Abstract
Maltreatment is associated with increased risk of a range of psychiatric disorders, many of which are characterized by altered risk-taking propensity. Currently, little is known about the neural correlates of risk-taking in children exposed to maltreatment, nor whether their risk-taking is atypically modulated by peer influence. Seventy-five 10- to 14-year-old children [maltreated (MT) group: N = 41; non-maltreated Group (NMT): N = 34] performed a Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART), under three different peer influence conditions: while alone, while being observed by a peer and while being encouraged by a peer to take risks. The MT group engaged in less risk-taking irrespective of peer influence. There was no differential effect of peer influence on risk-taking behaviour across groups. At the neural level, the right anterior insula (rAI) exhibited altered risk sensitivity across conditions in the MT group. Across groups and conditions, rAI risk sensitivity was negatively associated with risk-taking and within the MT group greater rAI risk sensitivity was related to more anxiety symptoms. These findings suggest that children with a history of maltreatment show reduced risk-taking but typical responses to peer influence. Abnormal rAI functioning contributes to the pattern of reduced risk-taking and may predispose children exposed to maltreatment to develop future psychopathology.
Keywords: childhood maltreatment, risk-taking, peer influence, fMRI
Introduction
Childhood maltreatment is associated with significantly increased risk of a range of psychiatric disorders (Gilbert et al., 2009) as well as poor economic productivity across the lifespan (Currie and Widom, 2010). However, relatively little is known about the neurocognitive mechanisms that may underpin these associations. According to the theory of latent vulnerability, maltreatment results in measurable alterations in a number of neurocognitive systems that reflect calibration to neglectful and/or abusive early environments (McCrory and Viding, 2015; McCrory et al., 2017). A general principle of the theory is that these changes may represent (at least in part) an adaptation in response to an adverse caregiving environment during childhood. However, such alterations are equally thought to incur a longer term cost as they may mean that the individual is poorly optimized to negotiate the demands of other, more normative environments and be vulnerable to future stressors (McCrory and Viding, 2015).
To date, the field of maltreatment research has focussed on four candidate neurocognitive systems that may embed latent vulnerability, including threat processing, reward processing, emotion regulation and executive functioning (see McCrory et al., 2017 for a recent review). However children exposed to maltreatment are at risk for a wide range of psychiatric conditions, consistent with the concept of multifinality (Cicchetti and Rogosch, 1996); as such, it is likely that a range of other candidate information processing domains are shaped by maltreatment experience. One such domain that has attracted increasing interest in the developmental and psychiatric literature pertains to an individual’s propensity to take risks in the context of potential reward (e.g. Lejuez et al., 2007; Cheng et al., 2012). It is possible that an early adverse environment characterized by unpredictability and/or a paucity of developmentally normative reinforcers may lead to maladaptive calibration of risk-taking propensity. This may result in atypical weighting of risk or reward with serious consequences across development. Reduced risk-taking may lead to a failure to exploit resources within the environment. In contrast, increased risk-taking may lead to greater exposure to adverse outcomes. Only two behavioural experimental studies have investigated risk-taking propensity in maltreated and post-institutionalized children. Guyer et al. (2006) used a two-choice decision-making task with reward and punishment contingencies (Wheel of Fortune) and found that children who had experienced maltreatment presenting with depressive disorders tended to select safe over risky choices compared to their peers. A related study using the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART), which measures behaviour in the context of increased risk and reward, reported reduced risk-taking in post-institutionalized preadolescent children compared to youth who were internationally adopted early from foster care and non-adopted youth (Loman et al., 2014). These findings were interpreted as reflecting early stressful and unpredictable environments leading to a preference for safe over risky choices (Loman et al., 2014), by decreasing reward sensitivity and increasing punishment sensitivity (Guyer et al., 2006). These studies are consistent with the hypothesis that reduced risk-taking propensity may represent a latent vulnerability factor: similar patterns of altered risk-taking are seen in psychiatric disorders–associated maltreatment such as depression and anxiety disorders (Forbes et al., 2007; Giorgetta et al., 2012).
To date, no prior study has investigated the neurocognitive correlates of risk-taking in individuals exposed to maltreatment. At the neural level, risky decision-making has been related to the interplay of approach and avoidance circuits that have been suggested to form a ‘risk matrix’ (Knutson and Huettel, 2015). In particular, activation of the ventral striatum (VS) has been related to gain anticipation and precedes risky choices (Kuhnen and Knutson, 2005; Canessa et al., 2013), whereas activation of the anterior insula (AI) activation is thought to relate to loss anticipation and precedes safe choices (Paulus et al., 2003; Kuhnen and Knutson, 2005; Canessa et al., 2013).
Alterations in VS and AI function have been implicated in functional neuroimaging studies of childhood maltreatment. For example, adolescents and adults who have experienced childhood maltreatment show attenuated neural activity in VS during reward processing (Dillon et al., 2009; Goff et al., 2013; Hanson et al., 2015; Gerin et al., 2017) and altered neural activity to threat related cues in the amygdala and the AI (McCrory et al., 2011, 2013; Dannlowski et al., 2012; Puetz et al., 2016). Similar atypical functioning of these neurocognitive systems has been implicated in many psychiatric disorders associated with maltreatment such as depression and anxiety disorders (Wolfensberger et al., 2008; Stoy et al., 2012; McCrory and Viding, 2015).
