Abstract
Contemporary environments are saturated with food cues that stimulate appetites in the absence of hunger, which leads to maladaptive eating. These settings can induce persistent drive to eat, as learned behaviors can reappear after extinction. Behavioral paradigms of responding renewal provide a valuable framework to study how food cues contribute to the inability to resist palatable foods and change maladaptive eating habits. Using a rat model for this persistent food motivation, we determined sex differences in the causal function for the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) during context-mediated renewal of responding to food cues. Previously, we found behavioral sex differences (only males exhibited renewal) and differential recruitment within the vmPFC (increased Fos induction in males but decreased in females). Here, we used DREADDs (Designer Receptors Exclusively Activated by Designer Drugs) to silence vmPFC neurons in males and to stimulate vmPFC neurons in females specifically during renewal. Silencing vmPFC neurons in males disrupted renewal of responding to a food cue, while stimulating vmPFC neurons in females induced this behavior. These findings demonstrate sex differences in the vmPFC function in a model of food seeking relevant to environmentally driven appetites contributing to obesity and eating disorders.
Introduction
Persistent food cravings and the inability to resist palatable foods even when eating is maladaptive are hallmarks of overeating dysregulation1. Food cues are important contributors to these excessive drives. Cues previously associated with food can stimulate appetite and food consumption in the absence of hunger, which can lead to maladaptive overeating2–5. Food cue exposure, and associated cravings, significantly influence eating behavior and weight gain in humans6 and neural responding to food cues in sated states has been correlated with long-term weight gain7. A sharp rise in obesity has occurred over the last 30 years, and during the same period there have been profound changes in the locations where we consume food. The number of meals consumed outside the home has substantially increased, and these meals occur in distinct places such as fast-food restaurants8. Palatable foods are now so widely available that very few locations in our environments are not associated with food and eating.
Environments (contexts) previously associated with food can later stimulate consumption in the absence of hunger9. In addition to this function as conditioned stimuli, contextual cues are critical mediators of food memory and reappearance of learned behaviors after extinction. Cognitive processes mediating learned behaviors after extinction, such as renewal, might explain the difficulty associated with changing unhealthy eating habits and weight control in our environment1,10. Indeed, the renewal (reinstatement) model was recently introduced as a framework to study mechanisms of relapse to palatable food seeking, similar to the reinstatement model for relapse of drug use1,11,12.
One important renewal mechanism is mediated by context. Contexts can mediate memory formation and later recall of discrete cues (e.g., a tone or light), particularly when those cues have different meaning in different contexts. When a cue is paired with food in one context but not in another, context is ‘setting the occasion’ for the cue’s meaning—signaling food or not signaling food13. In that preparation, acquisition of a cue-food association and extinction of that association occur in different contexts, and then the renewal of conditioned behavior (food seeking) is induced by return to the acquisition context. Context-mediated renewal is a well-suited behavioral model for persistent food motivation, relevant to our contemporary lifestyle and insatiable appetites.
Women are more likely than men to be overweight and obese, and obese women show greater impairments in food associative learning14–17. There are also sex differences in animal models of associative learning, particularly with contextual processing18–23, that underscore the importance of determining mechanisms in both sexes. Still, female subjects are underrepresented in both basic and clinical research24,25. Therefore, recently we began to investigate both sexes in context-mediated renewal of responding. We found sex differences in context-mediated renewal of responding to Pavlovian food cues19, and distinct recruitment of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) in male and female rats20. Increased Fos induction within the infralimibic and prelimbic areas in the vmPFC were associated with renewal behavior in males, while females did not show renewal or an increase in Fos induction. These distinct patterns suggest sex-specific vmPFC activation mediates behavioral sex differences. We posit context-mediated renewal is a decision-making process that involves retrieval, evaluation, and the use of context-mediated cue memory to guide responding, a function that requires vmPFC activation.
