Abstract
Early life experiences can have profound and persistent effects on traits expressed throughout the life course, with consequences for later life behavior, disease risk, and mortality rates. The shaping of later life traits by early life environments, known as ‘developmental plasticity’, has been well-documented in humans and non-human animals, and has consequently captured the attention of both evolutionary biologists and researchers studying human health. Importantly, the parallel significance of developmental plasticity across multiple fields presents a timely opportunity to build a comprehensive understanding of this phenomenon. We aim to facilitate this goal by highlighting key outstanding questions shared by both evolutionary and health researchers, and by identifying theory and empirical work from both research traditions that is designed to address these questions. Specifically, we focus on: (i) evolutionary explanations for developmental plasticity, (ii) the genetics of developmental plasticity and (iii) the molecular mechanisms that mediate developmental plasticity. In each section, we emphasize the conceptual gains in human health and evolutionary biology that would follow from filling current knowledge gaps using interdisciplinary approaches. We encourage researchers interested in developmental plasticity to evaluate their own work in light of research from diverse fields, with the ultimate goal of establishing a cross-disciplinary understanding of developmental plasticity.
Keywords: developmental plasticity, early life effects, gene-environment interaction, epigenetics, evolution of plasticity, developmental origins of health and disease
INTRODUCTION
Early life environments can profoundly shape traits related to both human health and Darwinian fitness. For example, humans exposed to famine in utero exhibit higher rates of obesity, heart disease and schizophrenia in adulthood than siblings conceived under better conditions [1, 2]. In addition, children exposed to many adverse psychological events experience more physiological wear-and-tear by midlife, and live shorter lives on average than individuals exposed to few adverse events [3, 4]. Similar effects of early life conditions are observed in non-human vertebrates. In red deer and Asian elephants, individuals born during ecologically challenging periods experience faster reproductive senescence than individuals born during better times [5, 6], and in zebra finches and great tits, the availability of resources in early life predicts clutch size in adulthood [7]. Together, these studies provide just a few striking examples of the impact early life environments can have on later life traits.
The capacity of genetically similar individuals to produce substantially different phenotypes depending upon environmental conditions during early life (defined here as the period between conception and reproductive maturation, following [8]) is known as ‘developmental plasticity’. Because the impact of early conditions can be so dramatic, with potent effects on reproduction and survival, developmental plasticity is of central interest to multiple disciplines. Researchers in medicine, public health, psychology, economics, and sociology seek to understand the link between early conditions and adult health because of its relevance to disease treatment and prevention (reviewed in [9–14]). Concurrently, evolutionary biologists seek to understand the impact of early environments on traits related to Darwinian fitness, because this knowledge is important for understanding the evolution of complex traits and the selection pressures that shape them (reviewed in references [15–19]).
The parallel importance of early environmental effects in both health and evolutionary research presents an opportunity to forge a more complete, interdisciplinary understanding of developmental plasticity. We argue that realizing this opportunity should be a priority, but doing so is challenging because conflicting theories and terminology have arisen in the different fields in the absence of the cross-talk necessary to resolve them. Our goal here is thus to initiate and encourage increased connectivity, specifically by outlining key questions shared by health and evolutionary researchers, as well as approaches from diverse fields that could be used to address them. In doing so, we do not comprehensively review developmental plasticity research (we refer interested readers to many excellent, recent reviews [9–19]). Instead, we aim to spark excitement about work in three distinct areas where the potential for cross-disciplinary gain is high. In the first section, we discuss evolutionary explanations for developmental plasticity and highlight critical tests that distinguish between potential explanations. In the second and third sections, we shift our emphasis to focus on the molecular basis of developmental plasticity, including the contribution of genetic variation (see ‘Genetics and genomics of developmental plasticity’ Section) and the molecular mechanisms that mediate plasticity within an individual’s lifetime (see ‘The molecular mechanisms that mediate developmental plasticity’ Section). In each area, we discuss current knowledge gaps, promising approaches for filling these gaps and the gains in human health and evolutionary biology that would follow. Because of our focus on human health, we primarily draw on studies of humans and other mammals, but where relevant, we include important work from birds, insects, and other taxa. Finally, we provide a roadmap to terminology commonly used by health researchers and evolutionary biologists (Table 1), as well as a brief guide to evolutionary frameworks that are discussed in both communities (Box 1).
Table 1.
Term | Range of definitions | Proposed usage |
---|---|---|
Selection |
|
Avoid the abbreviation; specify whether the topic is natural selection, social selection, health selection, selection bias, and so on. |
Heritable |
|
Use the term ‘inherited’ instead of ‘heritable’ for meaning number 2, i.e., in the absence of data to support heritability in the technical sense. |
Adaptation |
|
When using these terms, clarify whether evolutionary adaptation or short-term physiological adaptation is meant. If referring to evolutionary adaptation, the term should be reserved for traits with demonstrated (rather than assumed) fitness advantages in carriers, relative to other individuals in a population or species. |
Maladaptation |
|
Confine the use of this term to definition 1. In the absence of evidence that a trait is truly maladaptive, replace the term with another word, such as detrimental, and clarify that only immediate or short term costs are being considered. |
‘Development constraints’ models posit that, in the face of early adversity, natural selection favors developmental strategies that promote immediate survival, even if other aspects of development are impaired. Thus, in the face of constraints, organisms ‘make the best of a bad job’ and follow developmental trajectories that avoid immediate death or impairment, but that may generate negative consequences later in life. For instance, the ‘silver spoon ‘effect’ occurs when exposure to favorable conditions during development produce fitness advantages in adulthood, and conversely when adverse conditions during development produce disadvantages later in life [21]. The ‘thrifty phenotype hypothesis’ posits that inadequate early nutrition triggers ‘nutritional thrift’, impairing the development of pancreatic function and pre-disposing the individual to metabolic disorders in adulthood [163]. This hypothesis was later updated to posit that metabolic disorders in later life would be milder if adult nutritional conditions closely matched those in childhood ([164], reviewed in [15]). However, evidence is limited for health gains following matched early life and adult nutritional environments (see ‘Tests of developmental constraints versus predictive response models’ Section ); thus we view the thrifty phenotype, at its core, as a hypothesis about constraints. The ‘allostatic load hypothesis’ invokes stressors—during development and throughout life—as challenges to which the organism must respond by modifying its physiology and behavior. The accumulation of such challenges results in increased susceptibility to disease [165]. This hypothesis is often overlooked in the developmental plasticity literature, but we include it here because, by explicitly invoking stressors during development as contributors to poor health in later life, it has the hallmarks of a development constraints model. Contrary to some interpretations, developmental constraints models are models of an adaptive process: a developmentally plastic organism will generally have higher fitness than one that is unable to alter any development processes in the face of environmental limitations [18].
In contrast to constraints models, ‘predictive models’ posit that natural selection produces organisms that adjust their phenotype during development to optimize their performance in a future (as opposed to the current) environment. The standard ‘PAR’ model proposes that cues in early life trigger phenotypic responses that improve fitness at a later stage of development [9, 24, 25, 166]. This model, also called the ‘“external” PAR model’ (ePAR; [26, 34]), finds strong support in a few short-lived vertebrates and invertebrates [25, 39], weak support in a meta-analysis of 58 experimental plant and animal studies [39], and no or inconclusive support in the four species of long-lived vertebrates in which it has been tested [27–31]. Another problem for predictive models is that adaptive adjustments can only occur if a cue during development accurately predicts the adult environment [18, 33, 34], a condition that is rare for long-lived organisms. The recently proposed ‘“internal” PARr model’ (iPAR; [26]) offers a solution to this problem by suggesting that developmental responses to adversity are not adapted to future ‘external’ conditions, but rather to the predicted poor ‘internal’ somatic state of individuals growing up under adversity. The iPAR converges with developmental constraints models in invoking developmental tradeoffs as a primary driver of poor adult outcomes. However, it differs from constraints models by proposing that the developing organism will respond to these tradeoffs by maturing early to maximize reproduction under a shorter life expectancy ([26]; see [167, 168] for recent empirical tests of iPAR). The ‘adaptive calibration model’ (ACM), another recent, untested variant of predictive models, also predicts accelerated maturation when early adversity increases mortality risk [23, 32]. The ACM also invokes later life plasticity, making the difficult-to-test prediction that individuals may re-calibrate their physiological responses to adversity later in life in response to environmental conditions.