The frontrostriatal brain regions are known to undergo considerable change during development, particularly during adolescence (Steinberg, 2008; Smith et al., 2014). Heightened risk-taking in adolescents compared to adults is thought to relate to greater reward sensitivity of VS (Braams et al., 2014, 2016; van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2016). Adolescents’ risk-taking has also been shown to be particularly sensitive to the social context with the presence of peers associated with heightened VS activation preceding risky decisions (Gardner and Steinberg, 2005; Chein et al., 2011). How peer influence might alter risk-taking propensity in children who have experienced maltreatment remains unclear. Children with a history of maltreatment have been commonly reported to exhibit atypical peer functioning and poorer peer relationships (Bolger et al., 1998), which may alter their susceptibility to peer influence during risk-taking. For example, maltreatment experience has been associated with decreased trust and social motivation (Germine et al., 2015; Pitula et al., 2017), as well as disrupted attachment patterns (e.g. Van Ijzendoorn et al., 1999). Arguably, these responses might be associated with reduced susceptibility to peer influence. On the other hand, children with a history of maltreatment feel more excluded and frustrated after social exclusion (Puetz et al., 2014), and have a greater tendency to associate with deviant peers (e.g. Mandal and Hindin, 2013). These responses may promote susceptibility to peer influence. As such, it remains unclear how altered patterns of peer influence in children with a history of maltreatment would affect susceptibility to peer influence during risk-taking.
The aim of this study was to investigate the neural correlates of risk-taking under varying conditions of peer influence in 10- to 14-year-old children with and without a history of maltreatment. In an automatic version of the BART (Pleskac et al., 2008) during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), participants pumped up balloons, with each pump increasing the potential reward but also the risk of explosion and thus the loss of the reward. In the original manual version of the BART (Lejuez et al., 2007), the measure of risk-taking propensity (average number of pumps) is biased in the sense that it underestimates risk-taking propensity, as some trials end early (balloon explosion) as the consequence of risk-taking. A more recent version, the automated BART (Pleskac et al., 2008), requires participants to indicate at the beginning of each trial the degree of risk they want to take (how many pumps). This delivers an unbiased estimate of risk-taking propensity (Pleskac et al., 2008). Other advantages of the automated BART include shorter administration time and minimization of motor involvement (Pleskac et al., 2008). To investigate the influence of peers on risk-taking, the participants played the BART under three different conditions: when alone, knowing they were being observed by a peer and having a peer coax them to take risks.
At the behavioural level, we hypothesized that maltreated children would exhibit decreased risk-taking consistent with findings from prior experimental studies of maltreated and post-institutionalized children (Guyer et al., 2006; Loman et al., 2014). At the neural level, we hypothesized that maltreatment experience would be associated with differential modulation of VS and AI by risk level. More specifically, we expected that decreased risk-taking in children who have experienced maltreatment would be associated with altered sensitivity with which the VS and AI activation tracks the level of risk. Specifically, for these children, we predicted reduced modulation of neural activity in the VS by the risk-taking level, and increased modulation of neural activity in the AI by the risk-taking level, based on prior behavioural data. Finally, we explored whether peer influence differentially modulated behaviour and VS and AI functioning in children who had experienced maltreatment and their peers: prior data did not warrant directional hypotheses.
Materials and methods
Participants
A total of seventy-five 10–14 year olds were recruited for this study. Forty-one children who had experienced maltreatment (MT group) were recruited from a London Social Services (SS) Department and adoption agencies. Thirty-four non-maltreated children (NMT group) were recruited from schools, youth clubs and via newspaper and Internet advertisement. Exclusion criteria for the NMT group included previous contact with SS with regard to the quality of parental care or maltreatment. Exclusion criteria for all participants included a diagnosis of learning disability, pervasive developmental disorder, neurological abnormalities, standard MRI contraindications (e.g. ferromagnetic implants, past or present neurological disorder) and IQ < 70. Participants across groups were comparable in age, pubertal status, gender, IQ, socio-economic status (level of education of the parents) and ethnicity (see Table 1). Consent was obtained from the child’s legal guardian. Assent to participate in the study was obtained from all children. All procedures in the study were approved by University College London Committee (0895/002).
Table 1.
MT group (n = 41) | NMT group (n = 34) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Measure | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | P | |
Age (years) | 12.45 (1.49) | 12.44 (1.21) | 0.89 | |
WASI-IQa | 105.17 (12.70) | 107.68 (11.62) | 0.38 | |
Pubertal Development (PDS)b | 2.05 (0.70) | 1.80 (0.61) | 0.11 | |
n (%) | n (%) | P | ||
Gender (% female) | 21 (51) | 21 (62) | 0.49 | |
Ethnicity (% Caucasian) | 28 (67) | 20 (59) | 0.47 | |
SESc | 2.82 (1.55) | 3.14 (1.12) | 0.32 | |
Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | P | ||
CTQd(Total) | 37.98 (16.51) | 28.72 (4.73) | <0.01 | |
TSCCe | Depression | 47.20 (9.72) | 44.29 (8.09) | 0.17 |
Anxiety | 48.56 (12.22) | 44.44 (9.65) | 0.12 |
WASI-IQ, 2-subscale IQ derived from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence (Wechsler, 1999).
Self rating of Puberty Development Scale (Petersen et al., 1988).
Socioeconomic status (SES): Highest level education rated on 6-point scale from 0 = no formal qualifications to 5 = postgraduate qualification.
Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (Bernstein and Fink, 1998).
Trauma Symptom Checklist for Children (Briere, 1996).
Measures
Maltreatment experience
For children referred to SS, maltreatment history, including the estimated severity, onset and duration of maltreatment, was provided by the child’s social worker or adoptive parent (on the basis of SS records), using an established maltreatment scale (Kaufman et al., 1994) with an additional rating for intimate partner violence. Severity of each abuse type was rated on a scale from 0 (not present) to 4 (severe). Presence of maltreatment type was rated as follows: neglect N = 33; emotional abuse N = 40; sexual abuse N = 7; physical abuse N= 3; exposure to domestic violence N = 23. Overall across subtypes, maltreatment was characterized as follows: mean onset in years = 4.14 (s.d. = 4.39), mean duration in years= 5.92 (s.d. = 4.66) and mean severity = 1.54 (s.d. = 0.57) (see Supplementary material for onset, duration and severity by subtype). Additionally, all children completed the self-report Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (Bernstein and Fink, 1998).