Here we aimed to establish the necessity of the vmPFC and that its differential activation underlies sex differences in context-mediated renewal of responding to Pavlovian food cues. First, we established an ABA renewal procedure that is suitable for within-subject neural manipulations. To directly test the causal function of the vmPFC we used a chemogenetic approach to manipulate vmPFC neurons to disrupt renewal of responding in males and to selectively induce this behavior in females. We used DREADDs (Designer Receptors Exclusively Activated by Designer Drugs)26 to silence (hM4Di) vmPFC neurons in males and activate (hM3Dq) vmPFC neurons in females during renewal tests. Two control conditions were employed in order to establish specific DREADDs effects. One group of control rats received DREADDs but was given a vehicle at test. Rats in the other control group received a control viral vector without DREADDs (AAV-only) and were given CNO at test. This CNO group was added in order to rule out potential effects of CNO independent from its action on DREADDs. Inclusion of that control group was particularly important given recent evidence that CNO was not biologically inert in some cases27,28. Identifying the vmPFC as a critical site of sex differences in context-mediated renewal will further our fundamental understanding of contextual processing and learning and memory in both sexes, as well as clinical treatment of excessive cognitive drive to eat.
Results
Experiment 1
In order to manipulate vmPFC neurons in a within-subjects preparation, we first established a behavioral procedure (Methods, Fig. 1a,b, Supplementary Fig. 1). All rats received acquisition training in a distinct context, consisting of tone (conditioned stimulus; CS) and food (unconditioned stimulus; US) pairings and then extinction training, consisting of CS presentations alone in a different context. Rats were then tested for food seeking conditioned responding (CR; food cup behavior) during CSs in each context. Higher responding during the CS in the acquisition context (vs. the extinction context) indicates successful renewal. We replicated the sex differences found with a between-subjects ABA paradigm19,20. Both sexes showed similar acquisition learning and similar performance during extinction training (Fig. 1a). At test, males showed significantly higher responding in the acquisition context compared to the extinction context during the CS, while females showed similar low responding in both contexts (Fig. 1b). A mixed-design ANOVA (repeated (context) and between (sex)) found a significant effect for Context by Sex interaction (F(1,14) = 19.483, p = 0.001). Post hoc two-tailed t-test determined males showed significantly higher responding in the Acquisition context (p < 0.05; Fig. 1b). Males also responded significantly higher during preCS in the acquisition compared to extinction context, while females had low responding in both contexts (mixed-design ANOVA (repeated (context) and between (sex)) significant for Test Context by Sex interaction (F(1,14) = 10.286, p = 0.006); post hoc two-tailed t-test (p < 0.05); Fig. 1b). Additional two-tailed t-test determined males showed significantly higher responding during the CS compared to preCS period in Acquisition context (t(7) = −4.257, p = 0.004). The elevation (CS-preCS responding) difference in responding in the acquisition vs. extinction context was also higher for males compared to females but that difference was not statistically significant (Supplementary Fig. 1).
Experiment 2
To silence the vmPFC in males, we infused an adeno-associated virus (AAV) containing the gene for an inhibitory Gi-coupled hM4Di receptor into the vmPFC (prelimbic and infralimibic areas) to express DREADD receptors (Fig. 2a,b). DREADD-selective, biologically inert ligand, clozapine-N-oxide (CNO) was injected i.p. prior to testing, which selectively silenced the infected neurons. Rats in the 3 groups: DREADD + CNO, DREADD + Vehicle, Control Virus (AAV with EGFP-only) + CNO (Fig. 2c) acquired and extinguished CRs to the tone similarly (Supplementary Fig. 2). On tests, the DREADD + CNO group failed to show renewal responding with similar low responding to CSs in both contexts, while DREADD + Vehicle and Control Virus + CNO groups showed renewal responding with higher induction of CRs to the CSs in the acquisition context (Fig. 2d–f). A planned orthogonal contrast of the elevation difference (t(15) = 2.248, p = 0.040) confirmed that silencing vmPFC neurons disrupted renewal behavior in males (Fig. 2f). Responding during preCS remained low and did not differ between groups (p > 0.05).
