statements within messages, and the level of depicted susceptibility and/or severity within messages.
Amount of depicted fear
Perhaps the most central aspect of a fear appeal message is the amount of fear it is intended to arouse in message recipients. We will refer to this as depicted fear to emphasize that it reflects a property of the message’s content, rather than the subjective state of fear that message recipients experience.2 Two competing theories make predictions about amount of depicted fear, which we will refer to as the linear model (e.g., Witte & Allen, 2000) and the curvilinear model (Hovland et al., 1953; Janis, 1967; Janis & Feshbach, 1953; McGuire, 1968; McGuire, 1969). Both theoretical perspectives conceptualize depicted fear as a source of motivation, such that exposure to depicted fear increases motivation to adopt the message’s recommendations (Hovland et al., 1953; Witte & Allen, 2000). Further, both models predict that low levels of depicted fear will be relatively less motivating and thus less effective than moderate levels of fear. However, the linear model predicts that depicted fear has a positive and monotonic influences on attitudes, intentions, and behaviors, such that high depicted fear is more effective than moderate depicted fear (e.g., Witte & Allen, 2000). In contrast, the curvilinear model predicts that high depicted fear elicits defensive avoidance, a reaction in which message recipients disengage from the message, avoid further exposure to the message, and/or derogate the message because it is too frightening (Higbee, 1969; Hovland et al., 1953; Janis, 1967; 1968; Janis & Feshbach, 1953; Janis & Leventhal, 1968; McGuire, 1968; 1969; Millman, 1968). Consequently, the curvilinear theory predicts that high levels of depicted fear should be less effective than moderate levels of depicted fear.
The linear and curvilinear models have been tested in prior meta-analyses, and the linear model has consistently been supported by existing data, whereas the curvilinear model has not (e.g., Witte & Allen, 2000). One drawback to prior investigations of the linear and curvilinear models is that the analyses included comparisons from studies that used two levels of depicted fear, even though it is difficult to equate levels of depicted fear across different studies – what may qualify as moderate depicted fear in one study may qualify as low depicted fear in a different study. Thus, an appropriate test of the linear and curvilinear models requires depicted fear to be manipulated with at least three levels within the same study to ensure that moderate depicted fear is operationalized as an intermediate level between extremes. We therefore tested the linear and curvilinear models in the current meta-analysis by comparing the effects of high versus moderate depicted fear, using only studies that manipulated depicted fear across several levels. The linear model predicts that high depicted fear will be more effective than moderate depicted fear, whereas the curvilinear model predicts that high depicted fear will be less effective than moderate depicted fear.
Efficacy statements
According to the health belief model (HBM; Rosenstock, 1966; Becker, 1974; Becker et al., 1977; Becker et al., 1978; Rosenstock, 1974), the stage model (e.g., de Hoog et al., 2007), the parallel process model (PPM; Leventhal, 1970), and the extended parallel process model (EPPM; Witte, 1992; Witte, 1998), fear appeals “work only when accompanied by… efficacy messages” (Witte & Allen, 2000, p.606). An efficacy message is a statement that assures message recipients that they are capable of performing the fear appeal’s recommended actions (self-efficacy) and/or that performing the recommended actions will result in desirable consequences (response-efficacy). The HBM, stage model, PPM, and EPPM suggest that when message recipients are presented with a threat (i.e., depicted fear), resulting feelings of vulnerability lead them to evaluate whether or not adopting the message’s recommendations will protect them from the threat-related negative consequences. If recipients decide that adopting the recommended action(s) will protect them, the fear appeal should be more effective. As efficacy statements provide this assurance, fear appeal messages that include statements about self- or response-efficacy should be more effective than fear appeal messages that include neither (de Hoog et al., 2007; Witte & Allen, 2000).
There are two forms of the efficacy statement hypothesis. The strong hypothesis is that fear appeals without efficacy statements will produce negative effects (i.e., will backfire). The weak hypothesis is that fear appeals without efficacy statements will produce weaker (i.e., less positive or null) effects relative to fear appeals with efficacy statements. Three meta-analyses have tested whether the inclusion of efficacy statements in fear appeals leads to increased effectiveness, and all found support for the weak hypothesis (de Hoog et al., 2007; Mongeau, 1998; Witte & Allen, 2000). However, those studies were conducted using less comprehensive meta-analytic databases, and thus the current synthesis can provide a more thorough assessment of the strong and weak hypotheses.
Depicted susceptibility and severity
According to the stage model (de Hoog et al., 2007), the effectiveness of fear appeals should depend on their levels of depicted susceptibility and severity. A message high in depicted susceptibility emphasizes the message recipient’s personal risk for negative consequences (e.g., “One of fourteen women is destined to develop breast cancer during her life. So every woman may get breast cancer. You also run that risk!”; Siero et al., 1984), whereas a message low in depicted susceptibility does not personalize risk (e.g., “One of fourteen women is destined to develop breast cancer during her life.”; Siero et al., 1984). A message high in depicted severity describes the negative consequences of not taking action (e.g., “Breast cancer is a serious disease of which many women die, contrary to, for example, cancer of the uterus, where 90% to 95% recover.”; Siero et al., 1984), whereas a message low in depicted severity portrays manageable consequences (e.g., “If breast cancer is detected at an early stage it can be cured in a number of cases, contrary to, for example, lung cancer where 90% die of it.”; Siero et al., 1984). According to this model, high depicted severity (but not susceptibility) should improve attitudes, whereas high depicted susceptibility (but not severity) should improve intentions and behaviors. Consequently, only the combination of high-depicted susceptibility and severity should improve attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. A previous meta-analysis found mixed results concerning these predictions (de Hoog et al., 2007). Specifically, messages with high depicted severity positively influenced attitudes, intentions, and behaviors, whereas messages with high depicted susceptibility positively influenced intentions and behaviors but not attitudes. We tested these hypotheses on our more comprehensive database.
The Recommended Behavior
Three prominent theories make predictions about the impact of the recommended behaviors on fear appeal effectiveness: Robertson’s single action theory (Robertson, 1975; Rothman, Martino, Bedell, Detweiler, & Salovey, 1999), prospect theory (Rothman et al., 1999; Rothman & Salovey, 1997; Tversky & Kahneman, 1981) and terror management theory (Goldenberg & Arndt, 2008; Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & Solomon, 1999; Shehryar & Hunt, 2005; Solomon, Greenberg, & Pyszczynski, 1991). These theories concern whether the recommended behavior is a one-time or recurring activity, involves detection or prevention/promotion, occurs immediately or after a delay, can enhance self-esteem, and is intended to replace a self-esteem enhancing behavior.
One-time versus repeated behaviors
According to Robertson (1975; also see Rothman et al., 1999), persuasive messages should be more successful when they recommend one-time behaviors (e.g., getting vaccinated) compared to behaviors that must be repeated over an extended period of time (e.g., exercising). As it takes less effort to do something once than many times, people are likely to be more compliant when a single behavior is recommended. Using this principle, we compared the effectiveness of fear appeals recommending one-time versus repeated behaviors.
Detection versus prevention/promotion behaviors
According to prospect theory, negative outcomes can be categorized as incurring a loss or foregoing a gain, and losses tend to be more psychologically impactful than foregone gains of objectively equal magnitude (Tversky & Kahneman, 1981). Several researchers have extended the logic of prospect theory to fear appeals, hypothesizing that fear appeals should be more effective when recommending detection behaviors relative to prevention/promotion behaviors (Rothman, Martino, Bedell, Detweiler, & Salovey, 1999; Rothman & Salovey, 1997). Detection behaviors are enacted to obtain information about potential risk factors or existing health issues (e.g., being screened for cancer), and thus engaging in a detection behavior increases risk for incurring a loss (e.g., acquiring the unwanted and undesirable information that one has cancer). In contrast, prevention/promotion behaviors are enacted to obtain desirable outcomes (e.g., exercising to lose weight or avoid weight gain), and thus engaging in prevention/promotion behaviors does not increase risk for incurring a loss (e.g., exercising will only bring one closer to the desired outcome of losing weight or avoiding weight gain, so there is no potential for loss by engaging in exercise). Fear appeals are loss-framed messages because they emphasize negative consequences, and loss-framed information makes people more willing than usual to take risks (Meyerowitz & Chaiken, 1987; van’t Riet et al., 2014). Therefore, although fear appeals should be effective for both detection and prevention/promotion behaviors, they should be particularly effective for detection behaviors because the loss-framed nature of the message should make people more willing than usual to take on the risk of the detection behavior (Meyerowitz & Chaiken, 1987; Rothman, Martino, Bedell, Detweiler, & Salovey, 1999; Rothman & Salovey, 1997; van’t Riet et al., 2014).
Mentioning death, self-esteem relevance, and time delays
Many fear appeals explicitly mention death (89 of the 248 studies in our meta-analysis), and terror management theory (TMT) makes three predictions about this factor. According to TMT, when people are reminded of their mortality by being exposed to the concept of death, they often become motivated to buffer their self-esteem to reduce mortality related anxiety (Goldenberg & Arndt, 2008; Pyszczynski et al., 1999; Shehryar & Hunt, 2005; Solomon et al., 1991). Some fear appeals recommend behaviors that can enhance self-esteem (e.g., dieting, which can improve body image; Goldenberg & Arndt, 2008), whereas others attempt to persuade people to stop engaging in behaviors that enhance self-esteem (e.g., tanning, which can also improve body image; Janssen et al., 2013). When fear appeals mention death, message recipients should increase commitment to behaviors that enhance self-esteem, regardless of whether the fear appeals encourage or discourage those behaviors. Consequently, fear appeals recommending self-esteem enhancing behaviors (e.g., dieting) should be more effective when they mention death than when they do not. In contrast, fear appeals recommending the cessation of behaviors that enhance self-esteem (e.g., tanning abstinence) should be less effective when they mention death than when they do not.
TMT also posits that reminders of death activate two types of defensive responses: Short-term proximal defenses and long-term distal defenses. Proximal defenses involve refuting information to avoid considering one’s death, whereas distal defenses involve buffering one’s self-esteem and pursuing long-term goals (e.g., a healthy lifestyle; Goldenberg & Arndt, 2008). Consequently, fear appeals that mention death should be more effective if there is a delay between fear appeal exposure and occurrence of the outcome, rather than if outcomes occur immediately after exposure when proximal defenses are still active (e.g., Greenberg et al., 1990; Shehryar & Hunt, 2005).3
The Audience
Two prominent theories make predictions about the impact of the audience on fear appeal effectiveness: Regulatory fit theory (Higgins, Pierro, & Kruglanski, 2008; Kurman & Hui, 2011; Lockwood, Marshall, & Sadler, 2005) and the transtheoretical model (Prochaska & DiClemente, 1983; Prochaska et al., 1992; Prochaska & Velicer, 1997). These predictions concern whether the message’s audience is primarily female (versus male), from a collectivist culture (versus an individualistic culture), and already attempting to change risk behaviors (versus not).
Gender and culture
According to regulatory fit theory, people can be promotion or prevention focused, placing greater value on either the pursuit of positive outcomes or on the avoidance of negative outcomes, respectively (Higgins et al., 2008; Kurman & Hui, 2011; Lockwood et al., 2005). Message frames that match the promotion versus prevention tendencies of the audience are more persuasive (Cesario, Higgins, & Scholar, 2008), and fear appeals are definitionally prevention-framed messages because they emphasize what one should do to avoid negative outcomes. Consequently, prevention-focused populations should be more persuaded by fear appeals relative to promotion-focused populations. Cultural research in the area of regulatory focus has found that women tend to be more prevention focused than men, and members of collectivist groups tend to be more prevention focused than members of individualist ones (Kurman & Hui, 2011; Lockwood, Marshall, & Sadler, 2005). Therefore, fear appeals should be particularly effective for female (versus male) and collectivist (versus individualist) audiences.
Early versus late stages of change
According to the transtheoretical model, people engaging in risky behaviors can be classified as belonging to an early stage (the model’s precontemplation, contemplation, and preparation stages) or a late stage (the model’s action and maintenance stages) in the change process (Prochaska & DiClemente, 1983; Prochaska et al., 1992; Prochaska & Velicer, 1997). According to the early-effectiveness hypothesis, fear appeals should be more effective for individuals in the early (vs. late) stages because the former require motivational appeals to understand that a threat exists and to increase commitment to adopting desirable behaviors and/or abandoning undesirable behaviors. In contrast, late stage individuals are already committed to behavior change and do not require such motivational appeals (DiClemente et al., 1991; Nabi et al., 2008; Prochaska & DiClemente, 1983; Prochaska et al., 1992). The late-effectiveness hypothesis competes with the early one, and predicts that success at behavior change is associated with increases in self- and response efficacy (Cho & Salmon, 2000). As a result, exposure to a fear appeal should lead individuals who have already enacted change to process the fear appeal in the context of their high response efficacy (Cho & Salmon, 2006). Consequently, the late-effectiveness hypothesis predicts that fear appeals should be more effective for late stage relative to early stage individuals.4 To test the early-effectiveness and late-effectiveness hypotheses, we classified each study’s sample as belonging to one of the transtheoretical model’s first three stages or last two stages. We then compared the effectiveness of fear appeals for individuals in the early versus late stages.
The Present Research
We compiled the largest meta-analytic database of fear appeals to date to examine the effectiveness of fear appeals for changing attitudes, intentions, and behaviors, and also to test moderator predictions made by a variety of influential fear appeal theories. Each of these theories tends to focus on one of three things – the content of the message, the type of behavior recommended by the communication, or the characteristics of the audience receiving the message (see Table 1 for a full list of theories and related hypotheses). Of the 16 fear appeal hypotheses discussed, only seven have been tested in prior meta-analyses, and all of them fall under the message aspect of our framework (Table 1). Thus, the present research represents the first meta-analytic test for nine of the 16 hypotheses and the first meta-analytic test for any hypotheses related to the behavior and audience aspects of our framework.
Methods
Review and Inclusion Criteria
To locate studies, we conducted a search of the PsycInfo and Medline databases using the keywords (risk or fear or shock or severity or susceptibility) AND (persuasion or appeal or argument or tactic or campaign or communication or intervention). To supplement these database searches, we examined the reference lists of previous fear appeal meta-analyses, review articles, and chapters. We also contacted researchers to request unpublished data and sent requests to the e-mail lists of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, the European Health Psychology Society, and the American Academy of Health Behavior. Our search extended through February 2015 and yielded 430 potentially eligible articles, which were subsequently screened for inclusion in the current meta-analysis based on several inclusion criteria. For inclusion in this meta-analysis, studies had to meet the following eligibility criteria:
Studies were included if they contained an experimental research design in which a treatment group was exposed to a message designed to induce fear (i.e., a fear appeal).
Studies were included if they contained a comparison group. The comparison group could have been a group that was not exposed to any message, a group that was exposed to a message that was not designed to induce fear, or a message that was designed to induce less fear than the treatment group’s message. When a study included more than one potential comparison groups, we opted to compare the highest depicted fear condition with the lowest depicted fear condition, prioritizing them in the following order: No message comparison group, neutral message comparison group, and low depicted fear comparison group. Thus, for a study containing a low depicted fear group and a neutral message group, we used the neutral message group as the comparison group. Overall, all results should be interpreted as the effect of exposure to messages designed to depict relatively high levels of fear compared to conditions designed to depict relatively lower levels of fear (including no fear).5
Studies were included if they experimentally manipulated depicted fear across groups. Studies were excluded if they used correlational research designs or provided all groups with the same level of depicted fear.
Studies were included if they measured one or more of the following variables as an outcome in both the treatment and comparison groups: Attitudes, intentions, or behaviors.
Studies were excluded if they did not contain appropriate statistics (e.g., F ratios, means and standard deviations, frequencies, or exact p values) for calculating an effect size representing the difference of outcomes for treatment versus comparison groups. If a study was otherwise eligible but did not contain appropriate statistics (e.g., it provided path coefficients from a structural equation analysis but did not supply means and standard deviations for treatment and comparison groups), we attempted to contact the study’s authors to retrieve usable data such as means and standard deviations. We contacted authors of 39 papers for this purpose: Three provided us with the requested data, six responded but could not provide us with the relevant data, and the rest did not respond to multiple contact requests.
Of the 430 reports considered for inclusion in this meta-analysis, 127 met our inclusion criteria (9% unpublished), providing 248 statistically independent samples with a total N of 27,372 participants in the treatment and comparison groups combined. Samples ranged in age from 9-87 years (M = 22.77 years, SD = 9.24 years) and were on average 66% female (SD = 33%). An average of 81% of each sample had completed high school (SD = 37%). Further, samples were on average 71% White or European-American (SD = 34%), 14% Asian or Asian-American (SD = 31%), 8% Black or African-American (SD = 18%), and 5% Hispanic/Latino(a) (SD = 14%).
Coding of Outcomes (Effect Size Calculation)
We calculated a single effect size per sample that compared attitudes, intentions, and behaviors for the treatment group relative to the comparison group. First, for each sample we recorded all measures of attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. For each outcome, we calculated the standardized mean difference between treatment and comparison groups correcting for sample size bias (Johnson & Eagly, 2014, p. 686). Effect sizes (d) were calculated based on provided F-ratios, t-tests, odds ratios, or means and standard deviations. To produce d for any odds ratios, we divided the log of the odds ratio by 1.81 (Haddock, Rindskopf, & Shadish, 1998; Hasselblad & Hedges, 1995).
Note that outcomes could have concerned the negative behavior/issue targeted by the fear appeal (e.g., attitudes toward smoking) or the fear appeal’s recommendations (e.g., attitudes toward smoking cessation). Effect sizes were calculated such that higher positive values indicate the treatment group scored higher in the message’s direction. For example, if a study used anti-smoking messages, a positive d would indicate that the treatment group (relative to the comparison group) had more negative attitudes toward smoking or more positive attitudes toward smoking cessation. Thus, a positive effect size indicates the fear appeal worked, whereas a negative effect size indicates the fear appeal backfired.
The majority of samples (k = 170) included only one type of dependent measure (attitudes, intentions, or behaviors), but some samples included two types (k = 61) or all three (k = 17). Therefore, after calculating d for each outcome in a sample, we averaged all d values together to form a single effect size per sample that represents positive change in the direction advocated by the fear appeal. Further, if a sample included two or more measures of the same outcome type (e.g., attitudes toward smoking and attitudes toward smoking cessation), each was included in the average and weighted equally (the number of samples with multiple attitude, intention, and behavior measures was respectively k = 18, k = 24, and k = 12). This approach is justified on several grounds. First, for studies that included all three types of outcomes (attitudes, intentions, and behaviors), Cronbach’s alpha for the composite measure was .87, indicating that the three types of measures are highly internally consistent. Further, prior research has demonstrated that composite measures combining attitudes, intentions, and behaviors are a valid outcome of interest when investigating the relative persuasiveness of messages (O’Keefe, 2013). We therefore combined all attitude, intention, and behavior measures within each sample to form a single effect size per sample, which is how the results will be presented in the present manuscript. However, we also conducted all analyses separately for attitude, intention, and behavior measures; these results are presented in Appendix A and are consistent with the results based on the combined measure. Several hypotheses made specific predictions about attitudes, intentions, or behaviors, and for those hypotheses (see Table 1), we present the relevant outcomes of interest in the body of the manuscript.
Of note, attitudes were most commonly measured with semantic differential scales (e.g., positive/negative, beneficial/harmful, wise/foolish, etc.; Roskos-Ewoldsen, Yu, & Rhodes, 2004; Nabi et al., 2008) and Likert style scales (e.g., agreement with statements such as, “I don’t like speeding”; Cauberghe et al, 2009, p. 280). Intentions were frequently measured with Likert style scales (e.g., agreement with statements such as, “In the immediate future, I plan to find someone who will teach me to do an accurate breast self-examination”; Roskos-Ewoldsen et al., 2004, p. 58) and questions with dichotomous response options (e.g., “In the future, I intend to stop spending time outside strictly for the purpose of getting a tan,” with responses Yes and No; McMath & Prentice-Dunn, 2005, p.629). Finally, behaviors were often measured dichotomously with self-report questions (e.g., “As a direct result of this message, did you seek help?” with responses Yes and No; Smalec & Klingle, 2000, p. 45) or behavioral observation data (e.g., information obtained from medical records; Ordoñana et al., 2009).
Coding of Potential Moderators
To test each hypothesis from the message, behavior, and audience portions of our framework, we coded several relevant variables (moderator codes for each paper included in the meta-analysis are displayed in Table 2). The first author trained two independent coders, who then coded all study characteristics relevant to each report. Intercoder reliability was calculated on 20% of the overall database using Cohen’s kappa (κ) for categorical variables and Pearson’s r for continuous variables. Agreement for all variables was good: Categorical variables had average κ = .93 (SD = .06, minimum = .80), and continuous variables had average r = .92 (SD = .12, minimum = .73). Disagreements were resolved by discussion and further examination of the studies.
Table 2.
Paper | d | N | AIB | Eff | Sev | Sus | OR | DPP | DP | SE | Delay | %F | IC | SOC |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bagley & Low, 1992 | .08 | 41 | B | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | L | 66 | I | E |
Bang, 1994 | −.11 | 223 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | 54 | I | — |
Beach, 1966 | .38 | 28 | I | N | Y | N | O | PP | Y | — | L | — | I | E |
Beck & Davis, 1978 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Interest | −.05 | 31 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | 42 | I | — |
2: High Interest | 1.03 | 31 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | 42 | I | — |
Beck, 1984 | .77 | 226 | I | N | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | 47 | I | — |
Berkowitz, 1998 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Sensation-Seeking, Message Choice | .02 | 48 | AIB | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | M | 62 | I | E |
2: Low Sensation-Seeking, No Message Choice | −.23 | 34 | AIB | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | M | 62 | I | E |
3: High Sensation-Seeking, Message Choice | .21 | 42 | AIB | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | M | 62 | I | E |
4: High Sensation-Seeking, No Message Choice | .01 | 48 | AIB | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | M | 62 | I | E |
Brouwers & Sorrentino, 1993 | .25 | 149 | IB | Y | Y | Y | O | D | Y | — | S | 69 | I | — |
Brown, 1979 | 1.81 | 38 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | 0 | I | L |
Burnett, 1981 | 1.06 | 76 | AI | N | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
Calantone & Warshaw, 1985 | .96 | 180 | B | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
Carey, 1990 | .00 | 118 | I | N | Y | N | O | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | — |
Chang et al., 1989 | −.10 | 1425 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | E |
Cho & Salmon, 2006 | .42 | 239 | IB | N | Y | Y | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 61 | I | — |
Chu, 1966 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Efficacy | .36 | 240 | B | N | Y | Y | O | PP | Y | — | M | — | C | — |
2: Medium Efficacy | .52 | 242 | B | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | Y | — | M | — | C | — |
3: High Efficacy | 1.71 | 231 | B | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | Y | — | M | — | C | — |
Cooper et al., 2014 | ||||||||||||||
1: Appearance | .09 | 98 | I | — | Y | Y | R | PP | N | SEH | S | 65 | I | E |
2: Cancer | −.33 | 98 | I | — | Y | Y | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 65 | I | E |
Dabbs & Leventhal, 1966 | .68 | 120 | AIB | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | E |
Dahl et al., 2003 | .80 | 68 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | — |
Das et al., 2003 | ||||||||||||||
1: Study 1, Weak Arguments, Low Vulnerability | -1.09 | 52 | A | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
2: Study 1, Weak Arguments, High Vulnerability | 1.39 | 37 | A | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
3: Study 1, Strong Arguments, Low Vulnerability | 2.12 | 45 | A | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
4: Study 1, Strong Arguments, High Vulnerability | −.63 | 43 | A | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
5: Study 2, Weak Arguments, Low Vulnerability | −.33 | 28 | A | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
6: Study 2, Weak Arguments, High Vulnerability | .11 | 28 | A | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
7: Study 2, Strong Arguments, Low Vulnerability | −.26 | 23 | A | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
8: Study 2, Strong Arguments, High Vulnerability | .23 | 32 | A | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
9: Study 3, Weak Arguments | −.50 | 31 | AB | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
10: Study 3, Strong Arguments | .99 | 29 | AB | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
de Hoog et al., 2005 | .59 | 118 | AIB | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | N | — | S | 69 | I | — |
de Hoog et al., 2008 | ||||||||||||||
1: Study 1, Low Source Credibility | .41 | 30 | AI | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 71 | I | — |
2: Study 1, High Source Credibility | .25 | 30 | AI | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 71 | I | — |
3: Study 2, Weak Arguments | .51 | 32 | AI | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 75 | I | — |
4: Study 2, Strong Arguments | .65 | 32 | AI | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 75 | I | — |
Dembroski et al., 1978 | ||||||||||||||
1: Black Communicator | .22 | 40 | A | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | — | 52 | I | E |
2: White Communicator | 1.48 | 40 | A | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | — | 52 | I | E |
Dijkstra & Bos, 2015 | −.01 | 118 | IB | Y | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | L | 56 | I | E |
Duke et al., 2014 | ||||||||||||||
1: Threat vs. Control | .41 | 1540 | IB | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | L | 53 | I | E |
2: Threat + SE vs. SE | .67 | 970 | IB | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | L | 53 | I | E |
Evans et al., 1968 | −.53 | 49 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | M | — | I | E |
Evans et al., 1970 | .35 | 156 | IB | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | M | — | I | E |
Feenstra et al., 2014 | .32 | 1128 | AIB | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | L | 52 | I | E |
France et al., 2014 | ||||||||||||||
1: Threat vs. Control | .65 | 141 | I | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | 100 | I | E |
2: Threat + SE vs. SE | .48 | 213 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | 100 | I | E |
Frandsen, 1963 | .13 | 1080 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
Fukada, 1973 | .30 | 345 | IB | Y | Y | N | O | D | N | — | S | 57 | C | — |
Fukada, 1975 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Efficacy, Low Source Credibility | −.19 | 76 | AIB | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
2: Low Efficacy, High Source Credibility | .58 | 76 | AIB | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
3: High Efficacy, Low Source Credibility | .31 | 76 | AIB | Y | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
4: High Efficacy, High Source Credibility | .89 | 76 | AIB | Y | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
Fukada, 1983a | .94 | 48 | AIB | N | Y | N | O | D | N | — | L | 100 | C | — |
Fukada, 1983 (2) | ||||||||||||||
1: No Forewarnings | .99 | 76 | I | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
2: Topic Content Forewarning | .72 | 76 | I | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
3: Persuasive Intent Forewarning | .58 | 76 | I | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
4: Fear Arousal Forewarning | 1.08 | 76 | I | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
5: Topic Content & Fear Arousal Forewarnings | .94 | 76 | I | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
6: Topic Content & Persuasive Intent Forewarnings | 1.10 | 76 | I | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
7: Persuasive Intent & Fear Arousal Forewarnings | .64 | 76 | I | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
8: All Three Forewarnings | .55 | 76 | I | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
Fukada, 1988 | ||||||||||||||
1: Receive Counterargument | .86 | 42 | I | N | Y | N | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
2: Don’t Receive Counterargument | .4 | 42 | I | N | Y | N | O | D | N | — | S | 100 | C | — |
Fukada, 1991 | −.18 | 30 | A | N | N | N | R | PP | N | — | M | 100 | C | — |
Gleicher & Petty, 1992 | .23 | 336 | A | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | — | — | I | — |
Goldenbeld et al., 2008 | ||||||||||||||
1: Males | −.05 | 42 | AI | Y | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | 0 | I | — |
2: Females | .73 | 32 | AI | Y | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | 100 | I | — |
Griffeth & Rogers, 1976 | 1.17 | 137 | IB | Y | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEE | S | — | I | — |
Hass et al., 1975 | .60 | 56 | I | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
Hendrick et al., 1975 | ||||||||||||||
1: Study 1, Fear Reduction | .15 | 40 | AI | N | N | N | O | PP | N | — | S | 100 | I | — |
2: Study 1, No Fear Reduction | .72 | 40 | AI | N | N | N | O | PP | N | — | S | 100 | I | — |
3: Study 2 | .24 | 122 | AI | N | N | N | O | PP | N | — | S | 100 | I | — |
Hill & Gardner, 1980 | ||||||||||||||
1: Repressors | −.59 | 27 | B | N | Y | N | O | D | N | — | S | 0 | I | — |
2: Sensitizers | .44 | 25 | B | N | Y | N | O | D | N | — | S | 0 | I | — |
Hoeken & Geurts, 2005 | −.34 | 149 | I | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | 83 | I | — |
Horowitz & Gumenik, 1970 | .26 | 112 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | — |
Horowitz, 1969 | ||||||||||||||
1: Single Exposure | .37 | 60 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | 0 | I | — |
2: Multiple Exposures | .10 | 60 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | M | 0 | I | — |
Insko et al., 1965 | ||||||||||||||
1: Males | .00 | 72 | AI | N | Y | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | S | 0 | I | — |
2: Females | .00 | 72 | AI | N | Y | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | S | 100 | I | — |
Janis & Feshbach, 1954 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Anxiety | −.14 | 80 | AB | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | M | — | I | — |
2: High Anxiety | −.68 | 51 | AB | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | M | — | I | — |
Janis & Terwilliger, 1962 | −.72 | 31 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | 19 | I | — |
Janssens & De Pelsmacker, 2007 | ||||||||||||||
1: Non-Drivers | −.04 | 95 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEE | S | — | I | — |
2: Drivers | .01 | 89 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | — |
Johnston, 2006 | ||||||||||||||
1: No Pre-Test | .57 | 60 | A | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | N | — | S | 38 | I | E |
2: Pre-Test | .58 | 60 | A | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | N | — | S | 38 | I | E |
Jones & Owen, 2006 | ||||||||||||||
1: Ages 18–39 | .00 | 44 | I | Y | Y | N | R | D | N | — | S | 100 | I | E |
2: Ages 40–49 | .00 | 44 | I | Y | Y | N | R | D | N | — | S | 100 | I | E |
3: Ages 50+ | −.19 | 61 | I | Y | Y | N | R | D | N | — | S | 100 | I | L |
Kareklas & Muehling, 2014 | ||||||||||||||
1: No Verbal, Control vs. Visual | .25 | 112 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | 44 | I | E |
2: Verbal, Control vs. Visual | .10 | 112 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | 44 | I | E |
Keller & Block, 1996 | ||||||||||||||
1: Self-Reference | −.01 | 51 | I | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | E |
2: Other-Reference | .84 | 47 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | E |
Keller, 1999 | ||||||||||||||
1: Don’t Use Condoms | −.68 | 27 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | 100 | I | E |
2: Regularly Use Condoms | .66 | 34 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEE | S | 100 | I | L |
Kim et al., 2009 | .00 | 183 | B | N | N | N | R | PP | N | — | L | 53 | C | — |
Kirscht & Haefner, 1973 | ||||||||||||||
1: One Exposure | .22 | 30 | B | N | N | N | R | PP | N | — | L | 58 | I | — |
2: Two Exposures | .06 | 28 | B | N | N | N | R | PP | Y | — | L | 58 | I | — |
3: Three Exposures | −.10 | 27 | B | N | N | N | R | PP | Y | — | L | 58 | I | — |
Kirscht et al., 1978 | 1.23 | 109 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEE | M | 100 | I | E |
Kleinot & Rogers, 1982 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Efficacy | .77 | 22 | I | N | Y | Y | R | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | E |
2: High Efficacy | 1.16 | 22 | I | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | E |
Klohn & Rogers, 1991 | .68 | 85 | I | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | SEE | S | 100 | I | E |
LaTour et al., 1996 | .29 | 305 | AI | N | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | 100 | I | — |
LaTour & Tanner, 2003 | .13 | 124 | AI | N | Y | N | O | D | N | — | S | 43 | I | — |
Leventhal & Niles, 1964 | .45 | 209 | AI | Y | Y | N | O | D | N | — | S | — | I | E |
Leventhal & Watts, 1966 | ||||||||||||||
1: Smokers | −1.57 | 52 | B | Y | Y | N | O | D | N | — | S | — | I | E |
2: Non-Smokers | −.02 | 48 | B | Y | Y | N | O | D | N | SEE | S | — | I | — |
Leventhal et al., 1965 | ||||||||||||||
1: No Prior Vaccination | .60 | 59 | AI | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | E |
2: Prior Vaccination | .36 | 88 | AI | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | L |
Leventhal et al., 1967 | .53 | 106 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | E |
Levin et al., 2007 | −.41 | 222 | I | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | E |
Lewis et al., 2008 | ||||||||||||||
1: Male, Low Involvement | .23 | 35 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEE | L | 0 | I | L |
2: Male, High Involvement | .44 | 36 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | L | 0 | I | E |
3: Female, Low Involvement | .91 | 65 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEE | L | 100 | I | L |
4: Female, High Involvement | .87 | 65 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | L | 100 | I | E |
Lewis et al., 2010 | −.08 | 270 | I | N | N | N | R | PP | N | — | S | 66 | I | — |
Li, 2002 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Outcome | .27 | 28 | AI | N | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 44 | I | E |
2: High Outcome | .45 | 29 | AI | Y | Y | Y | O | D | N | — | S | 44 | I | E |
Liberman & Chaiken, 1992 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Relevance | .35 | 86 | I | N | N | Y | R | PP | N | — | S | 100 | I | — |