Cognitive ability
Cognitive ability was assessed using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence (Wechsler, 1999).
Psychiatric symptomatology
To measure symptoms of depression and anxiety, the Trauma Symptom Checklist for Children (TSCC), a self-report measure of affective and trauma-related symptomatology, was administered to all participants (Briere, 1996).
Balloon analogue risk task
In this study, we used an automatic version of the BART, as previously described by Pleskac et al. (2008), implemented with E-prime v2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.). During the BART, only one balloon was presented per trial, each having a maximum breaking point of 60 pumps. Participants selected the number of pumps using a button box (corresponding to risk) at the beginning of each trial. A coloured bar at the bottom of the screen indicated the increasing number of pumps. On the bottom left corner, the participants could see how many points were at stake. One pump was worth 10 points. On the bottom right corner, participants could see current earnings (see Figure 1). The decision screen remained visible until the participant made a response. A randomly jittered inter-stimulus interval followed (1.1–2.5 s). Afterwards, the balloon was inflated up to the number of indicated pumps or was interrupted because it exploded. A result screen followed for 1.5–5 s showing the points earned or lost. There were 18 trials per condition (alone, observed and peer pressure).
The BART in this study was adapted to investigate peer influence on risk-taking. Participants were told that there was a group of children who were part of another study at another university, and that the aim of this other study was to investigate whether observing someone playing the BART improved performance. Participants were also told that they would receive real-time feedback about their performance from those children via text messages appearing on the screen in between the trials. Preceding the peer conditions (observed or peer pressure), each child saw a staged Skype video call between the experimenters and another child, asking if the team on the other side was ready. In the alone condition, participants played the BART on their own, receiving general feedback after every 3 trials indicating how many of the 18 trials they had completed; a picture of themselves was present on the upper right corner of the screen. In the observed condition, participants were told another child would be watching them play the BART. This was indicated by a picture of the other child presented on the upper right corner of the screen. Again, for every 3 trials, participants received general feedback indicating how many of the 18 trials they had completed. In the peer pressure condition, participants received feedback from the other child encouraging them to take risks every three trials and the second last trial (e.g. pump it more next time). The three BART conditions were administered in three counterbalanced runs of 8 min each. To ensure that all children understood the task, a practice run (not containing any peer element) preceded the scanning session.
fMRI data acquisition
Participants were scanned on a 1.5 Tesla Siemens Avanto MRI scanner (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany) using a 32-channel head coil and whole-brain EPI sequence (parameters: voxel size: 3 × 3 × 3 mm, slices per volume: 35; slice thickness: 2 mm; TR: 2975 ms; TE: 50 ms; FoV: 192 mm; gap between slices: 1 mm; flip angle: 90°). A magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo sequence (MP-Rage) was used to obtain a high-resolution structural scan (parameters: 176 slices; slice thickness: 1 mm; gap between slices: 0.5 mm; TE: 2730 ms; TR: 3.57 ms; FoV: 256 mm; matrix: 256 × 256 mm; voxel size: 1 × 1 × 1mm). Stimuli were displayed on a front projector and viewed with a mirror mounted on the head coil. All children’s heads were foam padded to minimize head motion.
Data analysis
Brain images were analysed using SPM8 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8), implemented in Matlab 2015 (The MathWorks, 2012). The first three volumes were discarded to allow for T1 equilibrium effects. Pre-processing: Each participant’s scans were realigned within each run and subsequently across all three runs to the first image of run 1. Realigned images were coregistered with the individual anatomical T1-weighted images and subsequently spatially normalized by resampling to a voxel size of 3 × 3 × 3 mm to the standard MNI space (Montreal Neurological Institute). A 6-mm Gaussian filter was applied to smoothen the normalized images and high-pass filtered at 128 Hz.
The pre-processed images were subsequently analysed using the General Linear Model, including the three task regressors, representing: (i) pumping (risk-taking), outcome split by (ii) win outcome (cashout) or (iii) loss outcome (balloon explosion). The risk level in terms of number of pumps was also entered into the model as a linear parametric modulator of the pumping regressor. To reduce movement-related artefacts, we additionally included the six motion parameters and an additional regressor to model images that were corrupted due to head motion >1.5 mm and were replaced by interpolations of adjacent images (<10% of participant’s data for N =20 NMT and for N = 31 MT, no difference between the groups, P = 0.14). For each subject, a contrast of risk for each condition (alone, observed and peer pressure) against the implicit baseline was defined in order to examine the brain activations that covaried with the parametric level of risk. In addition to investigate average brain activity related to risk-taking and feedback processing, contrasts for win and loss outcome as well as pumping were defined for each peer influence condition against the implicit baseline.
A second-level group analysis was conducted using a repeated-measures mixed-effects analysis of variance (ANOVA) by entering the individual statistical parametric maps containing the parameter estimates of the three peer influence conditions as fixed effects and an additional ‘subject factor’ for random effects. This model included the parametric modulators (number of pumps) for the three conditions to investigate brain activation covarying with risk level during the pumping phase. In addition, a second second-level model was conducted that included the main regressors of pumping, win outcome and loss outcome for the three peer influence conditions, to examine average brain activation during risk-taking and outcome (for details and results, see Supplementary material).
In line with our aim to investigate the modulation of AI and VS by the level of risk, region of interest (ROI) analyses were performed using small volume correction (SVC) as implemented in SPM for the AI and the VS, applying family-wise error (FWE) corrections for multiple comparisons. The initial threshold was set to P < 0.005 (as for the whole-brain analyses), and an additional extent cluster threshold of ke = 5 was applied as an additional precaution to disregard very small activations. AI and VS volumes were functionally defined. The AI volume was based on a parcellation of resting state functional connectivity patterns of the human insula, as provided by Deen et al. (2011). The VS volume was based on Martinez et al. (2003) who used positron emission tomography to functionally define subdivisions of the striatum. Additional whole-brain analyses were conducted, using Monte-Carlo Simulation (3D ClusterSim; Ward, 2000) correcting for multiple comparisons. Cluster-size-corrected results are reported (voxel-wise P < 0.005, ke = 75) corresponding P = 0.05, FWE corrected.
