Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Assess. 2017 Aug 7;30(5):678–692. doi: 10.1037/pas0000511

Smoking Consequences Questionnaire: A Reevaluation of the Psychometric Properties Across Two Independent Samples of Smokers

Lorra Garey 1, Kara Manning 1, Charles Jardin 1, Adam M Leventhal 2, Matthew Stone 2, Amanda M Raines 3, Raina D Pang 2, Clayton Neighbors 1, Norman B Schmidt 3, Michael J Zvolensky 1,4
PMCID: PMC5803479  NIHMSID: NIHMS886500  PMID: 28782978

Abstract

Drug use outcome expectancies are a central construct to psychosocial theories of addictive disorders. In tobacco literature, the Smoking Consequences Questionnaire (SCQ; Brandon & Baker, 1991) is a tool used to assess this construct. Despite its common use, the SCQ has received little psychometric evaluation. In the current report, samples from two studies were employed to examine the assumed SCQ structure, develop a novel truncated scale, and evaluate the psychometric properties of the novel scale. In Study 1, the four-factor SCQ structure was examined using data from 343 (32.4% female; Mage = 43.7; SD = 10.8) adult non-treatment-seeking smokers. Results from Study 1 indicated that the four-factor SCQ structure did not adequately explain covariance between items. Instead, results provided evidence for a five-factor structure that tapped into outcome expectancies related to (1) immediate negative consequences, (2) long-term negative consequences, (3) sensory satisfaction, (4) negative affect reduction, and (5) appetite-weight control. In Study 2, the five-factor structure of the SCQ was confirmed and the construct validity was evaluated in 582 (48.2% female; Mage = 36.9; SD = 13.5) treatment-seeking adult smokers. Study 2 found evidence for measurement invariance across sex and overtime of the five-factor structure as well as substantial construct validity. Results from two independent samples challenge the traditional four-factor model of the SCQ, and instead, provide evidence for a novel five-factor SCQ structure with strong validity and reliability. Alternate scoring algorithms for the SCQ, including a five-subscale scheme, warrant consideration to ensure optimal measurement precision and construct differentiation.

Keywords: Psychometric Properties, Smoking Consequences Questionnaire, Measurement Invariance, Smokers


In the past two decades, there has been a growing interest to better understand cognitive processes that influence smoking behavior (King, Marcus, Pinto, Emmons, & Abrams, 1996; Schwarzer, 2008). Smoking outcome expectancies are beliefs about the positive and negative consequences or effects of smoking (Gwaltney, Shiffman, Balabanis, & Paty, 2005) and have been among the most promising cognitive processes identified thus far (Brandon, Juliano, & Copeland, 1999). Several influential theories and empirical studies underscore the importance of outcome expectancies as a key proximal mediator of smoking motivation (Brandon et al., 1999; Goldman, Brown, & Christiansen, 1987; Marlatt & Donovan, 2005; Niaura et al., 1988).

One measure used to assess smoking outcome expectancies is the Smoking Consequences Questionnaire (SCQ; Brandon & Baker, 1991). The original SCQ contains 50 self-report items assessing subjective positive and negative expectancies of smoking (Brandon & Baker, 1991); these items were retained from a pool of 80 items. Measure development work identified four SCQ factors: Negative Consequences (18 items that describe negative consequences of smoking), Negative Reinforcement/Negative Affect Reduction (12 items that assess expectancies that smoking will reduce negative affect), Positive Reinforcement/Sensory Satisfaction (15 items that evaluate expectancies for smoking to provide sensory satisfaction and promote positive consequences, such as alleviate boredom), and Appetite–Weight Control (5 items that assess beliefs for smoking to suppress one’s appetite). Brandon and Baker (1991) tested the psychometric properties of the SCQ across multiple score types. Specifically, participants reported the desirability and likelihood of each item; these score were used to derive (a) subjective expectancy unit score desirability, which were calculated by multiplying the desirability and likelihood ratings, and (b) likelihood ratings alone. Findings suggested that likelihood scores were better at discriminating smoking status and identifying sex differences than the subjective expectancy unit score (Brandon & Baker, 1991); this model was subsequently cross validated (Copeland, Brandon, & Quinn, 1995). Based on this data, with rare exception (Brandon, Wetter, & Baker, 1996), subsequent work has assessed SCQ items using a single Likert scale of likelihood.

The original four SCQ factors have a high degree of clinical utility, being related to smoking initiation (Doran et al., 2013), tobacco dependence (Myers, MacPherson, McCarthy, & Brown, 2003), tobacco withdrawal (Langdon & Leventhal, 2014; Wetter et al., 1994), and cessation outcome (Kenford et al., 2002; Wetter et al., 1999; Wetter et al., 1994). The SCQ factors also are associated with affective vulnerabilities that undergird problematic use and impede cessation, including anxiety sensitivity (Johnson et al., 2008; Zvolensky et al., 2004), negative affectivity (Johnson et al., 2008; Wetter et al., 1994), and smoking-specific experiential avoidance (Farris, Zvolensky, DiBello, & Schmidt, 2015). Although the original SCQ factors were extracted with an orthogonal rotation because they were believed to tap into unrelated dimensions of smoking expectancies (Brandon & Baker, 1991), extensive scientific data provide evidence that they are unique, although related, factors that assess different dimensions of smoking expectancies (Gonzalez, Hogan, McLeish, & Zvolensky, 2010; Gregor, Zvolensky, McLeish, Bernstein, & Morissette, 2008).

Despite the SCQ’s wide use and clinical utility, surprisingly little work has evaluated the psychometric properties of the original SCQ. Indeed, only three studies have evaluated the psychometric properties of the proposed four-factor SCQ structure (Brandon & Baker, 1991; Vidrine et al., 2009; Wetter et al., 1994)1 and one study has attempted to shorten the SCQ using a sample of adolescents and young adults (Myers et al., 2003). This work, although promising, has been limited in several key ways. First, confirmatory work on the SCQ has utilized less-than-ideal analytic approaches. Wetter et al. (1994) examined the SCQ structure using a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). These researchers employed parcels when evaluating the assumed structure of the SCQ, which is potentially problematic because it carries the assumption that items within parcels are unidimensional. To the extent that item parcels are not unidimensional, their use is likely to result in inflated model fit (Kline, 2015). Consequently, beyond this single study, no work has successfully replicated the four-factor SCQ structure. For example, Vidrine et al. (2009) attempted to replicate the SCQ structure using item-level data among a sample of Latino smokers, but found poor model fit. The model misfit may have resulted from poor item loadings (Matsunaga, 2015). Unfortunately, Vidrine et al. (2009) did not provide item-level data in their report, making this assertion speculative and an important area for research, particularly because misspecification at the item level may lead to poor models comprised of less precise constructs bombarded with error (Matsunaga, 2015).

Second, extant work has found sex differences across the SCQ factors (Brandon & Baker, 1991; Wetter et al., 1999; Wetter et al., 1994). Specifically, Brandon and Baker (1991) reported sex differences on Negative Reinforcement/Negative Affect Reduction and Appetite-Weight Control subscales, whereas Wetter et al. (1994) reported sex differences on these two subscales as well as Negative Consequences subscale. The latter finding was replicated in another sample of treatment seeking smokers (Wetter et al., 1999). Yet, measurement invariance, or stability of the measure, across sex has not been empirically evaluated with the SCQ. Measurement invariance (Vandenberg & Lance, 2000) assesses if indicators load onto underlying factors the same way across groups and provides empirical evidence that the fundamental meaning of a construct is not confounded by differences in perceptions of the construct (Little, Preacher, Selig, & Card, 2007; Vandenberg & Lance, 2000). Without establishing measurement invariance, bias may be introduced (McHorney & Fleishman, 2006) and differences cannot be unambiguously interpreted (Horn & McArdle, 1992). Thus, testing and establishing measurement invariance is necessary for future researchers to accurately conclude that observed differences in the SCQ across sex are the result of true changes in the construct.

In addition to testing measurement invariance across sex, it is clinically useful to evaluate measurement invariance across time with the SCQ. Indeed, recent work suggests that smoking outcome expectancies fluctuate in response to several internal and external factors (Doran, Schweizer, & Myers, 2011). Theoretically, smoking outcome expectancies may change in response to treatment. For example, expectancies for the health risks of smoking may increase during the study as participants learned new information about the negative effects of smoking on health (Vidrine et al., 2009). Yet, little work has focused on examining change in the SCQ over time. One potential reason for this lack of research may be that the comparability of the SCQ measured at two time points has not been established. Thus, an important ‘next-step’ is to evaluate measurement invariance across time with the SCQ. Providing evidence for the stability of the structure over time may encourage researchers to employ this measure more often when evaluating change in smoking expectancies.

Third, Brandon and Baker (1991) demonstrated validity for the measure to distinguish between groups of smokers and non-smokers. However, little to no attention has been given to examining the convergent/discriminant validity of the SCQ factors with constructs theorized to maintain smoking or interfere with quitting. As reviewed, other work provides evidence for correlates of the SCQ factors (Doran et al., 2013; Langdon & Leventhal, 2014; Myers et al., 2003; Wetter et al., 1994). Nevertheless, these relations have not been evaluated in the context of psychometric testing of the SCQ. Rigorous scientific work to validate the relations between the SCQ factors and other constructs related to smoking is central to (a) demonstrate that SCQ factors measure the psychological attributes they were developed to measure; and (b) to understand the meaningfulness of the measure in clinical work (Adcock, 2001). As work with the SCQ continues to develop, there is a need to demonstrate convergent/discriminant validity between the SCQ factors and affective vulnerabilities or other cognitive processes that may be involved in maintenance and relapse processes.

Finally, to add to the construct validity argument for the original SCQ structure, Wetter and colleagues (1994) examined the predictive validity of the SCQ subscales on withdrawal symptoms among participants who had successfully quit. All subscales except Negative Consequences predicted withdrawal. One limitation of this work, however, was that abstinence was defined as an expired carbon monoxide rating of less than 10 parts per million (PPM). Recent work suggests that such a high cutoff may not adequately differentiate abstainers from non-abstainers. Indeed, current guidelines recommend defining abstinence as less than 5 PPM (Perkins, Karelitz, & Jao, 2013). Thus, Wetter and colleagues (1994) may have erroneously classified smokers as abstinent. To respond to this limitation, it would be useful to investigate the predictive validity of SCQ factors on withdrawal among a sample of successful quitters defined using contemporary recommendations (i.e., PPM <5; Perkins et al., 2013).

