Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2017 Apr 16;23(1):515–526. doi: 10.1111/adb.12505

The involuntary nature of binge drinking: goal directedness and awareness of intention

Nuria Doñamayor 1, Daniela Strelchuk 1, Kwangyeol Baek 1,2, Paula Banca 1, Valerie Voon 1,3,4,5,
PMCID: PMC5811896  PMID: 28419776

Abstract

Binge drinking represents a public health issue and is a known risk factor in the development of alcohol use disorders. Previous studies have shown behavioural as well as neuroanatomical alterations associated with binge drinking. Here, we address the question of the automaticity or involuntary nature of the behaviour by assessing goal‐directed behaviour and intentionality. In this study, we used a computational two‐step task, designed to discern between model‐based/goal‐directed and model‐free/habitual behaviours, and the classic Libet clock task, to study intention awareness, in a sample of 31 severe binge drinkers (BD) and 35 matched healthy volunteers. We observed that BD had impaired goal‐directed behaviour in the two‐step task compared with healthy volunteers. In the Libet clock task, BD showed delayed intention awareness. Further, we demonstrated that alcohol use severity, as reflected by the alcohol use disorders identification test, correlated with decreased conscious awareness of volitional intention in BD, although it was unrelated to performance on the two‐step task. However, the time elapsed since the last drinking binge influenced the model‐free scores, with BD showing less habitual behaviour after longer abstinence. Our findings suggest that the implementation of goal‐directed strategies and the awareness of volitional intention are affected in current heavy alcohol users. However, the modulation of these impairments by alcohol use severity and abstinence suggests a state effect of alcohol use in these measures and that top‐down volitional control might be ameliorated with alcohol use cessation.

Keywords: alcohol dependence, binge drinking, goal‐directed behaviour, habitual behaviour, intention awareness

Introduction

Binge drinking is characterized by consuming large quantities of alcohol (blood alcohol concentration ≥ 80 mg%) in a short period of time (about 2 hours) followed by longer periods of abstinence. It has numerous negative social and individual consequences and is a prominent risk factor for the development of alcohol abuse disorders (Crabbe, Harris & Koob 2011). Binge drinking has been associated with attentional impairments, decreased sensitivity to the anticipation of high‐risk negative outcomes and increased premature responding and trait impulsivity (Bauer & Ceballos 2014; Sanchez‐Roige et al. 2014; Worbe et al. 2014; Morris, Kundu, Baek, et al. 2016a). Neuroimaging has further revealed grey matter abnormalities within ventral striatum (VS), prefrontal cortex (PFC) and limbic areas in binge drinkers (BD) (Howell et al. 2013; Doallo et al. 2014; Mashhoon et al. 2014), as well as PFC hyperactivity following high‐risk decisions (Worbe et al. 2014), but hypoactivity during working memory tasks (Crego et al. 2010). Several lines of evidence suggest potential abnormalities in involuntary automatic behaviours in addictions, from a shift away from goal‐directed behaviours and relapse triggers from incentive cues. Here, we assess the relationship between goal‐directed behaviours and motor intention in severe BD.

Dual‐system accounts suggest that action control is determined by the balance between goal‐directed or model‐based behaviour, driven by response–outcome information, and habitual or model‐free behaviour, driven by stimulus–response associations (Gläscher et al. 2010; van der Meer & Redish 2010; de Wit et al. 2012; Dolan & Dayan 2013). Overreliance on habits over model‐based behaviour has been reported in a number of neuropsychiatric disorders, including chronic addiction (Gillan et al. 2011; Hogarth, Chase & Baess 2012; Sjoerds et al. 2013; Gillan et al. 2014; Voon, Derbyshire, et al. 2015b). Furthermore, anatomical, functional and connectivity studies have evidenced an overlap between the neural substrate of the model‐based and model‐free systems, which involves lateral and ventromedial PFC, orbitofrontal cortex, inferior parietal cortex (IPC) and striatum (Valentin, Dickinson & O'Doherty 2007; Tanaka, Balleine & O'Doherty 2008; de Wit et al. 2009; Gläscher et al. 2010; de Wit et al. 2012; Voon, Derbyshire, et al. 2015b; Morris, Kundu, Dowell, et al. 2016b), and the structures altered in BD (Howell et al. 2013; Vargas et al. 2014; Whelan et al. 2014; Worbe et al. 2014; Morris, Kundu, Baek, et al. 2016a).