Contrast estimates from the peak voxels of clusters where significant group differences emerged were extracted using the MarsBaR Toolbox (Brett et al., 2002) implemented in SPM8 and subsequently correlated with the depression and anxiety scales of the TSCC (Briere, 1996) and maltreatment indices (onset, duration and severity; Kaufman et al., 1994) in SPSS version 21 (IBM Corp. 2012).
Results
Behavioural results
To investigate the differential effects of peer influence on risk-taking between MT and NMT groups, a 3 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA was performed on the mean number of pumps, with peer influence as within-subjects factor (alone, observed and peer pressure) and group as between-subjects factor (MT group vs NMT group).
There were significant main effects of group, F(1, 73) = 5.85, P < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.07, and condition, F(2, 146) = 135.28, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.65 (see Figure 2). There was no significant group by condition interaction, F(2, 146) = 0.94, P = 0.39, ηp2 = 0.01, suggesting that both groups were equally susceptible to peer influence. Post hoc t-tests showed that risk-taking was significantly reduced in the observed condition compared to the alone condition for both groups [MT group, t(40) = −3.41, P < 0.01; NMT group, t(33) = −2.51, P < 0.05]. In addition, risk-taking was significantly increased in the peer pressure condition compared to the observed [MT group, t(40) = 10.67, P < 0.001; NMT group, t(33) = 9.17, P < 0.001] and alone condition [MT group, t(40) = 8.72, P < 0.001; NMT group, t(33) = 6.80, P < 0.001] for both groups. The MT group engaged in significantly less risk-taking compared to the NMT group in the alone [t(73) = 2.15, P < 0.05] and the observed [t(73) = 2.48, P < 0.05] and at trend level in the peer pressure condition [t(73) = 1.75, P = 0.084].
fMRI results
In the following, the results of the parametric analysis are presented, investigating modulation of brain activation by level of risk (number of pumps) during the pumping phase (for results of the analyses of average brain activation during pumping and outcome, see Supplementary material).
F-contrast for maltreatment by peer influence interaction: parametric analysis of risk level
In line with the behavioural analysis, we first performed an F-contrast to investigate whether there was a significant peer influence (alone, observed and peer pressure) by group (MT group vs NMT group) interaction across the whole brain and the AI and VS. Similar to the behavioural findings, there were no significant interaction effects across the whole brain or the AI and VS.
Main effect of maltreatment: parametric analysis of risk level
To investigate differential modulation by risk level between the two groups, we then performed a t-contrast across all peer influence conditions. In our ROI analyses across all peer influence conditions, the MT group exhibited differential modulations of right AI (rAI) activity by level of risk (peak coordinate: x = 30, y = 17, z = −5; k = 13, t = 3.61, SVC:FWE < 0.05) relative to the NMT group. Whereas the MT group showed a positive rAI modulation by risk level, the NMT group showed a – rAI modulation by risk level (see Figure 3). There was no differential modulation by risk level in the VS between the groups. No whole-brain differences were found between the two groups in our parametric analysis.
Main effect of peer influence: parametric analysis of risk level
T-contrasts were performed incrementally to compare the different levels of peer influence in a systematic way and thus to isolate the unique effects of having another peer observing compared to playing the BART alone (‘observed vs alone’) and during peer pressure compared to a peer observing (peer pressure vs observed).
Across the MT and NMT groups, there was no difference for the contrast ‘observed > alone’ within the AI and VS. Across participants, whole-brain analyses revealed that risk level modulated the left amygdala and left parahippocampal gyrus more strongly in the observed compared to the alone condition (see Table 2 and Figure 4). Across the MT and NMT groups, there was also no difference for the contrast ‘peer pressure > observed’ within the AI and VS. Across participants, whole-brain analyses revealed that risk level modulated the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG) more strongly in the peer pressure compared to the observed condition (see Table 2 and Figure 5). No significant interaction effects were found between group and peer influence conditions for the above mentioned contrasts (‘observed > alone’, ‘peer pressure > observed’) within our regions of interest (AI, VS) or across the whole brain.
Table 2.
Brain region | R/L | x | y | z | ke | Z |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Main effect of maltreatment | ||||||
MT group > NMT group | ||||||
Anterior insulaa | R | 30 | 17 | −5 | 13 | 3.33 |
Main effect of peer influence | ||||||
Observed > alone | ||||||
Parahippocampal gyrus | R | 12 | −37 | −2 | 80 | 4.39 |
R | 27 | −25 | −17 | 3.10 | ||
R | 21 | −31 | −14 | 2.69 | ||
Parahippocampal gyrus/amygdala | L | −24 | −16 | −17 | 90 | 4.14 |
L | −24 | 2 | −23 | 3.62 | ||
L | −30 | −1 | −17 | 3.45 | ||
Peer pressure > observed | ||||||
Inferior frontal gyrus | R | 48 | 20 | 13 | 108 | 4.06 |
R | 48 | 35 | 10 | 3.81 | ||
R | 60 | 11 | 16 | 2.93 |
SVC (FWE < 0.05).
Abbreviations: R/L, Right/Left; ke, cluster extent.
Relation between risk sensitivity coded in rAI and risk-taking behaviour
To investigate the relation between risk sensitivity coded in the rAI and actual risk-taking behaviour, we extracted the contrast estimates of the significant rAI cluster (based on the main effect of group) and ran a correlation with the risk-taking behaviour.
