Abstract
The roles of kisspeptin signaling outside the hypothalamus in the brain are unknown. We examined here the impact of Kiss1r-deletion on hippocampus-related behaviors of anxiety and spatial learning in adult male mice using two mouse models. In the first, global Kiss1r-null and control mice were gonadectomized (GDX KISS1R-KO). In the second, KISS1R signalling was rescued selectively in gonadotropin-releasing hormone neurons to generate Kiss1r-null mice with normal testosterone levels (intact KISS1R-KO). Intact KISS1R-KO rescue mice were found to spend twice as much time in the open arms of the elevated plus maze (EPM) compared to controls (P < 0.01). GDX KISS1R-KO mice showed a similar but less pronounced trend. No differences were detected between intact KISS1R-KO mice and controls in the open field test (OFT), although a marked reduction in time spent in the centre quadrant was observed for all GDX mice (P < 0.001). No effects of KISS1R deletion or gonadectomy were detected in the Morris water maze. These observations demonstrate that KISS1R signalling impacts upon anxiogenic neural circuits operative in the EPM, while gonadal steroids appear important for anxiety behaviour observed in the OFT. The potential anxiogenic role of kisspeptin may need to be considered in the development of kisspeptin analogs for the clinic.
Introduction
Kisspeptin was identified in 1996 from human melanoma cell lines1 and the kisspeptin receptor identified shortly afterwards as G-protein-coupled receptor 542–4, now termed KISS1R5. In 2003, the critical importance of kisspeptin for reproduction was established by seminal studies reporting that humans with mutations in KISS1R failed to go through puberty and were infertile6,7. Investigations in various animal models over the last decade have now explained those clinical findings by demonstrating that kisspeptin activates KISS1Rs expressed by gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) neurons to play a central role in the control of fertility8–10.
In addition to the GnRH neurons, kisspeptin signaling occurs at cells in several body organs11,12 as well as within multiple brain regions outside the hypothalamus13. Indeed, kisspeptin-immunoreactive fibers are located throughout the limbic forebrain14–16 and cells expressing KISS1R are dispersed across an even wider distribution of the central nervous system17–19. In our own mapping studies, we were interested to note that the highest density of cells transcribing Kiss1r existed in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus18. Moreover, early electrophysiological studies by Arai and colleagues20 reported that kisspeptin selectively modulated excitatory synaptic activity in dentate granule neurons. These data suggested that kisspeptin signaling may have a role in hippocampal functioning.
In the present study, we have examined whether the deletion of Kiss1r impacts upon hippocampal-related behavioral tasks such as anxiety and spatial learning21,22. Kiss1r-null mice fail to go through puberty and remain sexually immature with very low levels of circulating gonadal steroid hormones23. This creates an experimental confound as gonadal steroids modulate the activity of many neuronal circuits in the brain, including those within the hippocampus controlling traits such as anxiety24,25. One strategy for overcoming this caveat has been to undertake experiments in gonadectomized (GDX) mice so that gonadal steroids are absent in both controls and Kiss1r-null mice26. However, this results in the investigation of GDX mice rather than normal animals and is, itself, problematic when gonadal steroids may play a role in the parameters being examined. An alternative approach is to use a mouse model in which Kiss1r has been deleted from all cells in the body but rescued selectively in the GnRH neurons27. This rescue approach results in Kiss1r-deleted male mice that have normal levels of circulating testosterone and fertility27,28. We have examined here the learning and anxiety traits of Kiss1r-null male mice in both the GDX and rescue models to explore whether kisspeptin may have a role in hippocampal function.
Results
Adult male 129S6 global Kiss1r−/− and littermate wild-type Kiss1r+/+ mice7 (Kiss1rtm1Coll, provided by Prof. William Colledge, University of Cambridge) were GDX three weeks before beginning experiments when 4–6 months old. Experimental mice were labelled as “GDX KISS1R-KO” and controls as “GDX control”. Adult male Kiss1r−/−; Tg+/− and littermate Tg+/− mice27 were used for experiments at 4–6 months of age. This was a 129P2 global Kiss1R-null mouse (Kiss1rtmGstn) crossed on to a C57BL/6 J line incorporating a transgene (Tg) in which the Gnrh1 promoter drives the expression of Kiss1R27,29 (available from AEH). Experimental mice were labelled “intact KISS1R-KO rescue” and controls as “intact controls”.
Elevated Plus Maze (EPM)
The number of times mice entered into the open arms during their 5 min in the EPM did not vary across the experimental groups; GDX control 7.0 ± 0.8, GDX KISS1R-KO 8.0 ± 0.8, intact control 7.1 ± 1.6, intact KISS1R-KO rescue 6.9 ± 0.8. However, the percentage of time spent in the different regions of the EPM was different for both GDX and intact experimental groups (Fig. 1A–C). The % of time spent in the open arms was significantly different across genotype (F(1,29) = 7.77, P = 0.009) and gonadal status (F(1,29) = 6.76, P = 0.015), with an interaction between the two (F(1,29) = 4.32, P = 0.046) and post-hoc tests showing that KISS1R-KO rescue mice spent approximately twice as much time in the open arms compared with controls (P = 0.008, Tukey’s post-hoc multiple comparisons) and all GDX mice (P < 0.01)(Fig. 1A). With regard to the % of time spent in the center of the EPM, a significant effect of gonadal status (F(1,29) = 20.47, P < 0.0001) and interaction between genotype and gonadal status (F(1,29) = 10.01, P = 0.003) was found. Post-hoc tests showed no differences between the two GDX groups or two intact groups but intact KISS1R-KO rescue mice spent significantly less time in the centre compared with both GDX groups (P < 0.05, Tukey’s post-hoc multiple comparisons)(Fig. 1B). For the % of time spent in the closed arms, a significant effect of genotype (F(1,29) = 10.39, P = 0.003) was identified with GDX KISS1R-KO mice spending less time in the closed arms compared with both GDX and intact controls (P < 0.05, Tukey’s post-hoc multiple comparisons)(Fig. 1C).
When compiled as percent event histograms for each group (Fig. 1D), the overall behaviour of GDX controls and intact controls appeared similar, whereas both KISS1R-KO mouse lines spent more time in the centre and open compartments of the EPM.
Open Field Test (OFT)
The average distance travelled by mice across all three trials did not vary between control and KISS1R-KO mice in either intact or GDX groups (Fig. 2A). However, as a group, intact mice were found to travel significantly further (~60%) than GDX animals (effect of gonadal status, F(1,29) = 17.04, P = 0.0003) with both GDX groups travelling less than both intact groups (P < 0.05, Tukey’s post-hoc multiple comparisons). When examined across the three trials, intact and GDX mice were found to travel the same distance as each other on Day 1 (F(1,29) = 4.02, P = 0.054), with intact mice travelling further than GDX animals on Days 2 (F(1,29) = 15.43, P = 0.0005) and Day 3 (F(1,29) = 21.78, P < 0.0001)(data not shown).
The number of crossings to the center quadrant also showed no differences between genotype (F(1,29) = 0.603, P = 0.444), but over two-fold more crosses in intact mice compared with GDX animals (F(1,29) = 23.3, P < 0.0001)(Fig. 2B). Both GDX groups made fewer crossing to the center quadrant than both intact groups (P < 0.05, Tukey’s post-hoc multiple comparisons) (Fig. 2B).
The % of time spent in the inner quadrant exhibited the same relationship with no differences between genotype (F(1,29) = 0.679, P = 0.417), but intact mice spending over 5-times longer in the inner quadrant compared to GDX mice (F(1,29) = 17.38, P = 0.0003) (Fig. 2C). The same relationship between intact and GDX mice was observed on each of the three trial days (data not shown).
Morris Water Maze (MWM)
All mice were found to learn the cued task at an equivalent rate (Fig. 3A) with no significant differences between genotype or gonadal status (P > 0.05 for all comparisons, repeated-measures 2-way ANOVA). Performance in the reference memory spatial learning task was also not found to be different between any groups. Swimming velocity was the same and distance travelled (Fig. 3B) did not differ depending on genotype or gonadal status (P > 0.05). Probe tests also failed to reveal any significant differences in swimming speed (Fig. 3C), the percentage of time in the target quadrant (Fig. 3D) or number of crossings over the training platform location (Fig. 3E)(P > 0.05 for all comparisons, 2-way ANOVA).
Discussion
We report here that KISS1R signaling in the male mouse is required for normal anxiety-like behavior in relation to the EPM. The full expression of this phenotype is dependent upon normal circulating levels of gonadal steroids. In addition, we find that gonadectomy has a potent anxiogenic effect on behavior in the OFT. No deficits in spatial learning were detected in relation to gonadectomy or the deletion of KISS1R signaling. Together, these observations suggest that kisspeptin and likely testosterone signaling can impact upon different neural circuitries mediating different aspects anxiety behavior in male mice.
Effect of gonadectomy on anxiety-like behavior in male mice
Studies in male rats have reported contradictory results regarding the effects of castration upon anxiety-like behaviors in the EPM. Whereas an early study described increased anxiety following gonadectomy30, others have found no effect or even decreased anxiety31–33. Although less has been done with mice, a similar situation exists where investigators have reported gonadectomy to increase anxiety-like behaviors34 or have little effect35,36. Studies with testicular feminized rats and mice, that have life-long dysfunctional androgen receptor signaling, have demonstrated increased aspects of anxiety in both species37,38. One common observation with rats and mice, is that the effects of adult castration are often dissimilar in different behavioral tests of anxiety33,34. For example, Benice and Raber reported that GDX male mice travelled less distance and spent less time in the center of the OFT, but that gonadectomy had no impact upon time in the open arms of an elevated zero maze34.
Our observations regarding the effects of gonadectomy on anxiety-like behaviors are essentially the same as those of Benice and Raber34. We also find that gonadectomy reduces the total distance travelled and time in the centre of the OFT, but has no significant impact upon the time spent in the open arms of the EPM. Discrepancies between measures of anxiety in the OFT and EPM are relatively common39,40 and likely arise from behavioural tests measuring different facets or dimensions of anxiety. Thus, gonadectomy, and presumably testosterone, appears to selectively impact upon anxiety circuitry modulating the impetus for male mice to explore and remain exposed in the open. As noted previously34,41, there appears to be no impact of gonadectomy on cued or spatial learning in the reference memory water maze task.
A role for KISS1R signaling in anxiety-like behavior
The major observation of this study is that intact KISS1R-KO rescue mice spend twice as much time in the open arms of the EPM compared to intact controls. Although a trend for reduced anxiety behavior was also evident in the OFT, this was not found to be statistically significant. This indicates that KISS1R-dependent signaling is normally facilitating the activity of anxiogenic neural circuits operative in the EPM. Interestingly, these circuits appear to be different to the anxiety networks addressed by testosterone as gonadectomy had a substantial impact upon performance in the OFT. The simplest interpretation of these observations would be that testosterone is involved in modulating anxiety circuits related to the fear of open spaces (OFT) whereas KISS1R signaling is important for anxiety related to the fear of heights (EPM). The possibility that these effects reflect strain differences seems unlikely as the C57BL/6 J and 129 strains perform very similarly in the EPM and OFT42. Compatible with the present results, a study in male rats has shown that intracerebroventricular administration of kisspeptin-13 causes anxiety in the EPM43.
We note that the anxiolytic phenotype of Kiss1r-null mice was only clearly observed in intact KISS1R-KO rescue mice. The GDX KISS1R-KO mice exhibited a similar but less robust phenomenon with less time spent in the closed arms and a trend to spend longer in the center and open arms combined. This may suggest that testosterone is involved in modulating KISS1R-dependent anxiety parameters in the EPM. More generally, this observation highlights the potential importance of normal levels of circulating gonadal steroids when interrogating kisspeptin-KISS1R signaling throughout the body.
The deletion of Kiss1r was found to have no effects upon performance in the Morris water maze. This indicates that there is no critical role for KISS1R signaling in brain circuits responsible for spatial learning and memory in male mice44. A recent study reported that infusion of kisspeptin-13 into the third ventricle ameliorated memory loss in amyloid beta-treated mice45. As this effect was inhibited by a GnRH receptor antagonist, it is possible that the memory changes in that study were indirect resulting from increases in circulating gonadal hormone levels secondary to kisspeptin activation of GnRH neurons.
A variety of limbic brain regions contribute to anxiety-related behaviors21 and it is conceivable that deleted KISS1R signaling in any of these areas could underlie the specific anxiolytic effects reported here in KISS1R-KO mice. However, of these various regions, it is only the granule cells of the hippocampal dentate gyrus that express Kiss1r at any appreciable level18. Recent hippocampal optogenetic studies have highlighted the importance of dorsal granule cells in the modulation of exploratory behavior and ventral granule cells in the regulation of anxiety46. As the EPM tests the conflict between exploration and fear of a potentially threatening environment, KISS1R signaling may be important in either or both the dorsal and ventral dentate. Although other ligands are possible for KISS1R13, it is notable that electrophysiological studies have shown that kisspeptin increases the excitability of granule cells in the dentate20. Thus, it is plausible that a potential reduction in granule cell excitability in KISS1R-KO mice may underlie their inclination to spend more time in the open arms of the EPM.
To our knowledge, the anxiety of the few humans identified with mutations in KISS1R has not been assessed. However, roles for kisspeptin outside the human hypothalamus have been suggested recently with the finding that the peripheral administration of kisspeptin can alter limbic brain activity, including the hippocampus, in relation to sexual and emotional stimuli47. Given our present observation of a specific anxiety deficit in Kiss1r -null male mice, it may be important to consider the possibility of effects of kisspeptin on anxiety when designing new therapies for treating reproductive disorders in the clinic.
Materials and Methods
Adult male 129S6 global Kiss1r−/− (GDX KISS1R-KO) and littermate Kiss1r+/+ (GDX control) mice7 were GDX three weeks before beginning experiments when 4–6 months old. Adult male 129P2/C57BL/6 J Kiss1r−/−; Tg+/− (KISS1R-KO rescue) and littermate Tg+/− (control) mice27 were used for experiments at 4–6 months of age. Littermate null and control mice for each experiment were housed together three to four mice per cage under a 12 h light-dark cycle (lights on 19:00) at 20 ± 1 °C with 40 ± 5% humidity, and food and water available ad libitum. Experiments were undertaken during the animals’ dark phase under sodium lighting in the University of Otago Behavioural Phenotyping Unit. All experimentation was approved by the University of Otago Ethics Committee and undertaken in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.
Mice (GDX control, N = 9; GDX KISS1R-KO, N = 7; control, N = 8; KISS1R-KO rescue, N = 9) were tested by an investigator blind to the experimental group and genotype. Mice were habituated to the investigator by daily handling for four days prior to behavioral testing. This consisted of one trial in the EPM (day 1), three trials in an OFT (days 1–3) and testing in a MWM (days 4–10). Mice were tracked and analysed using an overhead camera and the TopScan behavioural analysis system (Clever Sys Inc., Washington, USA).
EPM
The maze consisted of four arms (35 cm long, 5 cm wide) extending out from a central square (5 × 5 cm) with two arms having 20 cm high walls. Mice were placed in the central square facing an open arm and their activity tracked for 5 min. The number of times a mouse entered fully (whole body) into an open or closed arm and the % of time a mouse spent in the closed, centre, and open arms of the maze were measured.
OFT
Each mouse was tested in the OFT after completing the EPM on Day 1 and then tested again on Days 2 and 3. The open field was rectangular open box (40 cm × 40 cm with 20 cm high walls) with the mouse placed in the centre of the open field and its activity tracked for 10 min. The total distance travelled, the number of crossings into the middle portion of the arena (center quadrant, 24 × 24 cm), and percentage of time spent in the center quadrant were measured.
MWM
The day after completing the OFT, mice were tested in a circular water maze 100 cm in diameter and 35 cm deep maintained at 22 °C. A rectangular platform (6 cm diameter) was placed 1 cm above or below the surface of the water in various locations within the pool. Cued water maze training (Day 4) was undertaken by placing a blue flag on the platform located above the water in the middle of the maze and placing mice with their nose to the wall of each of the four quadrants. Each trial lasted until the mouse had located the platform or until 60 s had elapsed at which time mouse was placed on the platform for 10 s. Two trials were undertaken in the morning and a further two trials in the afternoon. The time taken for mice to locate the platform on each trial was determined. Spatial learning and memory (Days 5–9) was tested over the next 5 days by locating the platform below the water and without a flag in the middle of the north-east (NE) quadrant. Mice were then placed in the pool in each of the quadrants as above and this was repeated on 5 consecutive days. The swimming velocity and distance required for mice to reach the platform was measured. Probe tests were undertaken on Days 7 and 9 before the spatial learning test by removing the platform from the pool and placing the mice with their nose to wall into the south-west quadrant. Mice were allowed to swim freely for 60 s and their swim velocity, number of crossing over the previous location of the platform, and % time spent in the NE quadrant measured.
Statistical Analysis: Comparisons across the four experimental groups were assessed with normal or repeated measures 2-way ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc multiple comparison tests where required. Statistical analysis was performed with GraphPad Prism 7 software.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Prof. Cliff Abraham, Dept. Psychology, University of Otago for reviewing an early version of the manuscript. These studies were supported by the New Zealand Health Research Council.
Author Contributions
The study was designed by A.E.H. and D.B., experiments undertaken by S.D. and R.P., data analyzed by S.D., D.H.B. and A.E.H. and the paper written by A.E.H.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Lee JH, et al. KiSS-1, a novel human malignant melanoma metastasis-suppressor gene. J Natl Cancer Inst. 1996;88:1731–1737. doi: 10.1093/jnci/88.23.1731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kotani M, et al. The metastasis suppressor gene KiSS-1 encodes kisspeptins, the natural ligands of the orphan G protein-coupled receptor GPR54. J Biol Chem. 2001;276:34631–34636. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M104847200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ohtaki T, et al. Metastasis suppressor gene KiSS-1 encodes peptide ligand of a G-protein-coupled receptor. Nature. 2001;411:613–617. doi: 10.1038/35079135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Muir AI, et al. AXOR12, a novel human G protein-coupled receptor, activated by the peptide KiSS-1. J Biol Chem. 2001;276:28969–28975. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M102743200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gottsch ML, Clifton DK, Steiner RA. From KISS1 to kisspeptins: An historical perspective and suggested nomenclature. Peptides. 2009;30:4–9. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2008.06.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.de Roux N, et al. Hypogonadotropic hypogonadism due to loss of function of the KiSS1-derived peptide receptor GPR54. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:10972–10976. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1834399100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Seminara SB, et al. The GPR54 gene as a regulator of puberty. N Engl J Med. 2003;349:1614–1627. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa035322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Popa SM, Clifton DK, Steiner RA. The role of kisspeptins and GPR54 in the neuroendocrine regulation of reproduction. Annu Rev Physiol. 2008;70:213–238. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.70.113006.100540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lehman MN, Coolen LM, Goodman RL. Minireview: Kisspeptin/Neurokinin B/Dynorphin (KNDy) cells of the arcuate nucleus: A central node in the control of gonadotropin-releasing hormone secretion. Endocrinology. 