2: High Relevance | .35 | 86 | I | N | N | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | S | 100 | I | — |
Lwin & Malik, 2014 | ||||||||||||||
1: With Wii | .10 | 199 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | SEE | L | 42 | C | E |
2: Without Wii | −.24 | 199 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | SEE | L | 42 | C | E |
McMath & Prentice-Dunn, 2005 | 1.06 | 196 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | SEH | S | 74 | I | E |
Meijnders et al., 2001a | ||||||||||||||
1: Weak Arguments | −.27 | 54 | AI | N | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | 50 | I | — |
2: Strong Arguments | .47 | 54 | AI | N | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | 50 | I | — |
Meijnders et al., 2001b | ||||||||||||||
1: Weak Arguments | .46 | 40 | A | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | 67 | I | — |
2: Strong Arguments | .47 | 40 | A | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | 67 | I | — |
Morales et al., 2012 | ||||||||||||||
1: Study 1 (Methamphetamine Use) | .42 | 104 | I | N | N | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | — |
2: Study 2 (Sun Safety) | .43 | 94 | I | N | N | N | R | PP | N | SEH | S | — | I | — |
3: Study 3 (BPA Products) | −.20 | 54 | I | N | Y | N | O | PP | — | — | S | — | I | — |
Morris et al., 2014 | ||||||||||||||
1: Study 1, UV Photo | .31 | 31 | IB | N | Y | Y | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 100 | I | E |
2: Study 1, No UV Photo | −.53 | 28 | IB | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 100 | I | E |
3: Study 2, Appearance Focus | 1.05 | 24 | I | N | N | Y | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 100 | I | E |
4: Study 2, Health Focus | −.30 | 27 | I | N | N | Y | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 100 | I | E |
5: Study 2, No Photo | −.51 | 33 | I | N | N | N | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 100 | I | E |
Muthusamy et al., 2009 | ||||||||||||||
1: No Efficacy Message | −.20 | 124 | AIB | N | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | M | 68 | C | E |
2: Efficacy Message | .08 | 124 | AIB | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | M | 68 | C | E |
Ordoñana et al., 2009 | ||||||||||||||
1: No Efficacy Message | .59 | 45 | IB | N | Y | Y | O | PP | N | — | L | 83 | I | E |
2: Efficacy Message | .52 | 47 | IB | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | N | — | L | 83 | I | E |
Pengchit, 2010 | 1.25 | 124 | B | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
Pepper & Nettle, 2014 | ||||||||||||||
1: Study 1 | −.22 | 72 | IB | N | — | — | R | PP | Y | SEE | S | 46 | I | E |
2: Study 2 | .14 | 66 | IB | N | N | Y | R | PP | Y | SEE | S | 40 | I | E |
Powell, 1965 | ||||||||||||||
1: Threat to Listener | −.52 | 28 | A | N | N | N | O | PP | Y | — | S | 0 | I | — |
2: Threat to Family | .84 | 28 | A | N | N | N | O | PP | Y | — | S | 0 | I | — |
3: Threat to Nation | .01 | 24 | A | N | N | N | O | PP | Y | — | S | 0 | I | — |
Priolo & Milhabet, 2008 | ||||||||||||||
1: Study 1, Smokers Committed to Quitting | .72 | 60 | I | N | — | — | R | PP | Y | SEE | S | 85 | I | E |
2: Study 1, Smokers Not Committed to Quitting | −.16 | 60 | I | N | — | — | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 85 | I | E |
3: Study 2, Smokers Committed to Smoking | −.55 | 60 | I | N | — | — | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 85 | I | E |
4: Study 2, Smokers Not Committed to Smoking | .49 | 60 | I | N | — | — | R | PP | Y | SEE | S | 85 | I | E |
Radelfinger, 1965 | .90 | 131 | I | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | E |
Raleigh, 2002 | ||||||||||||||
1: Males, Low Response Costs | −.19 | 17 | I | Y | N | Y | R | PP | N | — | S | 0 | I | E |
2: Males, High Response Costs | −.25 | 13 | I | N | N | Y | R | PP | N | — | S | 0 | I | E |
3: Females, Low Response Costs | −.24 | 11 | I | Y | N | Y | R | PP | N | — | S | 100 | I | E |
4: Females, High Response Costs | −.78 | 10 | I | N | N | Y | R | PP | N | — | S | 100 | I | E |
Ramirez & Lasater, 1976 | .00 | 462 | B | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | E |
Ramirez & Lasater, 1977 | .30 | 196 | B | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | E |
Rippetoe & Rogers, 1987 | .69 | 128 | I | Y | Y | Y | R | D | N | — | S | 100 | I | E |
Rodriguez, 1995 | ||||||||||||||
1: Bicycle Safety | .98 | 124 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | E |
2: Drinking | .54 | 125 | A | N | N | Y | R | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | E |
3: Tetanus Vaccine | .42 | 120 | A | N | Y | Y | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
Rogers & Deckner, 1975 | ||||||||||||||
1: Study 1 | .25 | 116 | AIB | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | E |
2: Study 2 | .38 | 152 | AI | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | E |
Rogers & Mewborn, 1976 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Efficacy | −.04 | 44 | I | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
2: High Efficacy | .41 | 44 | I | N | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
Rogers & Thistlethwaite, 1970 | ||||||||||||||
1: Smokers | .47 | 40 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | E |
2: Non-Smokers | .82 | 40 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | SEE | S | — | I | — |
Rosen et al., 1982 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Self-Esteem | .14 | 28 | I | N | Y | Y | O | PP | Y | — | S | 49 | I | E |
2: High Self-Esteem | −.24 | 28 | I | N | Y | Y | O | PP | Y | — | S | 49 | I | E |
Rosenthal, 1997 | ||||||||||||||
1: Peptic Ulcers | .01 | 70 | AI | N | N | Y | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
2: Heart Disease | .26 | 70 | AI | N | N | Y | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
Roskos-Ewoldsen et al., 2004 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Efficacy | −.19 | 55 | AI | N | N | Y | R | D | Y | — | S | 100 | I | — |
2: High Efficacy | .10 | 55 | AI | Y | N | Y | R | D | Y | — | S | 100 | I | — |
Ruiter et al., 2003 | .17 | 130 | AI | N | Y | Y | R | D | Y | — | S | 100 | I | — |
Schmitt & Blass, 2008 | .56 | 30 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEE | S | — | I | — |
Schoenbachler & Whittler, 1996 | .00 | 248 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | — |
Self & Rogers, 1990 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Efficacy | −.55 | 42 | I | N | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | S | 55 | I | — |
2: High Efficacy | .64 | 42 | I | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | S | 55 | I | — |
Shehryar & Hunt, 2005 | ||||||||||||||
1: Study 1, Low Commitment to Drunk Driving | .01 | 45 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEE | S | 57 | I | — |
2: Study 1, High Commitment to Drunk Driving | -1.07 | 45 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 57 | I | — |
3: Study 2, Low Commitment to Drunk Driving, No Delay | −.79 | 25 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEE | S | 57 | I | — |
4: Study 2, High Commitment to Drunk Driving, No Delay | .12 | 25 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 57 | I | — |
5: Study 2, High Commitment to Drunk Driving, Delay | −1.17 | 25 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 57 | I | — |
Shelton & Rogers, 1981 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Empathy | .85 | 56 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
2: High Empathy | .26 | 56 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
Shen, 2011 | .59 | 174 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | 66 | I | L |
Siero et al., 1984 | .24 | 269 | B | N | Y | Y | R | D | Y | — | L | 100 | I | L |
Skilbeck et al., 1977 | ||||||||||||||
1: Single Exposure | −.99 | 40 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | SEE | M | 100 | I | E |
2: Multiple Exposures | −1.23 | 46 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | SEE | M | 100 | I | E |
Smalec & Klingle, 2000 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Efficacy | −.60 | 22 | B | N | Y | Y | O | PP | N | SEH | S | 81 | I | L |
2: High Efficacy | 1.40 | 22 | B | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | N | SEH | S | 81 | I | L |
Smart & Fejer, 1974 | ||||||||||||||
1: Marijuana, Non-Users | −.05 | 856 | I | N | Y | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | S | — | I | — |
2: Marijuana, Users | −.17 | 249 | I | N | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | E |
3: Fictional Drug | 1.66 | 194 | I | N | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
Smerecnik & Ruiter, 2010 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Efficacy | −.19 | 30 | I | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | 65 | I | L |
2: High Efficacy | .76 | 30 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | 65 | I | L |
Smith & Stutts, 2003 | ||||||||||||||
1: Males, Overall | .29 | 79 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | L | 0 | I | — |
2: Females, Overall | .38 | 76 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | L | 100 | I | — |
3: White Subjects | .51 | 61 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | L | 49 | I | — |
4: Hispanic Subjects | .29 | 55 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | L | 49 | I | — |
5: African-American Subjects | .41 | 24 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | L | 49 | I | — |
Stainback & Rogers, 1983 | ||||||||||||||
1: Immediate Post-Test | .65 | 38 | I | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | S | — | I | — |
2: Delayed Post-Test | 1.30 | 38 | I | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | S | — | I | — |
Stark et al., 2008 | ||||||||||||||
1: Lozenges | .24 | 90 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | 73 | I | E |
2: Reduced-Exposure Cigarettes | .42 | 90 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | 73 | I | E |
3: Oral Tobacco | .34 | 90 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | 73 | I | E |
Stephenson & Witte, 1998 | .47 | 92 | AI | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | SEH | S | 56 | I | E |
Struckman-Johnson et al., 1990 | ||||||||||||||
1: Males | −.10 | 96 | I | N | — | — | R | PP | N | — | S | 0 | I | — |
2: Females | −.03 | 95 | I | N | — | — | R | PP | N | — | S | 100 | I | — |
Sturges & Rogers, 1996 | ||||||||||||||
1: Kids, Low Coping | .02 | 30 | I | N | Y | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | S | 50 | I | — |
2: Kids, High Coping | .43 | 37 | I | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | S | 50 | I | — |
3: Teens, Low Coping | .05 | 23 | I | N | Y | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | S | 50 | I | — |
4: Teens, High Coping | .32 | 22 | I | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | S | 50 | I | — |
5: Adults, Low Coping | −.34 | 31 | I | N | Y | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | S | 50 | I | — |
6: Adults, High Coping | .27 | 38 | I | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | S | 50 | I | — |
Tanner et al., 1991 | .81 | 60 | I | N | — | — | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | — |
Taubman Ben-Ari et al., 2000 | ||||||||||||||
1: Study 1, Low Driving-Related Self-Esteem | 1.06 | 27 | I | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEE | S | 0 | C | — |
2: Study 1, High Driving-Related Self-Esteem | .08 | 27 | I | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 0 | C | — |
3: Study 2, Low Driving-Related Self-Esteem | −.76 | 27 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEE | S | 0 | C | — |
4: Study 2, High Driving-Related Self-Esteem | .20 | 28 | B | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | 0 | C | — |
ter Horst et al., 1985 | −.12 | 107 | B | Y | Y | Y | O | PP | N | — | S | — | I | E |
Thornton et al., 2000 | −.65 | 112 | I | N | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | — | I | L |
Umeh & Stanley, 2005 | −.04 | 100 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | N | — | S | 0 | I | — |
Umeh, 2012 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Credibility Source | .06 | 134 | I | Y | Y | N | R | D | Y | — | S | 100 | I | — |
2: High Credibility Source | .25 | 134 | I | Y | Y | N | R | D | Y | — | S | 100 | I | — |
Venkatesan, 2010 | 3.01 | 72 | A | Y | N | Y | O | PP | N | — | L | 100 | I | E |
Weinstein et al., 1990 | .24 | 264 | I | Y | Y | Y | O | D | Y | — | S | — | I | — |
Welbourne et al., 2008 | .00 | 308 | A | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | — |
Wheatley & Oshikawa, 1970 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Anxiety | .30 | 49 | A | N | N | N | O | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | — |
2: High Anxiety | −.14 | 47 | A | N | N | N | O | PP | Y | — | S | — | I | — |
Will et al., 2009 | .54 | 352 | AB | Y | Y | N | O | PP | N | — | S | 80 | I | E |
Witte & Morrison, 1995 | −.32 | 122 | AIB | Y | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | L | 45 | I | — |
Witte et al., 1998 | .03 | 96 | AIB | N | Y | Y | R | PP | N | — | S | 100 | I | — |
Wong & Cappella, 2009 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Efficacy | −.01 | 277 | I | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | 47 | I | E |
1: High Efficacy | .81 | 278 | I | Y | Y | N | R | PP | Y | — | S | 47 | I | E |
Wurtele & Maddux, 1987 | ||||||||||||||
1: No Efficacy Message | 1.42 | 40 | I | N | N | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | M | 100 | I | E |
2: Self-Efficacy Message | −.11 | 40 | I | Y | N | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | M | 100 | I | E |
3: Response-Efficacy Message | .75 | 40 | I | Y | N | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | M | 100 | I | E |
4: Both Efficacy Messages | 1.22 | 40 | I | Y | N | Y | R | PP | N | SEE | M | 100 | I | E |
Wurtele, 1988 | .82 | 49 | IB | Y | N | Y | R | PP | N | — | M | 100 | I | E |
Yoon & Tinkman, 2013 | ||||||||||||||
1: Low Past Threat, Nonhumor Ads | −.28 | 48 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | — | I | E |
2: Low Past Threat, Humor Ads | .60 | 48 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | — | I | E |
3: High Past Threat, Nonhumor Ads | .62 | 48 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | — | I | E |
4: High Past Threat, Humor Ads | −.58 | 48 | AI | N | Y | N | R | PP | Y | SEH | S | — | I | E |
Note: d = Standardized mean effect size. N = Sample size for treatment plus comparison. AIB = Whether d was based on attitude (A), intention (I), and/or behavior (B) outcomes. EFF = Whether an efficacy statement was included (Y) or not (N). Sev = Whether the treatment message was manipulated to be higher in depicted severity than the comparison message (Y) or not (N). Sus = Whether the treatment message was manipulated to be higher in depicted susceptibility than the comparison message (Y) or not (N). OR = Whether the recommended behavior was one-time (O) or repeated (R). DPP = Whether the recommended behavior was detection (D) or prevention/promotion (PP). DP = Whether the word death was present in the message (Y) or not (N). SE = Whether the recommended behavior was self-esteem enhancing (SEE) or self-esteem hindering (SEH). Delay = Whether the outcome followed exposure to the message by less than 24 hours (S), 1–14 days (M), or more than 14 days (L). %F = Percent of sample that was female (0–100%). IC = Whether the sample was from an individualist (I) or collectivist (C) culture. SOC = Whether the sample was in the early (E) or late (L) stages of change. Dash (–) indicates the variable was not relevant for the study.
Moderators related to message content
To test hypotheses concerning the message content, we coded messages’ amount of depicted fear, inclusion (or absence) of efficacy statements, and levels of depicted susceptibility and severity.
Amount of depicted fear
To test the linear and curvilinear hypotheses, we coded whether studies included a moderate depicted fear group. To qualify, studies had to contain at least three experimental groups that were exposed to different levels of depicted fear. Thus, a study containing a high depicted fear group, a moderate depicted fear group, and a low depicted fear group would be included, whereas a study containing a high depicted fear group, a low depicted fear group, and a neutral control group would not. As noted above, an appropriate test of the linear and curvilinear hypotheses requires a comparison between high and moderate depicted fear; thus, the moderate group must represent a level of depicted fear between high and low (rather than between high and none). In the entire database (k = 248), 21 samples included more than two experimental groups exposed to varying levels of depicted fear. To test the linear and curvilinear hypotheses, we calculated effect sizes (d) comparing outcomes for the highest versus middle depicted fear groups (the calculation of these effect sizes followed the same procedure detailed above for the calculation of treatment versus comparison effect sizes). The moderate depicted fear groups (total N = 1,626) were not included in other analyses (the studies and corresponding effect sizes included in this analysis can be found in Table 3)
Table 3.
d
|
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
FirstAuthor | NH | NM | Combined Outcomes | Attitudes | Intentions | Behaviors |
Beck & Davis, 1978 | ||||||
1: Low Interest | 14 | 15 | .28 | .28 | – | – |
2: High Interest | 14 | 16 | −.45 | −.45 | – | – |
Burnett, 1981 | 36 | 43 | .51 | .73 | .28 | – |
Chu, 1966 | ||||||
1: Low Efficacy | 100 | 125 | 1.06 | – | – | 1.06 |
2: Medium Efficacy | 112 | 121 | −.18 | – | – | −.18 |
3: High Efficacy | 120 | 112 | .36 | – | – | .36 |
Hill & Gardner, 1980 | ||||||
1: Repressors | 11 | 13 | −.07 | – | – | −.07 |
2: Sensitizers | 15 | 14 | .65 | – | – | .65 |
Leventhal et al., 1965 | ||||||
1: No Prior Vaccination | 22 | 34 | .09 | – | – | .09 |
2: Prior Vaccination | 29 | 30 | −2.58 | – | – | −2.58 |
Ramirez & Lasater, 1976 | 231 | 231 | .00 | – | – | .00 |
Schoenbachler & Whittler, 1996 | 125 | 123 | .00 | .00 | .00 | – |
Skilbeck et al., 1977 | ||||||
1: Single Exposure | 25 | 18 | .58 | – | – | .58 |
2: Multiple Exposures | 17 | 18 | −.43 | – | – | −.43 |
Smart & Fejer, 1974 | ||||||
1: Marijuana, Non-Users | 122 | 119 | −.26 | – | −.26 | – |
2: Marijuana, Users | 414 | 441 | −.03 | – | −.03 | – |
Yoon & Tinkman, 2013 | ||||||
1: Low Past Threat, Nonhumor Ads | 24 | 24 | −.13 | −.23 | −.04 | – |
2: Low Past Threat, Humor Ads | 24 | 24 | .30 | .41 | .19 | – |
3: High Past Threat, Nonhumor Ads | 24 | 24 | .19 | .11 | .26 | – |
4: High Past Threat, Humor Ads | 24 | 24 | −.48 | −.64 | −.32 | – |
Thornton et al., 2000 | 56 | 57 | −.72 | – | −.72 | – |
Note: d = Standardized mean effect size. NH = Sample size for the high depicted fear group. NM = Sample size for the medium depicted fear group. Combined outcomes = Average of all attitude, intention, and behavior measures. Dash (–) indicates the variable was not relevant for the study. The attitude, intention, and behavior measures are analyzed separately in Appendix A.