It has been shown that alcohol dependence affects the ability to base behaviour on response–outcome information (Sjoerds et al. 2013), which is at the core of model‐based behaviour. A further study showed that heavy drinkers, including alcohol‐dependent subjects, showed increased alcohol cue reactivity in the dorsal striatum, whereas light social drinkers showed increased VS and PFC activities (Vollstädt‐Klein et al. 2010), suggesting a shift from ventral to dorsal striatal involvement, such as that seen in the development of addiction, and from goal‐directedness to habitual behaviour (Corbit, Nie & Janak 2012; Everitt & Robbins 2013). Similarly, alcohol‐dependent subjects tested early in abstinence showed impaired model‐based behaviours tested using the two‐step task (Sebold et al. 2014). However, this bias towards model‐free behaviour in alcohol‐dependent individuals has been reported to decrease with prolonged abstinence, suggesting a possible role for top‐down volitional control in increasing goal directedness (Voon, Derbyshire, et al. 2015b). We have also previously shown that BD take more risks when anticipating loss outcomes. The behaviour shifts towards a more risk‐averse attitude when exposed explicitly to the actual loss outcome, suggesting potentially that the explicit nature of experiencing the loss might counteract underlying automatic biases towards risk (Worbe et al. 2014).

Model‐based behaviour involves active deliberation and prospective planning in order to direct actions towards desirable outcomes (Gläscher et al. 2010; van der Meer & Redish 2010; de Wit et al. 2012; Dolan & Dayan 2013). Therefore, the fact that individuals drink excessively or go in drinking binges, despite their negative consequences (Crabbe et al. 2011), appears to contradict successful goal‐directed behaviour. Furthermore, this ability to prospectively track motivationally relevant outcomes most probably also requires a certain level of volitional awareness, more specifically, of both the planning or intention to act and the action itself.

Intention awareness has been extensively studied in the field of motor intention. Libet et al. (1983) designed a task aimed at discerning between the awareness of the intention to move and of the movement itself, showing that the former (W judgement) occurred around 200 milliseconds before the latter (M judgement). Although their task has been subject to some criticism (Gomes 1998; Danquah, Farrell & O'Boyle 2008), numerous studies have replicated the basic findings of the original study (Lau et al. 2004; Walsh et al. 2010; Fried, Mukamel & Kreiman 2011). Anatomically, intention awareness has been reported to involve supplementary motor complex, lateral PFC and IPC (Fried et al. 1991; Lau et al. 2004; Fried et al. 2011; Desmurget & Sirigu 2012), thus sharing part of the neural substrate of model‐based behaviour.

The interval between W and M judgements can be used as an implicit measure of conscious awareness of volitional intention, and it has been argued that this interval would allow for intentional stopping or vetoing of the movement (Libet 1999; Haggard & Libet 2001). Several neuropsychiatric disorders have been associated with delayed awareness of motor intention (Edwards et al. 2011; Moretto et al. 2011; Tabu et al. 2015; Baek et al. in press), reflected as a reduced veto period or a shift of the W towards the M judgement. Moreover, this veto period might be used not only to stop the initiated movement but also to evaluate whether the selected action might be optimal to obtain the desired effect (Haggard & Libet 2001). As stated previously, optimal action selection is paramount to goal‐directed behaviour. Interestingly, at least two of the disorders associated with reduced W–M intervals, Parkinson's disease and Tourette's syndrome, have also been associated with alterations in model‐based and model‐free behaviours (Marsh et al. 2004; de Wit et al. 2011; Hadj‐Bouziane et al. 2013; Wylie et al. 2013), further supporting the notion that maintaining optimal balance between these two systems might require a certain level of intention awareness.

In the current study, we aimed to study goal‐directed behaviour and intention awareness in BD and healthy volunteers (HV). To this end, we used a two‐step task (Daw et al. 2011), specifically designed to computationally differentiate model‐free and model‐based behaviours, as well as the Libet clock task (Libet et al. 1983). We hypothesized that BD subjects with current ongoing heavy alcohol use would have lower goal‐directed learning compared to HV. We further hypothesized that BD would be impaired in intention awareness relative to HV.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Thirty‐one BD (12 women, 21.29 ± 2.52 years old) and 35 HV (20 women, 23.17 ± 2.83 years old) aged between 19 and 29 years were recruited from community and university‐based advertisements. Both groups were gender matched, χ2 = 2.24, P > 0.1, and age matched within 5 years, but the healthy controls were slightly older as a group, t y(51) = −3.22, d = 0.70, P = 0.002.