Risk sensitivity coded in the rAI correlated negatively with risk-taking behaviour over the entire sample, r = −0.31, P < 0.01. This suggests that heightened risk sensitivity coded in the rAI is associated with decreased risk-taking at the behavioural level.
A further mediation analysis was performed to investigate whether differences in rAI risk sensitivity would partially mediate difference in risk-taking between the MT and NMT group. According to Baron and Kenny (1986), three criteria have to be fulfilled for a mediation analysis: (i) the causal variable (in this case group) has to be related to the outcome (in this case risk-taking), (ii) the causal variable has to correlate with the mediator (in this case rAI risk sensitivity) and (iii) the mediator has to have an effect on the outcome variable. Analyses were conducted using bootstrapping procedures recommended for smaller samples and operationalized in an SPSS Macro (Preacher and Hayes, 2008). We used 5000 bootstrap resamples of the data with replacement. Statistical significance with alpha at 0.05 is indicated by the 95% confidence intervals not crossing zero. We found a significant mediation effect of rAI risk sensitivity with respect to the difference in overall risk-taking between the MT and the NMT group (indirect effect = −1.25, SE = 0.70, 95% CI = −2.85 to − 0.04; see Figure 6). In addition, this mediation was total, meaning that individual differences in rAI risk sensitivity accounted fully for the group differences in overall risk-taking.
Post hoc correlations exploring the associations between risk-taking behaviour and maltreatment severity and symptoms of depression and anxiety
We investigated whether risk-taking behaviour in children with a history of maltreatment would relate to maltreatment onset, duration and severity (Kaufman et al., 1994). There was no significant correlation between risk-taking behaviour and maltreatment onset (r = 0.06, P = 0.69) and duration (r = −0.10, P = 0.55). There was an association at trend level between risk-taking behaviour and maltreatment severity (r = −0.28, P = 0.07), suggesting that the more severe the maltreatment experience was, the more risk-taking was decreased in children with a history of maltreatment. We also investigated whether risk-taking behaviour in children with a history of maltreatment would relate to self-reported symptoms of anxiety and depression as measured with the TSCC (Briere, 1996). There was no significant association between overall risk-taking and symptoms of anxiety (r = −0.19, P = 0.23) or depression (r = −0.27, P = 0.09).
Post hoc correlations exploring the associations between risk-sensitivity coded in rAI and maltreatment severity and symptoms of depression and anxiety
We investigated whether risk sensitivity coded in the rAI in children with a history of maltreatment would relate to maltreatment onset, duration and severity (Kaufman et al., 1994). To do so, we extracted the contrast estimates of the significant rAI cluster (based on the main effect of group) and ran correlations with maltreatment onset, duration and severity. There was no significant correlation between risk sensitivity in the rAI and maltreatment onset (r = 0.25, P = 0.12), duration (r = −0.03, P = 0.84) and severity (r = −0.20, P = 0.22). We also investigated whether risk sensitivity coded in the rAI in children with a history of maltreatment would relate to self-reported symptoms of anxiety and depression as measured by the TSCC (Briere, 1996). To do so, we extracted the contrast estimates of the significant rAI cluster (based on the main effect of group) and ran correlations with self-reported symptoms of anxiety and depression. Risk-sensitivity coded in the rAI in children who had experienced maltreatment was positively associated with anxiety symptoms (r = 0.32, P < 0.05) but not depression symptoms (r = 0.26, P = 0.10).
Discussion
Using fMRI, we investigated risk-taking propensity under varying conditions of peer influence in 10- to 14-year-old children with and without a history of maltreatment. There were three main findings. First, children who had experienced maltreatment engaged in less risk-taking overall during the BART compared to their non-maltreated peers. Second, children who had experienced maltreatment exhibited heightened risk sensitivity in the rAI across peer influence conditions relative to children without a history of maltreatment. Third, experience of maltreatment was not associated with any differential effects of peer influence on risk-taking at the behavioural level, nor at the neural level in our regions of interest (AI, VS) or at the whole-brain level. This suggests that peer influence exerted similar effects during risk-taking irrespective of maltreatment experience.
In line with previous studies (Guyer et al., 2006; Loman et al., 2014), our behavioural findings indicated that children who had experienced maltreatment display decreased risk-taking propensity, indicating a basic preference for safe choices over risky choices. Decreased risk-taking in children with a history of maltreatment potentially reflects heightened loss aversion. In other words, a decreased risk-taking propensity may reflect an adaptation to early adverse environments, in which a ‘safety first’ approach potentially represents the most optimal behavioural strategy. Early adverse environments are characterized by unpredictability and/or a paucity of developmentally normative reinforcers. For many children exposed to such environments, the costs of risk-taking likely outweigh the potential rewards and may be accorded greater salience during decision-making. Albeit speculative, we suggest that decreased risk-taking may serve an adaptive function for these children in early adverse environments, minimizing the likelihood of further experiences of loss and disappointment.
At the neural level, children who had experienced maltreatment showed a differential modulation of the rAI by risk level, irrespective of peer influence, relative to non-maltreated children. Whereas the rAI showed increased activation with risk level in children with a history of maltreatment, the rAI showed decreased activation with risk level in non-maltreated children. Over the entire sample, heightened risk sensitivity in the rAI (greater activation with risk level) was related to reduced risk-taking behaviourally, consistent with the view that AI functioning during risky decision-making is implicated in the inhibition of risky choices, representing a form of loss aversion (Kuhnen and Knutson, 2005; Knutson and Huettel, 2015). Indeed risk sensitivity in the rAI mediated differences in risk-taking between children with and without a history of maltreatment, albeit within a cross-sectional design. This finding needs to be replicated in a longitudinal sample. Abnormal risk sensitivity coded in the rAI in the MT group is also in line with reports of heightened AI activation during threat processing in children who have experienced maltreatment (McCrory et al., 2011). Accumulating evidence thus suggests that maltreatment experience is associated with alteration in neural circuits involved in detecting and anticipating threatening and negative stimuli in the environment. From a developmental perspective, such changes may be hypothesized to disrupt normative risk-taking and reward-seeking behaviour. As we speculate above, greater loss aversion may be adaptive for children in early adverse environments, decreasing potential future losses avoiding repeated disappointments. However, later in life in more normative and stable environments, such heightened loss aversion (indexed by abnormal risk sensitivity in the rAI), might become maladaptive, reducing the degree to which a child successfully explores and exploits the potential for rewards in their new surroundings. Heightened loss aversion and altered rAI functioning, which also characterizes patients with anxiety and depression (Pammi et al., 2015), may thus confer latent vulnerability to future psychiatric disorder for individuals exposed to maltreatment (McCrory and Viding, 2015). Consistent with this notion, our post hoc analyses showed that risk sensitivity coded in the rAI was associated with symptoms of anxiety (and at trend level with symptoms of depression) in children who have experienced maltreatment. Interestingly, abnormal AI functioning during learning and decision-making has been found in children with conduct problems and substance abuse (Crowley et al., 2006; White et al., 2013, 2016), who generally tend to exhibit more risk-taking behaviours (Verdejo-García and Pérez-García, 2008; Byrd et al., 2014). Maltreatment has been associated with both conduct problems and substance abuse outcomes (McCrory and Viding, 2015; Puetz and McCrory, 2015) and future research is needed to more fully investigate the neurocognitive risk factors related to these outcomes.
Based on previous findings showing a blunted response of the VS during reward processing in children who have experienced maltreatment (Dillon et al., 2009; Goff et al., 2013; Hanson et al., 2015), we hypothesized reduced modulation by risk level of the VS for children who had experienced maltreatment. However, no differences were observed between the groups in this region. This suggests that decreased risk-taking in children who have experienced maltreatment may primarily be related to increased loss aversion rather than decreased reward seeking, but future tasks that probe punishment avoidance and reward seeking using separate tasks are needed to further elucidate this question.
An additional aim of the study was to investigate whether there were differential effects of peer influence on risk-taking between children with and without a history of maltreatment. Based on previous studies, there were grounds to expect that maltreatment experience may be associated either with greater susceptibility to peer influence (Van Ijzendoorn et al., 1999; Puetz et al., 2014) or reduced susceptibility to peer influence (Mandal and Hindin, 2013; Germine et al., 2015; Pitula et al., 2017). In fact, we found that children who had experienced maltreatment showed normal susceptibility to peer influence during risk-taking. In line with these behavioural findings, no differential effects of peer influence were detected at the neural level across groups. In the observed condition, risk-taking was significantly lower than in the alone condition. Previous studies using the ‘driving task’ in typically developing adolescents reported increased risk-taking in the context of a peer being present (Gardner and Steinberg, 2005; Chein et al., 2011). However, a recent study that also used the BART reported that typically developing adolescents show reduced risk-taking when observed by a peer, consistent with the findings of this study (Kessler et al., 2017). This suggests that the effect of peer presence on risk-taking behaviour is influenced by the specific situational context.
In this study, no differences were found between the observed and the alone conditions in the AI or VS. However, increasing risk level modulated the left and right medial temporal lobe and left amygdala more strongly in the observed condition across groups relative to the alone condition. Heightened amygdala reactivity has been related to loss aversion in previous research (De Martino et al., 2010; Sokol-Hessner et al., 2013), suggesting that the observed pattern of reduced risk-taking in the presence of a peer may in part be associated with increased salience signalling in response to risk level.
During the peer pressure condition, all participants engaged in more risk-taking relative to the observed condition. There was no differential modulation of the AI and VS between these conditions. However, relative to the observed condition, the peer pressure condition modulated the right rIFG more strongly. Heightened risk sensitivity coded in the rIFG in the peer pressure condition, relative to the observed condition, might suggest increased integration of information prior to executing a risky behavioural choice in the peer pressure condition (Dippel and Beste, 2015).
A number of limitations should be noted. First, due to the cross-sectional design, it was not possible to examine the developmental trajectories of altered risk-taking propensity in this sample. Future studies employing longitudinal designs could examine if altered risk sensitivity in rAI predicts future psychopathology in children who have experienced maltreatment, consistent with the suggestion that this may represent a marker of latent vulnerability (McCrory and Viding, 2015). The modest correlations with anxiety and depression symptoms within our non-clinical sample of young adolescents (in the direction expected based on neuroimaging data from clinical samples using risk-taking paradigms) are consistent with this possibility. Second, many clinical studies have reported that maltreatment is associated with an increase of a wide range of complex behaviours understood to reflect ‘risk-taking’ (such as substance misuse or risky sexual behaviours: Bornovalova et al., 2008; Fergusson et al., 2008; Felsher et al., 2010). However, such behavioural outcomes are likely to be underpinned by a diverse set of cognitive processes of which risk-taking propensity, as measured in this study, may only be one. It is not known (for example) whether substance use behaviours associated with childhood maltreatment might represent some form of self-medication behaviour (Puetz and McCrory, 2015) independent of general risk-taking propensity. Future studies should shed light on the likely complex interactions of altered risk-taking propensity and a variety of risk-taking behaviours in adolescents and adults with a history of childhood maltreatment.
In conclusion, the current findings indicate that maltreatment experience is associated with reduced risk-taking and altered risk sensitivity coded in the rAI, but normal susceptibility to peer influence in the context of the BART. Furthermore, altered risk sensitivity in the rAI in children who have experienced maltreatment is related to symptoms of anxiety. Abnormal rAI functioning in children who have experienced maltreatment may therefore disrupt normative risk-taking during development and serve to increase latent vulnerability to future psychopathology. Longitudinal studies are required to test this prediction.