Efficacy statements
For each article, we dichotomously coded whether or not an efficacy message was embedded in the fear appeal. The efficacy message could have focused on self-efficacy (e.g., emphasizing that people have a built-in urge for physical activity and this basic human physical need will make it easy to begin a regular exercise program; Wurtele & Maddux, 1987), response-efficacy (e.g., emphasizing that exercise leads to higher levels of high-density lipoprotein and thus prevents heart attacks; Wurtele & Maddux, 1987), or both (e.g., highlighting that condoms substantially reduce the risk of HIV transmission if used correctly and are easy to use consistently; Witte & Morrison, 1995).
Depicted susceptibility and severity
For each article, we coded whether depicted severity was manipulated to be higher in the treatment group relative to the comparison group (e.g., the treatment group received a message emphasizing the drastic consequences of not wearing bicycle helmets; Rodriguez, 1995) and whether depicted susceptibility was manipulated to be higher in the treatment group relative to the comparison group (e.g., the treatment group received a message focusing on how coffee consumption will likely lead the message recipient to develop fibromyalgia; Lieberman & Chaiken, 1992).
Moderators related to behavior characteristics
To test hypotheses concerning the targeted behavior, we coded whether the fear appeals recommended behaviors that were one-time or recurring and whether the behavior was a detection or prevention/promotion behavior. We also coded whether death was mentioned when discussing the behavior, whether the behavior was measured immediately versus after a delay, and whether the recommended behaviors was self-esteem enhancing or self-esteem hindering.
One-time versus repeated behaviors
We coded whether the recommended behaviors concerned one-time-only instances (e.g., signing up for a stress management training; Das et al., 2003) or would need to be enacted over an extended period of time (e.g., regularly using child safety devices when traveling by car; Chang et al., 1989).
Detection versus prevention/promotion
For each article, we coded if the recommended behavior was a detection behavior (e.g., getting tested for syphilis; Fukada 1975) or a prevention/promotion behavior (e.g., attending a training to prevent repetitive stress injury; Pengchit, 2010). We initially attempted to code prevention and promotion behaviors separately. However, due to the nature of these constructs, it was often difficult to discern how participants would construe a behavior (e.g., did participants conceptualize exercising as promoting a healthy BMI or preventing obesity?). As the relevant hypothesis solely concerned fear appeals being more effective when recommending detection (vs. prevention/promotion) behaviors, promotion and prevention behaviors were collapsed into a single code.
Mentioning death, self-esteem relevance, and time delays
We created a dichotomous code for whether or not the message explicitly used the word death. Messages dealing with behaviors or issues that could clearly lead to death were still coded as non-death if the word death was not explicitly mentioned within the message itself (e.g., messages about smoking or HIV/AIDS that did not explicitly mention death as one of the potential consequences; Insko et al., 1965; McMath & Prentice-Dunn, 2005; Raleigh, 2002; Witte & Allen, 2000). This decision allowed for a more stringent test of TMT hypotheses, and provided an even distribution of death versus non-death conditions, which avoids the potential confound of death messages always being about more severe topics than non-death messages.
Self-esteem relevance
We coded whether the recommended behavior was self-esteem hindering or self-esteem enhancing. Self-esteem hindering behaviors were intended to replace existing behaviors that allowed message recipients to derive self-esteem. Samples were coded as containing a self-esteem hindering behavior if the researchers specifically measured self-esteem for the existing behavior being targeted by the fear appeal and described the sample as high (e.g., high driving-related self-esteem; Taubman Ben-Ari et al., 2000), if the sample was designated as committed to the existing behavior (e.g., smokers that were highly committed to smoking; Priolo & Milhabet, 2008), or if the existing behavior is one that people typically engage in to improve self-esteem and/or physical attractiveness (e.g., tanning or bulimia; Janssen et al., 2013; Smalec & Klingle, 2000).
In contrast, self-esteem enhancing behaviors have the potential to provide individuals with self-esteem. Samples were coded as containing a self-esteem enhancing behavior if the recommended behavior is commonly associated with the pursuit of improved self-esteem and/or physical attractiveness (e.g., fear appeals recommending a healthy diet to decrease BMI; Goldenberg & Arndt, 2008). Samples were also coded as self-esteem enhancing when fear appeals targeted behaviors that the audience had clearly already made the choice to forego (e.g., antismoking ads directed at non-smokers; Insko et al., 1965) because message recipients should generally be able to derive self-esteem by continuing to avoid engaging in the discouraged behavior (e.g., non-smokers who are told that smoking is bad and smoking abstinence is good should feel as though their decision to abstain from smoking reflects positively on them). Thus, studies were coded as self-esteem enhancing if the recommended behavior could improve self-esteem via the pursuit of physical attractiveness (e.g., exercise; Wurtele & Maddux, 1987), if the addressed behavior was not relevant for the sample (e.g., anti-smoking ads for non-smokers; Insko et al., 1965; Smart & Fejer, 1974), if the sample was designated as not committed to the behavior in question (e.g., smokers that were not committed to smoking; Priolo & Milhabet, 2008), or if the researchers specifically measured self-esteem related to the existing behavior being targeted by the fear appeal and described the sample as low (e.g., low driving-related self-esteem; Taubman Ben-Ari et al., 2000).
Time delay
We coded the amount of time between the fear appeal and the measurement of the outcome variable using three discrete categories: (a) The measure occurred the same day as the fear appeal exposure (e.g., Taubman Ben-Ari et al., 2000; Cho & Salmon, 2006; Nabi et al., 2008; Smart & Fejer, 1974; Stainback & Rogers, 1983); (b) the measure occurred one to fourteen days after fear appeal exposure (e.g., Berkowitz, 1998; Kirscht et al., 1978; Muthusamy et al., 2009); and (c) the measure occurred more than fourteen days after fear appeal exposure (e.g., Bagley & Low, 1992; Smith & Stutts, 2003; Witte & Morrison, 1995). We used categories because delayed outcomes often occurred within a specified range – e.g., participants returned to the lab during the following two weeks, but the exact number of days was not specified.
Moderators related to the audience
To test hypotheses concerning the audience portion of our framework, we coded the gender composition of the sample, whether the sample was from a collectivist or individualist country, and the transtheoretical model stage of change that was applicable to the sample.
Gender composition
We coded the percent of the sample that was female.
Collectivism and individualism
We dichotomously coded whether each study’s sample came from a primarily collectivist culture (e.g., East Asian cultures like South Korea, Japan, and Taiwan; Chu, 1966; Fukada, 1973; 1975; 1988; Kim et al., 2009) or a primarily individualist culture (e.g., Western cultures like Australia, Canada, and the United States; Beck, 1984; Brouwers & Sorrentino, 1993; Dahl et al., 2003; Hill & Gardner, 1980; Jones & Owen, 2006; LaTour & Tanner, 2003; Lewis et al., 2010; Smart & Fejer, 1974).
Stage of change
We coded the transtheoretical model’s stage of change that was most applicable to the audience. As most studies did not specifically measure this variable, we designed a conservative coding scheme to ensure we could include the maximum number of reports in this analysis while avoiding misclassifications. The early-effectiveness and late-effectiveness hypotheses both make predictions that compare individuals in the first three stages of the model (precontemplation, contemplation, and preparation) versus the last two stages of the model (action and maintenance). Thus, we created a dichotomous code indicating whether the sample was in the early or late stages of the model.
Samples were considered precontemplation if there was a clear indication that it was a sample merely at risk for a given behavior (e.g., participants who were designated as noncompliant with safe sex recommendations; Raleigh, 2002), or participants were being persuaded about a fictitious or not well-known disease/risk for which they had clearly not been engaging in protective action beforehand (e.g., hypoglycemia; de Hoog et al., 2008). We excluded samples in which the participants may have been in the precontemplation stage but for which there were no pretest measures available (e.g., if the sample was given a message about drinking and driving but there were no baseline measures available to indicate whether or not the sample had engaged in drunk driving in the past; Shehryar & Hunt, 2005). Samples were considered contemplation or preparation if there was a clear indication that they were already preparing to engage in the recommended action (e.g., a sample of women under 50 years old who had not yet received mammograms, but the majority of whom stated they intended to receive mammograms after age 50; Jones & Owen, 2006). Samples were classified into the action/maintenance category if participants had explicitly been engaging in the recommended behavior (e.g., a message promoted breast self-exams and 80% of the sample indicated they already performed breast self-exams regularly; Siero, Kok, & Pruyn, 1984) or if they were recruited from a population that would definitionally be in this stage (e.g., patients receiving treatment in alcohol rehabilitation clinics; Brown, 1979).
Results
All analyses were conducted in R using the meta-analytic software package metafor, version 1.9.4 (Viechtbauer, 2010). We conducted all analyses using random- and fixed-effects analyses. As both types of analyses produced comparable results, we present the random-effects analyses.
Distribution of Effect Sizes
We first analyzed the distribution of effect sizes in our sample to determine whether there were biases in study retrieval and inclusion. Figure 1 displays a forest plot for our meta-analytic database, and Figure 2 displays the corresponding funnel plot. In a forest plot, each study is represented by a horizontal line that indicates the confidence interval for the study’s effect size. By examining a forest plot, it is possible to assess the precision of effect size estimates from each study. Further, forest plots can also be used to assess the distribution of effect sizes across studies. As can be seen in the forest plot, the precision of effect size estimates varies across studies, with most studies displaying moderate precision. Further, the distribution of effect sizes appears to be roughly continuous and normal, which indicates a lack of inclusion bias. If no retrieval or inclusion bias is present in a meta-analytic database, the distribution of effect sizes in the funnel plot should be centered on and symmetric around the mean effect size, with smaller variability toward the top of the figure. If retrieval or inclusion biases are present, then the distribution should be asymmetric around the mean effect size. As can be seen in the figure, the distribution appears quite symmetric with smaller variability toward the top of the plot. We conducted a formal test of funnel plot asymmetry known as Begg and Mazumdar’s rank correlation test, which is a non-parametric correlation of the effect sizes with their corresponding standard errors (Begg & Mazumdar, 1994). If this correlation is significantly different from zero, there is evidence of inclusion bias. The rank correlation was r = −.02, p = .67. Thus, there is no evidence of retrieval or inclusion bias.
Another way of testing for biases is to use the normal quantile plot method (Wang & Bushman, 1999). In a normal quantile plot, the observed values of a variable are plotted against the expected values given normality. If the sample of effect sizes is from a normal distribution, data points cluster around the diagonal; if the sample of effect sizes is biased by publication practices or eligibility criteria, data points deviate from the diagonal (Wang & Bushman, 1999). As can be seen from Figure 3, the effect sizes followed a straight line and generally fell within the 95% confidence interval of the normality line, and thus there is no evidence of retrieval or inclusion bias.
Average Effect Size and Between-Effects Variability
The average weighted effect size comparing outcomes for treatment to comparison groups was d = 0.29 with a 95% CI of [0.22, 0.35]. Therefore, fear appeals have a significant and positive effect on outcomes. That is, relative to participants in comparison groups, participants in treatment groups (i.e., those exposed to relatively high levels of depicted fear) had attitudes, intentions, and behaviors that were more in line with the position advocated by the fear appeal. There was also significant heterogeneity among effect sizes Q(247) = 1,287, I2 = 85.11, p < .0001.
For studies that included a manipulation check of subjectively experienced fear, we coded this variable and calculated d for treatment versus comparison groups using the same methods employed for primary outcomes. We included all measures that asked respondents to report their current levels of fear (e.g., Cauberghe, De Pelsmacker, Janssens & Dens, 2009; Cho & Salmon, 2006; Nabi et al., 2008). Based on the 71 samples that included such manipulation checks, fear appeals were generally successful at inducing experienced fear, such that treatment groups reported more fear than comparison groups, d = 1.00 (95% CI: [0.83, 1.18]), Q(70) = 697, I2 = 90.67, p < .0001. Importantly, this result should be taken as an estimate of how much fear was induced by the particular messages used in this sample, rather than an estimate of how much fear is induced by fear appeal messages in general.
Moderator Tests
To test our hypotheses of interest (see Table 1), we primarily conducted moderator analyses by calculating weighted effect sizes and corresponding 95% CIs for each level of our moderator variables (i.e., we meta-analyzed samples within each moderator level separately to produce an overall effect size estimate for that level). If the CIs for two moderator levels are not overlapping, then those levels of the moderator are significantly different from each other. In contrast, if the CIs are overlapping, then those levels of the moderator are not different from each other. We also conducted moderated meta-regressions to analyze all moderator variables; the results were the same as the 95% CI analyses and are thus not presented here. Table 5 displays average weighted effect sizes and corresponding 95% CIs for all levels of our moderator variables.
Table 5.
MBA Aspect | Variable | Level | d | 95% CI | k |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Message | Efficacy statements | Included | .43 | [.31, .55] | 92 |
Excluded | .21 | [.13, .29] | 154 | ||
| |||||
Depicted susceptibility and severity | Both | .39 | [.28, .50] | 78 | |
Susceptibility | .43 | [.08, .79] | 20 | ||
Severity | .23 | [.13, .33] | 125 | ||
Neither | .12 | [−.03, .27] | 17 | ||
| |||||
Behavior | One-time versus repeated | One-time | .43 | [.30, .56] | 82 |
Repeated | .21 | [.14, .29] | 166 | ||
| |||||
Detection versus promotion/prevention | Detection | .35 | [.21, .49] | 40 | |
PP | .27 | [.20, .35] | 208 | ||
| |||||
Death and self-esteem | SEE, DP | .39 | [.11, .67] | 15 | |
SEE, DA | .22 | [−.04, .47] | 23 | ||
SEH, DP | −.11 | [−.41, .18] | 18 | ||
SEH, DA | .48 | [.00, .96] | 6 | ||
| |||||
Death and delay | DP, same day | .16 | [.04, .27] | 70 | |
DP, 1–14 days | .79 | [.21, 1.37] | 5 | ||
DP, 14+ days | .35 | [.19, .51] | 14 | ||
DA, same day | .34 | [.25, .44] | 124 | ||
DA, 1–14 days | .02 | [−.29, .33] | 18 | ||
DA, 14+ days | .46 | [.03, .88] | 13 | ||
| |||||
Audience | Culture | Collectivist | .47 | [.27, .66] | 29 |
Individualist | .26 | [.19, .33] | 219 | ||
| |||||
Stage of change | Early | .30 | [.21, .40] | 150 | |
Late | .34 | [.14, .54] | 30 |
Note: SE = Self-esteem. DP = Death present in message. DA = Death absent in message. PP = Promotion/prevention. SEE = Self-esteem enhancing recommended behaviors. SEH = Self-esteem hindering recommended behaviors. d = Standardized mean effect size estimated meta-analytically for the indicated moderator level. 95% CI = The 95% confidence interval for d. k = The number of studies for each moderator level.