Binge‐drinking criteria were based on the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism diagnostics that define consumption of greater than eight units for men or six units for women within a 2‐hour period at least once a week for the previous 6 months (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism 2004, Winter). Subjects also had to have the intention to get drunk and also have been drunk at least once per week for the previous 6 months. They were excluded if they had a major psychiatric disorder, substance addiction or medical illness or were on psychotropic medications. Psychiatric disorders were screened with the mini‐international neuropsychiatric interview (Sheehan et al. 1998), and participants completed the National Adult Reading Test to assess verbal IQ (BD: M = 116.69, SD = 4.03, n = 26; HV: M = 115.47, SD = 5.66, n = 32; effect of group: P > 0.1; effect of the age covariate: P > 0.1), the Beck Depression Inventory to assess depressive symptoms (BD: M = 9.54, SD = 8.01, n = 26; HV: M = 7.29, SD = 7.48, n = 34; effect of group: P > 0.1; effect of the age covariate: P > 0.1) and the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) (BD: M = 16.08, SD = 5.32, n = 26; HV: M = 4.53, SD = 3.34, n = 34; effect of group: F(1) = 86.33, P < 0.001; effect of the age covariate: P > 0.1) and the Alcohol Use Questionnaire to assess alcohol use severity (BD: M = 33.41, SD = 14.25, n = 22; HV: M = 8.87, SD = 8.90, n = 27; effect of group: F(1) = 57.35, P < 0.001; effect of the age covariate: P > 0.1). The mean number of times drunk over the last 6 months was greater for BD (BD: M = 20.70, SD = 14.25, n = 23; HV: M = 2.21, SD = 4.21, n = 34; effect of group: F(1) = 63.29, P < 0.001; effect of the age covariate: P > 0.1). Subjects were questioned on their patterns of alcohol consumption, to ensure they fit the criteria, and their last alcohol binge prior to testing was recorded. Subjects were administered a breathalyser test on the day of the study.

Participants were tested on two separate days, with the two‐step task tested on day 1 and the Libet clock and working memory tasks tested on day 2. All subjects provided written informed consent before enrolling and were compensated for their time. The study was approved by the University of Cambridge Research Ethics Committee.

Task description

Two‐step task

Participants performed the two‐step task as described in our previous work (Fig. 1a) (Voon, Derbyshire, et al. 2015b). In stage 1, participants had to choose between two stimuli, each of which led to one of the two stimulus pairs with a fixed probability (P = 0.70) and to the other stimulus pair with opposite probability (P = 0.30). The stimulus choice made in stage 2 led to a reward with probability varying slowly and independently over time between 0.25 and 0.75. Each of the four stimuli in stage 2 was attached to a different probability distribution, and the association between each stage 2 stimulus and its reward probability was counterbalanced across participants. Choices at each stage had to be made within 2 seconds, and the result of each choice was presented after 1.5 seconds. The stimulus chosen in stages 1 and 2 remained on screen as a reminder in stage 2 and the outcome stage, respectively. If the stage 2 choice was rewarded, participants saw a £1 coin for 1 second; otherwise, they saw a grey circle for 1 second.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Results from the analysis of the two‐step and Libet clock tasks. Schematic depiction of the (a) two‐step and (b) Libet clock tasks. Mean (c) weighting parameter (w), (d) learning rate in stage 1 (α1) and (e) perseveration rate from the two‐step task. (f) Mean estimated times of intention (W judgement), movement (M judgement) and difference between intention and movement (W–M) relative to the recorded button press from the Libet clock task. Black bars represent binge drinkers (BD) and white healthy volunteers (HV). Error bars represent the standard error of the mean

The task consisted of three blocks of 67 trials. Prior to the task, participants underwent extensive computer‐based instructions, which included explanatory examples of changes in transition and probability, and a short block of 50 trials in the same format as the experimental task but with different stimuli. The task was run with Cogent 2000 (http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent.php) on Matlab R2011a (MathWorks, Natick, USA).

Libet's clock task

In a separate session, participants also performed the Libet clock task (Fig. 1b) (Libet et al. 1983). In each trial, a hand revolved inside a clock face marked at 5‐minute intervals at 2560 milliseconds per cycle. Participants were instructed to make a button press with their right index finger after a random time interval. They were asked to act as spontaneously as possible and to avoid preselecting a position of the hand to trigger the button press.

Participants were required to make either a W judgement or an M judgement. In W judgement trials, they were instructed to pay attention to when they first felt the urge to press the button; in M judgement trials, they were instructed to pay attention to when they actually pressed the button. After the button press, the clock hand continued rotating for 1500–2500 milliseconds. Participants then verbally reported its position either where they first felt the urge to press the button or when they actually pressed it, depending on the type of trial.