The evolution of developmental plasticity
Shared interests, current knowledge, and gaps in knowledge
Dobzhansky’s famous message—that ‘nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution’ [20]—has been taken to heart by researchers interested in developmental plasticity. The result has been a plethora of explanations for how natural selection has produced a trait, namely early environmental sensitivity, that can sometimes lead to detrimental health or fitness outcomes. These explanations fall into two broad categories, both of which assume a role for adaptive evolution (Box 1). However, the two categories of models differ in whether they view plastic responses as determined by immediate constraints or as anticipatory.
‘Developmental constraints models’ propose that, in resource-limited environments, developing organisms make tradeoffs to protect critical functions (e.g. investing in brain development at the expense of growth). These tradeoffs may improve the organism’s chance of survival in early life, but reduce long-term somatic quality and compromise adult health [8, 15, 21, 22]. In other words, natural selection may attempt to optimize overall fitness (relative to individuals that exhibit no plasticity) by accepting tradeoffs that carry long-term costs (relative to individuals who did not experience resource limitation). In contrast, ‘predictive models’ describe a situation in which environmental cues in early life predict the adult environment; hence organisms evolve the ability to adjust their phenotype during development to maximize fitness in the predicted adult environment ([9, 23–26]; see Box 1). Predictive models thus invoke the presence of ‘informational’ cues in early life that, over evolutionary history, have reliably predicted the nature of the adult environment [18]. If these early cues induce an incorrect predictive response (e.g. fail to correctly predict the adult environment), the resulting mismatch results in poor later life health.
Despite the conceptual impact of both developmental constraints and predictive models, few studies have attempted to empirically distinguish between them, especially in humans or other long-lived mammals (but see [27–31]). In addition, the last few years have brought new hypotheses that merge some aspects of constraints and predictive frameworks [23, 26, 32] (Box 1). Only recently have researchers attempted to identify the contexts in which predictive versus constraints-induced plasticity evolves [18, 33–38]. Expanding on these two areas of research—testing predictions from theory and identifying contexts that promote plasticity evolution—is critical for understanding the evolution and maintenance of early life effects, as well as the health costs incurred by early adversity. Below we discuss approaches for doing so, as well as the specific gains that would follow.
Tests of developmental constraints versus predictive response models
A critical test of predictive versus constraints models requires comparing the fitness of individuals born in high-quality environments with those born in low-quality environments, when both sets of individuals experience both high- and low-quality conditions as adults [15, 27, 29, 30, 39]. Under a predictive model, fitness will be maximized when individuals encounter matched early life and adult environments, whereas under a constraints model, individuals born in high-quality environments will consistently outperform individuals born in low-quality environments. Experiments that satisfy this ‘fully factorial design’ have now been conducted in dozens of short-lived insect, amphibian, reptile and plant species, with stronger support for constraints than predictive plasticity [39]. However, in a few striking cases, experimental manipulations of early life auditory cues that signal environmental quality have produced strong evidence for predictive plasticity (e.g. in red squirrels [40] and zebra finches [41]).
In humans, inferences about the evolution of developmental plasticity have been largely based on cross-population or between-cohort comparisons [42–45], as well as longitudinal studies that do not satisfy the fully factorial design. This limitation exists because identifying human populations that are appropriate for fully factorial, within-individual tests is challenging, as is obtaining individual-based, longitudinal data. Consequently, in spite of enthusiasm for the idea that mismatches between early and later life environments produce pathology in humans, evidence in support of this idea is very limited. Indeed, the only critical tests of predictive models in humans that we know of, in pre-industrial Finnish populations, find no support for predictive adaptive responses (PARs). Instead, these studies find that pre-industrial Finns born in poor environments exhibit fertility and survival detriments, rather than enhancements, when they re-encounter challenging environments in adulthood [20, 23].
Inspite of the challenges of studying developmental plasticity in long-lived species, several longitudinal studies of human populations exposed to varying levels of early adversity are underway [46–48]. With time, these studies have the potential to identify the environments and traits that are best explained by constraints versus predictive models. Meanwhile, long-term studies of long-lived mammals offer great potential for performing fully factorial tests, especially in cases where longitudinal data exist or are being collected. Already, three groups have leveraged data from well-studied populations (of roe deer, bighorn sheep and yellow baboons) to examine fitness outcomes when individuals naturally encounter environments that both match and mismatch their early life conditions [27, 30, 31]. All three studies found stronger support for developmental constraints than predictive plasticity, with no or limited support for predictive plasticity overall (see Box 1). Other long-term studies of wild mammals [49] are poised to contribute similar tests. In addition to circumventing limitations faced by human studies, such work will be important for interpreting the evolutionary history of developmental plasticity in a comparative framework, including the degree to which aspects of early life effects in humans are unique.
Identifying contexts in which predictive responses and developmental constraints evolve
Recently, several groups have used simulations and mathematical models to identify the contexts in which predictive plasticity should evolve [18, 33, 34, 50]. This work has highlighted two predictions relevant to human disease, as well as the evolution of plasticity more generally. First, when the environment varies stochastically on a timescale shorter than the organism’s generation time, early life conditions will be a poor predictor of the adult environment, and predictive plasticity will be unlikely to evolve [26, 33, 34, 51]. This situation is commonly encountered by long-lived animals, who often experience unpredictable heterogeneity in rainfall, food availability, and other environmental variables over the course of their multi-year lives [27–30]. Thus, we should expect predictive plasticity to be rare in these contexts. Second, organisms can only evolve plastic responses to environments they are repeatedly and consistently exposed to over evolutionary time, and responses to novel or atypical environments may generally be disadvantageous [52, 53] (Box 1) [33, 54]. If true, this prediction implies that for experimental studies focusing on extreme or novel early life challenges (e.g. exposure to recently introduced toxins), it may be inappropriate to interpret the results in an adaptive evolutionary framework.
Testing these two predictions—(i) that predictive responses are unlikely to evolve in stochastic environments and (ii) that highly novel early environments trigger maladaptive responses—is fundamental for assessing and refining current frameworks. Such tests have already begun in systems amenable to experimental evolution (e.g. fungal and bacterial species, as well as Caenorhabditis elegans and Drosophila melanogaster), and this work has supported the idea that different types of environmental predictability select for different types of plasticity [35, 38, 55–59]. For example, Dey et al. [35] exposed C. elegans to an environmental regime that predictably alternated between normoxia and anoxia every generation. Because the future environment could be reliably predicted from current conditions, populations evolved the ability to ‘prepare’ offspring for the alternate environment (by manipulating glycogen provisioning to their embryos).