Study Characteristics
For descriptive purposes, we recorded the following for each sample: (a) Study source (journal article, unpublished dissertation or thesis, or conference paper); (b) institution of the paper’s first author (university/college, research center); (c) the sampled population (general population, college students, high school students, children, other); (d) whether participants were run individually or in groups; (e) the study setting (laboratory, field); (f) the specificity of the message – whether the message targeted a single specific outcome (e.g., signing up for a training to prevent stress-related illness; Das et al., 2003), multiple specific outcomes (e.g., consuming calcium and performing weight-bearing exercises to prevent osteoporosis; Klohn & Rogers, 1991), or multiple non-specific outcomes (e.g., general recommendations to improve diet and increase exercise without mentioning specific dietary concerns or specific forms of exercise; Kirscht & Haefner, 1973); (g) whether the study measured subjective fear; (h) the type of media used to present the message (text information, pictures/videos); (i) whether the message targeted a health relevant outcome; and (j) the domain of the study’s targeted issue (dental hygiene, driving safety, HIV/STDs, drinking/drugs, smoking, cancer prevention, disease prevention, general safety, environment/society, other). As can be seen in Table 4, none of these methodological factors moderated fear appeal effectiveness – within each factor, 95% CIs for each factor level overlap for all levels of all factors. In addition to these factors, we also recorded publication year, average age of participants, and sample size. Separate meta-regressions for each of these three variables revealed that none were related to fear appeal effectiveness: b = −0.0029 (SE = 0.0022, p = .18, 95% CI: [−0.0072, 0.0013]), b = −0.0046 (SE = 0.0039, p = .23, 95% CI: [−0.0122, 0.0030]), and b = 0.0000 (SE = 0.0002, p = .91, 95% CI: [−0.0003, 0.0003]), respectively for publication year, average age of sample, and sample size.
Table 4.
Variable | Level | d | 95% CI | k |
---|---|---|---|---|
Study source | Journal article | .28 | [.21, .35] | 226 |
Other | .32 | [.00, .63] | 22 | |
| ||||
Institution of first author | University or college | .29 | [.21, .36] | 228 |
Research center | .25 | [−.05, .55] | 14 | |
| ||||
Sampled population | General population | .14 | [.00, .29] | 45 |
University students | .34 | [.24, .43] | 145 | |
High school students | .35 | [.09, .60] | 17 | |
Children | .25 | [−.03, .53] | 13 | |
Other | .18 | [−.04, .39] | 24 | |
| ||||
Participants run in groups | Yes | .30 | [.21, .38] | 135 |
No | .32 | [.20, .43] | 75 | |
| ||||
Study setting | Laboratory | .25 | [.15, .35] | 137 |
Field | .31 | [.22, .41] | 107 | |
| ||||
Message specificity | Single specific target | .30 | [.22, .39] | 182 |
Multiple specific targets | .22 | [.02, .42] | 26 | |
Multiple non-specific targets | .26 | [.10, .42] | 35 | |
| ||||
Measured fear in the study | Yes | .30 | [.18, .41] | 71 |
No | .28 | [.20, .36] | 177 | |
| ||||
Media of message | Text information | .36 | [.25, .47] | 93 |
Pictures/videos | .20 | [.09, .31] | 73 | |
| ||||
Health related message | Yes | .28 | [.20, .35] | 202 |
No | .31 | [.13, .49] | 43 | |
| ||||
Study domain | Dental hygiene | .06 | [−.16, .28] | 14 |
Driving safety | .11 | [−.10, .33] | 27 | |
HIV/STDs | .37 | [.20, .54] | 33 | |
Drinking/drugs | .49 | [.25, .74] | 20 | |
Smoking | .26 | [.13, .40] | 40 | |
Cancer prevention | .16 | [−.01, .34] | 26 | |
Disease prevention | .40 | [.19, .61] | 51 | |
General safety | .22 | [.03, .40] | 13 | |
Environment/society | .24 | [.02, .45] | 13 | |
Other | .39 | [.11, .68] | 11 |
Note: d = Standardized mean effect size estimated meta-analytically for the indicated moderator level. 95% CI = The 95% confidence interval for d. k = The number of studies for each moderator level.
Tests of message content hypotheses
Message content: Depicted fear
To test the linear and curvilinear hypotheses, we calculated an average weighted effect size comparing groups that were exposed to moderate depicted fear versus high depicted fear. The linear hypothesis predicts that this effect size should be positive and significant, whereas the curvilinear hypothesis predicts that this effect size should be negative and significant. The combined effect size was d = −0.05 with a 95% CI of [−0.34, 0.24] and Q(20) = 154 (I2 = 92.89, p < .0001). Therefore, outcomes did not differ for groups exposed to moderate versus high depicted fear. Instead of supporting either the linear or curvilinear hypothesis, this result suggests that depicted fear may have a maximum effective value, beyond which there is no impact of depicting additional fear. This finding may have implications for practitioners using fear appeals - i.e., once a message depicts moderate fear, there is no value in depicting additional fear, but depicting additional fear will not lead to negative effects.
One caveat is that this analysis was only based on 21 samples. However, to our knowledge, this is the largest and most valid test of the linear and curvilinear hypotheses to date. Specifically, to ensure that the test concerned high depicted fear versus moderate depicted fear, we only included studies with at least three levels of depicted fear. Given that we obtained an overall positive effect of depicted fear when comparing treatment and comparison groups, the results here can be interpreted as supporting a modified version of the linear hypothesis. Specifically, depicted fear has significant positive effects, but depicted fear cannot be effectively manipulated indefinitely and results in diminishing returns beyond a certain point (rather than negative effects causing the message to backfire, as suggested by the curvilinear hypothesis). However, given the limited sample size, this conclusion should be confirmed in future research.
Message content: Efficacy statements
The strong and weak efficacy hypotheses both predict that inclusion of efficacy statements in a fear appeal will lead to increased effectiveness. The results support this hypothesis: Fear appeals were more effective when they included efficacy statements (95% CI: [0.31, 0.55]) than when they did not (95% CI: [0.13, 0.29]). However, the strong hypothesis predicts that fear appeals without efficacy messages will backfire and produce negative effects, whereas the weak hypothesis predicts that fear appeals without efficacy statements will simply produce less positive or null effects. The results clearly support the weak efficacy hypothesis and disconfirm the strong efficacy hypothesis. Thus, fear appeals are effective with or without efficacy statements, but the inclusion of efficacy statements is associated with increased effectiveness. These results confirm the conclusions of prior meta-analyses concerning the use of efficacy statements (de Hoog et al., 2007; Peters et al., 2012; Witte & Allen, 2000).
Message content: Depicted susceptibility and severity
The first hypothesis concerning depicted susceptibility and severity states that fear appeals high in depicted severity (but not depicted susceptibility) will positively influence attitudes but will not influence intentions or behaviors. The 95% CIs indicated that fear appeals that were only high in depicted severity had positive effects for attitudes (95% CI: [0.06, 0.37]) and intentions (95% CI: [0.20, 0.39]) but not behaviors (95% CI: [−0.08, 0.42]) (see Appendix A for the results of all analyses done separately for attitudes, intentions, and behavior). Although this hypothesis was not supported, our results partially replicated a previous meta-analytic finding in which high depicted severity influenced all three outcome measures (de Hoog et al., 2007). The second hypothesis is that fear appeals high in depicted susceptibility (but not severity) will positively influence intentions and behaviors but will not influence attitudes. The 95% CIs indicated that fear appeals that were only high in depicted susceptibility had positive effects for intentions (95% CI: [0.15, 0.59]) and behaviors (95% CI: [0.01, 0.88]) but not attitudes (95% CI: [−0.51, 1.47]). Therefore, this hypothesis was supported. The third hypothesis is that fear appeals with high depicted severity and high depicted susceptibility will positively influence attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. The 95% CIs confirmed this prediction and indicated that fear appeals high on both moderators had positive effects for attitudes (95% CI: [0.05, 0.38]), intentions (95% CI: [0.23, 0.47]), and behaviors (95% CI: [0.24, 0.63]). Further, the 95% CI for the focal outcome in our meta-analysis (the average of attitude, intention, and behavior outcomes) also supported this result: [0.28, 0.50]. Thus, when testing all three hypotheses, fear appeals generally had positive effects on attitudes, intentions, and behaviors when they were high in depicted severity and/or susceptibility.
Tests of the recommended behavior hypotheses
Recommended behavior: One-time versus repeated behaviors
According to Robertson’s (1975) single action theory, fear appeals that attempt to persuade people about one-time behaviors (e.g., getting vaccinated) should be more effective than fear appeals that attempt to persuade people about repeated behaviors (e.g., exercising multiple times per week every week). The results supported this hypothesis, such that fear appeals recommending one-time behaviors (95% CI: [0.30, 0.56]) were more effective than fear appeals recommending repeated behaviors (95% CI: [0.14, 0.29]). However, it is worth noting that fear appeals were effective for both types of recommended behaviors, and they were simply more effective for one-time behaviors.
Recommended behavior: Detection versus prevention/promotion behaviors
Based on hypotheses derived from prospect theory, several researchers have hypothesized that fear appeals should be more effective when recommending detection behaviors relative to prevention/promotion behaviors. The results did not support this hypothesis, as fear appeals recommending detection behaviors (95% CI: [0.21, 0.49]) and prevention/promotion behaviors (95% CI: [0.20, 0.38]) were equally effective.
Recommended behavior: Death and self-esteem
Based on predictions from TMT, fear appeals that mention death (versus not) should be more effective when the recommended behavior is self-esteem enhancing but less effective when the recommended behavior is self-esteem hindering. The results did not support these predictions, as fear appeals were equally effective when they mentioned death and recommended a self-esteem hindering behavior (95% CI: [−0.41, 0.18]), did not mention death and recommended a self-esteem hindering behavior (95% CI: [0.00, 0.96]), mentioned death and recommended a self-esteem enhancing behavior (95% CI: [0.11, 0.67]), or did not mention death and recommended a self-esteem enhancing behavior (95% CI: [−0.04, 0.47]). Thus, neither self-esteem hypotheses derived from TMT was supported.
Recommended behavior: Death and delay
A separate prediction derived from TMT is that fear appeals that mention death will be more effective if the recommended behavior is measured after a delay rather than immediately. These predictions were not supported. When fear appeals mentioned death, they were equally effective for outcomes that occurred the same day (95% CI: [0.04, 0.27]), between one and fourteen days after fear appeal exposure (95% CI: [0.21, 1.37]), or more than fourteen days later (95% CI: [0.19, 0.51]). Similarly, when fear appeals did not mention death, they were equally effective for outcomes that occurred the same day (95% CI: [0.25, 0.44]), between one and fourteen days after fear appeal exposure (95% CI: [−0.29, 0.33]), or more than fourteen days later (95% CI: [0.03, 0.88]). Therefore, the death and delay hypothesis was not supported.
Tests of the audience hypotheses
Audience: Gender
Based on predictions derived from regulatory fit theory, fear appeals should be more effective for women than men. We tested this hypothesis via meta-regression, using percent of the sample that was female as a predictor of effect size. This analysis produced a small but significant effect, b = 0.0031 (SE = 0.0012, 95% CI for the slope: [0.0007, 0.0055]), p < .0001. Therefore, for every 10% increase in the percent of the sample that is female, fear appeal effectiveness increases by approximately d = 0.03. Thus, the hypothesis was supported: Fear appeals are more effective for audiences with a larger percentage of female message recipients than male message recipients.
Audience: Collectivism versus individualism
Based on predictions derived from regulatory fit theory, fear appeals should be more effective for collectivist samples than individualist samples. The results did not support this hypothesis. Fear appeals were equally effective in studies conducted in collectivist countries (95% CI: [0.27, 0.66]) and individualist countries (95% CI: [0.19, 0.33]).
Audience: Stages of change
Based on the early-effectiveness hypothesis, fear appeals should be more effective for samples that occupy the first three stages of the stages of change model relative to the last two stages. In contrast, the late-effectiveness hypothesis predicts the opposite. Neither hypothesis was supported by the data because audiences in the early stages (95% CI: [0.21, 0.40]) and late stages (95% CI: [0.14, 0.54]) were equally impacted by fear appeals.
General Discussion
Fear appeals are effective. The present meta-analysis found that fear appeals were successful at influencing attitudes, intentions, and behaviors across nearly all conditions that were analyzed. Even when a moderator was unrelated to fear appeal effectiveness, fear appeals were still more effective than comparison treatments. Further, there was not one level of any moderator that we tested for which fear appeals backfired to produce worse outcomes relative to the comparison groups. These results are striking given the wide range of theories that attempt to specify conditions under which fear appeals should be ineffective or counter-productive (e.g., the curvilinear model, the strong efficacy hypothesis, the stage model) and given the numerous practitioners who make bold claims stating that fear appeals are futile or even dangerous (e.g., Drug Free Action Alliance, 2013; Kok et al., 2014; Ruiter et al., 2014). Rather, fear appeals consistently work, and through our meta-analysis we were able to identify various factors that can enhance their effectiveness to make them work even better. We believe that these results make important contributions to theory, practice, and policy.
A Message-Behavior-Audience Framework of Fear Appeals
We structured our review around a framework that considers three important aspects of any fear appeal communication: The message’s content, the recommended behavior, and the audience. This model is meant to be an organizing thread to help connect existing theories and research, and to identify areas in need of future research. Specifically, we believe this framework is useful for several reasons. First, each aspect (message, behavior, and audience) has the potential to vary independently of the others and may impact the communication’s effectiveness in ways scholars must consider. Second, this structure connects and organizes seemingly unrelated theories and hypotheses concerning fear appeals, including the linear model, the stage model, and hypotheses derived from prospect theory. Specifically, we found that fear appeals were more effective when the message depicted relatively high amounts of fear, included an efficacy message, and stressed susceptibility and severity related to the concerns being addressed (i.e., factors concerning the message). We also found that fear appeals were more effective when they recommended one-time only behaviors (i.e., a factor concerning the recommended behavior) and when audiences included a higher percentage of women (i.e., a factor concerning the audience).
Our framework also highlights that prior research has strongly focused on one particular aspect of fear appeals somewhat to the exclusion of the other aspects. Specifically, the bulk of prior research on fear appeals has investigated questions about the message’s content – indeed, of the prior meta-analyses on fear appeals, all of them addressed questions related to the message’s content while overlooking questions related to the recommended behavior and audience. However, this bias is clearly not due to a lack of interesting or potentially important effects concerning the behavior or audience, as significant effects emerged pertaining to each. Thus, we hope that our framework will help generate interest in research directed toward these previously under-studied aspects of fear appeal effectiveness.
Limitations
Four specific limitations are worth mentioning. First, as discussed in the introduction, the present results concern fear appeals rather than fear. That is, our meta-analysis did not compare people who were subjectively afraid to people who were subjectively unafraid, but rather we compared groups that were exposed to more or less fear inducing content. Consequently, all comparisons between the treatment and comparison groups must be interpreted as effects of exposure to depicted levels of fear rather than effects of fear per se. However, this feature is not unique to our analyses, and prior meta-analyses of fear appeals are subject to the same considerations (e.g., Boster & Mongeau, 1984; de Hoog et al., 2007; Peters et al., 2012; Sutton, 1984; Witte & Allen, 2000). As researchers and practitioners alike are typically concerned with how to design effective communications, knowledge of the effectiveness of fear appeals is quite useful.
Relatedly, although we were able to determine that the treatment groups generally experienced more subjective fear than the comparison groups by analyzing fear-related manipulation check questions, the majority of samples included no assessment of subjective fear (k = 177, which is 71% of samples in our database). This is a serious limitation of the existing literature for three reasons. First, if fear appeals are presumed to have an effect on outcomes by instilling fear in message recipients, it is important to verify that these messages actually evoke fear, and that it is the evoked fear that mediates the relation between message presentation and response. Indeed, many fear appeals may evoke emotions in addition to fear (e.g., disgust, anger), and these other emotions may partially (or in some cases fully) mediate the effects of fear appeals. Second, the lack of subjective fear measures makes it difficult (if not impossible) to equate fear appeal intensity across studies. What one research team refers to as low fear may represent what another research teams refers to as moderate fear or a control condition. However, the inclusion of subjective measures of fear in response to fear appeals would enable researchers to equate fear appeal intensity across studies and more precisely investigate effects via well-calibrated levels of fear. Finally, the lack of subjective fear measures makes it difficult for researchers interested in the effects of fear (rather than fear appeals) to investigate relevant hypotheses meta-analytically. All three of these issues can be easily resolved by including measures of subjective fear in future studies on fear appeals, and we therefore urge researchers to do so.
Third, our meta-analysis exclusively included experimental studies. As experiments often allow for increased internal validity at the cost of decreased external validity, it will be important for future research to investigate whether the present results generalize to naturalistic settings. For example, do fear appeals produce the same effects when used in real-world public health campaigns as they do when used in highly controlled experimental studies? Although we expect the results will generalize to such settings, future research will be necessary to definitively test this question.
The final limitation of note concerns the coding of variables in the current meta-analysis. Specifically, to test hypotheses related to TMT, studies were coded as either containing the word death or not. However, some studies did not include full texts for fear appeal messages, and thus it is possible that some messages did contain the word death but were nonetheless coded as not containing this word (however, studies were only coded as containing the word death if a portion of the message’s text was available that showed this word). Overall, it is likely that such miscodings would attenuate potential differences across conditions.