The task consisted of one M judgement block followed by a W judgement block, each consisting of 20 trials. The programme was developed with LabView (National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA).

Memory tests

In order to control for possible working memory effects in the two‐step task, we also tested participants with the spatial working memory (SWM) and paired associates learning (PAL) tasks of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (http://www.cambridgecognition.com/technology).

Statistical analysis

For the two‐step task, choice behaviour was fit to a computational model, which learned action values through both model‐based reinforcement learning and model‐free State‐Action‐Reward‐State‐Action (SARSA) (λ) temporal difference learning. Participants' choices were driven by the weighted combination of these two types of learning, with the relative weighting controlled by a free parameter w, which was used as a measure of the reliance on model‐free (w = 0) or model‐based (w = 1) strategies. Additional behavioural measures were the indices of choice reliability in each stage (β), the learning rate in each stage (α), the reinforcement eligibility parameter (λ) and the perseveration rate (for further information on the model and the equations used, see Daw et al. 2011; Voon, Baek, et al. 2015a). We also examined the behavioural parameters of model‐based (interaction between transition and outcome) and model‐free (effect of outcome) measures. Each parameter was analysed by means of analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) to test for differences between BD and HV, while controlling for the effect of the age covariate.

For Libet's clock task, average latencies (excluding values over/under 3 SD) were computed for each participant's W and M judgements. Latencies were introduced in a 2 × 2 model to test for the effects of group and type of judgement and using age as a covariate. The difference between W and M judgements (W–M interval) was used as an implicit measure of conscious awareness of volitional intention. This measure was tested using an ANCOVA with age as a covariate.

The relationship between test outcomes (w, model‐free and model‐based scores from the two‐step task, and W–M interval from the Libet clock task) and alcohol use severity, as reflected by AUDIT score, was subsequently tested on an exploratory basis. To this end, variables were introduced in regression models. If AUDIT score significantly predicted the behavioural measure, the Fisher r‐to‐z transformation was used to assess the significance of the difference between the correlation coefficients of BD and HV. Further, k‐means clustering was performed to test whether the days since the last binge drinking episode had an influence on the behavioural measures in the BD group. This method was chosen over regression because of the limited number of data points in the time variable.

Task outcomes were analysed using r (https://www.r‐project.org/). Data that did not conform to normality of distribution and/or had outliers were tested using robust statistical methods (Yuen's t‐test with 10 percent winsorization, robust rank‐based ANCOVA and least trimmed squares regression). Otherwise, parametric testing was used (linear regression). k‐means clustering used the Calinski–Harabasz index to estimate the number of clusters, with a maximum of 10 iterations and two clusters, given the reduced number of data points; the Duda–Hart test with a significance level of P = 0.005 was applied to assess whether the data set should be split into clusters.

Results

Two‐step task

Thirty BD and 35 HV completed the two‐step task. One BD did not complete this task.

After controlling for the effect of age, BD (M = 0.322, SD = 0.224) had significantly lower w scores than HV (M = 0.464, SD = 0.255), F(1) = 5.03, P = 0.029 (Fig. 1c); the age covariate was not significantly related to the w score, P > 0.1. However, when analysing the model‐based and model‐free scores separately, measures that are less robust than the computational analysis, there were no differences between groups in either score, P > 0.05, nor was the age covariate related to the scores, P > 0.05.

There were no significant differences in choice reliability in stage 1 (β1) between BD (M = 4.63, SD = 2.97) and HV (M = 5.69, SD = 3.78), P > 0.05, but there was a significant relationship between this parameter and the age covariate, F(1) = 6.43, P = 0.014. There were no differences in choice reliability in stage 2 (β2), nor was this parameter related to the age covariate, P > 0.1.

After controlling for the effect of age, BD (M = 0.294, SD = 0.217) also had significantly lower learning rates in stage 1 (α1) than HV (M = 0.519, SD = 0.348), F(1) = 4.68, P = 0.034 (Fig. 1d); but there was no relationship between α1 and participants' age, P > 0.1. There were no differences between the groups' learning rates in stage 2 (α2), nor was this parameter related to the age covariate, P > 0.1.

Binge drinkers (M = 0.197, SD = 0.180) had significantly higher perseveration rates than HV (M = 0.139, SD = 0.145) after controlling for the effect of age, F(1) = 4.21, P = 0.044 (Fig. 1e); but age was not related to perseveration rates, P > 0.1.

There were no significant differences between groups in the λ parameter, nor was this parameter related to the subjects' age, P > 0.1.

Libet's clock task

Twenty‐two BD and 31 HV completed the Libet clock task. