Abstract
Numerical cognition relies on interactions within and between multiple functional brain systems, including those subserving quantity processing, working memory, declarative memory, and cognitive control. This chapter describes recent advances in our understanding of memory and control circuits in mathematical cognition and learning. The working memory system involves multiple parietal–frontal circuits which create short-term representations that allow manipulation of discrete quantities over several seconds. In contrast, hippocampal–frontal circuits underlying the declarative memory system play an important role in formation of associative memories and binding of new and old information, leading to the formation of long-term memories that allow generalization beyond individual problem attributes. The flow of information across these systems is regulated by flexible cognitive control systems which facilitate the integration and manipulation of quantity and mnemonic information. The implications of recent research for formulating a more comprehensive systems neuroscience view of the neural basis of mathematical learning and knowledge acquisition in both children and adults are discussed.
Keywords: Memory, Cognitive, Control, Prefrontal, Parietal, Hippocampus, Numerical, Problem solving, Circuits, Function
1 INTRODUCTION
Knowledge of numerical magnitude and manipulations of symbolic and nonsymbolic quantity (number sense) are critical building blocks from which all mathematical knowledge is constructed. These basic building blocks rely on visual and auditory association cortices which help decode the visual form and phonological features of numerical stimuli, and the parietal attention system (Dehaene et al., 2003) which helps to build semantic representations of quantity (Ansari, 2008) from visuo-spatial primitives including object identification, spatial attention, eye gaze, and pointing (Simon et al., 2002). With increased proficiency, these functions are sub-served by a “core” visuospatial number system anchored in the fusiform gyrus (FG) and intraparietal sulcus (IPS).
The IPS subdivision of the posterior parietal cortex has been the focus of most neurobiological research on mathematical cognition and learning (Ansari, 2008; Butterworth, 1999; Dehaene et al., 2003). The IPS and FG form core building blocks from which number form and quantity representations are constructed in the brain (Ansari, 2008). But these regions do not function in isolation. They receive input from multiple brain regions and send outputs to several others. Furthermore, the development of core systems is supported by its engagement with multiple brain systems. Recent research is beginning to emphasize a multisystem approach (Arsalidou and Taylor, 2011; Fias et al., 2013; Qin et al., 2014). Multiple distributed neural processes involved in number form, magnitude and quantity representations, working memory, and declarative memory have been identified as being important for numerical problem solving and mathematical learning (Fig. 1). This chapter synthesizes emerging findings on multiple memory and cognitive control systems which play a critical, but heretofore underappreciated, role in mathematical cognition in adults, as well as in scaffolding children’s mathematics learning and skill development.
We focus on two memory systems—working memory and declarative memory— that play distinct roles in mathematical cognition and learning. The working memory system anchored in parietal–frontal circuits creates short-term representations that allow manipulation of multiple discrete quantities over several seconds. In contrast, declarative memory systems anchored in hippocampal–frontal circuits play an important role in formation of associative memories and binding of new and old information, contributing to long-term memory and generalization beyond individual problem attributes. Both these processes require flexible integration of functional circuits anchored in prefrontal cognitive control systems. We describe cognitive control systems which guide allocation of attention resources and retrieval of facts from memory in the service of goal-directed numerical problem solving.
The roles of the two memory systems are greatly amplified during key developmental stages of learning, and their involvement in mathematical cognition and learning is best studied in the context of cognitive development. There is now growing evidence to suggest that functional circuits engaged by children are not the same as those engaged by adults who have evolved multiple strategies for learning and cognitive skill acquisition. Studies comparing children with adults are therefore likely to be insensitive to major changes that occur during specific stages of development. A number of scaffolding systems are likely to be engaged during development, and new studies are beginning to investigate the role of parietal–frontal and hippocampal–frontal circuits that might otherwise be missed in studies involving adults. The precise nature of this engagement is a function of developmental stage, domain knowledge, problem complexity, and individual proficiency in use of efficient problem-solving strategies. Accordingly, this review has a strong focus on both typical and atypical neurodevelopmental processes associated with the two memory systems and their associated prefrontal cognitive control systems.
The next sections are organized as follows. In Section 2, we first consider the relation between core systems and working memory and describe multiple parietal–frontal working memory circuits anchored in different subdivisions of the posterior parietal cortex, with a specific focus on functional circuits associated with the IPS and supramarginal gyrus. The role of parietal–frontal working memory circuits in the typical and atypical development of mathematical cognition is then discussed. In Section 3, we turn to the declarative memory system highlighting key hippocampal–prefrontal circuits. Emerging findings on the role of the hippocampal memory system in mathematical learning and development are then described. In Section 4, we consider how distinct prefrontal cortex (PFC) control systems facilitate mathematical cognition and learning. Section 5 summarizes the main points of this review. This chapter builds on related topical reviews (Menon, 2015, 2016) and attempts an integrative view of distinct, but overlapping, memory and cognitive control circuits involved in mathematical cognition and learning.
2 PARIETAL–FRONTAL WORKING MEMORY SYSTEMS
2.1 CORE AND NONCORE PARIETAL SYSTEMS OVERLAP IN THE IPS
Parietal–frontal circuits play a prominent role in mathematical cognition. Functional neuroimaging research has revealed significant overlap in multiple parietal and prefrontal cortical regions involved in working memory and numerical problem solving (Arsalidou and Taylor, 2011; Metcalfe et al., 2013; Rottschy et al., 2012). Common patterns of coactivation have most prominently been detected in the IPS, supramarginal gyrus, premotor cortex, and ventral and dorsal aspects of the lateral PFC.
A critical locus of intersection between “core” number system and “noncore” working memory systems is the IPS, a region important for representing and manipulating numerical quantity. It is now well known that “core” IPS regions implicated in quantity and numerosity judgement are also involved in a broader class of cognitive functions, including sequential ordering and manipulation of working memory contents for nonnumerical stimuli, as shown in several elegant studies by Fias and colleagues (Van Opstal et al., 2009). Furthermore, these IPS regions are also integral to short-term visual WM for object locations in space over a period lasting 1–2 s (Luck and Vogel, 2013). Thus, multiple lines of evidence suggest that parietal systems for numerical quantity processing and working memory show a prominent overlap in the parietal cortex. The crucial point these examples illustrate is that the distinction between “core” quantity and “noncore” working memory systems is not functionally segregated and that they draw on similar mechanisms for dynamic manipulation of representations over a timescale of several seconds.