2010;151:3479–3489. doi: 10.1210/en.2010-0022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Herbison AE. Control of puberty onset and fertility by gonadotropin-releasing hormone neurons. Nature reviews. Endocrinology. 2016;12:452–466. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2016.70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hussain MA, Song WJ, Wolfe A. There is Kisspeptin - And Then There is Kisspeptin. Trends Endocrinol Metab. 2015;26:564–572. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2015.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bhattacharya M, Babwah AV. Kisspeptin: beyond the brain. Endocrinology. 2015;156:1218–1227. doi: 10.1210/en.2014-1915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Liu X, Herbison AE. Kisspeptin Regulation of Neuronal Activity throughout the Central Nervous System. Endocrinol Metab (Seoul) 2016;31:193–205. doi: 10.3803/EnM.2016.31.2.193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Mikkelsen JD, Simonneaux V. The neuroanatomy of the kisspeptin system in the mammalian brain. Peptides. 2009;30:26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2008.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Clarkson J, d’Anglemont de Tassigny X, Colledge WH, Caraty A, Herbison AE. Distribution of kisspeptin neurones in the adult female mouse brain. J Neuroendocrinol. 2009;21:673–682. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2009.01892.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Desroziers E, et al. Mapping of kisspeptin fibres in the brain of the pro-oestrous rat. J Neuroendocrinol. 2010;22:1101–1112. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2010.02053.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lee DK, et al. Discovery of a receptor related to the galanin receptors. FEBS Lett. 1999;446:103–107. doi: 10.1016/S0014-5793(99)00009-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Herbison AE, de Tassigny X, Doran J, Colledge WH. Distribution and postnatal development of Gpr54 gene expression in mouse brain and gonadotropin-releasing hormone neurons. Endocrinology. 2010;151:312–321. doi: 10.1210/en.2009-0552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Higo, S., Honda, S., Iijima, N. & Ozawa, H. Mapping of Kisspeptin Receptor mRNA in the Whole Rat Brain and its Co-Localisation with Oxytocin in the Paraventricular Nucleus. J Neuroendocrinol28 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 20.Arai AC, et al. Cancer metastasis-suppressing peptide metastin upregulates excitatory synaptic transmission in hippocampal dentate granule cells. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:3648–3652. doi: 10.1152/jn.00590.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Allsop SA, Vander Weele CM, Wichmann R, Tye KM. Optogenetic insights on the relationship between anxiety-related behaviors and social deficits. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:241. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Buzsaki G, Moser EI. Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:130–138. doi: 10.1038/nn.3304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Colledge WH. Transgenic mouse models to study Gpr54/kisspeptin physiology. Peptides. 2009;30:34–41. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2008.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Aikey JL, Nyby JG, Anmuth DM, James PJ. Testosterone rapidly reduces anxiety in male house mice (Mus musculus) Hormones and behavior. 2002;42:448–460. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2002.1838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Chen CV, Brummet JL, Lonstein JS, Jordan CL, Breedlove SM. New knockout model confirms a role for androgen receptors in regulating anxiety-like behaviors and HPA response in mice. Hormones and behavior. 2014;65:211–218. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Tolson KP, et al. Impaired kisspeptin signaling decreases metabolism and promotes glucose intolerance and obesity. The Journal of clinical investigation. 2014;124:3075–3079. doi: 10.1172/JCI71075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kirilov M, et al. Dependence of fertility on kisspeptin-Gpr54 signaling at the GnRH neuron. Nature Commun. 2013;4:2492. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Leon S, et al. Direct Actions of Kisspeptins on GnRH Neurons Permit Attainment of Fertility but are Insufficient to Fully Preserve Gonadotropic Axis Activity. Scientific reports. 2016;6:19206. doi: 10.1038/srep19206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Funes S, et al. The KiSS-1 receptor GPR54 is essential for the development of the murine reproductive system. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2003;312:1357–1363. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2003.11.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Frye CA, Seliga AM. Testosterone increases analgesia, anxiolysis, and cognitive performance of male rats. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2001;1:371–381. doi: 10.3758/CABN.1.4.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zimmerberg B, Farley MJ. Sex differences in anxiety behavior in rats: role of gonadal hormones. Physiol Behav. 1993;54:1119–1124. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90335-D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Naslund J, Studer E, Johansson E, Eriksson E. Effects of gonadectomy and serotonin depletion on inter-individual differences in anxiety-like behaviour in male Wistar rats. Behav Brain Res. 2016;308:160–165. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Domonkos E, et al. Sex differences and sex hormones in anxiety-like behavior of aging rats. Hormones and behavior. 2017;93:159–165. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Benice TS, Raber J. Dihydrotestosterone modulates spatial working-memory performance in male mice. J Neurochem. 2009;110:902–911. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2009.06183.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.McDermott CM, Liu D, Schrader LA. Role of gonadal hormones in anxiety and fear memory formation and inhibition in male mice. Physiol Behav. 2012;105:1168–1174. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.12.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Nikmahzar E, et al. Hippocampal serotonin-2A receptor-immunoreactive neurons density increases after testosterone therapy in the gonadectomized male mice. Anat Cell Biol. 2016;49:259–272. doi: 10.5115/acb.2016.49.4.259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Zuloaga DG, Morris JA, Jordan CL, Breedlove SM. Mice with the testicular feminization mutation demonstrate a role for androgen receptors in the regulation of anxiety-related behaviors and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Hormones and behavior. 2008;54:758–766. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Zuloaga DG, Poort JE, Jordan CL, Breedlove SM. Male rats with the testicular feminization mutation of the androgen receptor display elevated anxiety-related behavior and corticosterone response to mild stress. Hormones and behavior. 2011;60:380–388. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Rizk A, Curley J, Robertson J, Raber J. Anxiety and cognition in histamine H3 receptor−/− mice. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;19:1992–1996. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03251.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Benice TS, Rizk A, Kohama S, Pfankuch T, Raber J. Sex-differences in age-related cognitive decline in C57BL/6J mice associated with increased brain microtubule-associated protein 2 and synaptophysin immunoreactivity. Neuroscience. 2006;137:413–423. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Spritzer MD, Gill M, Weinberg A, Galea LA. Castration differentially affects spatial working and reference memory in male rats. Arch Sex Behav. 2008;37:19–29. doi: 10.1007/s10508-007-9264-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.O’Leary TP, Gunn RK, Brown RE. What are we measuring when we test strain differences in anxiety in mice? Behav Genet. 2013;43:34–50. doi: 10.1007/s10519-012-9572-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Csabafi K, Jaszberenyi M, Bagosi Z, Liptak N, Telegdy G. Effects of kisspeptin-13 on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, thermoregulation, anxiety and locomotor activity in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2013;241:56–61. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.11.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Vorhees CV, Williams MT. Value of water mazes for assessing spatial and egocentric learning and memory in rodent basic research and regulatory studies. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2014;45:75–90. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2014.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Jiang JH, et al. Kisspeptin-13 enhances memory and mitigates memory impairment induced by Abeta1-42 in mice novel object and object location recognition tasks. Neurobiology of learning and memory. 2015;123:187–195. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kheirbek MA, et al. Differential control of learning and anxiety along the dorsoventral axis of the dentate gyrus. Neuron. 2013;77:955–968. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Comninos AN, et al. Kisspeptin modulates sexual and emotional brain processing in humans. The Journal of clinical investigation. 2017;127:709–719. doi: 10.1172/JCI89519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.