It is important to note that alternative versions of original SCQ measure are currently in circulation. Specifically, the SCQ-Adult was developed from the original 80 item pool which proposed 10-factors across 55 items (see Copeland et al., 1995). Additionally, an adolescent version of the SCQ has been developed from the original 80 items (Adolescent-SCQ; (Adolescent-SCQ; Lewis-Esquerre, Rodrigue, & Kahler, 2005) as well as a shortened version of the original SCQ 50-item measure with adolescents and young adults (S-SCQ; Myers et al., 2003), a shortened version of the SCQ-Adult (Brief SCQ-Adult; Rash & Copeland, 2008), and a Spanish version derived from the SCQ-Adult (SCQ-Spanish; Cepeda-Benito & Reig Ferrer, 2000). Despite the development of alternative versions of the SCQ, the original 50-item measure continues to be the most highly used instrument to examine smoking outcome expectancies (Aguirre et al., 2016; Garey et al., 2016; Goldenson, Pang, & Leventhal, 2016). The continued use of the SCQ may stem from its appropriateness with today’s smoker. Specifically, the alternative adult version, namely, the SCQ-Adult, was developed among a sample of heavy smokers who smoked roughly 27 cigarettes per day, whereas the original SCQ was developed among a more general sample of smokers who smoked 11 cigarettes per day. Because the average cigarettes per day smoked in the U.S. has been declining since 1995 to the 2015 average of 14 cigarettes per day (Jamal, 2016), the original factor structure developed by Brandon and Baker (1991) may be more applicable to today’s smokers. Thus, this issue may partly explain its popularity, and adds further justification to examine the psychometric properties of the original SCQ using refined methodology in a sample of general smoker.

Together, the current study addressed several notable, clinically-relevant gaps related to the psychometric evaluation of the SCQ. Study 1 examined the four-factor SCQ structure using item-level data on a sample of community-recruited non-treatment seeking smokers. Based on model fit indices, the structure of the SCQ was reevaluated and refined in a theoretically and empirically informed exploratory manner. Ultimately, a novel, truncated five-factor structure was supported. To encourage and support the use of this novel measure in clinical and research setting, the five-factor model underwent rigorous psychometric evaluation in Study 2 to assess its construct validity. Indeed, many of the shortcomings outlined above related to the original SCQ measure were addressed using the novel, truncated measure. Specifically, Study 2 used a confirmatory analytic method to evaluate the novel five-factor scale in an independent sample of treatment-seeking smokers. The stability (i.e., measurement invariance) of the five-factor structure was examined across sex and over two time points (baseline and quit-week). In both studies, factors of the five-factor structure were evaluated for convergent and discriminant validity. In Study 2, factors were evaluated for internal consistency and more extensive construct validity, including convergent and discriminant validity across a wider range of variables as well as predictive validity.

METHOD

Study 1

Participants

A sample of 343 (32.4% female; Mage = 43.7; SD = 10.8) adult smokers who responded to study advertisements for a study on personality and smoking were included in the study. Inclusion criteria were (a) age≥18 years old; (b) daily cigarette smoking for≥2 years; (c) currently smoking≥10 cigarettes/day; and (d) fluency in English. Exclusion criteria were (a) current DSM–IV dependence on substances other than nicotine in the past 30 days (to prevent modulation of responses due to withdrawal from other substances); (b) current DSM–IV mood disorder, psychotic symptoms, or use of psychiatric medications (to prevent cognitive or behavioral impairment that might interfere with completing a behavioral smoking task or modulation of tobacco abstinence effects by psychiatric medication); (c) breath carbon monoxide (CO) levels <10 ppm at intake (to exclude individuals who may be over reporting their smoking level); (d) use of non-cigarette tobacco or nicotine products; and (e) currently pregnant. The racial/ethnic distribution of this sample was as follows: 51.9% Black, 34.4% White, 7.0% Hispanic, 1.2% American Indian/Alaskan Native, 0.9% Asian, 0.3% Middle Eastern, 4.1% Multi-racial. On average, participants reported being a regular smoker since age 19.2 years old (SD = 5.5) and currently smoke an average of 16.7 cigarettes per day (SD = 6.9). A moderate level of tobacco dependence was observed within the sample based on the Fagerström Test for Cigarette Dependence (M = 5.3, SD = 1.9; Heatherton, Kozlowski, Frecker, & Fagerström, 1991).

Procedures

The data for the current study was taken from a larger study on personality and smoking (Leventhal, Piper, Japuntich, Baker, & Cook, 2014). Interested persons responding to community-based advertisements contacted the research team via phone. During the initial telephone interaction, research staff provided participants a detailed description of the study and administered a preliminary phone screener. Participants deemed eligible at the preliminary phone screener were scheduled for a baseline appointment. At the baseline appointment, participants provided written informed consent, were screened for study eligibility and completed self-report baseline measures. Eligible participants then attended two experimental visits (one 16-hr smoking abstinent and one nonabstinent) that began at 12 p.m. and were conducted within 2 to 14 days of each other; abstinence condition order was counterbalanced across participants. The current study is based on secondary analyses of baseline data collected from participants who were eligible for the larger trial and who provided data for the SCQ. All study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board.

Measures

Demographics

Demographic information included sex, age, and race. Participants provided smoking history information, including number of cigarettes per day and age when they became a regular smoker.

Carbon Monoxide

Biochemical verification of smoking status was assessed by expired carbon monoxide (CO) analysis of breath samples collected using a CMD/CO Carbon Monoxide Monitor (Model 3110; Spirometrics, Inc.). In the present study, baseline CO was used to demonstrate convergent validity.

Fagerström Test for Cigarette Dependence (FTCD)

The FTCD is a 6-item scale that assesses gradations in tobacco dependence (Fagerström, 2012; Heatherton et al., 1991). Scores range from 0–10, with higher scores reflecting high levels of physiological dependence on cigarettes. The FTCD has adequate internal consistency, positive relations with key smoking variables (e.g., saliva cotinine), and high test-retest reliability (Heatherton et al., 1991; Pomerleau, Carton, Lutzke, Flessland, & Pomerleau, 1994). In the current study, the FTCD total score was used to characterize tobacco dependence across the sample (α = .58).

Anxiety Sensitivity Index (ASI)

The ASI (Reiss & McNally, 1985) is a 16-item self-report measure of the sensitivity to and fear of the potential negative consequences of anxiety-related symptoms and sensations. Respondents are asked to indicate, on a 5-point Likert-type scale (0 = “very little” to 4 = “very much”), the degree to which they are concerned about these possible negative consequences. The ASI has sound psychometric properties and has good test-retest reliability and high levels of internal consistency (Brown, Kahler, Zvolensky, Lejuez, & Ramsey, 2001; Reiss & McNally, 1985). The ASI demonstrated excellent internal consistency in the present study (α = .91).

Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire-Short Form (MASQ)

The MASQ (Clark & Watson, 1991) is a 62-item self-report measure of emotional symptoms based upon the tripartite model of anxiety and depression. Participants rate how much they experienced each symptom in the previous week (1 = not at all; 5 = extremely). The MASQ contains four symptom scales: (a) general distress-anxiety, which assesses anxious/tense mood and other nonspecific anxiety symptoms (11 items; e.g., “felt nervous,” “felt uneasy”); (b) anxious arousal, which assesses somatic tension and arousal (17 items; e.g., “was trembling or shaking,” “felt dizzy or lightheaded”) specific to anxiety; (c) general distress-depression, which assesses depressed/sad mood other non- specific depressive symptoms (12 items; e.g., “felt sad,” “felt like a failure”); and (d) anhedonic depression, which assesses low interest, pleasure, and positive affect (22 items; e.g., “felt like there wasn’t anything interesting or fun to do,” “felt like nothing was very enjoyable”), specific to depression. In the current study, the general distress-anxiety (α = .87), anxious arousal (α = .88), general distress-depression (α = .86) and anhedonic symptoms (α = .90) subscales were used.

Smoking Consequences Questionnaire (SCQ)

The SCQ (Brandon & Baker, 1991) is a 50-item self-report measure that assesses tobacco use outcome expectancies believed to underlie smoking motivation on a Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (not true of me at all) to 7 (very true of me). The measure consists of four key factors: Positive Reinforcement/Sensory Satisfaction (PR; 15 items), Negative Reinforcement/Negative Affect Reduction (NR; 12 items), Negative Consequences (NC; 18 items), and Appetite-Weight Control (AW; 5 items).

Data Analytic Strategy

The four-factor SCQ structure was examined via confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) within a structural equation modeling framework (Brown, 2015). Analyses were conducted using Mplus 7.31(Muthén & Muthén, 2012). The distribution of SCQ items was outside the range of normal (skewness range: |0.05|–|2.45|; kurtosis range: |0.02|–|5.99|); therefore, maximum likelihood robust estimation was employed (Muthén & Muthén, 2012). Against considerable empirical evidence for correlations across factors (Brandon & Baker, 1991; Gonzalez et al., 2010; Gregor et al., 2008; Wetter et al., 1994), factors could correlate. Model fit was examined using the following criteria: root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), with values less than .06 indicating excellent fit, values less than .08 indicating acceptable fit, and values above .10 suggesting poor fit; the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), with values between 0.95 and 1.00 indicating excellent fit and values between .90 and .94 indicating acceptable fit; and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), with values less than .08 indicating acceptable fit (Awang, 2012; Hu & Bentler, 1999). Full information maximum likelihood was employed to handle missing data (Allison, 2012). At baseline, SCQ had the most missing data (4.1%–4.7%); missingness across other baseline variables ranged from 1.2% to 4.4%. Convergent and discriminant validity across the subscales was examined with anxiety sensitivity (ASI), mood and anxiety symptoms (MASQ-SF subscales), and expired CO at baseline. Based on prior research (Guillot, Zvolensky, & Leventhal, 2015; Leyro, Zvolensky, Vujanovic, & Bernstein, 2008; Pang, Khoddam, Guillot, & Leventhal, 2014; Wetter et al., 1994), ASI and mood and anxiety symptoms were hypothesized to positively and significantly relate to positive and negative smoking expectancy outcome subscales, whereas expired CO at baseline was hypothesized to relate only to positive-reinforcing properties associated with smoking outcome expectancies.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Four-Factor Confirmatory Factor Analysis

The factor structure proposed by Brandon and Baker (1991) demonstrated poor model fit (X2[1169] = 4405.25, p < .001; RMSEA = .09 [90% CI: .09, .10]; CFI = .69; SRMR = .14). Factor loadings were evaluated to determine item appropriateness. Inspection of standardized factor loadings indicated that several items loaded poorly on the NC and PR factors (see Table 1). For each of these factors, items that loaded strongly and those that loaded poorly appeared to form unique theoretical factors. To provide evidence for an alternative structure, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) using all 50 items from the SCQ was employed.