2.2 MULTIPLE PARIETAL–FRONTAL WORKING MEMORY CIRCUITS
Analysis of intrinsic functional circuits associated with the posterior parietal cortex hints at multiple parietal–frontal working memory-related circuits involved in mathematical cognition. The three distinct subdivisions of the inferior parietal cortex, the IPS, supramarginal gyrus, and angular gyrus are associated with distinct but overlapping parietal–frontal circuits (Fig. 2). These circuits contribute to different aspects of mathematical cognition and learning by virtue of their differential large-scale functional organization. The IPS is part of an intrinsically connected parietal–frontal system that includes the frontal eye fields, supplementary motor area, anterior insula, and ventrolateral PFC (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Corbetta et al., 2008; Menon and Uddin, 2010; Supekar and Menon, 2012; Uddin et al., 2010a). A key distinguishing feature of the IPS is its connectivity with the frontal eye field: the IPS is more strongly connected with this region than the supramarginal gyrus (Uddin et al., 2010a). In contrast, the supramarginal gyrus is more tightly linked to the dorsolateral PFC, together with which it forms the canonical parietal–frontal central executive network (Bressler and Menon, 2010). The IPS also shows a distinct pattern of connectivity from the adjoining angular gyrus. The angular gyrus is strongly connected with ventromedial PFC and posterior cingulate regions, comprising the default mode network (Greicius et al., 2003, 2004; Raichle et al., 2001), a system with no direct involvement in working memory. Thus, the IPS and supramarginal gyrus form distinct parietal–frontal working memory-related circuits.
It should be noted that the IPS itself is not a homogeneous structure. Cytoarchitectonic maps obtained from postmortem brains suggest that the human IPS has a more finely grained parcellation than previously suggested by the classical Brodmann map (Caspers et al., 2008). While all three subdivisions identified to date have strong links to the frontal eye field, their large-scale connectivity patterns can also be dissociated. The anterior-most IPS subdivisions (hIP2 and hIP1) are linked with ventral premotor cortex, anterior insula, and dorsolateral PFC, while the posterior-most IPS subdivision (hIP3) has higher connectivity with extrastriate visual areas (Fig. 2). This functional connectivity profile suggests a strong coupling between the anterior IPS and PFC control regions. In sharp contrast, the posterior IPS region shows strong coupling with posterior occipital regions. Thus, connectivity analyses of networks associated with the IPS suggest a general principle of organization whereby posterior IPS regions that are closely linked to the visual system translate stimuli into motor action through anterior IPS connections with the PFC.
A key point suggested by extant findings is that IPS and supramarginal gyrus involved in mathematical cognition form overlapping but segregated working memory circuits. Precisely how these circuits work to serve the dual purpose of segregation and integration of core and noncore parietal systems involved in mathematical cognition and learning is less clear. A crucial hint comes from their distinct functional roles. Unlike the IPS, the supramarginal gyrus is not critically involved in quantity judgement per se, but it is strongly engaged during numerical problem-solving tasks that require active storage and manipulation of the contents of working memory. It is likely that the supramarginal gyrus provides a more general mechanism for manipulating the contents of working memory in a more flexible and context-dependent manner over an extended period of time, thus freeing up “core” IPS resources necessary for quantity processing.
2.3 PARIETAL–FRONTAL WORKING MEMORY SYSTEMS IN MATHEMATICAL COGNITION AND ITS DEVELOPMENT
The particular emphasis on parietal–frontal working memory systems in mathematical cognition is most prominent in developmental studies. This has origins in children’s immature problem-solving abilities which require them to break down numerical problems into more basic components. The use of such strategies requires greater reliance on working memory systems for problem solving in children. For example, children rely more on counting strategies during simple arithmetic problem solving and need to access multiple working memory components including short-term storage, rule-based manipulation, and updating of the stored contents. With increased proficiency and a shift to fact retrieval strategies, there is less demand and need for working memory resources (Bailey et al., 2012; Geary et al., 2007a). Consistent with this view, neuroimaging studies in typical and atypical development have provided strong evidence for coactivation of parietal and prefrontal regions that overlap with parietal–frontal circuits highlighted in the previous section.
The involvement of working memory in mathematical cognition had initially been surmised based on overlapping responses in parietal and PFC in the two domains (Cantlon et al., 2009; Grabner et al., 2009; Ischebeck et al., 2007; Rivera et al., 2005). Studies of typical development provided initial evidence for the changing role of working memory with age. For example, Rivera and colleagues found that relative to adults, children tend to engage the posterior parietal cortex less, and the PFC more, when solving arithmetic problems, likely reflecting the increased role of visuospatial processing and the concurrent decrease in demands on cognitive control. Other studies have more directly addressed the link between working memory abilities and numerical problem-solving skills. Dumontheil and Klingberg (2012) found that IPS activity during a visuospatial working memory task predicted arithmetic performance 2 years later in a sample of 6–16-year-old children and adolescents. This finding further reinforced the role of parietal–frontal working memory systems in mathematical cognition and pointed to the overlap between core and noncore functional systems in the IPS.
More detailed analyses of the neural correlates of individual components of working memory have provided evidence for the fractionation of neurofunctional systems associated with distinct working memory components during numerical problem solving (Arsalidou and Taylor, 2011; Metcalfe et al., 2013; Rottschy et al., 2012). Analysis of the relation between the central executive, phonological and visuospatial components of working memory, and brain activation during an arithmetic verification task in a large group of 7–9-year-old children revealed that visuospatial working memory is a strong predictor of mathematical ability in children in this age group and is associated with increased problem complexity-related responses in left dorsolateral and right ventrolateral PFC as well as in the bilateral IPS and supramarginal gyrus. Metcalfe and colleagues also found that visuospatial working memory and the central executive component were associated with largely distinct patterns of brain responses during arithmetic problem solving, and overlap was only observed in the ventral aspects of the left supramarginal gyrus, suggesting that this region is an important locus for the integration of information in working memory during numerical problem solving in children (Ansari, 2008; Dehaene et al., 2003; Kawashima et al., 2004; Kucian et al., 2008; Menon et al., 2000; Rivera et al., 2005; Rosenberg-Lee et al., 2014).
Finally, analysis of intrinsic functional connectivity suggests that a network of prefrontal cortical areas supports the longitudinal development of numerical abilities. Intrinsic functional connectivity between the IPS and dorsolateral and ventrolateral PFC predicted longitudinal gains in numerical problem-solving abilities over a 6-year period in children (Evans et al., 2015). These findings further confirm the pivotal role of overlapping parietal–frontal circuits in children’s mathematical skill development.
2.4 PARIETAL–FRONTAL IMPAIRMENTS IN CHILDREN WITH MATHEMATICAL DISABILITIES
Although mathematical disability (MD) was initially conceptualized as a disorder of a single brain region characterized by a localized deficit in the IPS (Cohen Kadosh et al., 2007; Isaacs et al., 2001; Price et al., 2007), more recently, prominent neurocognitive models of MD have posited that the disorder stems from more extensive functional aberrations in a distributed network of brain areas encompassing not only posterior parietal, but also prefrontal, as well as ventral temporal–occipital cortices that are known to serve multiple cognitive functions necessary for successful numerical problem solving. Studies have variably reported aberrant responses in the IPS, supramarginal gyrus, and multiple prefrontal cortical areas implicated in working memory (Butterworth et al., 2011; Davis et al., 2009; Iuculano et al., 2015; Kaufmann et al., 2009a; Kucian and von Aster, 2015; Kucian et al., 2006, 2011; Menon, 2014; Price et al., 2007; Rosenberg-Lee et al., 2014).
Behavioral studies have shown that disruptions to working memory are a prominent factor contributing to persistent deficits in arithmetic problem solving in children with MD (Geary et al., 2007b). Evidence to date suggests that visuospatial working memory is a specific source of vulnerability in symbolic numerical calculation deficits and thus needs to be seriously considered as a key component in neurobiological and developmental models of typical and atypical mathematical skill acquisition (Ashkenazi et al., 2013). Rotzer et al. (2009) found that compared to typically developing children, children with low math abilities had lower visuospatial abilities and lower activity levels in the right anterior IPS, ventrolateral PFC, and insular cortex during a visuospatial working memory task. In a different vein, Ashkenazi et al. (2013) identified impaired working memory components in children with MD and then examined their role in modulating brain responses to numerical problem solving. Children with MD had specific deficits in visuospatial working memory in addition to deficits in arithmetic task performance, even though they were matched on IQ and verbal abilities to their typically developing peers. Crucially, activations in IPS, and dorsolateral and ventrolateral PFC are positively correlated with visuospatial working memory ability in typically developing children, but no such relation was seen in children with MD. This result suggests that children with MD fail to appropriately exploit parietal–frontal working memory resources during problem solving.
2.5 HYPERACTIVE PARIETAL–FRONTAL WORKING MEMORY CIRCUITS IN CHILDREN WITH MD
While previous studies have focused on regional profiles of deficits, understanding cognitive deficits requires knowledge not only about aberrations in localized patterns of brain responses but also distributed functional circuits that might be impaired or organized in unusual ways (Uddin et al., 2010b). This is particularly true for numerical problem solving which typically requires the coordinated interaction of multiple brain regions (Rosenberg-Lee et al., 2011; Varma and Schwartz, 2008). A few recent studies have begun to probe parietal–frontal circuits in children with MD.
Analysis of both task and task-free data suggests that parietal–frontal working memory circuits are impaired in children with MD. Surprisingly, contrary to what might be predicted, relative to typically developing children, children with MD show hyperconnectivity of the IPS, rather than reduced connectivity, with several cortical areas (Rosenberg-Lee et al., 2014). Hyperconnectivity of the IPS with multiple regions in lateral PFC and parietal regions is most prominent in affected children. These regions include the bilateral ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC and the supramarginal gyrus. This pattern of hyperconnectivity was observed during both a simpler addition task and a more cognitively challenging subtraction task (Fig. 3). Thus, children with MD engage multiple parietal–frontal working memory circuits differently from their typically developing peers—they require greater engagement of these circuits despite lower performance levels. The greater engagement of these circuits may arise from the activation of problem-irrelevant information that in turn disrupts problem solving. This view is consistent with behavioral studies that show the intrusion of problem-irrelevant information into working memory when children with MD attempt to retrieve arithmetic answers from long-term memory (Barrouillet et al., 1997; Geary et al., 2000, 2012). Further studies are needed to disentangle intrusive vs compensatory effects in this complex pattern of hyperconnectivity in parietal–frontal working memory circuits.
Notably, this pattern of hyperconnectivity is also manifest in intrinsic functional circuits (Fig. 4). Compared to their typically developing peers, children with MD show aberrant IPS connectivity with multiple prefrontal and parietal regions (Jolles et al., 2016). Specifically, children with MD show greater functional connectivity between left and right IPS, as well as between IPS and dorsolateral and ventrolateral PFC. It is plausible that intrinsic hyperconnectivity in these parietal–frontal circuits in children with MD may underlie the increased activation and connectivity of these regions reported in several studies of numerical problem solving (Iuculano et al., 2015; Kaufmann et al., 2009a,b, 2011; Rosenberg-Lee et al., 2014). Interestingly, however, not all studies have found greater parietal–frontal activation in children with MD, but discrepancies between studies may be explained by the type of baseline conditions used. Specifically, it has been suggested that MD is characterized by reduced modulation of brain responses with increasing task complexity, rather than reduced activation per se (Ashkenazi et al., 2012). For example, children with MD show reduced activation in parietal–frontal regions for small vs large distances in number comparison tasks (Mussolin et al., 2010; Price et al., 2007; but see Kucian, 2011), and for complex vs simple problems in arithmetic tasks (Ashkenazi et al., 2012). Finally, aberrant connectivity within these parietal–frontal areas in children with MD is consistent with deficits in spatial attention that have been reported in domains outside mathematical problem solving (Ashkenazi and Henik, 2010; Szucs et al., 2013).
An important question for future research is how aberrant parietal circuits impact the ability to modulate parietal–frontal responses in a context-specific manner and how this in turn influences skill development and learning.
3 HIPPOCAMPAL–FRONTAL DECLARATIVE MEMORY SYSTEM
3.1 THE MEDIAL TEMPORAL LOBE: A SYSTEM FOR ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING
Over the past few years, evidence has been accumulating for the differential involvement of the declarative memory system in mathematical learning, especially during key stages of skill acquisition in children (Qin et al., 2014). The importance of the medial temporal lobe, particularly its hippocampal subdivision, in learning and memory for events in space and time is well known (Davachi, 2006; Davachi et al., 2003; Diana et al., 2007; Eichenbaum et al., 2007; Tulving, 1983). Theories of memory consolidation posit that the hippocampus plays an important role in the early stages of learning and retrieval, but its involvement decreases over time with concomitant increase in reliance on neocortical memory systems (Eichenbaum et al., 2007). This might explain why despite its critical role in learning and memory formation, hippocampal contributions to mathematics learning and cognitive development more broadly have received little attention until recently.
3.2 HIPPOCAMPAL–FRONTAL CORTEX CIRCUITS
The hippocampus forms the structural core of the declarative memory system. Research over the past two decades has clarified the specific roles of its functional subdivisions in different aspects of encoding and retrieval of novel information (Kumaran et al., 2009; Schacter et al., 1998, 2007; Tulving, 1983, 2002). The hippocampus and its associated functional circuits play an important role in memory encoding and retrieval in both children and adults (Ghetti et al., 2010; Menon et al., 2005; Ofen et al., 2007). The hippocampus is thought to contribute to declarative memory through binding inputs from multiple cortical areas (Davachi, 2006; Eichenbaum, 2004; Eichenbaum et al., 2007), while its functional interactions with the PFC are thought to facilitate memory formation and retrieval through cognitive control processes acting on the contents of memory (Qin et al., 2007, 2009, 2011a,b). Declarative memory relies on the coordinated interactions of distributed brain areas, most prominently, the hippocampus and the PFC (Diekelmann et al., 2009; Frankland and Bontempi, 2005; McGaugh, 2000; Norman and O’Reilly, 2003; Qin et al., 2011a; Simons and Spiers, 2003). These studies suggest that newly acquired memories are strongly dependent on the hippocampus and its interactions with the PFC, and become increasingly independent of the hippocampus and MTL over time.
3.3 HIPPOCAMPAL–PREFRONTAL COACTIVATION IN CHILDREN’S MATHEMATICAL SKILL DEVELOPMENT
The first evidence for the differential engagement of the hippocampal memory system in arithmetic skill acquisition came from a cross-sectional study in children, adolescents, and adults who ranged in age from 8 to 19 (Rivera et al., 2005). Importantly, children exhibited significantly greater engagement of multiple medial temporal lobe regions including the hippocampus. Similarly, De Smedt et al. (2011) found greater hippocampal response in children compared to adults when solving addition problems; hippocampal activation was not detected for subtraction problems which are less well rehearsed and more difficult to memorize because subtraction problems are not commutative. These findings highlight the dynamic role of the hippocampus in the maturation of memory-based problem-solving strategies and its greater engagement in childhood followed by decreased involvement in adolescence and adulthood (Fig. 5).
As noted earlier, children’s gains in problem-solving skills during the elementary school years are characterized by the gradual replacement of inefficient procedural strategies with direct retrieval of domain-relevant facts (Cho et al., 2011; Geary, 2011; Geary and Brown, 1991; Geary and Hoard, 2003). Cho et al. (2012) examined neurodevelopmental changes related to increased use of retrieval strategies and found that higher retrieval fluency was associated with greater response in multiple brain regions, including the hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus subdivisions of the medial temporal lobe. Thus, children’s use of retrieval strategies, far from being idiosyncratic, is in fact associated with a predictable profile of hippocampal responses. A related study found that retrieval and counting strategies were associated with different activation patterns in hippocampal regions important for memory encoding and retrieval, including bilateral hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus (Cho et al., 2011, 2012). The existence of decodable fine-scale pattern differences in fMRI signals suggests not only that the hippocampus is differentially engaged in relation to retrieval but also that the underlying neural resources are accessed and used differently in each strategy.
Qin and colleagues further investigated the transition from procedure-based to memory-based problem-solving strategies using longitudinal fMRI data from 7- to 9-year-old children (Qin et al., 2014). Children’s transition from counting to memory-based retrieval strategies over a 1.2-year interval was mediated by increased hippocampal activation and decreased parietal–frontal engagement. Following an initial increase in hippocampal engagement during middle childhood, this hippocampal dependency decreased during adolescence and adulthood despite further improvements in memory-based problem solving. This pattern of initial increase and subsequent decrease in activation provides novel support for models of long-term memory consolidation which posit that the hippocampus plays a time-limited role in the early phase of knowledge acquisition (McClelland et al., 1995; Tse et al., 2007). Consistent with this pattern of developmental change, previous studies in adults have reported no reliable hippocampal engagement during arithmetic tasks. Thus, the hippocampal system is critical to children’s early learning of arithmetic facts (Cho et al., 2011, 2012; De Smedt et al., 2011), while retrieval is largely dependent on the neocortex in adults (Dehaene et al., 2003; Menon, 2014).
3.4 HIPPOCAMPAL–FRONTAL CIRCUITS IN CHILDREN’S MATHEMATICAL SKILL DEVELOPMENT AND LEARNING
Dynamic coordination between the hippocampus and PFC plays an important role in memory formation (Norman and O’Reilly, 2003; Qin et al., 2011a; Simons and Spiers, 2003), and connectivity analyses are beginning to shed light on how the hippocampus and PFC interact to support memory formation for arithmetic facts. Analysis of task-related hippocampus connectivity has identified distributed functional circuits associated with retrieval fluency. In particular, right hippocampal connectivity with bilateral ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC is strongly correlated with retrieval fluency (Cho et al., 2012). Analysis of longitudinal data has further clarified the relation between hippocampal–prefrontal circuits and individual differences in children’s mathematical skill development. In particular, the shift from counting to memory-based retrieval strategy and increased hippocampal activation is accompanied by decreased parietal–frontal engagement. Longitudinal improvements in retrieval fluency are best predicted by increased functional connectivity in hippocampal–neocortical circuits (Qin et al., 2014). Increased hippocampal functional coupling with prefrontal and parietal cortices is positively correlated with individual gains in memory-based strategy use. Finally, in a tutoring study designed to facilitate rapid retrieval, hippocampal–PFC functional circuits predicted performance gains over an 8-week interval (Fig. 6). Children who exhibited higher intrinsic functional connectivity in these circuits prior to tutoring showed the greatest performance improvement in math problem solving (Supekar et al., 2013). Hippocampal– neocortical circuit reorganization therefore plays an important role in children’s shift from effortful counting to more efficient memory-based problem solving.
4 COGNITIVE CONTROL SYSTEMS IN MATHEMATICAL COGNITION
4.1 FLEXIBLE HUBS FOR COGNITIVE CONTROL
Prefrontal control processes are important for virtually every complex cognitive task, including mathematical cognition. The role of both working memory and declarative memory systems in mathematical cognition must therefore be considered in the context of cognitive control processes that support flexible problem solving and learning. Prefrontal control processes serve several functions in numerical cognition including maintenance of attention on goal-relevant numerical representations, manipulation of information in working memory, inhibition of irrelevant information, and implementation of task-relevant activations. Implementation of such control relies on dynamic functional interactions between multiple frontal regions (Cai et al., 2014, 2016; Cole et al., 2013; Ham et al., 2013; Seeley et al., 2007; Sridharan et al., 2008), and recent research has begun to elucidate the role of parietal–frontal and hippocampal–frontal circuits in different aspects of cognitive control during mathematical cognition.