Though compelling, evolutionary experiments of this type are infeasible for long-lived organisms, and alternative approaches are needed. Cross-taxon analysis can be used to test whether predictive plasticity evolves when future environments are predictable: once empirical tests have been performed in a sufficiently diverse set of species, researchers can examine the relationship between life history variation, ecological variation and predictive plasticity evolution. Amassing the required set of empirical tests is clearly a tall order, but points to an important goal for the research community. Testing the second theoretical prediction—that highly novel early environments trigger maladaptive responses (Table 1)—can be addressed with comparative data from species that encounter rapid environmental change or urbanization. For instance, studies that examine behavioral responses to human-induced environmental change are generating considerable data on the extent and nature of maladaptive responses to highly novel environments [60, 61]. This framework could be expanded to incorporate early life effects. Importantly, the prediction that novel environments trigger maladaptive responses would hold under both a constraints and predictive plasticity framework: both models assume plasticity has evolved through natural selection, a process that usually requires many generations of environmental exposure.
Gains from answering outstanding questions
For researchers interested in human health, understanding when predictive models do and do not explain developmental plasticity is essential for knowing what the optimal adult environment ‘looks like’ for each individual, and which mitigation strategies will best combat the effects of early adversity. Under a predictive model, health is maximized when early and adult environments are concordant, suggesting, for instance, that manipulating adult diet or lifestyle could mitigate the effects of undernourishment in early life. In contrast, if adult phenotypes arise from developmental constraints, ‘matching’ the adult environment to the early one will be unproductive or even detrimental, and intervention efforts should focus on improving early conditions. Sound theory and a robust body of empirical work are essential for understanding whether predictive plasticity is expected to be common or rare in long-lived species such as humans, and for which traits.
For evolutionary biologists, understanding the distribution of predictive versus constraints-induced plasticity in nature will help reveal the tradeoffs organisms make under resource-limited conditions, as well as the selective pressures that determine variation in tradeoffs across species. In addition, understanding the drivers of plasticity evolution is important for predicting how species will cope with environmental change [62], including through the evolution of plastic responses [33, 63, 64]. Accurately modeling responses to environmental change will require an understanding of the properties of the environment and the organism’s life history that determine whether and how different types of plasticity evolve.
Genetics and genomics of developmental plasticity
Shared interests, current knowledge, and gaps in knowledge
Identifying genes and genetic variants that contribute to plasticity is critical for understanding the evolutionary history of plastic traits, as well as sources of inter-individual variation in the capacity for plasticity. We focus on two questions related to this topic. First, what genes are involved in generating developmental plasticity within a given species? To date, work on this question has largely focused on polyphenisms, which occur when two or more discontinuous phenotypes are produced from the same genotype. Classic examples include caste differentiation in eusocial insects and seasonal morphs in butterflies, which have been well-studied at the molecular level [65–69]. A second, related question is: to what degree does genetic variation for plasticity itself exist among individuals of a given species? Work in both humans and non-human animals indicates that variation in the magnitude or direction of an environmental response is often a function of genotype, a phenomenon known as gene × environment (G × E) interactions [70–73] (Fig. 1).
In spite of these advances, major gaps in our knowledge remain. First, the genes involved in producing plasticity have only been identified in a few organisms (reviewed in [17, 69]), motivating expansion of this work to a larger set of species. Second, while many populations clearly harbor genetic variation for plasticity, few attempts have been made to identify the loci involved, especially using unbiased genome-wide approaches. For example, molecular psychiatrists have largely focused tests of gene × early environment interactions (referred to here as ‘G × early E’ interactions; Fig. 2A) on a handful of candidate genes [73, 74]. Such studies are often low powered and susceptible to unidentified confounds (e.g. population structure), leading to results that have been controversial and difficult to replicate [75]. Transitioning to well-powered whole genome scans, though challenging, would address these concerns [76], and provide a more nuanced view of the variants involved in a highly complex trait. Below, we discuss each of these gaps in knowledge, namely: (i) identifying genes that contribute to plasticity, and (ii) mapping individual-level differences in environmental sensitivity (i.e. G × early E interactions), with a focus on tractable molecular traits such as gene expression.
Identifying genes that contribute to developmental plasticity in diverse organisms
To understand the genetics of plasticity in a wide range of organisms—especially long-lived species that are not amenable to experimental manipulations—researchers will need to add additional approaches to their toolkit. One non experimental approach is to identify genetic differences between closely related species that exhibit differences in plasticity, specifically in cases where plasticity has been repeatedly gained or lost across a phylogenetic tree. For example, several Daphnia (water flea) species that develop helmets following early exposure to predators (a morphological change that protects these individuals from future predation) have sister species that do not exhibit this form of plasticity [58]. Similarly, closely related locust species vary in whether they display density-dependent polyphenisms [60], and aphid species differ in their capacity to produce winged versus unwinged morphs as a function of ecological conditions [77]. Scans for consistent genetic differentiation across multiple related species (or populations) that do versus do not display developmental plasticity could identify possible plasticity-related genes. Importantly, this study design has already proven useful for identifying the genetic basis of morphological and physiological traits in non-model systems [78, 79]. For example, Reid and colleagues compared genome sequences in 4 natural populations of killifish that had independently evolved tolerance to pollutants with four populations that had not, and found that genes involved in a single signaling pathway (the aryl hydrocarbonreceptor pathway) were repeated targets of selection in pollution-tolerant populations [78].
Mapping individual-level differences in environmental sensitivity on a genome-wide scale
Two genome-wide approaches could be harnessed to map G × early E interactions (Fig. 2B and C), both of which focus on how genetic variation affects gene expression, an environmentally sensitive molecular trait. The first approach is the study of ‘response eQTL:’ genetic variants that interact with the environment to influence gene expression levels (such that the magnitude or direction of the genotype-expression correlation varies across environments; Fig. 2C). Response eQTL have typically been identified in vitro by exposing cells to differing conditions and identifying variants that predict gene expression levels in a given condition (usually focusing on variants close to a given gene, known as ‘cis’ eQTL, to limit multiple hypothesis testing [80–82]).
Adapting the response eQTL framework to study developmental plasticity in other systems (including through in vivo approaches) is challenging but increasingly feasible as genomic resources accumulate for non-model systems. For example, one could identify genetic variants that affect gene expression in Daphnia clones exposed or unexposed to predators, or in spadefoot toad tadpoles fed a diet of shrimp versus detritus (an early life exposure that leads to striking differences in developmental trajectories) [83, 84]). Importantly, previous work in both humans and in non-model organisms has highlighted that early life experiences commonly affect long-term patterns of gene expression [17, 85–87], setting the stage for studies that investigate how these changes interact with genetic variation. In addition to designs that rely on in vivo exposures, culturing cells from non-model organisms is increasingly achievable. Thus, it may be possible to measure gene expression levels, in cells derived from the same individual, when these cells are cultured in the presence or absence of an in vitro cue that induces developmental plasticity in the system (e.g. hormone [68, 69] or kairomone [88, 89] cues).
A related approach for mapping G × early E interactions is the use of within-individual measures of allele specific gene expression (ASE). This approach capitalizes on the fact that when one allele of a gene within a heterozygous individual is overexpressed relative to the other allele of the same gene (i.e. when there is ‘allelic imbalance’), a nearby, cis-acting heterozygous regulatory variant must exist that modulates gene expression. If the degree of allelic imbalance within an individual varies across environments, a G × E interaction is implicated (Fig. 2B). ASE approaches have already been used to find variants associated with sensitivity to smoking, exercise, and medication use in human genome-wide gene expression datasets [90, 91], and early efforts have used ASE to identify genetic variation for developmental plasticity at candidate genes [92]. These approaches [91, 93–95] could be leveraged to investigate G × early E interactions in a range of organisms. Indeed, population-scale gene expression datasets are increasingly being collected for non-model organisms [96, 97], including in some long-term study populations [85, 98].