Table 1.

Standardized factor loadings from confirmatory factor analysis in Study 1.

Item Loading Item Loading
Factor 1: Negative Consequences Factor 2: Positive Reinforcement Cont.
Smoking is taking years off my life. 0.71 Cigarettes are good for dealing with boredom. 0.34
I will probably die earlier if I continue to smoke. 0.70 I like to watch the smoke from my cigarette. 0.34
Each cigarette I smoke maintains my addiction. 0.67 I feel more at ease with other people if I have a cigarette. 0.32
I become more addicted the more I smoke. 0.67 Smoking temporarily reduces those repeated urges for cigarettes. 0.25
I will become more dependent on nicotine if I continue smoking. 0.65 If I’m feeling irritable, a smoke will help me relax. 0.24
The more I smoke, the more I risk my health. 0.64 If I have nothing to do, a smoke can help kill time. 0.24
Smoking is hazardous to my health. 0.63 Cigarettes give me something to do with my hands. 0.16
By smoking I risk heart disease and lung cancer. 0.62  Factor 3: Negative Reinforcement
Cigarettes control me more and more, the longer I smoke. 0.62 Cigarettes help me deal with anger. 0.88
Smoking makes me seem less attractive. 0.59 Smoking reduces my anger. 0.87
Smoking will make me cough. 0.59 Cigarettes help me deal with anxiety or worry. 0.87
Smoking irritates my mouth and throat. 0.59 Cigarettes help me reduce or handle tension. 0.86
My mouth tastes bad after smoking. 0.57 When I’m upset with someone, a cigarette helps me cope. 0.86
Cigarettes make my lungs hurt. 0.57 If I’m tense, a cigarette helps me to relax. 0.81
I look ridiculous while smoking. 0.55 When I’m angry, a cigarette can calm me down. 0.80
People think less of me if they see me smoking. 0.53 Smoking calms me down when I feel nervous. 0.79
The longer I smoke, the harder it will be to quit. 0.52 Smoking helps me deal with depression. 0.78
My throat burns after smoking. 0.49 If I’m disappointed in myself, a good smoke can help. 0.77
Factor 2: Positive Reinforcement When I am sad, smoking makes me feel better. 0.74
When I smoke, the taste is pleasant. 0.92 Cigarettes help me concentrate. 0.65
I will enjoy the flavor of a cigarette. 0.90 Factor 4: Appetite/Weight Control
Cigarettes taste good. 0.85 Cigarettes keep me from eating more than I should. 0.94
I enjoy the taste sensations while smoking. 0.83 Smoking controls my appetite. 0.93
I enjoy feeling the smoke hit my mouth and the back of my throat. 0.57 Cigarettes keep me from overeating. 0.92
I will enjoy feeling a cigarette on my tongue and lips. 0.57 Smoking keeps my weight down. 0.91
I really enjoy a cigarette when I’m relaxed and feeling good. 0.52 Smoking helps me control my weight. 0.86
I enjoy parties more when I am smoking. 0.35
Exploratory Factor Analysis

Results from a parallel analysis (Horn, 1965) with 1000 repetitions and an oblique rotation supported a six-factor structure. Standardized factor loadings were examined to evaluate item appropriateness, and number of item was used to evaluate strength and stability of factors. Criterion for an adequate loading ranges from .40 (Matsunaga, 2015) to .70 (Kline, 2015). For the current study, items with a significant factor loading of at least .50 were considered meaningful indicators of the underlying construct. Regarding strength and stability of factors, recommendations for best practices in exploratory factor analysis suggest that a factor with three or fewer items is generally weak and unstable (Costello & Osborne, 2005). Therefore, only factors with four or more items with significant loadings of at least .50 were retained. Based on this criterion, 35 items were retained that constituted five factors (see Table 2); one of the six factors supported by the parallel analysis had only one item that loaded at greater than .50 and therefore was not retained. The revised five-factor structure included the following subscales: (1) Long Term Negative Consequences [LT], (2) Immediate Negative Consequences [IC], (3) Sensory Satisfaction [SS], (4) the original NR, and (5) the original AW.

Table 2.

Standardized factor loadings from exploratory factor analysis in Study 1.

Item Loading Item Loading
Factor 1: Long Term Negative Consequences Factor 4: Negative Reinforcement
By smoking I risk heart disease and lung cancer. 0.87 Cigarettes help me deal with anger. 0.95
Smoking is hazardous to my health. 0.87 Smoking reduces my anger. 0.93
The more I smoke, the more I risk my health. 0.79 When I’m upset with someone, a cigarette helps me cope. 0.85
Smoking is taking years off my life. 0.60 Cigarettes help me deal with anxiety or worry. 0.81
Each cigarette I smoke maintains my addiction. 0.58 Cigarettes help me reduce or handle tension. 0.81
I will become more dependent on nicotine if I continue smoking. 0.54 When I’m angry, a cigarette can calm me down. 0.79
I will probably die earlier if I continue to smoke. 0.53 Smoking helps me deal with depression. 0.77
Factor 2: Immediate Negative Consequences If I am disappointed in myself, a good smoke can help. 0.70
Cigarettes make my lungs hurt. 0.79 If I’m tense a cigarette helps me to relax. 0.67
Smoking irritates my mouth and throat. 0.79 Smoking calms me down when I feel nervous. 0.67
My throat burns after smoking. 0.76 When I am sad, smoking makes me feel better. 0.65
Smoking will make me cough. 0.64 Cigarettes help me concentrate. 0.51
I look ridiculous while smoking. 0.61 Factor 5: Appetite-Weight Control
People think less of me if they see me smoking. 0.58 Cigarettes keep me from eating more than I should. 0.94
Smoking makes me seem less attractive. 0.52 Smoking controls my appetite. 0.92
Factor 3: Sensory Satisfaction Smoking keeps my weight down. 0.91
When I smoke, the taste is pleasant. 0.95 Cigarettes keep me from overeating. 0.90
I will enjoy the flavor of a cigarette. 0.89 Smoking helps me control my weight. 0.84
Cigarettes taste good. 0.84
I enjoy the taste sensations while smoking. 0.83
Construct Validity

LT was positively associated with anxiety sensitivity (r = .14, p = .01). IC was positively related to baseline measures of anxiety sensitivity (r = .25, p < .001), general distress related to anxiety (r = .25, p < .001), anxious arousal (r = .30, p < .001), general distress related to depression (r = .20, p < .001), and anhedonic symptoms (r = .16, p = .004). SS negatively related to anhedonic symptoms (r = −.14, p = .02) and expired CO (r = −.10, p = .05). The NR and AW factors were both positively correlated with anxiety sensitivity (r = .36, p < .001 and r = .21, p = .001, respectively), general distress related to anxiety (r = .24, p < .001 and r = .14, p = .01, respectively), anxious arousal (r = .24, p < .001 and r = .20, p < .001, respectively), and anhedonic symptoms (r = .12, p = .04 and r = .11, p = .04, respectively). Additionally, NR was positively correlated with general distress related to depression (r = .17, p = .001).

Discussion

Findings failed to provide evidence for the proposed four-factor SCQ solution using the original 50-items. Instead, the current data supported a novel, truncated five-factor solution consisting of 35-items. The observed associations across the proposed five factors and constructs believed to interfere with quit success and maintain smoking provides initial evidence for the construct validity of these factors. Importantly, results indicated unique factor- affective/mood construct associations. For example, all factors, except SS, positively related to anxiety sensitivity. These associations ranged from small to moderate. Further, the IC and NR subscales were positively associated with all affective constructs, and AW as positively associated with all except general distress related to depression; relations ranged from small to moderate in effect. SS was the only construct related to an index of smoking behavior. Thus, the differential pattern and strength of associations provide initial evidence for the uniqueness of the factors. Yet, considering that measurement invariance across sex was not tested prior to evaluating the construct validity of the newly proposed structure, findings should be interpreted with caution. Indeed, it is only after measurement invariance has been demonstrated that the current findings can be considered an unbiased, true reflection of the observed associations.

To provide further evidence for the five-factor SCQ structure using 35-items, address the limitations of the current study (i.e., lack of tests for measurement invariance), and more compressively evaluate the construct validity of these factors, Study 2 was conducted. Based on current findings, all SCQ factors, except SS, were hypothesized to positively relate to anxiety sensitivity, negative affectivity, smoking specific experiential avoidance, and tobacco dependence. The IC, NR, and AW were hypothesized to positively relate to dysphoria, panic, and social anxiety; whereas LT and SS were expected to be unrelated to these variables. Lastly, positive smoking expectancy outcome (i.e., SS, NR and AW) subscales were hypothesized to negatively relate to positive affect and well-being; negative smoking expectancies (i.e., LT and IC) were expected to be unrelated to positive affectivity and well-being. Prior research informed the selection of variables used to evaluate convergent and discriminant validity (Guillot et al., 2015; Leventhal, Zvolensky, & Schmidt, 2011; Leyro et al., 2008).

Study 2

Participants

A sample of 582 treatment-seeking adult smokers who responded to study advertisements (e.g., flyers, newspaper ads, radio announcements; 48.2% female; Mage = 36.9; SD = 13.5) were included in the current study. Exclusion criteria included suicidality and psychosis. The racial/ethnic distribution of this sample was as follows: 82.9% White/Caucasian; 9.8% Black/Non-Hispanic; 0.9% Black/Hispanic; 2.6% Hispanic; 1.0% Asian; and 2.8% ‘Other.’At least one current (past year) Axis I diagnosis was endorsed by 54.8% of the sample, most commonly social anxiety disorder (10.1%), generalized anxiety disorder (5.4%), current major depressive episode (4.7%), and posttraumatic stress disorder (3.0%). On average, participants reported smoking 16.7 cigarettes per day (SD = 10.0), had been a daily smoker for 18.6 years (SD = 13.4), and had an average expired carbon monoxide level of 19.4 parts per million (SD = 12.3). A moderate level of tobacco dependence was observed within the sample based on the FTCD (M= 5.2, SD= 2.3; Heatherton et al., 1991).