In a subset of rats, we examined Fos induction during a test for renewal in the acquisition context to verify differential activation in the DREADD + CNO group compared to the control groups (Fig. 2g–j). The AAV expression was similar across groups (Fig. 2h). The percentage of AAV neurons that were double-labeled with Fos (AAV + Fos/AAV) was lowest in the DREADD + CNO group; however, the decrease was not significantly different from the controls (mean ± SEM by group: DREADD + CNO: 35 ± 5%, DREADD + vehicle: 42 ± 6%, Control Virus + CNO: 38 ± 3%; p > 0.05). Interestingly, the DREADD + CNO group had significantly higher number of total Fos positive neurons compared to controls (Fig. 2i; planned orthogonal contrast, t(8) = −3.429, p = 0.009), possibly due to disinhibition of neurons receiving inhibitory inputs from the hM4Di infected neurons. Furthermore, the percentage of total Fos expressing neurons that were double-labeled (AAV + Fos/Fos) was significantly lower in the DREADD + CNO group compared to controls (Fig. 2j; planned orthogonal contrast, t(8) = 5.616, p = 0.001), further suggesting increased total Fos may be due to a release from inhibition by hM4Di infected neurons.
Experiment 3
To activate vmPFC neurons in females, we infected vmPFC neurons with AAV containing hM3Dq vector and then trained the rats in the same behavioral design as described above (Fig. 3a,b; Experiment 3). All rats acquired and extinguished CRs similarly (Supplementary Fig. 3). At test, the DREADD + CNO group had higher CRs to the CSs in the acquisition context (Fig. 3d–f; planned orthogonal contrast of the elevation difference t(16) = −2.528, p = 0.022.), while control groups had similar responding to the CSs in both contexts. Responding during preCS remained low and did not differ between groups (p > 0.05). Stimulating vmPFC neurons in females induced renewal of behavior, thereby eliminating behavioral sex differences. Similar to Experiment 2, to confirm hM3Dq effectiveness, we analyzed Fos induction during a test for renewal in the acquisition context. We found no differences in the total number of AAV labeled or total Fos labeled neurons (p > 0.05). In the DREADDs + CNO group compared to controls there were significantly more double-labeled neurons (AAV + Fos) (planned orthogonal contrast, t(7) = −3.649, p = 0.008, data not shown), and a significantly higher percentage of total AAV-labeled neurons were double labeled with Fos (AAV + Fos/AAV) (Fig. 3j; planned orthogonal contrast, t(7) = −4.787, p = 0.002). In addition, a higher percentage of total Fos labeled neurons were double labeled (AAV + Fos/Fos) in the DREADDs + CNO group compared to controls (mean ± SEM by group: DREADD + CNO: 39 ± 8%, DREADD + vehicle: 25 ± 4%, Control Virus + CNO: 23 ± 6%; planned orthogonal contrast, t(7) = −2.526, p = 0.039).
Discussion
These findings are the first evidence that manipulating vmPFC activity can impact behavioral sex differences. Chemogenetic silencing of vmPFC neurons in male rats disrupted context-mediated renewal of cue driven food seeking while stimulating vmPFC neurons in female rats induced renewal of this behavior, demonstrating vmPFC activity is crucial for renewal in both sexes. These results reveal a likely sex-specific function of the vmPFC in context-mediated renewal, a model of persistent food seeking relevant to environmentally driven appetites. Nevertheless, whether manipulations in the opposite direction (i.e., activating the vmPFC in males and inhibiting it in females) would have an effect on context-mediated renewal of responding to food cues remains to be tested.
The current evidence of vmPFC’s function during renewal of responding to food cues is in agreement with prior evidence for the vmPFC’s role in the control of appetitive behaviors by associative cues in males. The vmPFC is recruited during cue-food associative learning29, is critical for context-mediated renewal of instrumental responding for drug and food30–32, and feeding stimulated by contextual food cues33,34. However, the vmPFC is not solely important for contextual processing, it is also recruited by discrete cues during learning and cue-potentiated feeding35,36. It seems likely the vmPFC function involves accessing different memory traces in order to guide feeding behavior.