Gains from answering outstanding questions
Together, this work will clarify the genes involved in plasticity, as well as the loci that generate inter-individual variation. Doing so is important for understanding the biology of environmentally induced disease, and for identifying people who are particularly vulnerable to early life stressors. For example, recent work on another type of plasticity—the immune response—has leveraged the response eQTL framework to identify key transcription factors that mobilize the response to infection [82, 99], and to understand why individuals of European versus African genetic ancestry differ in their responses to pathogens [81]. Such approaches promise to improve our understanding of the mechanistic basis of plasticity, contributing to improved prediction and, over the long run, better options for therapeutic interventions.
For evolutionary biologists, the proposed work will identify genes involved in developmental plasticity in diverse species, and provide estimates of the prevalence, effect sizes, and genomic targets of G × early E effects. Such data will provide insight into the evolutionary history of plasticity, and will help reveal how early environmental perturbations alter the genotype–phenotype relationship. Further, understanding the genetic architecture of developmental plasticity (e.g. the number of genes that underlie this trait, as well as their effect sizes and pleiotropic or epistatic effects) will inform models of how rapidly plasticity can evolve when the environment shifts. Theoretical work in this area has assumed a simple architecture underlying plasticity (one or a few loci of large effect) [33]. If empirical data indicate this assumption is unwarranted [100, 101], we will need to radically readjust our models and predictions.
The molecular mechanisms that mediate developmental plasticity
Shared interests, current knowledge and gaps in knowledge
What are the molecular mechanisms that allow early environmental variation to produce diverse phenotypes from static gene sequences? And further, following an early life exposure, which genes are targeted by such mechanisms (i.e. which genes are ‘differentially regulated’)? Answering these questions can advance our understanding of the causal chain linking environmental inputs with health or fitness-related outcomes.
Recent work on the molecular mechanisms that mediate developmental plasticity has focused most heavily on generating genome–wide gene expression or epigenetic data for individuals with known environmental histories. Here, we use the term ‘epigenetic’ to refer to a class of environmentally or developmentally sensitive mechanisms that can stably alter gene expression without changing the underlying DNA sequence (including DNA methylation, the best studied epigenetic mark to date [102–105]; histone modifications [102, 106, 107], chromatin accessibility; and non-coding RNAs [108]). Importantly, changes in the epigenome represent a major avenue through which environmental variation is translated into variation in organism-level traits. For example, groundbreaking work in laboratory mice found that maternal diet during pregnancy influences offspring methylation near the agouti gene, which in turn affects agouti gene expression, fur color, body mass, and susceptibility to diabetes [109, 110].
Studies like these have generated substantial interest in the degree to which changes in the epigenome explain early life effects on fitness-related traits [111–114]. However, several key gaps remain. First, as with studies of the genes involved in plasticity, studies of early environmental effects on genome-wide gene regulation (i.e. epigenetic marks or gene expression levels) have been concentrated in a handful of systems and contexts: model organisms and humans that experienced extreme early life challenges (e.g. famine, institutionalization, or child abuse) [17, 69, 104, 105, 115–117]). As a result, general principles governing plastic responses at the molecular level—including how they evolve and function in nature—remain unclear. Second, a robust body of work has shown that environmentally responsive gene regulatory mechanisms are sensitive to stimuli across the life course, not just in early life [118–120]. Thus, a key outstanding question is: to what degree are epigenetic marks and gene expression patterns stably affected by conditions in early life (e.g. during the prenatal, neonatal, and juvenile periods) versus reversible in adulthood? Below we discuss approaches for addressing these missing pieces.
Early life environmental effects on gene regulation in diverse species
Molecular studies of captive or laboratory animals can provide powerful insight into causality, but two common features of their design can limit their generality. First, animal models of early adversity are often extreme, and thus differ from the conditions animals have encountered over their evolutionary history [87, 121–124]. Second, lab-based studies frequently compare the outliers of an early environmental distribution in a case/control framework, an approach that fails to address common variation that lies near the mean (Fig. 1). Work in human medical epigenetics has followed a similar pattern, and has largely focused on early life challenges with major health consequences (e.g. famine, child abuse or institutionalization [105, 115, 117, 125, 126]). Currently, the degree to which responses to ‘atypical’ stressors (e.g. evolutionarily novel, rare, or extreme stressors) recapitulate responses to stimuli that organisms have encountered repeatedly throughout their evolutionary history is unknown. Importantly, theory suggests that animals evolve plasticity in stress response pathways over many generations of stabilizing selection, and extreme or novel environments may break down or ‘decanalize’ such responses, increasing variability in unexpected ways [127–129]. Going forward, we therefore suggest dual emphases on both (i) understanding gene regulatory responses to atypical early environmental perturbations with large health or fitness effects, and (ii) covering the spectrum of naturally occurring early environmental variation. This dual approach will allow us to understand whether regulatory responses to naturally occurring stressors are conserved, and how such systems are perturbed when the stressor falls outside the species’ evolutionary range.
Developing a general picture of the gene regulatory consequences of early environmental stress would be greatly accelerated by transcriptomic and epigenomic datasets from diverse, longitudinally studied populations. Studies of humans and non-human mammals are likely to be mutually informative, as epigenetic mechanisms are largely conserved across these species [130, 131]. This strategy has already been modeled by a handful of studies focusing on gene expression or DNA methylation [85, 98, 132, 133], providing important proof of principle. Work on other epigenetic marks such as histone modifications, chromatin accessibility and non-coding RNAs is less advanced, presumably because these marks are more challenging to measure outside of the laboratory (e.g., for certain marks, protocols must be performed on large numbers of live cells immediately upon collection). However, genomic protocols are rapidly becoming more streamlined and optimized for low-input [134–136], making it increasingly possible to study multiple epigenetic marks in longitudinally followed populations of wild animals or remotely living humans.
The timing and stability of environmental epigenetic effects
Initial work on epigenetic marks, especially DNA methylation, emphasized the stable effects of events that occurred during specific early life critical periods, a phenomenon referred to as ‘embedding’ or ‘programming’ [103, 137–139]. However, recent work has shown that conditions throughout early life (i.e. the period between conception and reproductive maturity [8]) and in adulthood can both have potent effects on the epigenome [118, 120, 140]. This duality implies that it may be possible to reverse (or further exacerbate) epigenetic changes induced by early adversity, depending upon the environment later in life (as in [141]). However, the degree to which variation in any epigenetic mark is determined by early life versus adult conditions remains poorly understood. Early studies that have tackled this important question (all focused on DNA methylation in humans) have suffered from low power, confounds between adult environments and health habits, and a reliance on retrospective self-reporting [115, 116, 138].
Here again, long-term studies of natural or captive animal populations—where blood or other tissue is routinely collected from individuals followed over the life course—could be leveraged to circumvent problems posed by human population studies. In addition, such work could be fruitfully complemented by (i) prospective, longitudinal studies of human cohorts with repeated biological sampling (many of which are in progress, and will be invaluable once completed [142]); and (ii) experiments in lab-based systems. For example, studies of laboratory rodents and captive rhesus macaques have identified epigenetic changes following early life manipulations [102, 103, 143, 144], but they have rarely been extended to coupled manipulations of both early life and adult environments (but see [145]). Additionally, studies of birds that produce altricial hatchlings offer the ability to experimentally investigate early life stressors that are ecologically relevant, and to combine these with the ability to monitor longitudinal fitness outcomes and collect molecular samples [146–150]. In systems such as these, and others that share similar features, researchers could expand on study designs used to investigate evolutionary explanations for early life effects [16], where individuals are exposed to high- or low-quality early environments followed by high- or low-quality adult environments in a ‘fully factorial’ design. In this case, however, the phenotype of interest would be genome-wide epigenetic patterns, rather than fitness.