Future Directions
Experimental manipulations and mechanisms
The present meta-analysis only included experimental studies that compared treatment and comparison groups, and thus internal validity is good when considering the effects of relatively high versus low depicted fear. However, meta-analyses are a correlational research design, and thus many of the moderator analyses we conducted should be interpreted with this in mind. For example, does using fear appeals to target one-time behaviors versus recurring behaviors actually cause the fear appeals to be more effective, or are fear appeals that target one-time behaviors systematically different from fear appeals that target recurring behaviors along some other dimension that results in the observed difference? Future experimental work will be necessary to address such questions, and we therefore encourage researchers to experimentally test our moderator findings concerning variables that were not manipulated in the primary studies.
It is also important for future research to uncover the mechanisms behind the moderation effects we identified. For example, why are fear appeals more effective for one-time behaviors? A number of the hypotheses that we substantiated are relatively agnostic concerning mechanisms, and this is a serious gap in the current fear appeal literature. To truly understand fear appeal effectiveness, it is necessary to know why they work. This knowledge could then be used to design more effective fear appeals, and it could potentially be used for other types of communications as well. Although some of the theories investigated do discuss mechanism to some degree (e.g., EPPM; Witte, 1992), our updated review of the literature is consistent with conclusions from prior reviews that these mechanisms are often under-studied and are in need of additional research (e.g., Popova, 2012).
Relatedly, future research could benefit from developing methods to manipulate perceptions of certain variables that were found to be significant moderators. For example, fear appeals were more effective for one-time behaviors, but this knowledge is currently of little use to researchers or practitioners who address recurring behaviors. However, this knowledge could become useful if methods were developed to successfully re-frame recurring behaviors as one-time behaviors. Such methods would also allow for experimental tests of the relevant dimensions and mechanisms (e.g., test whether fear appeals can be made more effective for a particular behavior if the behavior is framed as one-time rather than recurring).
Linear effect of fear
Another important question to address in future research concerns the linear and curvilinear hypotheses tested in the present study. Strictly speaking, we did not find support for either model. High levels of depicted fear did not lead to different outcomes than moderate depicted fear, suggesting that high and moderate depictions of fear produce similar results. However, the reason for this is unclear – were the high fear messages unsuccessful at evoking more subjective fear than the moderate messages, or is there simply a point beyond which additional fear (depicted or subjective) confers no benefit? To explore these possibilities, future studies should examine a large range of depicted fear along with measures of subjectively experienced fear.
Integration of findings
Finally, we believe that an additional benefit of our framework is its ability to guide researchers in generating future research questions. As mentioned, organizing the existing literature under this framework highlights the relative dearth of research addressing the behavior and audience aspects of the model relative to the message aspect. A number of interesting questions have yet to be explored in these areas. For example, are fear appeals more effective if they address behaviors concerning the self or close others (e.g., one’s children, romantic partners), public or private behaviors (e.g., exercising at a gym versus alone), or socially desirable or undesirable behaviors? Further, are fear appeals differentially effective for target populations that differ in age, education, social class, or personality? Such questions have received relatively little attention, but they have the potential to inform fear appeal theory and practice.
Additionally, what kinds of interactions exist when crossing aspects of message, behavior, and audience? We investigated two such questions in the present study with the hypotheses related to terror management theory – i.e., message content (presence versus absence of the word death) crossed with the recommended behavior (self-esteem enhancing versus hindering behaviors, immediate versus delayed outcomes). Although neither of these hypotheses was supported, the potential to test these types of interactions prompts the question of which variables may interact, particularly variables from separate aspects of the model. For example, might fear appeal effectiveness be moderated by interactions of culture (a factor of the audience) with the kind of behavior addressed by the fear appeal? Cross-cultural differences have rarely been explored in the effectiveness of fear appeals, and it is possible that cultural sensitivity to a behavior/issue may moderate the effectiveness of fear appeals addressing that behavior/issue. For example, East Asian countries have extremely low HIV prevalence rates and thus may be less susceptible to fear appeals on that topic relative to other topics. Whether this is true and whether it interacts with related findings is an empirical question that could be fruitfully explored in future research.
Importantly, aspects other than message content, behavior, and audience may moderate the effectiveness of fear appeal communications. However, based on our review of the literature, there simply appeared to be too little research on other aspects to include them in the current framework. Three potential aspects worth noting are the source of the communication, the subjective experience of the message recipient, and the channel used to transmit the message. First, based on a well-established body of literature in persuasion demonstrating that aspects of a message’s source can influence the persuasiveness of the message (Briñol & Petty, 2009; Kumkale et al., 2010; Pornpitakpan, 2004; Wilson & Sherrell, 1993), the source of a fear appeal communication should be an important moderator for fear appeal effectiveness. For example, fear appeals from benevolent groups (e.g., a respected government institution, a close personal friend) may be more effective than fear appeals from self-interested groups (e.g., corporations or other for-profit entities). However, most empirical studies did not detail source information in a manner that allowed us to test such hypotheses. Further, many fear appeals are delivered in the form of public service announcements, and thus there is relatively little variation across existing studies on this dimension. Second, drawing on our previous distinction between fear appeals and fear, the subjective experience of the message recipient should be an important aspect of fear appeal communications. Although most empirical studies simply do not measure participants’ subjective states, such measures could be very informative to test a variety of interesting questions. For example, is fear the only emotion evoked by fear appeals? If not, what other negative emotions are evoked (e.g., disgust, shame, guilt, anger), and are they partially responsible for the effectiveness of fear appeals? Similarly, perhaps the effects of fear appeals are simply driven by induced negative affect or high arousal, and the specific experience of fear is superfluous? Future research using measures of subjective experience are needed to address these questions. The paucity of existing research addressing source characteristics and subjective experience led us to not include these as aspects of the current review framework, but they would be welcome additions in the future. Third, consistent with the focus of the persuasion literature on source, message, audience, and channel of communication as key components to understand in the persuasion process (Shannon & Weaver, 1949), are certain channels of communication more likely to be effective in delivering fear appeals? For example, are graphic videos more likely to be effective than audio fear appeals without video? How do social media channels (generally more linked to liked peers) differ from mass media in effectiveness of delivered fear appeals?
Conclusion
To conclude, fear appeals are effective, and our synthesis organized and identified factors that make them even more effective. Specifically, fear appeals are particularly effective when the communication depicts relatively high amounts of fear, includes an efficacy message, stresses severity and susceptibility, recommends one-time only behaviors, and targets audiences that include a larger percentage of female message recipients. We formed these conclusions by meta-analytically testing a wide variety of influential fear appeal theories using the largest and most comprehensive fear appeals database to date. We believe our analysis has provided a thorough overview of the state of the literature and also generated a variety of important and exciting future directions.
Acknowledgments
This paper was facilitated by grants R01 MH094241, R01 NR08325, and R56 AI114501.
Appendix A
In the body of the manuscript, we presented random-effects analyses for a combined measure averaging across attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. Here, we present the analyses done separately for each type of measure.
First, the overall average effect size comparing treatment to comparison groups separately for attitudes, intentions, and behaviors was respectively d = 0.23 (95% CI: [0.11, 0.34]), d = 0.31 (95% CI: [0.24, 0.38]), and d = 0.27 (95% CI: [0.13, 0.42]). The heterogeneity statistics for each measure were respectively Q(109) = 614 (I2 = 86.52, p < .0001), Q(162) = 615 (I2 = 75.48, p < .0001), and Q(69) = 733 (I2 = 92.37, p < .0001).
To examine the linear and curvilinear hypotheses for each outcome, we computed the average weighted effect size comparing outcomes for high fear versus moderate fear groups. For attitudes, intentions, and behaviors, the results were respectively d = 0.05 (95% CI: [−0.27, 0.36]), d = −0.09 (95% CI: [−0.29, 0.11]), and d = −0.04 (95% CI: [−0.63, 0.56]). The heterogeneity statistics for each measure were respectively Q(7) = 19 (I2 = 66.10, p = .009), Q(8) = 19 (I2 = 65.95, p = .01), and Q(9) = 118 (I2 = 96.12, p < .0001).
To examine the gender hypothesis, we regressed outcomes onto the percent of the sample that was female. The results for attitudes, intentions, and behaviors were respectively b = 0.0019 (SE = 0.0022, 95% CI for the slope: [−0.0024, 0.0061], p = .38, k = 72), b = 0.0043 (SE = 0.0013, 95% CI for the slope: [0.0016, 0.0069], p = .002, k = 119), and b = 0.0037 (SE = 0.0028, 95% CI for the slope: [−0.0018, 0.0091], p = .19, k = 49).
The results for all categorical moderator analyses are presented in Table A.1.
Table A.1.
|
|||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Attitudes | Intentions | Behaviors | |||||||||
|
|||||||||||
MBA Aspect |
Variable | Level | d | 95% CI | k | d | 95% CI | k | d | 95% CI | k |
Message | Efficacy statements | Included | .39 | [.13, .64] | 38 | .40 | [.30, .49] | 61 | .43 | [.20, .66] | 32 |
Excluded | .14 | [.04, .25] | 72 | .27 | [.17, .36] | 100 | .14 | [−.05, .33] | 38 | ||
| |||||||||||
Depicted susceptibility and severity | Both | .22 | [.05, .38] | 33 | .35 | [.23, .47] | 62 | .44 | [.24, .63] | 29 | |
Susceptibility | .48 | [−.51, 1.47] | 6 | .37 | [.15, .59] | 18 | .45 | [.01, .88] | 2 | ||
Severity | .22 | [.06, .37] | 62 | .29 | [.20, .39] | 68 | .17 | [−.08, .42] | 34 | ||
Neither | .19 | [−.05, .43] | 9 | .20 | [−.10, .49] | 7 | .02 | [−.22, .26] | 4 | ||
| |||||||||||
Behavior | One-time versus repeated | One-time | .38 | [.17, .59] | 48 | .46 | [.35, .57] | 48 | .49 | [.24, .74] | 26 |
Repeated | .11 | [−.00, .22] | 62 | .24 | [.16, .33] | 115 | .15 | [−.02, .33] | 44 | ||
| |||||||||||
Detection versus promotion/prevention | Detection | .22 | [.03, .42] | 16 | .46 | [.33, .58] | 35 | .23 | [−.15, .61] | 12 | |
PP | .22 | [.10, .35] | 94 | .27 | [.19, .34] | 128 | .28 | [.12, .45] | 58 | ||
| |||||||||||
Death and self-esteem | SEE, DP | .10 | [−.32, .51] | 6 | .36 | [.14, .58] | 8 | .61 | [−.30, 1.53] | 5 | |
SEE, DA | −.10 | [−.33, .14] | 4 | .35 | [.14, .56] | 20 | −.74 | [−1.48, .01] | 3 | ||
SEH, DP | −.29 | [−.87, .29] | 7 | .05 | [−.23, .32] | 14 | −.02 | [−.62, .59] | 4 | ||
SEH, DA | .42 | [.01, .83] | 1 | .54 | [.13, .95] | 4 | .39 | [−1.56, 2.35] | 2 | ||
| |||||||||||
Death and delay | DP, same day | .03 | [−.13, .19] | 33 | .21 | [.10, .32] | 49 | .35 | [−.11, .82] | 11 | |
DP, 1–14 days | .10 | [−.41, .60] | 1 | – | – | – | .95 | [.33, 1.57] | 4 | ||
DP, 14+ days | .68 | [.37, .98] | 4 | .22 | [−.10, .55] | 2 | .21 | [.06, .36] | 9 | ||
DA, same day | .36 | [.20, .51] | 54 | .37 | [.27, .46] | 91 | .27 | [.03, .52] | 23 | ||
DA, 1–14 days | −.15 | [−.31, .02] | 9 | .34 | [.04, .65] | 12 | −.17 | [−.46, .13] | 13 | ||
DA, 14+ days | .44 | [−.57, 1.45] | 6 | .25 | [.05, .45] | 8 | .48 | [.11, .85] | 10 | ||
| |||||||||||
Audience | Culture | Collectivist | .08 | [−.18, .34] | 10 | .51 | [.32, .70] | 22 | .41 | [.10, .71] | 14 |
Individualist | .24 | [.12, .36] | 100 | .27 | [.20, .35] | 141 | .24 | [.07, .41] | 56 | ||
| |||||||||||
Stages of change | Early | .32 | [.17, .47] | 69 | .31 | [.22, .39] | 98 | .24 | [.05, .43] | 46 | |
Late | .42 | [.07, .77] | 9 | .22 | [.03, .42] | 21 | .61 | [−.32, 1.53] | 5 |
Note: SE = Self-esteem. DP = Death present in message. DA = Death absent in message. PP = Promotion/prevention. SEE = Self-esteem enhancing recommended behaviors. SEH = Self-esteem hindering recommended behaviors. d = Standardized mean effect size estimated meta-analytically for the indicated moderator level. 95% CI = The 95% confidence interval for d. k = The number of studies for each moderator level. Dash (−) indicates there were no observations at a particular moderator level.
Footnotes
We use the term effectiveness to indicate whether exposure to a fear appeal message resulted in more persuasion than a comparison condition. Thus, a fear appeal is considered effective if the effect size comparing treatment to control is significantly positive. Consequently, when testing moderation, fear appeals will be considered more effective for one level of a moderator versus another if the average effect size for the first level of the moderator is significantly larger than the average effect size for the second level of the moderator. In other words, when we compare fear appeal effectiveness for a moderator, we are comparing whether treatment led to more persuasion relative to control for one level of a moderator versus another level of that moderator.
Our framework addresses the relation between fear appeals and outcomes of interest (e.g., intentions) rather than the relation between fear and outcomes of interest. Although many fear appeal theories discuss fear, empirical studies typically test the impact of fear appeal messages on outcomes, and subsequently infer that message effects were mediated by experienced fear even though fear itself is rarely measured (for a discussion, see Popova, 2012, p.466). Indeed, only 71 of the 248 studies in the current meta-analysis measured fear directly, and such measures were typically treated as manipulation checks rather than independent variables or mediators. We are therefore careful to discuss the influence of depicted message characteristics rather than subjectively experienced states (e.g., depicted fear versus experienced fear). This distinction applies to prior meta-analyses and primary studies as well, though the distinction is rarely made. We would like to thank an anonymous reviewer for encouraging us to frame our results in line with this distinction.
TMT theories also predict a higher order interaction between mentions of death, time delays, and self-esteem, such that the predicted effects of self-esteem discussed above become stronger after a delay (Goldenberg & Arndt, 2008). Of the 12 conditions represented by this prediction (2 death × 3 delay × 2 self-esteem), four had zero observations in our meta-analysis. Thus, we are only able to test the simpler predictions concerning self-esteem and time delay in isolation.
Although many researchers investigate stage progression in the transtheoretical model (the process by which people move from one stage of the model to the next; Prochaska & DiClemente, 1983), this outcome is not directly relevant for our investigation because we are examining the effect of fear appeals on attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. It is possible that individuals would be classified as moving from one stage of the model to the next due to changes in attitudes, intentions, or behaviors, but such classification decisions are not the focus of the present study. The transtheoretical model also includes three dimensions other than the stages of change — the processes of change, self-efficacy, and decisional balance. Although we test predictions derived from the transtheoretical model more broadly, we limited our predictions to the areas that are relevant to fear appeal audiences (stages of change).
A number of papers did not provide the full text of the messages that were presented to each group, which made it impossible to determine if comparison groups labeled with the terms neutral message or control message were actually presented with neutral messages or with low depicted fear messages. Similarly, groups labeled with the term low depicted fear may have actually been presented with a neutral message but were nonetheless labeled as low fear because they were designed to induce relatively less fear than the experimental group. Thus, we could consistently compare relative levels of depicted fear across studies (more depicted fear vs. less depicted fear), but not absolute levels of fear (high depicted fear vs. low depicted fear vs. no depicted fear). Consequently, no message groups, neutral message groups, and low depicted fear groups were all considered appropriate comparison groups. Further, it was generally not possible to combine different potential comparison groups because information about standard deviations for the outcomes of each group was often lacking from reports, which made it unfeasible to calculate correct standard errors for combined comparison groups.
Contributor Information
Melanie B. Tannenbaum, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
Justin Hepler, University of Nevada, Reno.
Rick S. Zimmerman, University of Missouri – St. Louis
Lindsey Saul, Virginia Commonwealth University.
Samantha Jacobs, Virginia Commonwealth University.
Kristina Wilson, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Dolores Albarracin, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.
- *.Bagley JG, Low KG. Enhancing flossing compliance in college freshmen. Clinical Preventive Dentistry. 1992;14:25–30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Bang HK. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Michigan State University; 1993. The effectiveness of the use of fear appeals depicting legal and physical consequences in anti-drunk driving television public service announcements. [Google Scholar]
- *.Beach RI. The effect of a ‘fear-arousing’ safety film on physiological, attitudinal, and behavioral measures: A pilot study. Research Review. 1966;10:53–56. [Google Scholar]
- *.Beck KH. The effects of risk probability, outcome severity, efficacy of protection and access to protection on decision making: A further test of protection motivation theory. Social Behavior and Personality. 1984;12:121–125. doi: 10.2224/sbp.1984.12.2.121. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Beck KH, Davis CM. Effects of fear-arousing communications and topic importance on attitude change. Journal of Social Psychology. 1978;104:81–95. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1978.9924040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker MH. The health belief model and personal health behavior. Health Education Monographs. 1974;2:324–473. [Google Scholar]
- Becker MH, Maiman LA, Kirscht JP, Haefner DP, Drachman RH. The health belief model and prediction of dietary compliance: A field experiment. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1977;18:348–366. doi: 10.2307/2955344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker MH, Radius SM, Rosenstock IM. Compliance with a medical regimen for asthma: A test of the health belief model. Public Health Reports. 1978;93:268–277. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Begg CB, Mazumdar M. Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics. 1994;50:1088–1101. doi: 10.2307/2533446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Berkowitz JM. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Michigan State University; 1998. The influence of sensation-seeking and message choice on responses to fear appeals. [Google Scholar]
- Boster FJ, Mongeau P. Fear-arousing persuasive messages. In: Bostrom RN, Westley BH, editors. Communication Yearbook. Vol. 8. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage; 1984. pp. 330–375. [Google Scholar]
- Briñol P, Petty RE. Source factors in persuasion: A self-validation approach. European Review of Social Psychology. 2009;20:49–96. doi: 10.1080/10463280802643640. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Brouwers MC, Sorrentino RM. Uncertainty orientation and protection motivation theory: The role of individual differences in health compliance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1993;65:102–112. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.102. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Brown RA. Fear-induced attitude change as a function of conformity and drinking pattern in alcoholics. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1979;35:454–456. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(197904)35:2<454::AID-JCLP2270350246>3.0.CO;2-S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Burnett JJ. Internal-external locus of control as a moderator of fear appeals. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1981;66:390–393. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.66.3.390. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Calantone RJ, Warshaw PR. Negating the effects of fear appeals in election campaigns. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1985;70:627–633. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.70.4.627. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Carey JC. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. The University of Texas at Austin; 1990. The influence of involvement on cognitive and emotional responses to fear appeals. [Google Scholar]
- Cauberghe V, de Pelsmacker P, Janssens W, Dens N. Fear, threat and efficacy in threat appeals: Message involvement as a key mediator to message acceptance. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2009;41:276–285. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2008.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. CDC’s Tips From Former Smokers campaign provided outstanding return on investment [Press release] 2014 Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/media/releases/2014/p1210-tips-roi.html.
- Cesario J, Higgins ET, Scholer AA. Regulatory fit and persuasion: Basic principles and remaining questions. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2008;2:444–463. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00055.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Chang A, Hearey CD, Gallagher KD, English P, Chang PC. Promoting child passenger safety in children served by a health maintenance organization. Patient Education and Counseling. 1989;13:297–307. doi: 10.1016/0738-3991(89)90023-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Cho H, Salmon CT. Fear appeals for individuals in different stages of change: Intended and unintended effects and implications on public health campaigns. Health Communication. 2006;20:91–99. doi: 10.1207/s15327027hc2001_9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Chu GC. Fear arousal, efficacy, and imminency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1966;4:517–524. doi: 10.1037/h0021189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Cooper DP, Goldenberg JL, Arndt J. Perceived efficacy, conscious fear of death and intentions to tan: Not all fear appeals are created equal. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2014;19:1–15. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Dabbs JM, Leventhal H. Effects of varying the recommendations in a fear-arousing communication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1966;4:525–531. doi: 10.1037/h0021190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Dahl DW, Frankenberger KD, Manchanda RV. Does it pay to shock? Reactions to shocking and nonshocking advertising content among university students. Journal of Advertising Research. 2003;43:268–280. doi: 10.1017/S0021849903030332. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Das EHHJ, de Wit JBF, Stroebe W. Fear appeals motivate acceptance of action recommendations: Evidence for a positive bias in the processing of persuasive messages. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2003;29:650–664. doi: 10.1177/0146167203029005009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.de Hoog N, Stroebe W, de Wit JBF. The impact of fear appeals on processing and acceptance of action recommendations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2005;31:24–33. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Hoog N, Stroebe W, de Wit JBF. The impact of vulnerability to and severity of a health risk on processing and acceptance of fear-arousing communications: A meta-analysis. Review of General Psychology. 2007;11:258. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.11.3.258. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.de Hoog N, Stroebe W, de Wit JBF. The processing of fear-arousing communications: How biased processing leads to persuasion. Social Influence. 2008;3:84–113. doi: 10.1080/15534510802185836. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Dembroski TM, Lasater TM, Ramirez A. Communicator similarity, fear arousing communications, and compliance with health care recommendations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1978;8:254–269. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1978.tb00781.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- DiClemente CC, Prochaska JO, Fairhurst SK, Velicer WF, Velasquez MM, Rossi JS. The process of smoking cessation: An analysis of precontemplation, contemplation, and preparation stages of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1991;59:295–304. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.59.2.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Dijkstra A, Bos C. The effects of repeated exposure to graphic fear appeals on cigarette packages: A field experiment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2015;29:82–90. doi: 10.1037/adb0000049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dillard JP, Plotnick CA, Godbold LC, Freimuth VS, Edgar T. The multiple affective outcomes of AIDS PSAs: Fear appeals do more than scare people. Communication Research. 1996;23:44–72. doi: 10.1177/009365096023001002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Drug Free Action Alliance. Why scare tactics in drug messaging prevention don’t work. Columbus, OH: Drug Free Action Alliance; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- *.Duke JC, Nonnemaker JM, Davis KC, Watson KA, Farrelly MC. The impact of cessation media messages on cessation-related outcomes: Results from a national experiment of smokers. American Journal of Health Promotion. 2014;28:242–250. doi: 10.4278/ajhp.120920-QUAN-452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Earl A, Albarracin D. Nature, decay, and spiraling of the effects of fear-inducing arguments and HIV counseling and testing: A meta-analysis of the short- and long-term outcomes of HIV-prevention interventions. Health Psychology. 2007;26:496–506. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.4.496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Evans RI, Rozelle RM, Lasater TM, Dembroski TM, Allen BP. New measures of effects of persuasive communications: A chemical indicator of toothbrushing behavior. Psychological Reports. 1968;23:731–736. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1968.23.3.731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Evans RI, Rozelle RM, Lasater TM, Dembroski TM, Allen BP. Fear arousal, persuasion, and actual versus implied behavioral change: New perspective utilizing a real-life dental hygiene program. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1970;16:220–227. doi: 10.1037/h0029834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Feenstra H, Ruiter RA, Kok G. Evaluating traffic informers: Testing the behavioral and social-cognitive effects of an adolescent bicycle safety education program. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2014;73:288–295. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2014.09.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floyd DL, Prentice-Dunn S, Rogers RW. A meta-analysis of research on protection motivation theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2000;30:407–429. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02323.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.France KE, Donovan RJ, Bower C, Elliott EJ, Payne JM, D’Antoine H, Bartu AE. Messages that increase women’s intentions to abstain from alcohol during pregnancy: Results from quantitative testing of advertising concepts. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:30. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Frandsen KD. Effects of threat appeals and media of transmission. Speech Monographs. 1963;30:101–104. doi: 10.1080/03637756309375365. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Fukada H. Effects of fear arousal, sex, and anxiety predisposition of subjects on attitude change. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1973;13:40–54. doi: 10.2130/jjesp.13.40. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Fukada H. Fear arousal and persuasion: A re-examination of the defensive avoidance hypothesis. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1975;15:12–24. doi: 10.2130/jjesp.15.12. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Fukada H. Forewarning effects in fear-arousing communications. Japanese Journal of Psychology. 1983;54:286–292. doi: 10.4992/jjpsy.54.286. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Fukada H. Persuasion facilitating effects under irrelevant fear-arousing situations. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1983;23:83–90. doi: 10.2130/jjesp.23.83. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Fukada H. Effects of fear-arousing communications on resistance to subsequent counterpropaganda. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1988;27:149–156. doi: 10.2130/jjesp.27.149. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Fukada H. Effects of communicator credibility on persuasion under irrelevant fear-arousing situations. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1991;31:94–103. doi: 10.2130/jjesp.31.94. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Gleicher F, Petty RE. Expectations of reassurance influence the nature of fear-stimulated attitude change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1992;28:86–100. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(92)90033-G. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Goldenbeld C, Twisk D, Houwing S. Effects of persuasive communication and group discussions on acceptability of anti-speeding policies for male and female drivers. Transportation Research Part F. 2008;11:207–220. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2007.11.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Goldenberg JL, Arndt J. The implications of death for health: A terror management health model for behavioral health promotion. Psychological Review. 2008;115:1032–1053. doi: 10.1037/a0013326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Griffeth RW, Rogers RW. Effects of fear-arousing components of driver education on students’ safety attitudes and simulator performance. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1976;68:501–506. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.68.4.501. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Haddock CK, Rindskopf D, Shadish WR. Using odds ratios as effect sizes for meta-analysis of dichotomous data: a primer on methods and issues. Psychological Methods. 1998;3:339. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.3.3.339. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Hass JW, Bagley GS, Rogers RW. Coping with the energy crisis: Effects of fear appeals upon attitudes toward energy consumption. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1975;60:754–756. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.60.6.754. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hasselblad V, Hedges LV. Meta-analysis of screening and diagnostic tests. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117:167. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Hendrick C, Giesen M, Borden R. False physiological feedback and persuasion: Effect of fear arousal vs. fear reduction on attitude change. Journal of Personality. 1975;43:196–214. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1975.tb00702.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higbee KL. Fifteen years of fear arousal: Research on threat appeals: 1953-1968. Psychological Bulletin. 1969;6:426–444. doi: 10.1037/h0028430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins ET, Pierro A, Kruglanski AW. Re-thinking culture and personality: How self-regulatory universals create cross-cultural differences. In: Sorrentino RM, Yamaguchi S, editors. Handbook of motivation and cognition across cultures. New York: Elsevier; 2008. pp. 161–190. [Google Scholar]
- *.Hill D, Gardner G. Repression-sensitization and yielding to threatening health communications. Australian Journal of Psychology. 1980;3:183–193. doi: 10.1080/00049538008254688. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Hoeken H, Geurts D. The influence of exemplars in fear appeals on the perception of self-efficacy and message acceptance. Information Design Journal & Document Design. 2005;13:238–248. doi: 10.1075/idjdd.13.3.09hoe. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Horowitz IA. Effects of volunteering, fear arousal, and number of communications on attitude change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1969;11:34–37. doi: 10.1037/h0027024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Horowitz IA, Gumenick WE. Effects of the volunteer subject, choice, and fear arousal on attitude change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1970;6:293–303. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(70)90064-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hovland C, Janis I, Kelly H. Communication and persuasion. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1953. [Google Scholar]
- *.Insko CA, Arkoff A, Insko VM. Effects of high and low fear-arousing communications upon opinions toward smoking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1965;1:256–266. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(65)90030-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Janis IL. Effects of fear arousal on attitude change: Recent developments in theory and experimental research. In: Berkowitz L, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 3. New York: Academic Press; 1967. pp. 166–225. [Google Scholar]
- Janis IL. The Contours of Fear. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 1968. [Google Scholar]
- Janis IL, Feshbach S. Effects of fear-arousing communications. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1953;48:78–92. doi: 10.1037/h0060732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Janis IL, Feshbach S. Personality differences associated with responsiveness to fear-arousing communications. Journal of Personality. 1954;23:154–166. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1954.tb01145.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janis IL, Leventhal H. Human reactions to stress. In: Borgatta E, Lambert W, editors. Handbook of personality theory and research. Chicago, IL: Rand McNally Press; 1968. [Google Scholar]
- *.Janis IL, Terwilliger RF. An experimental study of psychological resistances to fear arousing communications. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1962;65:403–410. doi: 10.1037/h0047601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janssen E, van Osch L, de Vries H, Lechner L. The influence of narrative risk communication on feelings of cancer risk. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2013;18:407–419. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8287.2012.02098.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Janssens W, de Pelsmacker P. Fear appeal in traffic safety advertising: The moderating role of medium context, trait anxiety, and differences between drivers and non-drivers. Psychologica Belgica. 2007;47:173–193. doi: 10.5334/pb-47-3-173. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson BT, Eagly AH. Meta-analysis of research in social personality and personality psychology. In: Reis HT, Judd CM, editors. Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology. 2nd. London: Cambridge University Press; 2014. pp. 675–707. [Google Scholar]
- *.Johnston AC. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Mississippi State University; 2006. An empirical investigation of the influence of fear appeals on attitudes and behavioral intentions associated with recommended individual computer security actions. [Google Scholar]
- *.Jones SC, Owen N. Using fear appeals to promote cancer screening – are we scaring the wrong people? International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing. 2006;11:93–103. doi: 10.1002/nvsm.48. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Kareklas I, Muehling DD. Addressing the texting and driving epidemic: Mortality salience priming effects on attitudes and behavioral intentions. Journal of Consumer Affairs. 2014;48:223–250. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.2324987. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Keller PA. Converting the unconverted: The effect of inclination and opportunity to discount health-related fear appeals. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1999;84:403–415. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.84.3.403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Keller PA, Block LG. Increasing the persuasiveness of fear appeals: The effect of arousal and elaboration. Journal of Consumer Research. 1996;22:448–459. doi: 10.1086/209461. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Kim P, Sorcar P, Um S, Chung H, Lee YS. Effects of episodic variations in web-based avian influenza education: Influence of fear and humor on perception, comprehension, retention, and behavior. Health Education Research. 2009;24:369–380. doi: 10.1093/her/cyn031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Kirscht JP, Becker MH, Haefner DP, Maiman LA. Effects of threatening communications and mothers’ health beliefs on weight change in obese children. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1978;1:147–157. doi: 10.1007/BF00846636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Kirscht JP, Haefner DP. Effects of repeated threatening health communications. International Journal of Health Education. 1973;16:268–277. [Google Scholar]
- *.Kleinot MC, Rogers RW. Identifying effective components of alcohol misuse prevention programs. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1982;43:802–811. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1982.43.802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Klohn LS, Rogers RW. Dimensions of the severity of a health threat: The persuasive effects of visibility, time of onset, and rate of onset on young women’s intentions to prevent osteoporosis. Health Psychology. 1991;10:323–329. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.10.5.323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kok G, Bartholomew LK, Parcel GS, Gottlieb NH, Fernández ME. Finding theory- and evidence-based alternatives to fear appeals: Intervention Mapping. International Journal of Psychology. 2014;49:98–107. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumkale GT, Albarracín D, Seignourel PJ. The effects of source credibility in the presence or absence of prior attitudes: Implications for the design of persuasive communication campaigns. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2010;40:1325–1356. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00620.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurman J, Hui C. Promotion, prevention, or both: Regulatory focus and culture, revisited. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture. 2011 doi: 10.9707/2307-0919.1109. Unit 5. Retrieved from http://scholarworks.qvsu.edu/orpc/vol5/iss3/3. [DOI]
- *.LaTour MS, Snipes RL, Bliss SJ. Don’t be afraid to use fear appeals: An experimental study. Journal of Advertising Research. 1996;2:59–67. [Google Scholar]
- *.LaTour MS, Tanner JF. Radon: Appealing to our fears. Psychology & Marketing. 2003;20:377–394. doi: 10.1002/mar.10078. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Leventhal H. Findings and theory in the study of fear communications. In: Berkowitz L, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 5. New York: Academic Press; 1970. pp. 119–186. [Google Scholar]
- *.Leventhal H, Niles P. A field experiment on fear arousal with data on the validity of questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality. 1964;32:459–479. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1964.tb01352.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Leventhal H, Singer R, Jones S. Effects of fear and specificity of recommendation upon attitudes and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1965;2:20–29. doi: 10.1037/h0022089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Leventhal H, Watts JC. Sources of resistance to fear-arousing communications on smoking and lung cancer. Journal of Personality. 1966;34:155–175. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1966.tb01706.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Leventhal H, Watts JC, Pagano F. Effects of fear and instructions on how to cope with danger. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1967;6:313–321. doi: 10.1037/h0021222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Levin AM, Dato-on MC, Manolis C. Deterring illegal downloading: The effects of threat appeals, past behavior, subjective norms, and attributions of harm. Journal of Consumer Behavior. 2007;6:111–122. doi: 10.1002/cb.211. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Lewis I, Watson B, White KM. An examination of message-relevant affect in road safety messages: Should road safety advertisements aim to make us feel good or bad? Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour. 2008;11:403–417. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2008.03.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Lewis IM, Watson B, White KM. Response efficacy: The key to minimizing rejection and maximizing acceptance of emotion-based anti-speeding messages. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2010;42:459–467. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Li Y. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Minnesota; 2002. Outcome as a determinant of threat ads’ effectiveness. [Google Scholar]