Supplementary data
Supplementary data are available at SCAN online.
Funding
This work was funded by a grant from the U.K. Economic and Social Research Council (ES/K005723/1) to E.J.M. (PI) and to E.V. who is also a Royal Society Wolfson Research Merit Award holder. We would like to thank the children, parents, carers and social workers who generously participated in this research.
Conflict of interest. None declared.
Supplementary Material
References
- Baron R.M., Kenny D.A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–82.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein D.P., Fink L. (1998). Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: A Retrospective Self-Report: Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Bolger K.E., Patterson C.J., Kupersmidt J.B. (1998). Peer relationships and self-esteem among children who have been maltreated. Child Development, 69(4), 1171–97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornovalova M.A., Gwadz M.A., Kahler C., Aklin W.M., Lejuez C.W. (2008). Sensation seeking and risk-taking propensity as mediators in the relationship between childhood abuse and HIV-related risk behavior. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32(1), 99–109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braams B.R., Peper J.S., van der Heide D., Peters S., Crone E.A. (2016). Nucleus accumbens response to rewards and testosterone levels are related to alcohol use in adolescents and young adults. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 83–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braams B.R., Peters S., Peper J.S., Güroğlu B., Crone E.A. (2014). Gambling for self, friends, and antagonists: differential contributions of affective and social brain regions on adolescent reward processing. NeuroImage, 100, 281–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brett, M., Anton, J.L., Valabregue, R., Poline, J.B. (2002). Region of interest analysis using the MarsBar toolbox for SPM 99. NeuroImage, 16, S497. [Google Scholar]
- Briere J. (1996). Trauma Symptom Checklist for Children (TSCC): Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
- Byrd A.L., Loeber R., Pardini D.A. (2014). Antisocial behavior, psychopathic features and abnormalities in reward and punishment processing in youth. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 17(2), 125–56.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-013-0159-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canessa N., Crespi C., Motterlini M., et al. (2013). The functional and structural neural basis of individual differences in loss aversion. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(36), 14307–17.http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0497-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chein J., Albert D., O’Brien L., Uckert K., Steinberg L. (2011). Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Developmental Science, 14(2), F1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng G.L., Tang J.C., Li F.W., Lau E.Y., Lee T.M. (2012). Schizophrenia and risk-taking: impaired reward but preserved punishment processing. Schizophrenia Research, 136(1-3), 122–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D., Rogosch F.A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8(04), 597–600.http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400007318 [Google Scholar]
- Crowley T.J., Raymond K.M., Mikulich-Gilbertson S.K., Thompson L.L., Lejuez C.W. (2006). A risk-taking “set” in a novel task among adolescents with serious conduct and substance problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(2), 175–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Currie J., Widom C.S. (2010). Long-term consequences of child abuse and neglect on adult economic well-being. Child Maltreatment, 15(2), 111–20.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1077559509355316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dannlowski U., Stuhrmann A., Beutelmann V., et al. (2012). Limbic scars: long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 71(4), 286–93.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Martino B., Camerer C.F., Adolphs R. (2010). Amygdala damage eliminates monetary loss aversion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(8), 3788–92.http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0910230107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deen B., Pitskel N.B., Pelphrey K.A. (2011). Three systems of insular functional connectivity identified with cluster analysis. Cerebral Cortex, 21(7), 1498–506.http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dillon D.G., Holmes A.J., Birk J.L., Brooks N., Lyons-Ruth K., Pizzagalli D.A. (2009). Childhood adversity is associated with left basal ganglia dysfunction during reward anticipation in adulthood. Biological Psychiatry, 66(3), 206–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dippel G., Beste C. (2015). A causal role of the right inferior frontal cortex in implementing strategies for multi-component behaviour. Nature Communications, 6, 6587.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Felsher J.R., Derevensky J.L., Gupta R. (2010). Young adults with gambling problems: the impact of childhood maltreatment. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 8(4), 545–56.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11469-009-9230-4 [Google Scholar]
- Fergusson D.M., Boden J.M., Horwood L.J. (2008). The developmental antecedents of illicit drug use: evidence from a 25-year longitudinal study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 96(1-2), 165–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forbes E.E., Shaw D.S., Dahl R.E. (2007). Alterations in reward-related decision making in boys with recent and future depression. Biological Psychiatry, 61(5), 633–9.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner M., Steinberg L. (2005). Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 41(4), 625–35.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.625 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Germine L., Dunn E.C., McLaughlin K.A., Smoller J.W. (2015). Childhood adversity is associated with adult theory of mind and social affiliation, but not face processing. PLoS One, 10(6), e0129612.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerin, M.I., Puetz, V.B., Blair, J., et al. (2017). A neurocomputational investigation of reinforcement-based decision making as a candidate latent vulnerability mechanism in maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 1689–1705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert R., Widom C.S., Browne K., Fergusson D., Webb E., Janson S. (2009). Burden and consequences of child maltreatment in high-income countries. The Lancet, 373(9657), 68–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giorgetta C., Grecucci A., Zuanon S., et al. (2012). Reduced risk-taking behavior as a trait feature of anxiety. Emotion, 12(6), 1373–83.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goff B., Gee D.G., Telzer E.H., et al. (2013). Reduced nucleus accumbens reactivity and adolescent depression following early-life stress. Neuroscience, 249, 129–38.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.12.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guyer A.E., Kaufman J., Hodgdon H.B., et al. (2006). Behavioral alterations in reward system function: the role of childhood maltreatment and psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(9), 1059–67.http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000227882.50404.11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanson J.L., Hariri A.R., Williamson D.E. (2015). Blunted ventral striatum development in adolescence reflects emotional neglect and predicts depressive symptoms. Biological Psychiatry, 78(9), 598–605.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- IBM Corp. Released (2012). IBM SPSS statistics for Macintosh version 21.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.