Here, during extinction, the levels of conditioned responding were somewhat low initially compared to responding during acquisition and therefore instead of a traditional extinction curve there was overall low responding. Previously, in a between-subjects ABA preparation, we found rats tested in a novel context during extinction had lower conditioned responding compared to rats tested in the acquisition context19. In the current study all rats were placed in a novel context during extinction training, which is likely the reason for their low responding.
The lack of renewal in females may be due to differences in learning strategies or differences in the brain mechanisms mediating contextual learning and extinction. Females may be relying more on the extinguished cue-food association than contextual information and thus generalizing across contexts more than males. In support of the idea that differences in extinction learning or recall may be mediating renewal differences, sex differences have been found during fear extinction learning37,38. In general, females showed better fear extinction learning; however, other evidence suggests the differences are more complex22,39,40. Furthermore, in a preparation that tested food consumption in the presence of a fear cue (a tone predictive of electric footshocks), during extinction testing, female rats showed longer inhibition of feeding compared to males41. Finally, a study that compared freezing behavior and food consumption in neutral and aversive contexts also found sex differences in context generalization23. Males and females showed similar freezing behavior (high in aversive and low in neutral contexts), indicating similar context discrimination across sexes, but only males selectively inhibited feeding in an aversive context, while females inhibited feeding in both contexts.
Our prior work19,20 demonstrated that renewal of responding in females is modulated by estradiol. Estradiol replacement to ovariectomized rats induced renewal responding compared to intact and ovariectomized females without estradiol replacement. Interestingly, testing rats during different estrous phases did not impact renewal responding in females, indicating that estradiol effects are not mediated acutely during renewal expression. Instead, estradiol may be important during extinction learning, similar to its impact on fear extinction37. In that regard, previous fear conditioning work found that higher generalization across contexts in females is in part dependent on estradiol42.
The vmPFC’s function in renewal may be mediated via glutamatergic pyramidal neurons or inhibitory interneurons or both43,44. Recent work has suggested that there is heterogeneity in the vmPFC substrates controlling different aspects of feeding behavior, and that AMPA- but not NMDA-type glutamatergic receptors are critical for behavioral organization during feeding45, while μ opioid system mediates food consumption, likely via changes in motivation and palatability46. Interestingly, μ opioid receptors are located on GABA neurons and it was hypothesized that their stimulation disinhibits the pyramidal output46 indicating that at least some aspects of feeding behavior are mediated by GABA transmission. Similarly, orexin receptor 1 blockade during cue-potentiated feeding resulted in increased Fos induction in the vmPFC36 also indicating inhibitory mechanisms. Finally, GABA transmission in the medial prefrontal cortex increased when male rats were placed in a cocaine-associated environment47. Therefore, inhibitory mechanisms may be critical during renewal responding, which would explain the unexpected Fos induction patterns we found using inhibitory DREADDs in male rats (Experiment 2). Inhibition of inhibitory neurons could have caused disinhibition of their targets and higher Fos induction in the experimental (hM4Di) group. However, future work is needed to determine if hM4Di inhibition of the vmPFC during renewal silenced GABA neurons and to determine which glutamatergic inputs were interrupted. In that regard, our recent study found the vmPFC inputs from the ventral hippocampal formation, paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus and basolateral amygdala were differently recruited during context-mediated renewal in males and females, suggesting under-recruitment in females may be the reason for lack of renewal (Anderson, L. C. & Petrovich, G. D., Neurobiology of learning and memory, Under Review (2018)).
These novel results are expected to propel future molecular and translational work investigating sex differences in associative learning and memory and how learned cues contribute to maladaptive eating habits and eating disorders. This work points to the vmPFC as a novel neural target for therapeutic intervention and sex-specific treatment relevant to sex differences in obesity and maladaptive eating behaviors16. More generally, this work suggests dissociable vmPFC functioning in males and females in decision-making processes that guide our behavior48,49.