Gains from answering outstanding questions
For human health researchers, understanding the gene regulatory effects of early life stressors—both typical and atypical—is essential for developing generalizable, predictive frameworks for environmentally induced disease. Epigenetic studies of wild animals have an important potential role here: they can illuminate whether organisms living in their natural environments respond to stressors in ways that recapitulate responses in laboratory animal models or humans. Further, such comparisons can help to empirically test whether certain modern early life exposures, which may be atypical relative to human evolutionary history, perturb evolutionarily conserved gene regulatory programs in ways that lead to poor health outcomes. With respect to timing, understanding the genome-wide stability of early environment-induced epigenetic changes will reveal the degree to which early adversity can be exacerbated or reversed by adult conditions. This information is key for understanding when interventions would be most meaningful.
Answers to these same questions are also important for testing and advancing evolutionary theory. As noted earlier, some types of plasticity (e.g. predictive plasticity, Box 1) are expected to evolve only under particular environmental scenarios. For example, when the environment varies on a timescale greater than the lifespan of the organism and not predictable during development, a risk-spreading strategy known as ‘diversifying bet-hedging’ is expected to evolve [33, 151, 152]. Under this strategy, individuals randomly develop one of several phenotypes, each of which is beneficial in one possible adult environment. Conditions selecting for both predictive plasticity and bet-hedging are common in nature [33, 153] and thus an empirical study of whether epigenetic mechanisms adhere to these predictions should be feasible in natural systems. Specifically, if the environment selects for irreversible, predictive responses to early conditions, do we see a strong and stable correlation between early environmental variation and epigenetic variation? And where bet-hedging is the favored form of plasticity, are epigenetic profiles randomly allocated among individuals at birth in a stable manner? In this way, epigenomic data could be used to test and refine theories of the evolution of developmental plasticity, and to understand whether molecular mechanisms follow theoretical predictions about plasticity in organism-level traits.
CONCLUSIONS
We have highlighted key outstanding questions in developmental plasticity research, along with suggestions for how to answer them. Our list is by no means exhaustive, but it is meant to ignite conversations between evolutionary and health researchers, and to highlight the acute relevance of work in each field to that in the other. The gains of establishing cross-disciplinary ground in developmental plasticity research will only be realized when human health researchers and evolutionary biologists go beyond the simple recognition that they share overlapping interests, and begin to critically evaluate their own work in the light of research in the other tradition.
The emergence of cross-disciplinary journals such as Evolution, Medicine, and Public Health represents a critical step in the right direction. However, researchers must also seek out papers and conversations with colleagues from unfamiliar disciplines. For instance, evolutionary researchers studying wild animals must look to the vast human literature on early life effects to inform their work, as several groups have already done in compelling ways [6, 15, 27, 149, 154, 155]. The power of merging evolutionary frameworks with questions and literature related to human health is also well demonstrated by research focused on longitudinal studies of human populations, where groundbreaking work has begun on evolutionary explanations for developmental plasticity and the developmental origins of disease [14, 28, 29, 156, 157]. At the same time, human health researchers must appreciate the insight provided by work in non-model animal systems, exemplified by several recent studies [158, 159]. More ambitiously, by collaborating on theoretical and empirical work (as in [9, 160–162]), human health and evolutionary researchers can increasingly leverage their disciplinary expertise to push forward the boundaries of our knowledge.
Acknowledgements
We thank members of the Tung, Archie, and Alberts labs, and the Amboseli Baboon Research Project for helpful discussions and insight related to this work.
FUNDING
This work was supported by NIH P01-AG031719 and NSF IOS-1456832 to S.C.A., R21-AG049936 and 1R01GM102562 to J.T., NSF BCS-1455808 to J.T. and A.J.L., a Leakey Foundation Research Grant to A.J.L. and S.C.A., and a Triangle Center for Evolutionary Medicine Fellowship to A.J.L. We declare no conflicts of interest.
Conflict of interest: None declared.
REFERENCES
- 1. Jones P. Schizophrenia after prenatal exposure to the Dutch hunger winter of 1944-1945. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994;51:333–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Roseboom T, de Rooij S, Painter R.. The Dutch famine and its long-term consequences for adult health. Early Hum Dev 2006;82:485–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Barboza Solís C, Kelly-Irving M, Fantin R. et al. Adverse childhood experiences and physiological wear-and-tear in midlife: findings from the 1958 British birth cohort. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2015;112:E738–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Felitti V, Anda R, Nordenberg D. et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med 1998;14:245–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Nussey DH, Kruuk LEB, Morris A. et al. Environmental conditions in early life influence ageing rates in a wild population of red deer. Curr Biol 2007;17:R1000–1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Mumby HS, Mar KU, Hayward AD. et al. Elephants born in the high stress season have faster reproductive ageing. Sci Rep 2015;5:1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Haywood S, Perrins CM.. Is clutch size in birds affected by environmental conditions during growth? R Soc Publ 1992;249:195–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Lindström J. Early development and fitness in birds and mammals. Trends Ecol Evol 1999;14:343–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Bateson P, Barker D, Clutton-Brock T. et al. Developmental plasticity and human health. Nature 2004;430:419–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Kuzawa CW, Quinn E. a.. Developmental origins of adult function and health: evolutionary hypotheses. Annu Rev Anthropol 2009;38:131–47. [Google Scholar]
- 11. Hostinar CE, Gunnar MR.. The developmental effects of early life stress: an overview of current theoretical frameworks. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22:400–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. French JA, Carp SB.. Early-life social adversity and developmental processes in nonhuman primates. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2016;7:40–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Gluckman PPD, Hanson M, Cooper C. et al. Effect of in utero and early-life conditions on adult health and disease. N Engl J Med 2008;359:61–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. McDade TW. Early environments and the ecology of inflammation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012;109:17281–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Monaghan P. Early growth conditions, phenotypic development and environmental change. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2008;363:1635–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Uller T. Developmental plasticity and the evolution of parental effects. Trends Ecol Evol 2008;23:432–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Beldade P, Mateus AR, Keller R.. Evolution and molecular mechanisms of adaptive developmental plasticity. Mol Ecol 2011;20:1347–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Nettle D, Bateson M.. Adaptive developmental plasticity: what is it, how can we recognize it and when can it evolve? Proc R Soc B 2015;282:20151005.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. West-Eberhard 2003. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. New York: Oxford University Press,. [Google Scholar]
- 20. Dobzhansky T. Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. Am Biol Teach 1973;35:125–9. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Grafen A.. On the uses of data on lifetime reproductive success In: Clutton-Brock TH. (ed.). Reproductive Success. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1988, 454–71 [Google Scholar]