4.2 DYNAMIC PARIETAL–FRONTAL CONTROL SIGNALS
As noted in Section 2, a common recurring theme in numerical problem solving is the coengagement of parietal and prefrontal regions associated with working memory. Children as young as 7 show reliable, and consistent, patterns of brain activity during arithmetic problem solving in multiple PFC regions (Houde et al., 2010). Commonly activated PFC regions include the anterior insula, and ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC (Fig. 1). It should be noted that these regions are also implicated in a wide range of cognitive control tasks in adults as well as children (Cole et al., 2013; Cai et al., 2014, 2016; Ham et al., 2013; Seeley et al., 2007; Sridharan et al., 2008; Ordaz et al., 2013).This profile of anatomical overlap suggests a common mechanism by which maturation of basic cognitive control can influence skill development across multiple cognitive domains.
Efficient control requires the concerted coordination between multiple brain regions, and there is growing evidence to suggest that this is implemented via dedicated neurocognitive networks. Two-key networks play a fundamental role in cognitive control processes in the human brain: the insula–cingulate salience network, which includes the anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortex, and the dorsal parietal–frontal working memory network, which includes the ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC and supramarginal gyrus. Supekar and Menon (2012) examined functional connectivity and dynamic causal interactions between the major nodes of these networks to investigate the maturation of control processes underlying numerical problem-solving skills in 7–9-year-old children, relative to adults. They found that, by age 9, the anterior insula node of the salience network is a major causal hub initiating control signals during problem solving (Fig. 7). The anterior insula, part of a larger network of regions previously shown to be important for salience processing and generating influential control signals, showed weaker influence over the ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC and anterior cingulate cortex in children compared to adults. Despite higher levels of PFC activation in children, the strength of their causal modulatory influences to the parietal cortex was significantly weaker relative to adults.
Notably, weaker PFC control signals were associated with lower levels of arithmetic performance, and network interactions better predicted reaction time in both children and adults. In children, the strength of casual signals from the anterior insula to supramarginal gyrus and ventrolateral PFC predicted reaction times, while the strength of anterior insula to supramarginal gyrus, ventrolateral PFC, and anterior cingulate cortex predicted reaction times in adults. Reaction times were better predicted in adults, compared to children. It is noteworthy that even though a different set of links predicted reaction times in both groups, the anterior insula to supramarginal gyrus link was common in both. Similar results were observed when accuracy instead of reaction time was used as the performance measure. Thus, multiple PFC control signals contribute to efficient problem-solving skills in adults and weak signaling mechanisms contribute to lower levels of performance in children.
4.3 DYNAMIC HIPPOCAMPAL–FRONTAL CONTROL SIGNALS
As described in Section 3, both the ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC are associated with increased use of memory-based strategies in children (Cho et al., 2012). The ventrolateral PFC is known to play a prominent role in cognitive control over memory retrieval processes both in adults (Badre and Wagner, 2007; Koechlin et al., 2003; Miller, 2000) and in children (Adleman et al., 2002; Bunge and Wright, 2007; Houde et al., 2010; Kwon et al., 2002). These control processes are thought to be important for accurate retrieval of relevant facts and inhibition of irrelevant information (Destefano and Lefevre, 2004; Kaufmann, 2002; Kaufmann et al., 2004; Logie et al., 1994).
Dynamic causal modeling of fMRI data has provided further insights into the temporal profile of interactions between the hippocampus and PFC regions involved in mediating retrieval fluency. Causal analysis has revealed strong bidirectional interactions between the hippocampus and both the left ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC (Fig. 8). Crucially, causal influences from the left ventrolateral PFC to the hippocampus act as the main “top-down” component, while causal influences from the hippocampus to the left dorsolateral PFC serve as the main “bottom-up” component of this retrieval network. While still preliminary, these analyses highlight the differential contribution of hippocampal–prefrontal circuits to the early development of retrieval fluency in arithmetic problem solving and provide a novel framework for studying dynamic developmental processes involving the hippocampus and PFC that accompany the maturation of cognitive skills. Further research is needed to investigate how these processes contribute to concomitant improvements in cognitive control over retrieval, including successful inhibition of irrelevant information, such as incorrect answers, intermediate steps, and operand intrusions (Barrouillet and Lepine, 2005; Passolunghi and Siegel, 2004).
5 SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
Multiple lines of evidence affirm that numerical cognition relies on interactions within and between multiple functional brain circuits, including those underlying numerical quantity representations (FG–IPS), working memory (IPS–SMG–ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC), declarative memory (hippocampus–ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC), and cognitive control (anterior insula–ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC) (Fig. 1). We have highlighted the role of distinct memory and cognitive control systems that play distinct roles in mathematical cognition and learning.
We have reviewed evidence that the parietal–frontal working memory system is engaged during a wide range of numerical problem-solving tasks. Multiple working memory circuits anchored in different subdivisions of the inferior parietal cortex help create short-term representations that support the manipulation of multiple discrete quantities over several seconds. The IPS and supramarginal gyrus are associated with differential patterns of connectivity with the frontal eye field and ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC. Findings to date suggest that the IPS plays an essential role not only in quantity representations but also in maintaining quantity-related information in short-term working memory. Rule-based and context-specific manipulation of these representations in working memory is in turn supported by multiple prefrontal cortical areas—here the supramarginal gyrus emerges as a key locus for integrating frontal control systems with quantity representations supported by the IPS.
The hippocampal–frontal declarative memory system has an entirely different role in mathematical cognition and learning. This system plays a critical, but time limited, role in the early phase of knowledge acquisition, and this hippocampal dependence is reduced following reconfiguration of neocortical connections and stabilization of newly acquired knowledge. Hippocampal–neocortical reorganization facilitates fluent retrieval and long-term neocortical memory consolidation in children, eventually resulting in retrieval processes that are independent of the hippocampus (Qin et al., 2014).
The working memory and declarative memory systems intersect most prominently in the ventrolateral and dorsolateral PFC, which together with the anterior insula likely serve as flexible hubs for integrating information across attentional and memory systems. As reviewed in Section 4, weak control signals from these PFC regions negatively impact the ability to maintain task-relevant representations needed for achieving mature levels of performance. Better understanding of control processes mediated by these PFC regions is essential for a more mechanistic characterization of mathematical cognition, skill development, and learning.
A challenging question for future research is to understand how the distinct functional circuits highlighted in this review interact dynamically to support different aspects of mathematical cognition and learning and how they change with different stages of development. Addressing this question will require developing appropriate computational models of dynamic causal interactions between brain regions, analyzing different stages of information processing, and utilizing more appropriate experimental designs that involve the controlled manipulation of symbolic and nonsymbolic quantity representations in posterior brain regions including the FG and IPS.
References
- Adleman NE, Menon V, Blasey CM, White CD, Warsofsky IS, Glover GH, Reiss AL. A developmental fMRI study of the Stroop color-word task. Neuroimage. 2002;16:61–75. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ansari D. Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:278–291. doi: 10.1038/nrn2334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arsalidou M, Taylor MJ. Is 2 + 2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations. Neuroimage. 2011;54:2382–2393. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashkenazi S, Henik A. Attentional networks in developmental dyscalculia. Behav Brain Funct. 2010;6:2. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashkenazi S, Rosenberg-Lee M, Tenison C, Menon V. Weak task-related modulation and stimulus representations during arithmetic problem solving in children with developmental dyscalculia. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2012;2:S152–S166. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashkenazi S, Rosenberg-Lee M, Metcalfe AW, Swigart AG, Menon V. Visuo-spatial working memory is an important source of domain-general vulnerability in the development of arithmetic cognition. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51:2305–2317. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badre D, Wagner AD. Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2883–2901. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey DH, Littlefield A, Geary DC. The codevelopment of skill at and preference for use of retrieval-based processes for solving addition problems: individual and sex differences from first to sixth grades. J Exp Child Psychol. 2012;113:78–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.04.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrouillet P, Lepine R. Working memory and children’s use of retrieval to solve addition problems. J Exp Child Psychol. 2005;91:183–204. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrouillet P, Fayol M, Lathuliere E. Selecting between competitors in multiplication tasks: an explanation of the errors produced by adolescents with learning difficulties. Int J Behav Dev. 1997;21:253–275. [Google Scholar]
- Bressler SL, Menon V. Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:277–290. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bunge SA, Wright SB. Neurodevelopmental changes in working memory and cognitive control. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17:243–250. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butterworth B. The Mathematical Brain. Macmillan; London: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Butterworth B, Varma S, Laurillard D. Dyscalculia: from brain to education. Science. 2011;332:1049–1053. doi: 10.1126/science.1201536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cai W, Ryali S, Chen T, Li CS, Menon V. Dissociable roles of right inferior frontal cortex and anterior insula in inhibitory control: evidence from intrinsic and task-related functional parcellation, connectivity, and response profile analyses across multiple datasets. J Neurosci. 2014;34:14652–14667. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3048-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cai W, Chen T, Ryali S, Kochalka J, Li CS, Menon V. Causal interactions within a frontal-cingulate-parietal network during cognitive control: convergent evidence from a multisite-multitask investigation. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26:2140–2153. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cantlon JF, Libertus ME, Pinel P, Dehaene S, Brannon EM, Pelphrey KA. The neural development of an abstract concept of number. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21:2217–2229. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspers S, Eickhoff SB, Geyer S, Scheperjans F, Mohlberg H, Zilles K, Amunts K. The human inferior parietal lobule in stereotaxic space. Brain Struct Funct. 2008;212:481–495. doi: 10.1007/s00429-008-0195-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho S, Ryali S, Geary DC, Menon V. How does a child solve 7 + 8? Decoding brain activity patterns associated with counting and retrieval strategies. Dev Sci. 2011;14:989–1001. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01055.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho S, Metcalfe AW, Young CB, Ryali S, Geary DC, Menon V. Hippocampal-prefrontal engagement and dynamic causal interactions in the maturation of children’s fact retrieval. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012;24:1849–1866. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen Kadosh R, Cohen Kadosh K, Schuhmann T, Kaas A, Goebel R, Henik A, Sack AT. Virtual dyscalculia induced by parietal-lobe TMS impairs automatic magnitude processing. Curr Biol. 2007;17:689–693. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole MW, Reynolds JR, Power JD, Repovs G, Anticevic A, Braver TS. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:1348–1355. doi: 10.1038/nn.3470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, Shulman G. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008;58:306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davachi L. Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006;16:693–700. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davachi L, Mitchell JP, Wagner AD. Multiple routes to memory: distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:2157–2162. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0337195100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis N, Cannistraci CJ, Rogers BP, Gatenby JC, Fuchs LS, Anderson AW, Gore JC. Aberrant functional activation in school age children at-risk for mathematical disability: a functional imaging study of simple arithmetic skill. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2470–2479. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Smedt B, Holloway ID, Ansari D. Effects of problem size and arithmetic operation on brain activation during calculation in children with varying levels of arithmetical fluency. Neuroimage. 2011;57:771–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Piazza M, Pinel P, Cohen L. Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2003;20:487–506. doi: 10.1080/02643290244000239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Destefano D, Lefevre JA. The role of working memory in mental arithmetic. Eur J Cogn Psychol. 2004;16:353–386. [Google Scholar]
- Diana RA, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C. Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: a three-component model. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:379–386. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diekelmann S, Wilhelm I, Born J. The whats and whens of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Sleep Med Rev. 2009;13:309–321. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2008.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumontheil I, Klingberg T. Brain activity during a visuospatial working memory task predicts arithmetical performance 2 years later. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22:1078–1085. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H. Hippocampus: cognitive processes and neural representations that underlie declarative memory. Neuron. 2004;44:109–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C. The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:123–152. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans TM, Kochalka J, Ngoon TJ, Wu SS, Qin S, Battista C, Menon V. Brain structural integrity and intrinsic functional connectivity forecast 6 year longitudinal growth in children’s numerical abilities. J Neurosci. 2015;35:11743–11750. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0216-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fias W, Menon V, Szucs D. Multiple components of developmental dyscalculia. Trends Neurosci Educ. 2013;2:43–47. [Google Scholar]
- Frankland PW, Bontempi B. The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:119–130. doi: 10.1038/nrn1607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geary DC. Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: a 5-year longitudinal study. Dev Psychol. 2011;47:1539–1552. doi: 10.1037/a0025510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geary DC, Brown SC. Cognitive addition: strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in gifted, normal, and mathematically disabled children. Dev Psychol. 1991;27:398–406. [Google Scholar]
- Geary DC, Hoard MK. Learning disabilities in basic mathematics: deficits in memory and cognition. In: Royer JM, editor. Mathematical Cognition. Information Age Publishing; Greenwich, CT: 2003. pp. 93–115. [Google Scholar]
- Geary DC, Hamson CO, Hoard MK. Numerical and arithmetical cognition: a longitudinal study of process and concept deficits in children with learning disability. J Exp Child Psychol. 2000;77:236–263. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geary D, Hoard M, Nugent L, Byrd-Craven J, Berch D, Mazzocco M. Strategy use, long-term memory, and working memory capacity. In: Berch DB, Mazzocco MMM, editors. Why Is Math So Hard For Some Children? Paul H Brookes Publishing Co; Baltimore, MD: 2007a. pp. 83–105. [Google Scholar]
- Geary DC, Hoard MK, Byrd-Craven J, Nugent L, Numtee C. Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. Child Dev. 2007b;78:1343–1359. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01069.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geary DC, Hoard MK, Bailey DH. Fact retrieval deficits in low achieving children and children with mathematical learning disability. J Learn Disabil. 2012;45:291–307. doi: 10.1177/0022219410392046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghetti S, Demaster DM, Yonelinas AP, Bunge SA. Developmental differences in medial temporal lobe function during memory encoding. J Neurosci. 2010;30:9548–9556. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3500-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabner RH, Ansari D, Koschutnig K, Reishofer G, Ebner F, Neuper C. To retrieve or to calculate? Left angular gyrus mediates the retrieval of arithmetic facts during problem solving. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:604–608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greicius MD, Srivastava G, Reiss AL, Menon V. Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:4637–4642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308627101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ham T, Leff A, de Boissezon X, Joffe A, Sharp DJ. Cognitive control and the salience network: an investigation of error processing and effective connectivity. J Neurosci. 2013;33:7091–7098. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4692-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houde O, Rossi S, Lubin A, Joliot M. Mapping numerical processing, reading, and executive functions in the developing brain: an fMRI meta-analysis of 52 studies including 842 children. Dev Sci. 2010;13:876–885. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00938.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isaacs EB, Edmonds CJ, Lucas A, Gadian DG. Calculation difficulties in children of very low birthweight: a neural correlate. Brain. 2001;124:1701–1707. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.9.1701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ischebeck A, Zamarian L, Egger K, Schocke M, Delazer M. Imaging early practice effects in arithmetic. Neuroimage. 2007;36:993–1003. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iuculano T, Rosenberg-Lee M, Richardson J, Tenison C, Fuchs L, Supekar K, Menon V. Cognitive tutoring induces widespread neuroplasticity and remediates brain function in children with mathematical learning disabilities. Nat Commun. 2015;6:8453. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jolles D, Ashkenazi S, Kochalka J, Evans T, Richardson J, Rosenberg-lee M, Zhao H, Supekar K, Chen T, Menon V. Parietal hyper-connectivity, aberrant brain organization, and circuit-based biomarkers in children with mathematical disabilities. Dev Sci. 2016;19:1–19. doi: 10.1111/desc.12399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufmann L. More evidence for the role of the central executive in retrieving arithmetic facts—a case study of severe developmental dyscalculia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2002;24:302–310. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.3.302.976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufmann L, Lochy A, Drexler A, Semenza C. Deficient arithmetic fact retrieval— storage or access problem? A case study. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:482–496. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufmann L, Vogel SE, Starke M, Kremser C, Schocke M. Numerical and non-numerical ordinality processing in children with and without developmental dyscalculia: evidence from fMRI. Cogn Dev. 2009a;24:486–494. [Google Scholar]
- Kaufmann L, Vogel SE, Starke M, Kremser C, Schocke M, Wood G. Developmental dyscalculia: compensatory mechanisms in left intraparietal regions in response to nonsymbolic magnitudes. Behav Brain Funct. 2009b;5:35. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-5-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufmann L, Wood G, Rubinsten O, Henik A. Meta-analyses of developmental fMRI studies investigating typical and atypical trajectories of number processing and calculation. Dev Neuropsychol. 2011;36:763–787. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawashima R, Taira M, Okita K, Inoue K, Tajima N, Yoshida H, Sasaki T, Sugiura M, Watanabe J, Fukuda H. A functional MRI study of simple arithmetic—a comparison between children and adults. Cogn Brain Res. 2004;18:227–233. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koechlin E, Ody C, Kouneiher F. The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science. 2003;302:1181–1185. doi: 10.1126/science.1088545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kucian K. Non-symbolic numerical distance effect in children with and without developmental dyscalculia: a parametric fMRI study. Dev Neuropsychol. 2011;36:741. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kucian K, von Aster M. Developmental dyscalculia. Eur J Pediatr. 2015;174:1–13. doi: 10.1007/s00431-014-2455-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kucian K, Loenneker T, Dietrich T, Dosch M, Martin E, von Aster M. Impaired neural networks for approximate calculation in dyscalculic children: a functional MRI study. Behav Brain Funct. 2006;2:31. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-2-31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kucian K, von Aster M, Loenneker T, Dietrich T, Martin E. Development of neural networks for exact and approximate calculation: a fMRI study. Dev Neuropsychol. 2008;33:447–473. doi: 10.1080/87565640802101474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kucian K, Grond U, Rotzer S, Henzi B, Schonmann C, Plangger F, Galli M, Martin E, von Aster M. Mental number line training in children with developmental dyscalculia. Neuroimage. 2011;57:782–795. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumaran D, Summerfield JJ, Hassabis D, Maguire EA. Tracking the emergence of conceptual knowledge during human decision making. Neuron. 2009;63:889–901. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kwon H, Reiss AL, Menon V. Neural basis of protracted developmental changes in visuo-spatial working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:13336–13341. doi: 10.1073/pnas.162486399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logie RH, Gilhooly KJ, Wynn V. Counting on working memory in arithmetic problem solving. Mem Cognit. 1994;22:395–410. doi: 10.3758/bf03200866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luck SJ, Vogel EK. Visual working memory capacity: from psychophysics and neurobiology to individual differences. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17:391–400. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClelland JL, Mcnaughton BL, O’Reilly RC. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol Rev. 1995;102:419–457. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGaugh JL. Memory—a century of consolidation. Science. 2000;287:248–251. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5451.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V. Arithmetic in child and adult brain. In: Cohen Kadosh R, Dowker A, editors. Handbook of Mathematical Cognition. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2014. pp. 502–530. [Google Scholar]
- Menon V. A neurodevelopmental perspective on the role of memory systems in children’s math learning. In: Berch DB, Geary DC, Mann Koepke K, editors. Development of Mathematical Cognition-Neural Substrates and Genetic Influences. Elsevier Science Publishing Co Inc.; United States: 2015. pp. 79–107. In revision. [Google Scholar]
- Menon V. Working memory in children’s math learning and its disruption in dyscalculia. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.014. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214:655–667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V, Rivera SM, White CD, Eliez S, Glover GH, Reiss AL. Functional optimization of arithmetic processing in perfect performers. Cogn Brain Res. 2000;9:343–345. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(00)00010-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V, Boyett-Anderson JM, Reiss AL. Maturation of medial temporal lobe response and connectivity during memory encoding. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;25:379–385. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metcalfe AWS, Ashkenazi S, Rosenberg-Lee M, Menon V. Fractionating the neural correlates of individual working memory components underlying arithmetic problem solving skills in children. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2013;6:162–175. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller EK. The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2000;1:59–65. doi: 10.1038/35036228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mussolin C, De Volder A, Grandin C, Schlogel X, Nassogne MC, Noël MP. Neural correlates of symbolic number comparison in developmental dyscalculia. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22:860–874. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norman KA, O’Reilly RC. Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: a complementary-learning-systems approach. Psychol Rev. 2003;110:611–646. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.4.611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ofen N, Kao YC, Sokol-Hessner P, Kim H, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Gabrieli JD. Development of the declarative memory system in the human brain. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:1198–1205. doi: 10.1038/nn1950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ordaz S, Foran W, Velanova K, Luna B. Longitudinal growth curves of brain function underlying inhibitory control through adolescence. J Neurosci. 2013;33:18109–18124. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1741-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passolunghi MC, Siegel LS. Working memory and access to numerical information in children with disability in mathematics. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;88:348–367. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price GR, Holloway I, Rasanen P, Vesterinen M, Ansari D. Impaired parietal magnitude processing in developmental dyscalculia. Curr Biol. 2007;17:R1042–R1043. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qin S, Piekema C, Petersson KM, Han B, Luo J, Fernandez G. Probing the transformation of discontinuous associations into episodic memory: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2007;38:212–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qin S, Rijpkema M, Tendolkar I, Piekema C, Hermans EJ, Binder M, Petersson KM, Luo J, Fernandez G. Dissecting medial temporal lobe contributions to item and associative memory formation. Neuroimage. 2009;46:874–881. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qin S, Hermans EJ, Rijpkema M, Fernández G. Adaptive Memory: Imaging Medial Temporal and Prefrontal Memory Systems. Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen, PhD; Nijmegen, the Netherlands: 2011a. [Google Scholar]
- Qin S, van Marle HJ, Hermans EJ, Fernandez G. Subjective sense of memory strength and the objective amount of information accurately remembered are related to distinct neural correlates at encoding. J Neurosci. 2011b;31:8920–8927. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2587-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qin S, Cho S, Chen T, Rosenberg-Lee M, Geary DC, Menon V. Hippocampal-neocortical functional reorganization underlies children’s cognitive development. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:1263–1269. doi: 10.1038/nn.3788. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle ME, Macleod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98:676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rivera SM, Reiss AL, Eckert MA, Menon V. Developmental changes in mental arithmetic: evidence for increased functional specialization in the left inferior parietal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:1779–1790. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg-Lee M, Barth M, Menon V. What difference does a year of schooling make? Maturation of brain response and connectivity between 2nd and 3rd grades during arithmetic problem solving. Neuroimage. 2011;57:796–808. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg-Lee M, Ashkenazi S, Chen T, Young CB, Geary DC, Menon V. Brain hyper-connectivity and operation-specific deficits during arithmetic problem solving in children with developmental dyscalculia. Dev Sci. 2014;18:351–372. doi: 10.1111/desc.12216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rottschy C, Langner R, Dogan I, Reetz K, Laird AR, Schulz JB, Fox PT, Eickhoff SB. Modelling neural correlates of working memory: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. Neuroimage. 2012;60:830–846. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rotzer S, Loenneker T, Kucian K, Martin E, Klaver P, von Aster M. Dysfunctional neural network of spatial working memory contributes to developmental dyscalculia. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2859–2865. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL, Norman KA, Koutstaal W. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory. Annu Rev Psychol. 1998;49:289–318. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL. Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:657–661. doi: 10.1038/nrn2213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci. 2007;27:2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon O, Mangin JF, Cohen L, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S. Topographical layout of hand, eye, calculation, and language-related areas in the human parietal lobe. Neuron. 2002;33:475–487. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00575-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Spiers HJ. Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-term memory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:637–648. doi: 10.1038/nrn1178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V. A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:12569–12574. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800005105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Supekar K, Menon V. Developmental maturation of dynamic causal control signals in higher-order cognition: a neurocognitive network model. PLoS Comput Biol. 2012;8:e1002374. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Supekar K, Swigart AG, Tenison C, Jolles DD, Rosenberg-Lee M, Fuchs L, Menon V. Neural predictors of individual differences in response to math tutoring in primary-grade school children. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:8230–8235. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1222154110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szucs D, Devine A, Soltesz F, Nobes A, Gabriel F. Developmental dyscalculia is related to visuo-spatial memory and inhibition impairment. Cortex. 2013;49:2674–2688. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tse D, Langston RF, Kakeyama M, Bethus I, Spooner PA, Wood ER, Witter MP, Morris RG. Schemas and memory consolidation. Science. 2007;316:76–82. doi: 10.1126/science.1135935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E. Elements of Episodic Memory. Oxford University Press; New York: 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Amin H, Rykhlevskaia E, Nguyen DA, Greicius MD, Menon V. Dissociable connectivity within human angular gyrus and intra-parietal sulcus: evidence from functional and structural connectivity. Cereb Cortex. 2010a;20:2636–2646. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Menon V. Typical and atypical development of functional human brain networks: insights from resting-state fMRI. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010b;4:21. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Opstal F, Fias W, Peigneux P, Verguts T. The neural representation of extensively trained ordered sequences. Neuroimage. 2009;47:367–375. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varma S, Schwartz DL. How should educational neuroscience conceptualise the relation between cognition and brain function? Mathematical reasoning as a network process. Educ Res. 2008;50:149–161. [Google Scholar]