Methods
Subjects
Adult male and female Long-Evans rats (250–275 g at arrival; total n = 88; Charles River Laboratories) were individually housed and maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00). Males and females were housed in separate colony rooms. 16 rats (8 males, 8 females) were used for Experiment 1. 36 males were used for Experiment 2; 36 females were used for Experiment 3. 35 rats total were excluded for the following reasons: poor health (1 male from Experiment 2, 1 female from Experiment 3), misplaced/insufficient viral expression (17 males from Experiment 2, 15 females from Experiment 3), or high preCS responding (1 female from Experiment 3; preCS responding higher than 3 standard deviations from the mean). Subjects in Exp. 2 and 3 underwent surgery after they were acclimated to the colony room for two days. Animals were given a week to recover post-surgery during which they were weighed and handled daily. All rats (Exp. 1–3) were given ad libitum access to standard laboratory chow (18% Protein Rodent Diet #2018, Harlan Teklad Global Diets, Madison, WI) and water, except as otherwise noted. All housing and testing procedures were in compliance with the National Institute of Health Guidelines for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the Boston College Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. None of the animals were used in prior experimental procedures unrelated to these studies. Animals were randomly assigned to experimental condition and renewal test day order (see Behavioral Training Procedure).
Surgical Procedure
Viral stocks of adeno-associated virus (AAV) carrying constructs AAV5-hSyn-HA-hM4D-IRES-mCitrine (Experiment 2), AAV5-hSyn-HA-hM3D-IRES-mCitrine (Experiment 3), and AAV5-hSyn-EGFP (Experiment 2 and 3) were purchased from the Vector Core at University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill. Animals were briefly anesthetized with isoflurane (5%; Baxter Healthcare Corporation, Deerfield, IL, USA), then deeply anesthetized with an intramuscular injection of a mixture (1 ml/kg body weight) of ketamine (50 mg/mL; Fort Dodge Animal Health, Fort Dodge, Iowa) and xylazine (10 mg/mL; LLOYD Laboratories, Shenandoah, IA, USA). While under anesthesia, animals received bilateral stereotaxically placed injections of the AAVs into the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC; volume 0.75 µl; flow rate 0.25 µl/min; titer 1 × 10e12, relative to bregma anterior-posterior [AP]: +2.8 mm, mediolateral [ML]: +/−0.7 mm, dorsoventral [DV]: −4.7 mm). A 10 µl Hamilton syringe with 32 gauge cannula driven by a motorized stereotaxic injector (Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL) was used to deliver microinjections. Stereotaxic surgeries were performed according to the procedures for aseptic technique in survival surgery and postoperative care approved by Boston College IACUC. The Biosafety Committee at Boston College has approved all protocols with AAVs. Behavior started three weeks after surgery to allow for recovery and sufficient expression of the viruses.
Apparatus
The behavioral training was conducted in identical behavioral chambers (30 × 28 × 30 cm; Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA) located in a room different from the colony housing rooms. The chambers had aluminum top and sides, clear Plexiglas rear wall and front hinged door and a floor of stainless steel rods 5 mm thick spaced 15 mm apart. Chambers contained a recessed food cup (3.2 × 4.2 cm) and a 4 W house light. Each chamber was located in a sound- and light-attenuating cubicle (79 × 53 × 53 cm), which was equipped with a ventilation fan (55 dB) and video camera attached to a recording system (Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA). The conditioned stimulus (CS) was a 10 second tone (75 dB, 2 kHz). The unconditioned stimulus (US) consisted of two food pellets (45 mg pellets, formula 5TUL; Test Diets, Richmond, IN, USA) delivered to the food cup. Chambers were modified in visual, tactile, and olfactory features, to create two distinct environments. For Context 1, a black Plexiglas panel was placed on top of the grid floor (so that rats could not see or feel the grids), and the doors to the cubicles were closed. For Context 2, a black Plexiglas panel was inserted diagonally across the side of the chamber creating a wall, and the doors to the cubicle were left open, and 1% acetic acid in water solution (Fisher Scientific, Fair Lawn, NJ) was sprayed onto the tray below the grid floor.