- 22. Ellison PT. Evolutionary tradeoffs. Evol. Med Public Health 2014;2014:93.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Del Giudice M, Ellis BJ, Shirtcliff E. a.. The adaptive calibration model of stress responsivity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011;35:1562–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Gluckman PD, Hanson MAT.. Living with the past: evolution, development, and patterns of disease. Science 2004;305:1733–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Gluckman PD, Hanson MT, Spencer HG.. Predictive adaptive responses and human evolution. Trends Ecol Evol 2005;20:527–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Wells JCK. A critical appraisal of the predictive adaptive response hypothesis. Int J Epidemiol 2012;41:229–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Douhard M, Plard F, Gaillard J-M. et al. Fitness consequences of environmental conditions at different life stages in a long-lived vertebrate. Proc R Soc London, Ser B 2014;281:20140276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Hayward AD, Rickard IJ, Lummaa V.. Influence of early-life nutrition on mortality and reproductive success during a subsequent famine in a preindustrial population. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013;110:13886–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Hayward AD, Lummaa V.. Testing the evolutionary basis of the predictive adaptive response hypothesis in a preindustrial human population. Evol Med Public Health 2013;2013:106–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Lea AJ, Altmann J, Alberts SC. et al. Developmental constraints in a wild primate. Am Nat 2015;185:809–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Pigeon G, Festa-Bianchet M, Pelletier F.. Long-term fitness consequences of early environment in a long-lived ungulate. Proc R Soc B 2017;284:20170222.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Del Giudice M. Early stress and human behavioral development: emerging evolutionary perspectives. J Dev Orig Health Dis 2014;5:270–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Botero C, Weissing FJ, Wright J. et al. Evolutionary tipping points in the capacity to adapt to environmental change. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2015;112:184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Nettle D, Frankenhuis W, Rickard I.. The evolution of predictive adaptive responses in human life history. Proc R Soc London, Ser B 2013;280:13–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Dey S, Proulx SR, Teotónio H.. Adaptation to temporally fluctuating environments by the evolution of maternal effects. PLoS Biol 2016;14:e1002388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Graham JK, Smith ML, Simons AM.. Experimental evolution of bet hedging under manipulated environmental uncertainty in Neurospora crassa. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2014;281:20140706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Garland T, Kelly S.. Phenotypic plasticity and experimental evolution. J Exp Biol 2006;209:2344–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Le Vinh Thuy J, VandenBrooks JM, Angilletta MJ.. Developmental plasticity evolved according to specialist–generalist trade-offs in experimental populations of Drosophila melanogaster. Biol Lett 2016;12:20160379.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Uller T, Nakagawa S, English S.. Weak evidence for anticipatory parental effects in plants and animals. J Evol Biol 2013;26:2161–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Dantzer B, Newman A, Boonstra R. et al. Density triggers maternal hormones that increase adaptive offspring growth in a wild mammal. Science 2013;340:1215–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Mariette MM, Buchanan KL.. Prenatal acoustic communication programs offspring for high posthatching temperatures in a songbird. Science 2016;353:812–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Schulz LC. The Dutch Hunger Winter and the developmental origins of health and disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010;107:16757–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Stanner S. a, Yudkin JS.. Fetal programming and the Leningrad Siege study. Twin Res 2001;4:287–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Li Y, He Y, Qi L. et al. Exposure to the Chinese famine in early life and the risk of hyperglycemia and type 2 diabetes in adulthood. Diabetes 2010;59:2400–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Nettle D, Bateson M.. Childhood and adult socioeconomic position interact to predict health in mid life in a cohort of British women. PeerJ 2017;5:e3528.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Adair LS, Popkin BM, Akin JS. et al. Cohort profile: The cebu longitudinal health and nutrition survey. Int J Epidemiol 2011;40:619–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Klein JD. The National Longitudinal Study on Adolescent Health: Preliminary results: Great expectations. J Am Med Assoc 1997;278:864–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Fuligni A, McCabe L, McLanahan S. et al. Four New National Longitudinal Surveys on Children In: Brooks-Gunn, Fuligni, Berlin (eds.). Early Childhood Development in the 21st Century. New York, NY: TC Press, 2003, 326–59. [Google Scholar]
- 49. Clutton-Brock T, Sheldon BC.. Individuals and populations: the role of long-term, individual-based studies of animals in ecology and evolutionary biology. Trends Ecol Evol 2010;25:562–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Frankenhuis WE, Panchanathan K, Belsky J.. A mathematical model of the evolution of individual differences in developmental plasticity arising through parental bet-hedging. Dev Sci 2016;19:251–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Rickard IJ, Lummaa V.. The predictive adaptive response and metabolic syndrome: challenges for the hypothesis. Trends Endocrinol Metab 2007;18:94–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Ghalambor CK, Hoke KL, Ruell EW. et al. Non-adaptive plasticity potentiates rapid adaptive evolution of gene expression in nature. Nature 2015;525:372–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Ghalambor CK, McKay JK, Carroll SP. et al. Adaptive versus non-adaptive phenotypic plasticity and the potential for contemporary adaptation in new environments. Funct Ecol 2007;21:394–407. [Google Scholar]
- 54. Hanson MA, Gluckman PD.. Early developmental conditioning of later health and disease: physiology or pathophysiology? Physiol Rev 2014;94:1027–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Huang Y, Agrawal AF.. Experimental evolution of gene expression and plasticity in alternative selective regimes. PLOS Genet 2016;12:e1006336.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Beaumont HJ, Gallie J, Kost C. et al. Experimental evolution of bet hedging. Nature 2009;462:90–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Yampolsky LY, Glazko GV, Fry JD.. Evolution of gene expression and expression plasticity in long-term experimental populations of Drosophila melanogaster maintained under constant and variable ethanol stress. Mol Ecol 2012;21:4287–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Hibshman JD, Hung A, Baugh LR, Ashrafi K.. Maternal diet and insulin-like signaling control intergenerational plasticity of progeny size and starvation resistance. PLOS Genet 2016;12:e1006396.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Proulx SR, Teotónio H.. What kind of maternal effects can be selected for in fluctuating environments? Am Nat 2017;189:E000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Sih A, Ferrari MCO, Harris DJ.. Evolution and behavioural responses to human-induced rapid environmental change. Evol Appl 2011;4:367–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Sih A, Bolnick DI, Luttbeg B. et al. Predator-prey naïveté, antipredator behavior, and the ecology of predator invasions. Oikos 2010;119:610–21. [Google Scholar]
- 62. Chevin L-M, Hoffmann AA.. Evolution of phenotypic plasticity in extreme environments. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 2016;372:20160138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Nussey D, Postma E, Gienapp P. et al. Selection on heritable phenotypic plasticity in a wild bird population. Science 2005;310:304–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Charmantier A, Charmantier A, Mccleery RH. et al. Adaptive phenotypic plasticity in response to climate change in a. 2008;320:800–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Brakefield PM, Gates J, Keys D. et al. Development, plasticity and evolution of butterfly eyespot patterns. Nature 1996;384:236–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Beldade P, Brakefield PM.. The genetics and evo-devo of butterfly wing patterns. Nature 2002;3:442–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Bento G, Ogawa A, Sommer RJ.. Co-option of the hormone-signalling module dafachronic acid-DAF-12 in nematode evolution. Nature 2010;466:494–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Suzuki Y, Nijhout HF.. Evolution of a polyphenism by genetic accommodation. Science 2006;311:650–2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Simpson SJ, Sword GA, Lo N.. Polyphenism in insects. Curr Biol 2011;21:R738–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Scheiner SM. Genetics and evolution of phenotypic plasticity. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 1993;24:35–68. [Google Scholar]