- *.Liberman A, Chaiken S. Defensive processing of personally relevant health messages. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1992;18:669–679. doi: 10.1177/0146167292186002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lockwood P, Marshall TC, Sadler P. Promoting success or preventing failure: Cultural differences in motivation by positive and negative role models. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2005;31:379–392. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Lwin MO, Malik S. Can exergames impart health messages? Game play, framing, and drivers of physical activity among children. Journal of Health Communication. 2014;19:136–151. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2013.798372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maddux JE, Rogers RW. Protection motivation and self-efficacy: A revised theory of fear appeals and attitude change. Journal of experimental social psychology. 1983;19:469–479. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(83)90023-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire WJ. Personality and susceptibility to social influence. In: Borgatta E, Lambert W, editors. Handbook of personality theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally Press; 1968. [Google Scholar]
- McGuire WJ. The nature of attitudes and attitude change. In: Lindzey G, Aronson E, editors. The handbook of social psychology. Vol. 3. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley; 1969. pp. 136–314. [Google Scholar]
- *.McMath BF, Prentice-Dunn S. Protection motivation theory and skin cancer risk: The role of individual differences in responses to persuasive appeals. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2005;35:621–643. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02138.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Meijnders AL, Midden CJH, Wilke HAM. Communications about environmental risks and risk-reducing behavior: The impact of fear on information processing. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2001;31:754–777. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb01412.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Meijnders AL, Midden CJH, Wilke HAM. Role of negative emotion in communication about CO2 risks. Risk Analysis. 2001;21:955–966. doi: 10.1111/0272-4332.215164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyerowitz BE, Chaiken S. The effect of message framing on breast self-examination attitudes, intentions, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;52:500. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Millman S. Anxiety, comprehension, and susceptibility to social influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1968;9:251–256. doi: 10.1037/h0021252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milne S, Sheeran P, Orbell S. Prediction and intervention in health-related behavior: A meta-analytic review of protection motivation theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2000;30:106–143. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02308.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mongeau P. Another look at fear-arousing persuasive appeals. In: Allen M, Preiss RW, editors. Persuasion: Advances through meta-analysis. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press; 1998. pp. 53–68. [Google Scholar]
- *.Morales AC, Wu EC, Fitzsimons GJ. How disgust enhances the effectiveness of fear appeals. Journal of Marketing Research. 2012;49:383–393. doi: 10.1509/jmr.07.0364. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Morris KL, Cooper DP, Goldenberg JL, Arndt J, Gibbons FX. Improving the efficacy of appearance-based sun exposure interventions with the terror management health model. Psychology & Health. 2014;29:1245–1264. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2014.922184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Muthusamy N, Levine TR, Weber R. Scaring the already scared: Some problems with HIV/AIDS fear appeals in Namibia. Journal of Communication. 2009;59:317–344. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2009.01418.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nabi RL, Roskos-Ewoldsen D, Dillman Carpentier F. Subjective knowledge and fear appeal effectiveness: Implications for message design. Health Communication. 2008;23:191–201. doi: 10.1080/10410230701808327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Keefe DJ. The relative persuasiveness of different message types does not vary as a function of the persuasive outcome assessed: Evidence from 29 meta-analyses of 2,062 effect sizes for 13 message variations. In: Cohen EL, editor. Communication Yearbook. Vol. 37. New York: Routledge; 2013. pp. 221–249. [Google Scholar]
- *.Ordonana JR, Gonzalez-Javier F, Espin-Lopez L, Gomez-Amor J. Self-report and psychophysiological responses to fear appeals. Human Communication Research. 2009;35:195–220. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2009.01344.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Pengchit W. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. The University of Utah; 2010. Not too scared to think carefully: Optimism fosters processing of self-relevant threatening persuasive messages. [Google Scholar]
- *.Pepper GV, Nettle D. Out of control mortality matters: the effect of perceived uncontrollable mortality risk on a health-related decision. PeerJ. 2014;2:e459. doi: 10.7717/peerj.459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters GJY, Ruiter RAC, Kok G. Threatening communication: a critical re-analysis and a revised meta-analytic test of fear appeal theory. Health Psychology Review. 2012;7:S8–S31. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2012.703527.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popova L. The extended parallel process model: Illuminating the gaps in research. Health Education and Behavior. 2012;39:455–473. doi: 10.1177/1090198111418108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pornpitakpan C. The persuasiveness of source credibility: A critical review of five decades’ evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2004;34:243–281. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02547.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Powell FA. The effect of anxiety-arousing messages when related to personal, familial, and impersonal referents. Speech Monographs. 1965;32:102–106. doi: 10.1080/03637756509375437. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Priolo D, Milhabet I. Quand l’engagement précѐde l’appel à la peur: Une forme de communication engageante. L’annee Psychologique. 2008;108:207–241. doi: 10.4074/S0003503308002029. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC. Stages and processes of self-change of smoking: Toward an integrative model of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1983;51:390–395. doi: 10.1037//0022-006X.51.3.390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC, Norcross JC. In search of how people change: Applications to addictive behavior. American Psychologist. 1992;47:1102–1114. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.47.9.1102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prochaska JO, Velicer WF. The transtheoretical model of health behavior change. American Journal of Health Promotion. 1997;12:38–48. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-12.1.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pyszczynski T, Greenberg J, Solomon S. A dual-process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: An extension of terror management theory. Psychological Review. 1999;106:835–845. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Radelfinger S. Some effects of fear-arousing communications on preventive health behavior. Health Education Monographs. 1965;19:2–15. doi: 10.1177/109019816500101901. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Raleigh KV. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Alabama; 2002. AIDS prevention: The effects of components of protection motivation theory on intentions to comply with safer sex recommendations. [Google Scholar]
- *.Ramirez A, Lasater TL. Attitudinal and behavioral reactions to fear-arousing communications. Psychological Reports. 1976;38:811–818. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1976.38.3.811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Ramirez A, Lasater TL. Ethnicity of communicator, self-esteem, and reactions to fear-arousing communications. Journal of Social Psychology. 1977;102:79–91. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1977.9713243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Rippetoe PA, Rogers RW. Effects of components of protection-motivation theory on adaptive and maladaptive coping with a health threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;52:596–604. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson LS. Behavioral research and strategies in public health: A demur. Social Science and Medicine. 1975;9:165–170. doi: 10.1016/0037-7856(75)90053-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Rodriguez JI. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Michigan State University; 1995. Confounds in fear-arousing persuasive messages: Do the paths less traveled make all the difference? [Google Scholar]
- *.Rogers RW, Deckner CW. Effects of fear appeals and physiological arousal upon emotion, attitudes, and cigarette smoking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1975;32:222–230. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.32.2.222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Rogers RW, Mewborn CR. Fear appeals and attitude change: Effects of a threat’s noxiousness, probability of occurrence, and the efficacy of coping responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1976;34:54–61. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.34.1.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Rogers RW, Thistlethwaite DL. Effects of fear arousal and reassurance on attitude change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1970;15:227–233. doi: 10.1037/h0029437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Rosen TJ, Terry NS, Leventhal H. The role of esteem and coping in response to a threat communication. Journal of Research in Personality. 1982;16:90–107. doi: 10.1016/0092-6566(82)90043-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenstock IM. Why people use health services. The Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly. 1966;44:94–127. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenstock IM. Historical origins of the health belief model. Health Education Monographs. 1974;2:328–335. doi: 10.1177/109019817400200403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Rosenthal LH. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Massachusetts at Amherst; 1997. A new perspective on the relation between fear and persuasion: The application of dual-process models. [Google Scholar]
- *.Roskos-Ewoldsen DR, Yu HJ, Rhodes N. Fear appeal messages affect accessibility of attitudes toward the threat and adaptive behaviors. Communication Monographs. 2004;71:49–69. doi: 10.1080/0363452042000228559. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rothman AJ, Martino SC, Bedell BT, Detweiler JB, Salovey P. The systematic influence of gain- and loss-framed messages on interest in and use of different types of health behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1999;25:1355–1369. doi: 10.1177/0146167299259003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rothman AJ, Salovey P. Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: The role of message framing. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;121:3–19. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruiter RA, Kessels LT, Peters GJY, Kok G. Sixty years of fear appeal research: Current state of the evidence. International Journal of Psychology. 2014;49:63–70. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Ruiter RAC, Verplanken B, Kok G, Werrij MQ. The role of coping appraisal in reactions to fear appeals: Do we need threat information? Journal of Health Psychology. 2003;8:465–474. doi: 10.1177/13591053030084006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Schmitt CL, Blass T. Fear appeals revisited: Testing a unique anti-smoking film. Current Psychology. 2008;27:145–151. doi: 10.1007/s12144-008-9029-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Schoenbachler DD, Whittler TE. Adolescent processing of social and physical threat communications. Journal of Advertising. 1996;25:37–54. doi: 10.1080/00913367.1996.10673511. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Self CA, Rogers RW. Coping with threats to health: Effects of persuasive appeals on depressed, normal, and antisocial personalities. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1990;13:343–357. doi: 10.1007/BF00844883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Shannon CE, Weaver W. The mathematical theory of communication. Urbana, Illinois: University of Illinois Press; 1949. [Google Scholar]
- *.Shehryar O, Hunt D. A terror management perspective on the persuasiveness of fear appeals. Journal of Consumer Psychology. 2005;15:275–287. doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp1504_2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Shelton ML, Rogers RW. Fear-arousing and empathy-arousing appeals to help: The pathos of persuasion. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1981;11:366–378. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1981.tb00829.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Shen L. The effectiveness of empathy-versus fear-arousing antismoking PSAs. Health Communication. 2011;26:404–415. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2011.552480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Siero S, Kok G, Pruyn J. Effects of public education about breast cancer and breast self-examination. Social Science and Medicine. 1984;18:881–888. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(84)90157-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Skilbeck C, Tulips J, Ley P. The effects of fear arousal, fear position, fear exposure, and sidedness on compliance with dietary instructions. European Journal of Social Psychology. 1977;7:221–239. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420070208. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Smalec JL, Klingle RS. Bulimia interventions via interpersonal influence: The role of threat and efficacy in persuading bulimics to seek help. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2000;23:37–57. doi: 10.1023/A:1005468220077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Smart RG, Fejer D. The effects of high and low fear messages about drugs. Journal of Drug Education. 1974;4:225–235. doi: 10.2190/1V5H-1NK5-QNXM-465F. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Smerecnik CMR, Ruiter RAC. Fear appeals in HIV prevention: The role of anticipated regret. Psychology, Health, and Medicine. 2010;15:550–559. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2010.498888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Smith KH, Stutts MA. Effects of short-term cosmetic versus long-term health fear appeals in anti-smoking advertisements on the smoking behaviour of adolescents. Journal of Consumer Behavior. 2003;3:157–177. doi: 10.1002/cb.130. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Solomon S, Greenberg J, Pyszczynski T. A terror management theory of social behavior: The psychological functions of self-esteem and cultural worldviews. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 1991;24:93–159. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60328-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Stainback RD, Rogers RW. Identifying effective components of alcohol abuse prevention programs: Effects of fear appeals, message style, and source expertise. The International Journal of the Addictions. 1983;18:393–405. doi: 10.3109/10826088309039356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Stark E, Kim A, Miller C, Borgida E. Effects of including a graphic warning label in advertisements for reduced-exposure products: Implications for persuasion and policy. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2008;38:281–293. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00305.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Stephenson MT, Witte K. Fear, threat, and perceptions of efficacy from frightening skin cancer messages. Public Health Reviews. 1998;26:147–174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Struckman-Johnson CJ, Gilliland RC, Struckman-Johnson DL, North TC. The effects of fear of AIDS and gender on responses to fear-arousing condom advertisements. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1990;20:1396–1410. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1990.tb01480.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Sturges JW, Rogers RW. Preventive health psychology from a developmental perspective: An extension of protection motivation theory. Health Psychology. 1996;15:158–166. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.15.3.158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton SR. Fear-arousing communications: A critical examination of theory and research. In: Eiser JR, editor. Social Psychology and Behavioral Medicine. London: Wiley; 1982. pp. 303–337. [Google Scholar]
- *.Tanner JF, Hunt JB, Eppright DR. The protection motivation model: A normative model of fear appeals. Journal of Marketing. 1991;55:36–45. doi: 10.2307/1252146. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Taubman Ben-Ari OT, Florian V, Mikulincer M. Does a threat appeal moderate reckless driving? A terror management theory perspective. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2000;32:1–10. doi: 10.1016/S0001-4575(99)00042-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.ter Horst G, Hoogstraten J, de Haan W. Stimulating dental attendance in the Netherlands: Comparison of three conceptual frameworks. Community Dental & Oral Epidemiology. 1985;13:136–139. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0528.1985.tb00428.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Thornton J, Rossiter J, White L. The persuasive effectiveness of varying levels of fear appeals: an anti-speeding advertising experiment. Proceedings of ANZMAC 2000 [Google Scholar]
- Tversky A, Kahneman D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science. 1981;211:453–458. doi: 10.1126/science.7455683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Umeh K. Does a credible source also need a fearful audience? Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2012;42:1716–1744. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00916.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Umeh K, Stanley SJ. Effects of communicator credibility and fear on adaptive and maladaptive coping reactions to the HIV threat. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research. 2005;10:183–198. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9861.2005.tb00011.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- van’t Riet J, Cox AD, Cox D, Zimet GD, De Bruijn GJ, Van den Putte B, Ruiter RA. Does perceived risk influence the effects of message framing? A new investigation of a widely held notion. Psychology & Health. 2014;29:933–949. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2014.896916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Venkatesan AM. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Rutgers University; 2010. An event-related potential (ERP) study of attention allocation in the processing of a fear appeal and its relation to HPV vaccine acceptance. [Google Scholar]
- Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software. 2010;36:1–48. [Google Scholar]
- Wang MC, Bushman BJ. Integrating results through meta-analytic review using SAS software. Cary, NC: SAS Institute; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- *.Weinstein ND, Sandman PM, Roberts NE. Determinants of self-protective behavior: Home radon testing. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1990;20:783–801. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1990.tb00379.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Welbourne JL, Hartley TA, Ott SD, Robertson S. Effects of risk-focused and recommendation-focused mental imagery on occupational risk communication. Health Communication. 2008;23:473–482. doi: 10.1080/10410230802342168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Wheatley JJ, Oshikawa S. The relationship between anxiety and positive and negative advertising appeals. Journal of Marketing Research. 1970;7:85–90. [Google Scholar]
- *.Will KE, Sabo CS, Porter BE. Evaluation of the ‘Boost ’em in the Back Seat Program’: Using fear and efficacy to increase booster seat use. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2009;41:57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2008.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson EJ, Sherrell DL. Source effects in communication and persuasion research: A meta-analysis of effect size. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science. 1993;21:101–112. doi: 10.1007/BF02894421. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Witte K. Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. Communication Mongraphs. 1992;59:329–349. doi: 10.1080/03637759209376276. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Witte K. Fear as motivator, fear as inhibitor: Using the EPPM to explain fear appeal successes and failures. In: Andersen PA, Guerrero LK, editors. The handbook of communication and emotion. New York: Academic Press; 1998. pp. 423–450. [Google Scholar]
- Witte K, Allen M. A meta-analysis of fear appeals: Implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Education & Behavior. 2000;27:591–615. doi: 10.1177/109019810002700506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Witte K, Berkowitz JM, Cameron KA, McKeon JK. Preventing the spread of genital warts: Using fear appeals to promote self-protective behaviors. Health Education & Behavior. 1998;25:571–585. doi: 10.1177/109019819802500505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Witte K, Morrison K. Using scare tactics to promote safer sex among juvenile detention and high school youth. Journal of Applied Communication Research. 1995;23:128–142. doi: 10.1080/00909889509365419. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Wong NCH, Cappella JN. Antismoking threat and efficacy appeals: Effects on smoking cessation intentions for smokers with low and high readiness to quit. Journal of Applied Communication Research. 2009;37:1–20. doi: 10.1080/00909880802593928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Wurtele SK. Increasing women’s calcium intake: The role of health beliefs, intentions, and health value. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1988;18:627–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1988.tb00041.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Wurtele SK, Maddux JE. Relative contributions of protection motivation theory components in predicting exercise intentions and behavior. Health Psychology. 1987;6:453–466. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.6.5.453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu X, Alexander RL, Jr, Simpson SA, Goates S, Nonnemaker JM, Davis KC, McAfee T. A cost-effectiveness analysis of the first federally funded antismoking campaign. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2015;48:318–325. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2014.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Yoon HJ, Tinkham SF. Humorous threat persuasion in advertising: The effects of humor, threat intensity, and issue involvement. Journal of Advertising. 2013;42:30–41. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2012.749082. [DOI] [Google Scholar]