Procedures

Data for the present study was collected during a large, multi-site randomized controlled clinical trial examining the efficacy of two smoking cessation interventions described in detail elsewhere (Schmidt, Raines, Allan, & Zvolensky, 2016). Interested persons responding to community-based advertisements (e.g., flyers, newspaper ads, radio announcements) contacted the research team and were provided with a detailed description of the study via phone. Participants were then screened for initial eligibility, and if eligible, scheduled for an appointment. Inclusion criteria included: (a) being between ages 18–65, (b) reporting smoking 8 or more cigarettes per day, and (c) reporting motivation to quit rated as at least 5 or higher on a 10-point scale. Exclusion criteria included: (a) current suicidal ideation requiring immediate intervention or (a) active psychosis. After providing written informed consent, participants were interviewed using the SCID-I/NP and completed a computerized self-report assessment battery as well as biochemical verification of smoking status. After completing the baseline assessment, participants were informed of their eligibility. For the current study, baseline data was included from all participants regardless of eligibility for the larger trial. Participants eligible for the larger trial were randomized to one of two treatment conditions. Each treatment was delivered across four intervention sessions with session four being quit day.

The current study is based on secondary analyses of baseline and quit day data from the larger trial. Of the 582 participants who provided data on the variables of interest at baseline, 486 were randomized to treatment. Of those randomized, 262 provided quit day data. Inclusion of participants regardless of eligibility for the larger trial is consistent with past psychometric work conducted in the context of a smoking cessation trial (see Farris, DiBello, et al., 2015). All study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Boards.

Measures

Demographics Questionnaire

Demographic information collected included sex, age, and race. Items from this measure were used to describe the sample.

Carbon Monoxide

Biochemical verification of smoking status was assessed by expired carbon monoxide (CO) analysis of breath samples collected using a CMD/CO Carbon Monoxide Monitor (Model 3110; Spirometrics, Inc.). In the present study, baseline CO was used to describe the sample and to demonstrate convergent validity. CO collected at quit day was used to classify quitters (e.g., nonsmokers) and non-quitters (e.g., smokers). A cut-off of 4 ppm has demonstrated excellent properties at distinguishing smokers from nonsmokers (Perkins et al., 2013).

Smoking History Questionnaire (SHQ)

The SHQ is used to assess smoking rate, years of daily smoking, and other characteristics (Brown, Lejuez, Kahler, & Strong, 2002). Smoking rate was obtained from the question, “Since you started regular daily smoking, what is the average number of cigarettes you smoked per day?” Furthermore, years as a daily smoker was assessed by the question, “For how many years, altogether, have you been a regular daily smoker?”.

Fagerström Test for Cigarette Dependence (FTCD)

Please see description above. In the current study, the FTCD total score was used to characterize tobacco dependence (α = .65). FTCD was used to describe the sample and to demonstrate convergent validity.

Structured Clinical Interview-Non-Patient Version for DSM-IV (SCID-I/NP)

Diagnostic assessments of past year Axis I psychopathology was conducted using the SCID-I/NP (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1994). All SCID-I/NP interviews were administered by trained research assistants or doctoral level staff and supervised by independent doctoral-level professionals. Interviews were audio-taped and the reliability of a random selection of 12.5% of interviews was checked (MJZ) for accuracy; there were no cases of disagreement. Data from the SCID-I/NP was used to describe psychopathology among the sample.

Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (ASI-3)

The ASI-3 is an 18-item self-report measure of the sensitivity to and fear of the potential negative consequences of anxiety-related symptoms and sensations. Respondents are asked to indicate, on a 5-point Likert-type scale (0 = “very little” to 4 = “very much”), the degree to which they are concerned about these possible negative consequences. The ASI-3, derived in part from the original ASI (Reiss & McNally, 1985), has sound psychometric properties, including excellent internal consistency, predictive validity, and reliability among smokers (Farris, DiBello, et al., 2015). In the present study, we utilized the total ASI-3 score (α = .93).

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS)

The PANAS (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) measured the extent to which participants experienced 20 different feelings and emotions on a scale ranging from 1 (Very slightly or not at all) to 5 (Extremely). The measure yields two factors, negative affect (NA) and positive affect (PA), and has strong documented psychometric properties (Watson et al., 1988). Both factors were utilized in the current study (NA α = .92; PA α = .90).

Smoking-specific Avoidance and Inflexibility Scale (AIS)

The AIS assessed avoidance and inflexibility related to smoking (Gifford & Lillis, 2009). Participants responded to 13-items according to a Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (Not at all) to 5 (Very much). Items included “How likely is it that these feelings will lead you to smoke?” and “To what degree must you reduce how often you have these thoughts in order not to smoke.” The AIS has demonstrated good internal consistency (Gifford & Lillis, 2009). Higher scores represent more smoking-based avoidance or inflexibility in the presence of uncomfortable or difficult sensations or thoughts, whereas lower scores suggest more ability to accept difficult feelings or thoughts without allowing them to trigger smoking. Past work has found good convergent and predictive validity of the AIS for smoking processes (Farris, Zvolensky, DiBello, et al., 2015; Zvolensky, Farris, Schmidt, & Smits, 2014). In the present study, we utilized the total AIS score (α = .93).

Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Symptoms (IDAS)

The IDAS (Watson et al., 2007) is a 64-item self-report instrument that assesses distinct affect symptom dimensions within the past two weeks. Items are answered on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “not at all” to “extremely.” The IDAS subscales show strong internal consistency, convergent and discriminant validity with psychiatric diagnoses and self-report measures; and short-term retest reliability (r = 0.79) with both community and psychiatric patient samples (Buckner et al., 2015; Capron, Allan, Norr, Zvolensky, & Schmidt, 2014; Leventhal et al., 2011; Watson et al., 2007). In the present study, we employed the well-being subscale (8 items; α = .91), dysphoria (9 items; α = .91), panic (8 items; α = .88), and social anxiety (5 items; α = .87).

Minnesota Nicotine Withdrawal Scale (MNWS)

The Minnesota Nicotine Withdrawal Scale (Hughes & Hatsukami, 1986) is an 8-item measure of nicotine withdrawal symptoms, which are rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 0 = Not present to 3 = Severe (e.g., insomnia, irritability/frustration, difficulty concentrating, and restlessness). The MNWS was administered on quit-day (α = .83).

Smoking Consequences Questionnaire (SCQ)

Please see description above. For the current study, response options ranged from 0 (completely unlikely) to 9 (completely likely).

Data Analytic Strategy

To corroborate findings from Study 1, the novel, truncated five-factor SCQ structure was examined with a CFA using the identified 35-items (i.e., LT [7 items], IC [7 items], SS [4 items], NR [12 items], AW [5 items]; Brown, 2015). Analyses were conducted using Mplus 7.31 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012). The distribution of select SCQ items was determined to be outside the range of normal (skewness range: |0.04|–|3.82|; kurtosis range: |0.10|–|18.57|); therefore, maximum likelihood robust estimation was used (Muthén & Muthén, 2012). Factors were allowed to correlate; however, a higher-order factor structure was deemed inappropriate given theoretical and conceptual support for the uniqueness of the different facets of the substance use expectancies (Brandon & Baker, 1991). Model fit and model comparison was examined using the same criteria as outlined for Study 1. Full information maximum likelihood was employed to handle missing data (Allison, 2012). At baseline, SCQ had the most missing data (12.7%) followed by FTND (12.5%); missingness across other baseline variables ranged from <1% to 12.2%.

Measurement invariance (Meredith, 1993) was assessed across sex and time from baseline to quit-week with the best fitting model. Specifically, configural, metric, and scalar invariance were assessed by constraining parameters and comparing nested models. Configural invariance assesses for similar factor-indicator patterns across sex. Metric invariance assesses for consistency in the strength of the association between items and factors across groups. Scalar invariance assesses for consistency in item means. CFI change of less than 0.010 and RMSEA change of less than 0.015 (Chen, 2007) provide statistical evidence for invariance between the less and more constrained model.

Factor determinacy coefficients were estimated as a measure of factor internal consistency, with values ranging from 0 to 1 and larger values indicating better measurement of the factor by the observed items. A factor determinacy coefficient of≥0.80 suggests strong correlation among items with their respective factor denoting high internal consistency (Gorsuch, 1983). Consistent with prior work (Barbaranelli, Lee, Vellone, & Riegel, 2014; Tabachnick, Fidell, & Osterlind, 2001), a factor determinacy coefficient of .70 indicated adequate reliability.

A series of zero-order correlations between baseline variables were conducted to demonstrate convergent and discriminant validity. Variables to evaluate convergent and discriminant validity included anxiety sensitivity, negative affectivity, smoking specific experiential avoidance, expired baseline CO, tobacco dependence, positive affect, well-being, dysphoria, panic, and social anxiety. Lastly, a path model was conducted to assess the predictive validity of the SCQ factors at baseline on withdrawal symptoms reported at quit week. Consistent with past work (Wetter et al., 1994), only those who met abstinence criteria were included in analyses (n = 76). Covariates included sex, tobacco dependence, presence of an Axis I disorder, and treatment condition.

RESULTS

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

The five-factor, 35-item SCQ structure identified in Study 1 provided acceptable fit for the data (X2[550] = 1393.95, p < .001; RMSEA = .06 [90% CI: .05, .06]; CFI = .89; SRMR = .06). Thus, this model was employed in all subsequent analyses (see Table 3).

Table 3.

Standardized factor loadings from confirmatory factor analysis in Study 2.