Behavioral training procedure
All behavioral training and testing occurred between 9:00 and 14:00. A week before start of training, all rats were food deprived and their daily food allotment was restricted to gradually reach 85% of their body weight; they were maintained at this weight for the duration of the experiment. All rats received 1 g of the food pellets (Unconditioned Stimulus, US) in the home cage the day before the training started to familiarize them with the pellets. The training consisted of three phases: conditioning (acquisition), extinction, and renewal tests. The conditioning and extinction occurred in two different contexts and the testing was conducted in both contexts (test order counterbalanced). The behavioral chambers used as distinct contexts for conditioning and extinction were counterbalanced (the acquisition context for half of the rats was in Context 1 and for the other half was in Context 2). During the acquisition phase, rats were trained for five days, with one 34-minute training session per day. During each session they received eight presentations of the tone CS, each immediately followed with delivery of food pellets (US) into the food cup. During the extinction, rats received two 34-minute sessions (one session per day), each with eight presentations of the tone CS alone with no USs. Rats were tested for renewal of responding with CS-only presentations in Context A and Context B, counterbalanced for order, on separate days (order randomly assigned). The tests for renewal were 34-minute sessions with eight CS presentations with no USs. All sessions were recorded and stored on DVDs for behavioral analysis. In Experiments 2 and 3, prior to the tests for renewal, clozapine-N-oxide (CNO; Enzo Life Sciences, Farmingdale, NY) was injected i.p. in half of the animals who received AAV5-hSyn-HA-hM4D-IRES-mCitrine (Experiment 2) or AAV5-hSyn-HA-hM3D-IRES-mCitrine (Experiment 3), the other half received saline. All of the animals that received the control viral vector (AAV5-hSyn-EGFP) received CNO. This resulted in 3 groups: DREADDs + CNO, DREADDs + Vehicle, Control Virus + CNO. CNO was administered at a dose of 3 mg/kg (1 mg/ml).
Behavioral Measures
Trained observers, ‘blind’ to experimental condition or sex of the rats, analyzed animals’ behavior from the video recordings. The primary measure of conditioning (conditioned response, CR) was the expression of ‘food cup behavior’ during the CS. The food cup behavior was defined by distinct nose pokes into the recessed food cup, or by rats standing in front of and directly facing the food cup. Behavior was scored every 1.25 seconds during each 10 second preCS and CS periods. At each observation only one behavior was recorded (food cup or other). The number of CRs was summed and converted to a percentage of the total time during each period an animal expressed food cup behavior.
Histological Procedures
After the end of renewal tests in Exp. 2 and 3, rats were anaesthetized with tribromoethanol (375 mg/kg body weigh, intraperitoneal injection) and transcardially perfused with 0.9% saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M borate buffer. A subset of rats (n = 4 per group) was perfused ninety minutes after the end of renewal test to be used for Fos induction detection. The brains were stored for 20–24 hrs at 4 °C in a paraformaldehyde and 12% sucrose mixture and then rapidly frozen in hexanes cooled with dry ice and stored at −80 °C. Brains were cut into 40 μm coronal sections using a microtome and collected into three adjacent series. For all brains, one tissue series was mounted unstained for identification of viral expression in the vmPFC. Another series was mounted and stained with thionin for identification of cytoarchitectonic borders. For the brains used for Fos detection, one tissue series was immediately processed with Fos immunohistochemistry, described below.
Tissue Processing and Immunohistochemistry
Immediately following slicing, sections were incubated for 72 h at 4 C in a blocking solution [KPBS containing 0.3% Triton X-100, 2% normal donkey serum (017-000-121; Jackson ImmunoResearch, West Grove, PA, USA)], with the primary antibody: c-Fos goat primary (1:2,000; sc-52-G; Santa Cruz, Dallas, TX). After rinses in KPBS, tissue was incubated for 1 h in the dark in the blocking solution containing the secondary antibody: Alexa 546 anti-goat (1:200; A11056; Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA). Following rinses in KPBS, in semidarkness tissue was mounted onto slides (SuperFrost Plus), dried, coverslipped with Vectashield HardSet Mounting Medium with DAPI (H-1500; Vector Labs, Burlingame, CA, USA), and stored at 4 C until analysis. This immunohistochemistry procedure was successfully repeated 8 times in the current study, and has been used successfully numerous additional times within our laboratory.