- Kaufman J., Jones B., Stieglitz E., Vitulano L., Mannarino A.P. (1994). The use of multiple informants to assess children‘s maltreatment experiences. Journal of Family Violence, 9(3), 227–48. [Google Scholar]
- Kessler L., Hewig J., Weichold K., Silbereisen R.K., Miltner W.H.R. (2017). Feedback negativity and decision-making behavior in the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) in adolescents is modulated by peer presence. Psychophysiology, 54(2), 260–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B., Huettel S.A. (2015). The risk matrix. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 5, 141–6. [Google Scholar]
- Kuhnen C.M., Knutson B. (2005). The neural basis of financial risk taking. Neuron, 47(5), 763–70.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.08.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lejuez C.W., Aklin W., Daughters S., Zvolensky M., Kahler C., Gwadz M. (2007). Reliability and validity of the youth version of the balloon analogue risk task (BART–Y) in the assessment of risk-taking behavior among inner-city adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36(1), 106–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loman M.M., Johnson A.E., Quevedo K., Lafavor T.L., Gunnar M.R. (2014). Risk‐taking and sensation‐seeking propensity in postinstitutionalized early adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(10), 1145–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mandal M., Hindin M.J. (2013). From family to friends: does witnessing interparental violence affect young adults’ relationships with friends? Journal of Adolescent Health, 53(2), 187–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez D., Slifstein M., Broft A., et al. (2003). Imaging human mesolimbic dopamine transmission with positron emission tomography. Part II: amphetamine-induced dopamine release in the functional subdivisions of the striatum. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism, 23(3), 285–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MathWorks (2012). MATLAB and statistics toolbox release. Natick, MA: MathWorks.
- McCrory E.J., De Brito S.A., Kelly P.A., et al. (2013). Amygdala activation in maltreated children during pre-attentive emotional processing. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 202(4), 269–76.http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.112.116624 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrory E.J., De Brito S.A., Sebastian C.L., et al. (2011). Heightened neural reactivity to threat in child victims of family violence. Current Biology, 21(23), R947–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrory E.J., Viding E. (2015). The theory of latent vulnerability: reconceptualizing the link between childhood maltreatment and psychiatric disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 27(02), 493–505.http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrory E.J., Gerin M.I., Viding E. (2017). Annual Research Review: Childhood maltreatment, latent vulnerability and the shift to preventive psychiatry—the contribution of functional brain imaging. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(4), 338–357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pammi V.C., Rajesh P.G., Kesavadas C., et al. (2015). Neural loss aversion differences between depression patients and healthy individuals: a functional MRI investigation. The Neuroradiology Journal, 28(2), 97–105.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1971400915576670 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus M.P., Rogalsky C., Simmons A., Feinstein J.S., Stein M.B. (2003). Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism. Neuroimage, 19(4), 1439–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen A.C., Crockett L., Richards M., Boxer A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitula C.E., Wenner J.A., Gunnar M.R., Thomas K.M. (2017). To trust or not to trust: social decision‐making in post‐institutionalized, internationally adopted youth. Developmental Science, 20(3), 1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pleskac T.J., Wallsten T.S., Wang P., Lejuez C.W. (2008). Development of an automatic response mode to improve the clinical utility of sequential risk-taking tasks. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16(6), 555–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puetz V.B., Kohn N., Dahmen B., et al. (2014). Neural response to social rejection in children with early separation experiences. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(12), 1328–37.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puetz V.B., McCrory E.J. (2015). Exploring the relationship between childhood maltreatment and addiction: a review of the neurocognitive evidence. Current Addiction Reports, 2(4), 318–25.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40429-015-0073-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puetz V.B., Viding E., Palmer A., et al. (2016). Altered neural response to rejection‐related words in children exposed to maltreatment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(10), 1165–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preacher K.J., Hayes A.F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879–91.http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith A.R., Steinberg L., Chein J. (2014). The role of the anterior insula in adolescent decision making. Developmental Neuroscience, 36(3-4), 196–209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sokol-Hessner P., Camerer C.F., Phelps E.A. (2013). Emotion regulation reduces loss aversion and decreases amygdala responses to losses. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(3), 341–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28(1), 78–106.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoy M., Schlagenhauf F., Sterzer P., et al. (2012). Hyporeactivity of ventral striatum towards incentive stimuli in unmedicated depressed patients normalizes after treatment with escitalopram. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 26(5), 677–88.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0269881111416686 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Duijvenvoorde A.C.K., Peters S., Braams B.R., Crone E.A. (2016). What motivates adolescents? Neural responses to rewards and their influence on adolescents’ risk taking, learning, and cognitive control. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 70, 135–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Ijzendoorn M.H., Schuengel C., Bakermans–Kranenburg M. j. (1999). Disorganized attachment in early childhood: meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Development and Psychopathology, 11(2), 225–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verdejo-García A., Pérez-García M. (2008). Substance abusers’ self-awareness of the neurobehavioral consequences of addiction. Psychiatry Research, 158(2), 172–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward, B. D. (2000). Simultaneous inference for FMRI data. Biophysics Research Institute, Medical College of Wisconsin.
- Wechsler D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation and Harcourt Brace. [Google Scholar]
- White S.F., Pope K., Sinclair S., et al. (2013). Disrupted expected value and prediction error signaling in youths with disruptive behavior disorders during a passive avoidance task. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(3), 315–23.http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12060840 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White S.F., Tyler P.M., Erway A.K., et al. (2016). Dysfunctional representation of expected value is associated with reinforcement‐based decision‐making deficits in adolescents with conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(8), 938–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfensberger S.P., Veltman D.J., Hoogendijk W.J., Boomsma D.I., de Geus E.J. (2008). Amygdala responses to emotional faces in twins discordant or concordant for the risk for anxiety and depression. NeuroImage, 41(2), 544–52.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.