Item Loading Item Loading
Factor 1: Long Term Negative Consequences Factor 4: Negative Reinforcement
The more I smoke, the more I risk my health. 0.83 When I’m upset with someone, a cigarette helps me cope. 0.80
Smoking is hazardous to my health. 0.83 Cigarettes help me deal with anger. 0.80
Each cigarette I smoke maintains my addiction. 0.79 Cigarettes help me deal with anxiety or worry. 0.79
By smoking I risk heart disease and lung cancer. 0.77 Cigarettes help me reduce or handle tension. 0.79
Smoking is taking years off my life. 0.75 When I’m angry, a cigarette can calm me down. 0.78
I will probably die earlier if I continue to smoke. 0.68 Smoking calms me down when I feel nervous. 0.78
I will become more dependent on nicotine if I continue smoking. 0.63 When I am sad, smoking makes me feel better. 0.77
Factor 2: Immediate Negative Consequences Smoking reduces my anger. 0.72
Smoking irritates my mouth and throat. 0.66 If I’m tense, a cigarette helps me relax. 0.72
Cigarettes make my lungs hurt. 0.62 If I’m disappointed in myself, a good smoke can help. 0.71
Smoking will make me cough. 0.60 Cigarettes help me concentrate. 0.65
I look ridiculous while smoking. 0.56 Smoking helps me deal with depression. 0.64
Smoking makes me seem less attractive. 0.56 Factor 5: Appetite-Weight Control
People think less of me if they see me smoking. 0.55 Cigarettes keep me from eating more than I should. 0.86
My throat burns after smoking. 0.52 Smoking keeps my weight down. 0.85
Factor 3: Sensory Satisfaction Smoking helps me control my weight. 0.84
When I smoke, the taste is pleasant. 0.93 Cigarettes keep me from overeating. 0.82
I will enjoy the flavor of a cigarette. 0.89 Smoking controls my appetite. 0.71
I enjoy the taste sensations while smoking. 0.85
Cigarettes taste good. 0.80
Attrition Analyses

Participants who provided quit day data for variables of interest (n = 262) and those who did not (n = 320) significantly differed in terms of sex (X2[1] = 5.66, p = .02). Specifically, more men did not provide quit day data (n = 179) compared to women (n = 139). Participants who did not provide quit day data were significantly younger than those who did (M = 34.86; SD = 12.77 versus M = 39.37; SD = 13.90; t[577] = 4.07, p < .001). Participants who did not provide quit day data reported being a regular, daily smoker for significantly fewer years than those who provided quit day data (M = 16.37; SD = 12.48 versus M = 21.02; SD = 13.83; t[510] = 4.00, p < .001). Moreover, participants who did not provide quit day data reported significantly greater negative affectivity (M = 20.20; SD = 8.04 versus M = 18.07; SD = 6.68, respectively) and anxiety sensitivity (M = 17.10, SD = 13.55 versus M = 13.87; SD = 11.70) at baseline relative to participants who provided quit day data (PANAS-NA: t[567] = −3.40, p = .001; ASI-3: t[576] = −3.04, p = .003).

Measurement Invariance

The five-factor structure demonstrated configural invariance across sex (X2[1100] = 2323.56, p < .001; RMSEA = .07 [90% CI: .06, .07]; CFI = .86; SRMR = .07). Results provided evidence for metric and scalar invariance for the five-factor structure across sex (see Table 3). Regarding configural invariance from baseline to quit-week, the five-factor structure demonstrated acceptable fit (X2[2265] = 4197.05, p < .001; RMSEA = .04 [90% CI: .04, .04]; CFI = .88; SRMR = .06), as well as metric and scalar invariance (see Table 4).

Table 4.

Measurement invariance of the six-factor SCQ model across sex and over time.

Models χ2 df CFI RMSEA SRMR ΔCFI ΔRMSEA
Across Sex
1. Configural 2323.56 1100 .86 .07 .07 -- --
2. Metric 2343.06 1130 .86 .07 .08 .001 .005
3. Scalar 2423.12 1165 .85 .07 .08 .005 .000

Across Time (Baseline to Quit Week)
1. Configural 4197.05 2265 .88 .04 .06 -- --
2. Metric 4269.52 2295 .88 .04 .07 .003 .001
3. Scalar 4378.42 2323 .88 .04 .07 .004 .000

Note. N = 508 baseline, 523 quit week; 15 participants who provided Smoking Consequences Questionnaire data at quit week only were included in tests of measurement invariance test over time. χ2 = Chi-square test; df = degrees of freedom; CFI = comparative fit index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; SRMR = standardized root-mean-square residual; ΔCFI = change in comparative fit index from previous model; ΔRMSEA = change in root mean square error of approximation from previous model.

Construct Validity

Internal consistency of the five factors at baseline was excellent across all factors (LT = .96; IC = .90; SS = .97; NR = .97, AW = .96). Table 5 presents results from tests of convergent and discriminant validity using scales from the SCQ and measures of interest. LT was positively associated with smoking-specific experiential avoidance, expired CO at baseline, and positive affectivity. IC was positively related to baseline measure of anxiety sensitivity, negative affectivity, smoking-specific experiential avoidance, expired CO, tobacco dependence, dysphoria, panic, and social anxiety. SS positively related to smoking-specific experiential avoidance and tobacco dependence. The NR and AW were both positively correlated with anxiety sensitivity, negative affect, smoking-specific experiential avoidance, tobacco dependence, dysphoria, and social anxiety, and negatively correlated with positive affectivity and well-being. The path model wherein withdrawal severity at quit week was regressed on the five SCQ factors and covariates accounted for significant variance in the outcome (R2 = 18.5%, p = .02). Only NR at baseline predicted withdrawal severity at quit day (b = 0.64, SE = 0.31, p = .04).

Table 5.

Associations across the SCQ and convergent/discriminant constructs.

Variable Mean (SD) Range LT IC SS NR AW
Convergent/Discriminant
 ASI-3 15.64 (12.83) 0–72 0.01 0.22*** 0.03 0.30*** 0.15**
 PANAS-NA 19.23 (7.52) 10–48 0.04 0.22*** 0.05 0.39*** 0.14**
 AIS 44.97 (10.71) 13–65 0.38*** 0.36*** 0.15** 0.47*** 0.23***
 Baseline Expired CO 19.34 (12.27) 0–90.5 0.09* 0.14* 0.04 0.03 0.01
 FTCD 5.24 (2.29) 0–10 0.08 0.16** 0.16*** 0.18*** 0.18***
 PANAS-PA 32.25 (7.42) 10–50 0.16** 0.07 −0.01 −0.13* −0.14**
 Well-Being 22.66 (6.99) 8–40 0.08 0.04 0.02 −0.22*** −0.16**
 Dysphoria 19.36 (8.00) 10–50 0.07 0.25*** 0.04 0.40*** 0.14**
 Panic 11.19 (4.37) 8–39 −0.01 0.23*** 0.01 0.25*** 0.14**
 Social Anxiety 8.07 (3.24) 5–25 −0.01 0.21*** 0.05 0.28*** 0.12*
Inter-Factor Correlations
 LT 7.96 (1.33) 0–9 -- 0.54*** 0.12* 0.34*** 0.14**
 IC 4.40 (1.48) 0–9 -- -- −0.12t 0.29*** 0.33***
 SS 5.25 (2.34) 0–9 -- -- -- 0.29*** 0.16**
 NR 5.63 (1.82) 0–9 -- -- -- -- 0.45***
 AW 4.15 (1.82) 0–9 -- -- -- -- --

Note.

t

p=.06,

*

p<.05,

**

p<.01,

***

p<.001.

SCQ scales were employed for tests of convergent/discriminate validity and factors were implored for inter-factor correlations. ASI-3 = Anxiety Sensitivity Index 3 (Taylor et al., 2007). PANAS-NA = Positive and Negative Affect Scale-Negative Affect subscale (Watson et al., 1988). Well-Being = Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Symptoms Well-Being Subscale (Watson et al., 2007). Dysphoria = Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Dysphoria subscale (Watson et al., 2007). Panic = Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Panic subscale (Watson et al., 2007). Social Anxiety = Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Social Anxiety subscale (Watson et al., 2007). AIS = Avoidance and Inflexibility Scale (Gifford & Lillis, 2009). FTCD= Fagerström Test for Cigarette Dependence (Fagerström, 2012). CO = Carbon Monoxide. LT = Long Term Negative Consequences. IC = Immediate Negative Consequences. SS = Sensory Satisfaction. NR = Negative Reinforcement. AW = Appetite-Weight Control.

GENERAL DISCUSSION

The current study evaluated the structure and psychometric properties of the SCQ in two independent samples of smokers. Findings suggested poor fit for the assumed four-factor structure and acceptable fit for a novel, truncated five-factor structure. The five-factor SCQ structure demonstrated stability across sex and from baseline to quit day as well as excellent internal consistency at baseline. Lastly, low to moderate factor correlations and unique predictive validity provided preliminary evidence that the five factors represented distinct, yet related, dimensions of smoking expectancies in a sample of treatment seeking smokers.

Despite previous evidence for a four-factor SCQ structure (Brandon & Baker, 1991; Wetter et al., 1994), the current study provides more convincing empirical support for a five-factor SCQ structure across two independent samples. Although this is the first investigation to isolate potential confounds that contribute to the poor fitting SCQ four-factor structure, it is noteworthy that another research group was unable to validate the four-factor structure in a sample of Latino smokers (Vidrine et al., 2009). The observed misfit of the four-factor structure across multiple studies may be a direct artifact of the original scale validation. Specifically, Brandon and Baker (1991) utilized scores that considered subjective expected utility scores (i.e., the cross-product of desirability and likelihood scores) to derive the original structure; yet, the SCQ is most commonly assessed with a single rating of likelihood on a Likert scale, as presented in Study 1 & 2. Moreover, Wetter et al. (1994) utilized parcels when they examined the four-factor structure in a CFA. The use of parcels over item-level data often results in improved model fit (Bandalos, 2002) and carries untested assumptions of unidimensionality among items within parcels, which can result in inflated model fit (Kline, 2015). In this case, improved model fit may have contributed to early conclusion that the SCQ is best modeled by a four-factor structure. Thus, the observed structural differences between past and present SCQ models illustrate that, as one explanation, assessment alterations and/or utilizing non-item-level data may have contributed to model misspecification.

Of the original 50 SCQ items, 35 items met cutoff recommendations to be included in the five-factor structure (See Costello & Osborne, 2005; Matsunaga, 2015). Therefore, 15 items were discarded. The discarded items, which included, for example, “The longer I smoke, the harder it will be to quit” and “I like to watch the smoke from my cigarette,” may be indicative of substantive cultural shift in how ‘today’s’ smokers perceive and report expectancies (Chassin, Presson, Sherman, & Kim, 2003). To illustrate, whereas the rate of smoking has decreased significantly since the initial development of the SCQ, current smoking among adults with some form of psychiatric distress or disorders has remained relatively stable (Control & Prevention, 2013). As such, current smokers may suffer from more global distress relative to smokers from the early 1990’s. The unique characteristics of ‘today’s’ smokers may have influenced item-factor relations and possibly contributed to the five-factor structure. Moreover, other factors, such as social/cultural awareness or individual smoking histories, may have contributed to the complexity of the currently proposed structure. Based on present results, future work using the SCQ should score the measure according to the five-factor structure using 35 of the original 50 SCQ items.