Image acquisition and analysis
The acquisition of images was conducted with a Zeiss Axio Image Z2 fluorescence microscope (Carl Zeiss Microscopy GmbH; Jena, Germany) and attached Hamamatsu ORCA-R2 camera (Bridgewater, NJ, USA). To determine the location and extent of the AAV expression, images of the areas with viral expression and the adjacent thionin-stained tissue were acquired at 10x. Neuroanatomical borders were drawn onto the thionin-stained image and then transposed to the adjacent fluorescent image using ImageJ software (NIH). The spread of AAV expression were then drawn on computerized versions of the standard rat brain atlas templates50 using illustration software (Adobe Illustrator CS5.5). Based on this analysis, well-defined and localized AAVs expressions were identified and included for data analysis (at least 50% in the prelimbic and infralimbic regions at bregma AP +3.60, +3.20 or +2.80; with less than 25% of the injection spread outside this target region). Only these subjects were included in behavioral data analysis. For Fos analysis, 2 × 2 tiled images were taken at 20× (tissue area approximately 820 × 630 μm) in the prelimbic area of the vmPFC (+3.20 mm AP from bregma). Three brains were excluded from Fos analysis (one male Control Virus + CNO from Experiment 2, one female DREADD + CNO and one female Control Virus + CNO from Experiment 3) due to inadequate viral expression in the prelimbic area at bregma +3.20 mm AP. Images were pseudocolored with green for DREADDs, red for Fos and blue for DAPI (nuclear counterstain). Single AAV, single Fos and double-labeled (AAV and Fos) cells were determined based on characteristic nuclear (Fos) or cytoplasmic (AAV) labeling by an investigator ‘blind’ to experimental condition. Single (AAV or Fos) and double-labeled (AAV and Fos) cells were counted. Double-labeled cells were expressed as a percentage of total AAV cells and total Fos cells.
Statistical analysis
Behavioral data analysis was conducted to compare CRs using a mixed-design ANOVA (repeated (context) and between (sex)) and significant main effects and interaction effects (p < 0.05) were followed by post hoc two-tailed t-tests. In addition, in Experiments 2 and 3, a priori planned orthogonal contrasts were used. All statistical analyses were conducted in SPSS. Data are presented as median and inner quartiles, as well as minimum and maximum when indicated. Statistical details of the experiments can be found in the figure legends and figures. Exclusion of subjects was determined based on the criteria described under Subjects and Image acquisition and analysis. Sample sizes were determined based on previous research in our lab19,20. All relevant data from the current study are available from the corresponding author upon request.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
Supported by NIDDK (R01DK085721) to GDP. We thank Amanda Jenkins and Connor Milone for technical assistance.
Author Contributions
G.D.P. conceptualized the study, L.C.A. and G.D.P. conceived experimental design, L.C.A. carried out the experiments and data analysis, L.C.A. and G.D.P. co-wrote the manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-018-20553-4.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Calu DJ, Chen YW, Kawa AB, Nair SG, Shaham Y. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76(Pt B):395–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.04.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Martin AA, Davidson TL. Physiology & behavior. 2014;136:185–193. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.02.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Levitsky DA. Physiology & behavior. 2005;86:623–632. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.08.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Berthoud HR, Munzberg H, Morrison CD. Gastroenterology. 2017;152:1728–1738. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2016.12.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Petrovich GD. Physiology & behavior. 2013;121:10–18. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Boswell RG, Kober H. Obesity reviews: an official journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity. 2016;17:159–177. doi: 10.1111/obr.12354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sun X, et al. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2015;35:7964–7976. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3884-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Nielsen SJ, Siega-Riz AM, Popkin BM. Preventive medicine. 2002;35:107–113. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2002.1037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Petrovich GD, Gallagher M. Physiology & behavior. 2007;91:397–403. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Boutelle KN, Bouton ME. Appetite. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.05.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Stewart J, de Wit H, Eikelboom R. Psychological review. 1984;91:251–268. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.91.2.251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Volkow ND, Wise RA. Nature neuroscience. 2005;8:555–560. doi: 10.1038/nn1452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Holland PC, Bouton ME. Current opinion in neurobiology. 1999;9:195–202. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)80027-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Brooks S, Prince A, Stahl D, Campbell IC, Treasure J. Clinical psychology review. 2011;31:37–51. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM. Jama. 2014;311:806–814. doi: 10.1001/jama.2014.732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Mitchell S, Shaw D. Best practice & research. Clinical obstetrics & gynaecology. 2015;29:289–299. doi: 10.1016/j.bpobgyn.2014.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Zhang Z, Manson KF, Schiller D, Levy I. Current biology: CB. 2014;24:1731–1736. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Dalla C, Papachristos EB, Whetstone AS, Shors TJ. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:2927–2932. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809650106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Anderson LC, Petrovich GD. Physiology & behavior. 2015;151:338–344. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.07.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Anderson LC, Petrovich GD. Neurobiology of learning and memory. 2016;139:11–21. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Farrell MR, Sengelaub DR, Wellman CL. Physiology & behavior. 2013;122:208–215. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Maren S, De Oca B, Fanselow MS. Brain research. 1994;661:25–34. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91176-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Reppucci CJ, Kuthyar M, Petrovich GD. Appetite. 2013;69:186–195. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Asarian L, Geary N. American journal of physiology. Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology. 2013;305:R1215–1267. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00446.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Zucker I, Beery AK. Nature. 2010;465:690. doi: 10.1038/465690a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Roth BL. Neuron. 2016;89:683–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Gomez JL, et al. Science. 2017;357:503–507. doi: 10.1126/science.aan2475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.MacLaren, D. A. et al. eneuro, 10.1523/eneuro.0219-16.2016 (2016).
- 29.Cole S, Hobin MP, Petrovich GD. Neuroscience. 2015;286:187–202. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.11.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Eddy MC, Todd TP, Bouton ME, Green JT. Neurobiology of learning and memory. 2016;128:33–39. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Willcocks AL, McNally GP. The European journal of neuroscience. 2013;37:259–268. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bossert JM, et al. Nature neuroscience. 2011;14:420–422. doi: 10.1038/nn.2758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Petrovich GD, Ross CA, Gallagher M, Holland PC. Physiology & behavior. 2007;90:362–367. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.09.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Petrovich GD, Ross CA, Holland PC, Gallagher M. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2007;27:6436–6441. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5001-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Holland PC, Petrovich GD. Physiology & behavior. 2005;86:747–761. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.08.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Cole S, Mayer HS, Petrovich GD. Scientific reports. 2015;5:16143. doi: 10.1038/srep16143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Cover KK, Maeng LY, Lebron-Milad K, Milad MR. Translational psychiatry. 2014;4:e422. doi: 10.1038/tp.2014.67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Lebron-Milad K, Milad MR. Biology of mood & anxiety disorders. 2012;2:3. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-2-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Pryce CR, Lehmann J, Feldon J. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1999;64:753–759. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(99)00147-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ribeiro AM, et al. Brain and cognition. 2010;74:145–151. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Petrovich GD, Lougee MA. Physiology & behavior. 2011;104:996–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lynch J, III., Cullen PK, Jasnow AM, Riccio DC. Learning & memory. 2013;20:628–632. doi: 10.1101/lm.032011.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Gabbott P, et al. The Journal of comparative neurology. 2012;520:2440–2458. doi: 10.1002/cne.23047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Sesack SR, Deutch AY, Roth RH, Bunney BS. The Journal of comparative neurology. 1989;290:213–242. doi: 10.1002/cne.902900205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Baldo BA, Spencer RC, Sadeghian K, Mena JD. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:960–970. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mena JD, Sadeghian K, Baldo BA. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2011;31:3249–3260. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2050-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Chefer VI, Wang R, Shippenberg TS. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:2018–2029. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Dalley JW, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2004;28:771–784. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.O’Doherty JP. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2011;1239:118–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06290.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Swanson, L. W. Brain maps: structure of the rat brain. A laboratory guide with printed and electronic templates for data, models and schematics. (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 2004).
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.