- 71. Pigliucci M. Evolution of phenotypic plasticity: where are we going now? Trends Ecol Evol 2005;20:481–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Heim C, Binder EB.. Review of human studies on sensitive periods, gene-environment interactions, and epigenetics. Exp Neurol 2012;233:102–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Caspi A, Moffitt T.. Gene – environment interactions in psychiatry: joining forces with neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 2006;7:583–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE. et al. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 2003;301:386–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Risch N, Herrell R, Lehner T. et al. Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events, and risk of depression. J Am Med Assoc 2009;301:2462–72., [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Ioannidis JPA, Tarone R, McLaughlin JK.. The false-positive to false-negative ratio in epidemiologic studies. Epidemiology 2011;22:450–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Brisson J. a. Aphid wing dimorphisms: linking environmental and genetic control of trait variation. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2010;365:605–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Reid NM, Proestou DA, Clark BW. et al. The genomic landscape of rapid repeated evolutionary adaptation to toxic pollution in wild fish. Science 2016;354:1305 LP–1308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Lamichhaney S, Han F, Berglund J. et al. A beak size locus in Darwin’s finches facilitated character displacement during a drought. Science 2016;352:470–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Çalışkan M, Baker SW, Gilad Y. et al. Host genetic variation influences gene expression response to rhinovirus infection. PLoS Genet 2015;11:e1005111.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Nédélec Y, Sanz J, Baharian G. et al. Genetic ancestry and natural selection drive population differences in immune responses to pathogens. Cell 2016;167:113.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Lee MN, Ye C, Villani A-C. et al. Common genetic variants modulate pathogen-sensing responses in human dendritic cells. Science 2014;343:1246980.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Pfennig DW. Proximate and functional causes of polyphenism in an anuran tadpole. Funct Ecol 1992;6:167–74. [Google Scholar]
- 84. Pfennig D. The adaptive significance of an environmentally-cued developmental switch in an anuran tadpole. Oecologia 1990;85:101–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Tung J, Zhou X, Alberts SC. et al. The genetic architecture of gene expression levels in wild baboons. eLife 2015;4:1–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. Miller GE, Chen E, Fok AK. et al. Low early-life social class leaves a biological residue manifested by decreased glucocorticoid and increased proinflammatory signaling. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009;106:14716–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87. Cole SW, Conti G, Arevalo JMG. et al. Transcriptional modulation of the developing immune system by early life social adversity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012;109:20578–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Laforsch C, Beccara L, Tollrian R.. Inducible defenses: the relevance of chemical alarm cues in Daphnia. Limnol Oceanogr 2006;51:1466–72. [Google Scholar]
- 89. Boersma M, Spaak P, De Meester L.. Predator-mediated plasticity in morphology, life history, and behavior of daphnia: the uncoupling of responses. Am Nat 1998;152:237–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. Grundberg E, Small KS, Hedman ÅK. et al. Mapping cis- and trans-regulatory effects across multiple tissues in twins. Nat Genet 2012;44:1084–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Knowles D, Davis JR, Raj A. et al. Allele-specific expression reveals interactions between genetic variation and environment. Nat Methods 2017;14:699–702. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Runcie DE, Wiedmann RT, Archie EA. et al. Social environment influences the relationship between genotype and gene expression in wild baboons. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 2013;368:1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Yan H, Yuan W, Velculescu VE. et al. Allelic variation in human gene expression. Science 2002;297:21218.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Pirinen M, Lappalainen T, Zaitlen N. a. et al. Assessing allele specific expression across multiple tissues from RNA-seq read data. Bioinformatics 2014;7211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Pastinen T. Genome-wide allele-specific analysis: insights into regulatory variation. Nat Rev Genet 2010;11:533–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Ekblom R, Galindo J.. Applications of next generation sequencing in molecular ecology of non-model organisms. Heredity (Edinb) 2011;107:1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. Todd EV, Black MA, Gemmell NJ.. The power and promise of RNA-seq in ecology and evolution. Mol Ecol 2016;25:1224–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98. Charruau P, Johnston R, Stahler DR. et al. Pervasive effects of aging on gene expression in wild wolves. Mol Biol Evol 2016;1–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99. Fairfax BP, Humburg P, Makino S. et al. Innate immune activity conditions the effect of regulatory variants upon monocyte gene expression. Science 2014;343:1246949.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100. Rockman MV. The QTN program and the alleles that matter for evolution: all that’s gold does not glitter. Evolution 2012;66:1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Boyle EA, Li YI, Pritchard JK.. Leading edge perspective an expanded view of complex traits: from polygenic to omnigenic. Cell 2017;169:1177–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102. Weaver ICG, Cervoni N, Champagne F. a. et al. Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat Neurosci 2004;7:847–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103. Murgatroyd C, Patchev AV, Wu Y. et al. Dynamic DNA methylation programs persistent adverse effects of early-life stress. Nat Neurosci 2009;12:1559–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Dominguez-Salas P, Moore SE, Baker MS. et al. Maternal nutrition at conception modulates DNA methylation of human metastable epialleles. Nat Commun 2014;5:3746.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105. Tobi EW, Goeman JJ, Monajemi R. et al. DNA methylation signatures link prenatal famine exposure to growth and metabolism. Nat Commun 2014;5:1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106. Simola DF, Graham RJ, Brady CM. et al. Epigenetic (re)programming of caste-specific behavior in the ant Camponotus floridanus. Science 2016;351:aac6633.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107. Xie L, Korkmaz KS, Braun K. et al. Early life stress-induced histone acetylations correlate with activation of the synaptic plasticity genes Arc and Egr1 in the mouse hippocampus. J Neurochem 2013;125:457–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108. Gapp K, Jawaid A, Sarkies P. et al. Implication of sperm RNAs in transgenerational inheritance of the effects of early trauma in mice. Nat Neurosci 2014;17:667–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109. Wolff GL, Kodell RL, Moore SR. et al. Maternal epigenetics and methyl supplements affect agouti gene expression in Avy/a mice. Am Soc Exp Biol 1998;12:949–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110. Klebig ML, Wilkinson JE, Geisler JG. et al. Ectopic expression of the agouti gene in transgenic mice causes obesity, features of type II diabetes, and yellow fur. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1995;92:4728–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111. Zhang T-Y, Meaney MJ.. Epigenetics and the environmental regulation of the genome and its function. Annu Rev Psychol 2010;61:439–66. C1-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112. Kappeler L, Meaney MJ.. Epigenetics and parental effects. Bioessays 2010;32:818–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113. Verhoeven KJF, VonHoldt BM, Sork VL.. Epigenetics in ecology and evolution: what we know and what we need to know. Mol Ecol 2016;25:1631–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114. Jirtle RL, Skinner MK.. Environmental epigenomics and disease susceptibility. Nat Rev Genet 2007;8:253–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115. Naumova OY, Lee M, Koposov R. et al. Differential patterns of whole-genome DNA methylation in institutionalized children and children raised by their biological parents. Dev Psychopathol 2012;24:143–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116. Borghol N, Suderman M, McArdle W. et al. Associations with early-life socio-economic position in adult DNA methylation. Int J Epidemiol 2012;41:62–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117. McGowan PO, Sasaki A, D'Alessio AC. et al. Epigenetic regulation of the glucocorticoid receptor in human brain associates with childhood abuse. Nat Neurosci 2009;12:342–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118. Pacis A, Tailleux L, Lambourne J. et al. Bacterial infection remodels the DNA methylation landscape of human dendritic cells. Genome Res 2015;25:1801–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119. Rönn T, Volkov P, Davegårdh C. et al. A six months exercise intervention influences the genome-wide DNA methylation pattern in human adipose tissue. PLoS Genet 2013;9:e1003572.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120. Tung J, Barreiro LB, Johnson ZP. et al. Social environment is associated with gene regulatory variation in the rhesus macaque immune system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012;109:6490–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121. Feng X, Wang L, Yang S. et al. Maternal separation produces lasting changes in cortisol and behavior in rhesus monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2011;108:14312–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122. van der Doelen RH. a, Deschamps W, D’Annibale C. et al. Early life adversity and serotonin transporter gene variation interact at the level of the adrenal gland to affect the adult hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. Transl Psychiatry 2014;4:e409.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123. Conti G, Hansman C, Heckman JJ. et al. Primate evidence on the late health effects of early-life adversity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012;109:8866–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124. Franklin TB, Russig H, Weiss IC. et al. Epigenetic transmission of the impact of early stress across generations. Biol Psychiatry 2010;68:408–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125. Heijmans BT, Tobi EW, Stein AD. et al. Persistent epigenetic differences associated with prenatal exposure to famine in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2008;105:17046–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126. Klengel T, Mehta D, Anacker C. et al. Allele-specific FKBP5 DNA demethylation mediates gene-childhood trauma interactions. Nat Neurosci 2013;16:33–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127. Gibson G. 2009. It Takes a Genome: How a Clash between Our Genes and Modern Life Is Making Us Sick. Upper Saddle River, NJ: FT Press Science,. [Google Scholar]
- 128. Gibson G. Decanalization and the origin of complex disease. Nat Rev Genet 2009;10:134–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129. Careau V, Buttemer WA, Buchanan KL.. Early-developmental stress, repeatability, and canalization in a suite of physiological and behavioral traits in female zebra finches. Integr Comp Biol 2014;54:539–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130. Champagne FA. Epigenetics and developmental plasticity across species. Dev Psychobiol 2013;55:33–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131. Lutz PE, Turecki G.. DNA methylation and childhood maltreatment: from animal models to human studies. Neuroscience 2014;264:142–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132. Lea AJ, Altmann J, Alberts SC. et al. Resource base influences genome-wide DNA methylation levels in wild baboons (Papio cynocephalus). Mol Ecol 2016;25:1681–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133. Janowitz Koch I, Clark MM, Thompson MJ. et al. The concerted impact of domestication and transposon insertions on methylation patterns between dogs and grey wolves. Mol Ecol 2016;25:1838–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134. Adey A, Shendure J.. Ultra-low-input, tagmentation-based whole-genome bisulfite sequencing. Genome Res 2012;22:1139–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135. Picelli S, Björklund AK, Reinius B. et al. Tn5 transposase and tagmentation procedures for massively scaled sequencing projects. Genome Res 2014;24:2033–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136. Buenrostro J, Giresi PG, Zaba L. et al. Transposition of native chromatin for multimodal regulatory analysis and personal epigenomics Jason. Nat Methods 2013;10:1213–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137. Hertzman C. Putting the concept of biological embedding in historical perspective. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012;109:17160–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138. Lam LL, Emberly E, Fraser HB. et al. Factors underlying variable DNA methylation in a human community cohort. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012;109:17253–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139. McEwen BS. Brain on stress: How the social environment gets under the skin. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012;109:17180–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140. Barrès R, Yan J, Egan B. et al. Acute exercise remodels promoter methylation in human skeletal muscle. Cell Metab 2012;15:405–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141. Vickers MH, Gluckman PD, Coveny AH. et al. Neonatal leptin treatment reverses developmental programming. Endocrinology 2005;146:4211–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142. Ng JW, Barrett LM, Wong A. et al. The role of longitudinal cohort studies in epigenetic epidemiology: challenges and opportunities. Genome Biol 2012;13:246.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143. Provençal N, Suderman MJ, Guillemin C. et al. The signature of maternal rearing in the methylome in rhesus macaque prefrontal cortex and T cells. J Neurosci 2012;32:15626–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144. Elliott E, Ezra-Nevo G, Regev L. et al. Resilience to social stress coincides with functional DNA methylation of the CRF gene in adult mice. Nat Neurosci 2010;13:1351–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145. Peña CJ, Kronman HG, Walker DM. et al. Early life stress confers lifelong stress susceptibility in mice via ventral tegmental area OTX2. Science 2017;356:1185–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146. Nettle D, Monaghan P, Gillespie R. et al. An experimental demonstration that early-life competitive disadvantage accelerates telomere loss. Proc Biol Sci 2014;282:20141610.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147. Nettle D, Andrews C, Reichert S. et al. Early-life adversity accelerates cellular ageing and affects adult inflammation: experimental evidence from the European starling. Sci Rep 2017;7:40794.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148. Briga M, Koetsier E, Boonekamp JJ. et al. Food availability affects adult survival trajectories depending on early developmental conditions. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2017;284:20162287.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149. Monaghan P, Heidinger BJ, D'Alba L. et al. For better or worse: reduced adult lifespan following early-life stress is transmitted to breeding partners. Proc Biol Sci 2012;279:709–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150. Nowicki S, Searcy WA, Peters S.. Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: A review and experimental test of the “nutritional stress hypothesis.” J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sensory, Neural, Behav Physiol 2002;188:1003–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151. Philippi T, Seger J.. Hedging one’s evolutionary bets, revisited. Trends Ecol Evol 1989;4:41–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152. Slatkin M. Hedging one’s evolutionary bets. Nature 1974;250:704–5. [Google Scholar]
- 153. Stamps JA, Frankenhuis WE.. Bayesian models of development. Trends Ecol Evol 2016;1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154. Criscuolo F, Monaghan P, Nasir L. et al. Early nutrition and phenotypic development: “catch-up” growth leads to elevated metabolic rate in adulthood. Proc Biol Sci 2008;275:1565–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155. Lummaa V, Clutton-Brock T.. Early development, survival and reproduction in humans. Trends Ecol Evol 2002;17:141–7. [Google Scholar]
- 156. McDade TW, Hoke M, Borja JB. et al. Do environments in infancy moderate the association between stress and inflammation in adulthood? Initial evidence from a birth cohort in the Philippines. Brain Behav Immun 2013;31:23–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157. Kuzawa C. The developmental origins of adult health: intergenerational inertia in adaptation and disease. Evol Health 2008;325–49. [Google Scholar]
- 158. Smith GD. Epidemiology, epigenetics and the “Gloomy Prospect”: embracing randomness in population health research and practice. Int J Epidemiol 2011;40:537–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159. Feinberg AP, Irizarry R. a.. Evolution in health and medicine Sackler colloquium: stochastic epigenetic variation as a driving force of development, evolutionary adaptation, and disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010;107:1757–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160. Herb BR, Wolschin F, Hansen KD. et al. Reversible switching between epigenetic states in honeybee behavioral subcastes. Nat Neurosci 2012;15:1371–3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161. Rubenstein DR, Skolnik H, Berrio A. et al. Sex-specific fitness effects of unpredictable early life conditions are associated with DNA methylation in the avian glucocorticoid receptor. Mol Ecol 2016;25:1714–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162. Gluckman PD, Hanson MA, Bateson P. et al. Towards a new developmental synthesis: adaptive developmental plasticity and human disease. Lancet 2009;373:1654–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163. Hales C, Barker D.. Type 2 (non-insulin-dependent) diabetes mellitus: the thrifty phenotype hypothesis. Diabetologia 1992;35:595–601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164. Hales CN, Barker DJ.. The thrifty phenotype hypothesis. Br Med Bull 2001;60:5–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165. McEwen BS, Stellar E.. Stress and the individual: mechanisms leading to disease. Arch Intern Med 1993;153:2093–101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166. Bateson P. Fetal experience and good adult design. Int J Epidemiol 2001;30:928–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167. Berghänel A, Heistermann M, Schülke O. et al. Prenatal stress effects in a wild, long-lived primate: predictive adaptive responses in an unpredictable environment. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2016;283:20161304.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168. Douhard M, Loe LE, Stien A. et al. The influence of weather conditions during gestation on life histories in a wild Arctic ungulate. Proc R Soc London B Biol Sci 2016;283:20161760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169. Chen E, Miller GE, Kobor MS. et al. Maternal warmth buffers the effects of low early-life socioeconomic status on pro-inflammatory signaling in adulthood. Mol Psychiatry 2011;16:729–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170. Maranville JC, Luca F, Richards AL. et al. Interactions between glucocorticoid treatment and cis-regulatory polymorphisms contribute to cellular response phenotypes. PLoS Genet 2011;7:e1002162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]