Factors that comprise the proposed five-factor structure are unique from and similar to the original factors in several fundamental ways. First, the original negative consequences factor assessed both immediate and long-term negative consequences of smoking. Underlying this factor construction is an assumption that negative consequence with differential time development tapped into the same unidimensional construct. Yet, research suggests that immediate and long-term negative consequences of smoking may be related, but unique, constructs that provide differential insight into smoking behavior and beliefs about the addictiveness of cigarettes (Slovic, 2000; Smith & Stutts, 2003). Consistent with this perspective, the newly proposed immediate (IC) and long-term negative consequences (LT) factors tap into multiple dimensions of smoking-related negative consequences which is overlooked when time-dependent negative consequences are culled into one factor.

Second, the originally proposed positive reinforcement/sensory satisfaction factor evaluated one’s expectancies for positive reinforcement properties of smoking as well as positive influences to the senses. Treating these two, seemingly unique, dimensions of smoking as a unidimensional construct is discordant with recent theoretical and empirical data that suggest that positive reinforcement aspects of smoking and taste/sensory properties are disparate facets of cognitive aspects of smoking (Piper et al., 2004). The proposed factor (i.e., SS), however, corroborates extant work implicating sensory satisfaction properties of smoking as unique dimensions of smoking expectancies. In addition, current findings did not support a unique factor to capture positive reinforcement smoking expectancies. The overlap in item content between items assumed to tap into positive reinforcement properties of smoking and negative reinforcement properties of smoking (i.e., NR subscale) may have contributed to this finding; thus, there may have been a concern of singularity had both subscales been preserved.

Third, the negative reinforcement and appetite-weight control factors are consistent across the original and newly proposed factor structures. Indeed, these factors demonstrated strong item-factor relations across all confirmatory factor analyses. Based on this empirical data as well as a concerted effort by the researchers to preserve continuity between the proposed SCQ factor structure and the original SCQ structure as theoretically and empirically supported, these factors were modeled as proposed by Brandon and Baker (1991). As a result, factor means for the negative reinforcement and appetite-weight control factors from the current study can be reliably compared to extant work with these factors.

Although the currently proposed newly specified factors warrant further validation, these factors have clear theoretical and clinical implications. Theoretically, smoking expectancies are conceptualized as a complex network of interrelated, yet distinct facets that may be limited by a four-factor structure (Copeland et al., 1995). The current structure addresses this limitation by expanding the structure of the SCQ to include additional theoretically-relevant facets of smoking outcome expectancies. In addition, each of the newly proposed factors taps into a more uniform, specific aspect of smoking expectancies relative to the originally proposed factors, as detailed above. This finding provides an improved opportunity for clinical symptom specificity and refined treatment planning. For example, the refined factors may provide slightly more nuanced insight into how specific expectancies relate to other smoking constructs and interfere with quit behavior.

The five-factor structure demonstrated full measurement invariance across sex and between baseline and quit day. These findings carry important interpretations and implications. First, the five-factor SCQ measure is similar in structure across sex and over time (e.g., the same factor-indicator pattern); therefore, the SCQ is conceptualized similarly across men and women, as well as if it is assessed at baseline or at quit-day following a smoking cessation treatment. Second, the strengths of the relations between each indicator and its associated factor is equal across sex and over time (e.g., the factor loading for each item did not significantly differ across sex or time); thus, men and women and participants at a baseline and quit-day assessment respond to the SCQ items in the same way. More specifically, scores on items can be compared across groups. Third, item means are equivalent across sex and over time (e.g., the item intercepts did not differ). This finding provides evidence that observed scores are related to latent scores; specifically, that participants with the same score on the latent construct have the same score on the observed items that constitute the latent construct. Conceptually, these findings provide strong empirical evidence to examine mean differences across sex and over time for each of the five proposed SCQ factors (Meredith, 1993). It is important to highlight that because the negative reinforcement and appetite-weight control factors were modeled in accordance with prior work, current findings can be extended to serve as initial evidence that reported differences in these factors across sex or over time are a result of true differences in the construct.

Beyond evidence for the stability of the five-factor structure, observed associations between the five SCQ factors and affective and smoking processes as well as indices of smoking behavior provide evidence of construct validity. All factors, except LT and SS, positively correlated with constructs known to be related to processes and psychopathological symptoms that maintain and impede quit success (Farris, Zvolensky, & Schmidt, 2015; Johnson, Stewart, Rosenfield, Steeves, & Zvolensky, 2012; Lasser et al., 2000; Leventhal et al., 2013). The observed association ranged from small to large in effect size. The LT and SS were positively associated with smoking-specific experiential avoidance with a medium and small effect size, respectively, but unrelated to anxiety sensitivity and negative affectivity. The specificity introduced with the newly constructed LT and SS factors may have contributed to their relation with a smoking-specific construct (i.e., smoking-specific experiential avoidance), and the lack of an association with more general measures of overall distress (i.e., anxiety sensitivity and negative affectivity). Second, the two consequences factors (LT and IC) positively related to baseline levels of expired carbon monoxide; both effect sizes were in the small range. Additionally, all factors, except LT, were positively correlated with tobacco dependence. The effect sizes were in the small to medium range. Thus, each factor related to some aspect of smoking severity. Third, all SCQ factors, except LT, were negatively correlated with or unrelated to constructs theorized to promote smoking cessation success (Barros, Kozasa, Formagini, Pereira, & Ronzani, 2015; Piper, Kenford, Fiore, & Baker, 2012). Unique to the LT, a positive, small/medium sized association emerged between this factor and positive affect. Importantly, although observed relations were identical across factors, the magnitude of these associations varied as discussed above. Thus, while the factors may relate to similar constructs, the strength of their associations identifies the unique contribution of each. Moreover, slightly different observations were observed regarding the LT and SS factors, providing evidence for the additional uniqueness of these factors. Together, findings provide initial evidence that general affective processes and smoking-specific processes share unique variance with each of the five SCQ factors.

The NR factor emerged as the only SCQ factor to significantly predict withdrawal severity. This finding is, in part, consistent with extant work (Langdon & Leventhal, 2014; Wetter et al., 1994). Despite the non-significant association between the other SCQ factors and withdrawal severity at quit week, the positive relation between NR and withdrawal severity at quit week provides further evidence for construct validity and the clinical utility of the NR subscale. Given the robust association between drug use to avoid or escape negative mood or affective states, such as withdrawal symptoms, and cessation (see Baker, Piper, McCarthy, Majeskie, & Fiore, 2004), elucidating pre-treatment predictors of withdrawal severity provides important information that has the potential to impact treatment development and could be used to help clinicians identify treatment seeking smokers who may be at greater risk for increased withdrawal severity and subsequently quit failure.

Clinically, the psychometric properties demonstrated by the SCQ suggest that it may be advisable for clinicians to administer a modified version of the SCQ to inform treatment and track smoking expectancies, particularly for smokers interested in or attempting to quit. Given that the proposed SCQ taps into a wider array of smoking outcome expectancies, clinicians may be able to isolate specific expectancies that may interfere with quitting with greater specificity. This approach would allow for a more individualized treatment plan that may promote greater quit success. Additionally, because both the original SCQ and presently proposed SCQ were developed among samples that parallel typical smoking behaviors among smokers today, there may be greater utility in administering the current modified measure over alternative forms of the SCQ.

There are several study limitations. First, the sample size and estimated parameters ratio across both studies may have impacted findings. Prior work suggests participant to parameter-estimated ratio of 20:1 (Kline, 2015), which was not met in either of the current studies. Importantly, results indicated interpretable, reliable estimates; yet, considering the number of parameters required to be estimated for the SCQ, future work may benefit from employing a Monte Carlo sample size estimation technique (Wolf, Harrington, Clark, & Miller, 2013) to inform appropriate sample size for ‘next stage’ evaluations of the SCQ. Second, consistent with extant work (Pang, Zvolensky, Schmidt, & Leventhal, 2014), both studies utilized a Likert response option scale for the SCQ that tapped into likelihood. However, the two studies differed in response anchors, such that Study 1 queried on how true the statement was for the respondent and Study 2 queried on how likely the statement was for the respondent. Along similar lines, factors could correlate in the current study, which is a deviation from the original testing of this measure considering that an orthogonal rotation was employed (Brandon & Baker, 1991). Yet, this is consistent with theoretical and recent analytic approaches used to evaluate smoking outcome measures (Lewis-Esquerre et al., 2005). Nevertheless, these are important considerations for future work that may attempt to cross-validate or extent the present findings in an independent sample of smokers. Third, the current study did not test the SCQ across ethnoracial factors. This limitation was due, in part, to an artifact of the sample size in Study 1 and limited diversity in Study 2. Thus, future research would benefit from employing strategic recruitment efforts to enroll a larger sample of ethnically/racially diverse smokers and evaluate the psychometric properties of the SCQ across ethnoracial factors. Fourth, as reviewed, several other factor structures for the SCQ have been developed. Unfortunately, different items included in the four-factor structure relative to alternative structures did not permit examination of previously proposed alternative model structures. Although each of the distinct SCQ measures have added uniquely to our understanding of the impact of smoking outcome expectancies on smoking behavior (for example: Gwaltney et al., 2005; Zvolensky et al. (2004), to allow for a more direct comparison of smoking outcome expectancies across studies, future research may benefit from evaluating the original SCQ, SCQ-Adult, Adolescent-SCQ, S-SCQ, Brief SCQ-Adult, and SCQ-Spanish, and the currently proposed novel, truncated scale, as well as other potential models including a hierarchical model, in a single study. Such work would permit a direct evaluation of the commonality in structure and may facilitate a more definitive consensus on the most appropriate measure/structure to evaluate smoking outcome expectancies. Finally, as the 35 items retained were not evaluated independently of the 50-items, additional work is needed to validate the proposed structure with a measure that only included the selected 35 items.

Findings from the current study offer a unique and necessary contribution to the smoking literature considering that the SCQ has been in circulation for more than 20 years and its use in clinical research settings is still ongoing alongside modified/revised version of the SCQ, including the SCQ-Adult (Brandon & Baker, 1991; Copeland et al., 1995). Indeed, work using this measure has contributed extensively to the current understanding of the role of smoking outcome expectancies in smoking maintenance and quit processes. Nevertheless, sociocultural shifts in society, specifically within the smoking community as well as in the broader population (Burns, 2014), may have contributed to changes in the structure and necessity of all original 50 items. Yet, prior to the present study, the construct validity argument for the SCQ-50 item measure has been relatively stagnate and may, potentially, be antiquated to the point of making the original measure arguably obsolete. To a degree, this is substantiated by the various versions of the SCQ that have been proposed since the development of the original scale as well as the currently proposed novel, truncated scale. The currently proposed scale offers a revitalized and novel analysis of this influential and widely used measure as well as a strong bridge to connect current, past, and future research;, particularly because two of the originally proposed scales are retained in their original form (i.e., NR and AW; Brandon & Baker, 1991). It is important to note, however, that the extant literature on smoking expectancies could benefit from further work devoted to evaluating and recommending a single measure of this construct to permit greater continuity across studies. The current findings provide a centrally important empirical road to stimulate such research in a more focused and timely manner.

Overall, the present findings provide initial evidence for a novel, truncated five-factor model structure of the SCQ that may prove helpful for elucidating individual, social, and contextual factors that are related to smoking behavior. As such, collective evidence from the current report proposes that future research utilize a 35-item SCQ measure with a scoring algorithm that models five unique factors (see Table 2 for factor items). Furthermore, these recommendations are derived from current findings that the five-factor SCQ structure demonstrated measurement invariance across sex and time, and the observation that each factor had excellent internal consistency. Current findings also highlight that the five SCQ factors are theoretically relevant facets of smoking expectancies that uniquely relate to processes posited to interfere with quitting or promote more problematic use. Together, the five-factor SCQ structure evinced properties that suggest it is a stable construct that assesses multiple domains of smoking outcome expectancies and is related to several affective and smoking processes the may interfere with smoking maintenance and relapse.

Public Significance Statements.

The present study provides initial evidence for the construct validity of a shorter, multifaceted Smoking Consequences Questionnaire. This briefer measure that taps into more specific smoking outcome expectancies may be easier to administer and score in clinical and research settings, and will assist clinicians in developing more accurate personalized treatment plans for smokers.

Acknowledgments

Funding: Funding for Study 1 was provided by the National Institute of Drug Abuse (R01-DA026831; PI: Adam M. Leventhal). Funding for Study 2 was provided by the National Institute of Mental Health (R01-MH076629; Co-PIs: Norman B. Schmidt and Michael J. Zvolensky). Work on this paper was supported by funding from the National Institute of Drug Abuse (1F31DA043390; PI: Lorra Garey).

Footnotes

1

Brandon and Baker (1991) and Wetter et al. (1994) both employed likelihood scores to assess the SCQ; Vidrine et al. (2009) did not explicitly indicate response options for the SCQ, but considering that this group compared variations of the SCQ that were derived from likelihood scores, it is assumed they also employed likelihood scores.

References

  1. Adcock R. Measurement validity: A shared standard for qualitative and quantitative research. Paper presented at the American Political Science Association.2001. [Google Scholar]
  2. Aguirre CG, Bello MS, Andrabi N, Pang RD, Hendricks PS, Bluthenthal RN, Leventhal AM. Gender, Ethnicity, and Their Intersectionality in the Prediction of Smoking Outcome Expectancies in Regular Cigarette Smokers. Behavior modification. 2016;40(1–2):281–302. doi: 10.1177/0145445515608146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allison PD. Handling missing data by maximum likelihood; Paper presented at the SAS global forum.2012. [Google Scholar]
  4. Awang Z. A Handbook on SEM (Structural Equation Modeling), Using AMOS Graphic. Universiti Teknologi Mara Kelantan; Kota Baharu: 2012. p. 309. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, Fiore MC. Addiction motivation reformulated: an affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological review. 2004;111(1):33. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bandalos DL. The Effects of Item Parceling on Goodness-of- Fit and Parameter Estimate Bias in Structural Equation Modeling. Structural equation modeling. 2002;9(1):78–102. [Google Scholar]
  7. Barbaranelli C, Lee CS, Vellone E, Riegel B. Dimensionality and Reliability of the Self - Care of Heart Failure Index Scales: Further Evidence From Confirmatory Factor Analysis. Research in nursing & health. 2014;37(6):524–537. doi: 10.1002/nur.21623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barros VV, Kozasa EH, Formagini TDB, Pereira LH, Ronzani TM. Smokers show lower levels of psychological well-being and mindfulness than non-smokers. PloS one. 2015;10(8):e0135377. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0135377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brandon TH, Baker TB. The Smoking Consequences Questionnaire: The subjective expected utility of smoking in college students. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1991;3(3):484. [Google Scholar]
  10. Brandon TH, Juliano LM, Copeland AL. Expectancies for tobacco smoking. 1999. [Google Scholar]
  11. Brandon TH, Wetter DW, Baker TB. Affect, expectancies, urges, and smoking: Do they conform to models of drug motivation and relapse? Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1996;4(1):29. [Google Scholar]
  12. Brown RA, Kahler CW, Zvolensky MJ, Lejuez C, Ramsey SE. Anxiety sensitivity: Relationship to negative affect smoking and smoking cessation in smokers with past major depressive disorder. Addictive behaviors. 2001;26(6):887–899. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(01)00241-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brown RA, Lejuez CW, Kahler CW, Strong DR. Distress tolerance and duration of past smoking cessation attempts. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111(1):180. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brown TA. Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. Guilford Publications; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  15. Buckner JD, Farris SG, Zvolensky MJ, Shah SM, Leventhal AM, Minnix JA, Schmidt NB. Dysphoria and smoking among treatment seeking smokers: the role of smoking-related inflexibility/avoidance. The American journal of drug and alcohol abuse. 2015;41(1):45–51. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2014.927472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Burns D. How far we have come in the last 50 years in smoking attitudes and actions. Annals of the American Thoracic Society. 2014;11(2):224–226. doi: 10.1513/AnnalsATS.201308-258PS. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Capron DW, Allan NP, Norr AM, Zvolensky MJ, Schmidt NB. The effect of successful and unsuccessful smoking cessation on short-term anxiety, depression, and suicidality. Addictive behaviors. 2014;39(4):782–788. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cepeda-Benito A, Reig Ferrer A. Smoking Consequences Questionnaire—Spanish. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2000;14(3):219. doi: 10.1037//0893-164x.14.3.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chassin L, Presson CC, Sherman SJ, Kim K. Historical changes in cigarette smoking and smoking-related beliefs after 2 decades in a midwestern community. Health Psychology. 2003;22(4):347. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.22.4.347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural equation modeling. 2007;14(3):464–504. [Google Scholar]
  21. Clark LA, Watson D. Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal of abnormal psychology. 1991;100(3):316. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.100.3.316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Control, C. f. D., Prevention. Vital signs: current cigarette smoking among adults aged≥ 18 years with mental illness-United States, 2009–2011. MMWR Morbidity and mortality weekly report. 2013;62(5):81. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Copeland AL, Brandon TH, Quinn EP. The Smoking Consequences Questionnaire-Adult: Measurement of smoking outcome expectancies of experienced smokers. Psychological Assessment. 1995;7(4):484. [Google Scholar]
  24. Costello AB, Osborne JW. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Practical Assessment Research & Evaluation. 2005;10:1–9. [Google Scholar]
  25. Doran N, Khoddam R, Sanders PE, Schweizer CA, Trim RS, Myers MG. A prospective study of the acquired preparedness model: The effects of impulsivity and expectancies on smoking initiation in college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2013;27(3):714. doi: 10.1037/a0028988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Doran N, Schweizer CA, Myers MG. Do expectancies for reinforcement from smoking change after smoking initiation? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2011;25(1):101. doi: 10.1037/a0020361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fagerström K. Determinants of tobacco use and renaming the FTND to the Fagerström Test for Cigarette Dependence. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2012;14(1):75–78. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntr137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Farris SG, DiBello AM, Allan NP, Hogan J, Schmidt NB, Zvolensky MJ. Evaluation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 among treatment-seeking smokers. Psychological Assessment. 2015;27(3):1123. doi: 10.1037/pas0000112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Farris SG, Zvolensky MJ, DiBello AM, Schmidt NB. Validation of the Avoidance and Inflexibility Scale (AIS) Among Treatment-Seeking Smokers. 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Farris SG, Zvolensky MJ, Schmidt NB. Smoking- specific experiential avoidance cognition: Explanatory relevance to pre- and post- cessation nicotine withdrawal, craving, and negative affect. Addictive behaviors. 2015;44:58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.07.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JB. Structured Clinical Interview for Axis I DSM-IV Disorders - Patient Edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0) New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  32. Garey L, Bakhshaie J, Brandt CP, Langdon KJ, Kauffman BY, Schmidt NB, … Zvolensky MJ. Interplay of dysphoria and anxiety sensitivity in relation to emotion regulatory cognitions of smoking among treatment - seeking smokers. The American Journal on Addictions. 2016;25(4):267–274. doi: 10.1111/ajad.12379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gifford EV, Lillis J. Avoidance and inflexibility as a common clinical pathway in obesity and smoking treatment. Journal of Health Psychology. 2009;14(7):992–996. doi: 10.1177/1359105309342304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Goldenson NI, Pang RD, Leventhal AM. Associations between ADHD symptoms and smoking outcome expectancies in a non - clinical sample of daily cigarette smokers. The American Journal on Addictions. 2016;25(2):152–159. doi: 10.1111/ajad.12343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Goldman MS, Brown SA, Christiansen BA. Expectancy theory-thinking about drinking. In: Blane, Leonard KE, editors. Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. New York, NY: Guilford Publications; 1987. p. 181. [Google Scholar]
  36. Gonzalez A, Hogan J, McLeish AC, Zvolensky MJ. An evaluation of pain-related anxiety among daily cigarette smokers in terms of negative and positive reinforcement smoking outcome expectancies. Addictive behaviors. 2010;35(6):553–557. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Gorsuch R. Factor analysis. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  38. Gregor KL, Zvolensky MJ, McLeish AC, Bernstein A, Morissette S. Anxiety sensitivity and perceived control over anxiety-related events: Associations with smoking outcome expectancies and perceived cessation barriers among daily smokers. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2008;10(4):627–635. doi: 10.1080/14622200801978706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Guillot CR, Zvolensky MJ, Leventhal AM. Differential associations between components of anxiety sensitivity and smoking-related characteristics. Addictive behaviors. 2015;40:39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gwaltney CJ, Shiffman S, Balabanis MH, Paty JA. Dynamic self-efficacy and outcome expectancies: prediction of smoking lapse and relapse. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2005;114(4):661. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerström KO. The Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence: A revision of the Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire. British Journal of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs. 1991;86(9):1119–1127. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Horn JL. A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1965;30(2):179–185. doi: 10.1007/BF02289447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Horn JL, McArdle JJ. A practical and theoretical guide to measurement invariance in aging research. Experimental aging research. 1992;18(3):117–144. doi: 10.1080/03610739208253916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hu Lt, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural equation modeling: a multidisciplinary journal. 1999;6(1):1–55. [Google Scholar]
  45. Hughes JR, Hatsukami D. Signs and symptoms of tobacco withdrawal. Archives of general psychiatry. 1986;43(3):289–294. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800030107013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Jamal A. Current cigarette smoking among adults—United States, 2005–2015. MMWR. Morbidity and mortality weekly report. 2016:65. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6544a2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Johnson KA, Stewart S, Rosenfield D, Steeves D, Zvolensky MJ. Prospective Evaluation of the Effects of Anxiety Sensitivity and State Anxiety in Predicting Acute Nicotine Withdrawal Symptoms During Smoking Cessation. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2012;26(2):289–297. doi: 10.1037/a0024133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Johnson KA, Zvolensky MJ, Marshall EC, Gonzalez A, Abrams K, Vujanovic AA. Linkages between cigarette smoking outcome expectancies and negative emotional vulnerability. Addictive behaviors. 2008;33(11):1416–1424. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kenford SL, Smith SS, Wetter DW, Jorenby DE, Fiore MC, Baker TB. Predicting relapse back to smoking: contrasting affective and physical models of dependence. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 2002;70(1):216. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. King TK, Marcus BH, Pinto BM, Emmons KM, Abrams DB. Cognitive–behavioral mediators of changing multiple behaviors: smoking and a sedentary lifestyle. Preventive medicine. 1996;25(6):684–691. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1996.0107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, NY: Guilford publications; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  52. Langdon KJ, Leventhal AM. Posttraumatic stress symptoms and tobacco abstinence effects in a non-clinical sample: Evaluating the mediating role of negative affect reduction smoking expectancies. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2014;28(11):1009–1017. doi: 10.1177/0269881114546708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lasser K, Boyd JW, Woolhandler S, Himmelstein DU, McCormick D, Bor DH. Smoking and mental illness: a population-based prevalence study. Jama. 2000;284(20):2606–2610. doi: 10.1001/jama.284.20.2606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Leventhal AM, Greenberg JB, Trujillo MA, Ameringer KJ, Lisha NE, Pang RD, Monterosso J. Positive and Negative Affect as Predictors of Urge to Smoke: Temporal Factors and Mediational Pathways. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2013;27(1):262–267. doi: 10.1037/a0031579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Leventhal AM, Piper ME, Japuntich SJ, Baker TB, Cook JW. Anhedonia, depressed mood, and smoking cessation outcome. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 2014;82(1):122. doi: 10.1037/a0035046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Leventhal AM, Zvolensky MJ, Schmidt NB. Smoking-related correlates of depressive symptom dimensions in treatment-seeking smokers. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2011;13(8):668–676. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntr056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lewis-Esquerre JM, Rodrigue JR, Kahler CW. Development and validation of an adolescent smoking consequences questionnaire. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2005;7(1):81–90. doi: 10.1080/14622200412331328475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Leyro TM, Zvolensky MJ, Vujanovic AA, Bernstein A. Anxiety sensitivity and smoking motives and outcome expectancies among adult daily smokers: Replication and extension. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2008;10(6):985–994. doi: 10.1080/14622200802097555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Little TD, Preacher KJ, Selig JP, Card NA. New developments in latent variable panel analyses of longitudinal data. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2007;31(4):357–365. [Google Scholar]
  60. Marlatt GA, Donovan DM. Relapse prevention: Maintenance strategies in the treatment of addictive behaviors. Guilford Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  61. Matsunaga M. How to Factor-Analyze Your Data Right: Do’s, Don’ts, and How-To’s. International Journal of Psychological Research. 2015;3(1):97–110. [Google Scholar]
  62. McHorney CA, Fleishman JA. Assessing and understanding measurement equivalence in health outcome measures: issues for further quantitative and qualitative inquiry. Medical care. 2006;44(11):S205–S210. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000245451.67862.57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Meredith W. Measurement invariance, factor analysis and factorial invariance. Psychometrika. 1993;58(4):525–543. [Google Scholar]
  64. Muthén L, Muthén B. CzeglédiE. Body dissatisfaction, trait anxiety and self-esteem in young men. Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen; 2012. Mplus User’s Guide; pp. 1998–2010. [Google Scholar]
  65. Myers MG, MacPherson L, McCarthy DM, Brown SA. Constructing a short form of the Smoking Consequences Questionnaire with adolescents and young adults. Psychological Assessment. 2003;15(2):163. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.15.2.163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Niaura RS, Rohsenow DJ, Binkoff JA, Monti PM, Pedraza M, Abrams DB. Relevance of cue reactivity to understanding alcohol and smoking relapse. Journal of abnormal psychology. 1988;97(2):133. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.97.2.133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Pang RD, Khoddam R, Guillot CR, Leventhal AM. Depression and anxiety symptoms moderate the relation between negative reinforcement smoking outcome expectancies and nicotine dependence. Journal of studies on alcohol and drugs. 2014;75(5):775–780. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2014.75.775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Pang RD, Zvolensky MJ, Schmidt NB, Leventhal AM. Gender differences in negative reinforcement smoking expectancies. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2014:ntu226. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntu226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Perkins KA, Karelitz JL, Jao NC. Optimal carbon monoxide criteria to confirm 24-hr smoking abstinence. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2013;15(5):978–982. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nts205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Piper M, Kenford S, Fiore M, Baker T. Smoking Cessation and Quality of Life: Changes in Life Satisfaction Over 3 Years Following a Quit Attempt. ann behav med. 2012;43(2):262–270. doi: 10.1007/s12160-011-9329-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Piper ME, Piasecki TM, Federman EB, Bolt DM, Smith SS, Fiore MC, Baker TB. A multiple motives approach to tobacco dependence: the Wisconsin Inventory of Smoking Dependence Motives (WISDM-68) Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 2004;72(2):139. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Pomerleau CS, Carton SM, Lutzke ML, Flessland KA, Pomerleau OF. Reliability of the fagerstrom tolerance questionnaire and the fagerstrom test for nicotine dependence. Addictive behaviors. 1994;19(1):33–39. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(94)90049-3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0306-4603(94)90049-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Rash CJ, Copeland AL. The Brief Smoking Consequences Questionnaire–Adult (BSCQ-A): development of a short form of the SCQ-A. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2008;10(11):1633–1643. doi: 10.1080/14622200802409990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Reiss S, McNally R. Expectancy model of fear. In: Reiss S, Bootzin RR, editors. Theoretical issues in behavior therapy inside. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  75. Schmidt NB, Raines AM, Allan NP, Zvolensky MJ. Anxiety sensitivity risk reduction in smokers: a randomized control trial examining effects on panic. Behaviour research and therapy. 2016;77:138–146. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2015.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Schwarzer R. Modeling health behavior change: How to predict and modify the adoption and maintenance of health behaviors. Applied Psychology. 2008;57(1):1–29. [Google Scholar]
  77. Slovic P. What does it mean to know a cumulative risk? Adolescents’ perceptions of short-term and long-term consequences of smoking. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 2000;13(2):259. [Google Scholar]
  78. Smith KH, Stutts MA. Effects of short - term cosmetic versus long - term health fear appeals in anti - smoking advertisements on the smoking behaviour of adolescents. Journal of Consumer Behaviour. 2003;3(2):157–177. [Google Scholar]
  79. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS, Osterlind SJ. Using multivariate statistics. 2001. [Google Scholar]
  80. Taylor S, Zvolensky MJ, Cox BJ, Deacon B, Heimberg RG, Ledley DR, … Stewart SH. Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: development and initial validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3. Psychological Assessment. 2007;19(2):176. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.2.176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Vandenberg RJ, Lance CE. A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational research methods. 2000;3(1):4–70. [Google Scholar]
  82. Vidrine JI, Vidrine DJ, Costello TJ, Mazas C, Cofta-Woerpel L, Mejia LM, Wetter DW. The Smoking Consequences Questionnaire: Factor structure and predictive validity among Spanish-speaking Latino smokers in the United States. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2009:ntp128. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntp128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1988;54(6):1063. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Watson D, O’Hara MW, Simms LJ, Kotov R, Chmielewski M, McDade-Montez EA, … Stuart S. Development and validation of the Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Symptoms (IDAS) Psychological Assessment. 2007;19(3):253. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.3.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Wetter DW, Kenford SL, Smith SS, Fiore MC, Jorenby DE, Baker TB. Gender differences in smoking cessation. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 1999;67(4):555. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.67.4.555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Wetter DW, Smith SS, Kenford SL, Jorenby DE, Fiore MC, Hurt RD, … Baker TB. Smoking outcome expectancies: factor structure, predictive validity, and discriminant validity. Journal of abnormal psychology. 1994;103(4):801. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.103.4.801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wolf EJ, Harrington KM, Clark SL, Miller MW. Sample size requirements for structural equation models an evaluation of power, bias, and solution propriety. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2013;73(6):913–934. doi: 10.1177/0013164413495237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Zvolensky MJ, Farris SG, Schmidt NB, Smits JA. The Role of Smoking Inflexibility/Avoidance in the Relation Between Anxiety Sensitivity and Tobacco Use and Beliefs Among Treatment-Seeking Smokers. 2014 doi: 10.1037/a0035306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Zvolensky MJ, Feldner MT, Leen-Feldner E, Bonn-Miller MO, McLeish AC, Gregor K. Evaluating the role of anxiety sensitivity in smoking outcome expectancies among regular smokers. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2004;28(4):473–486. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES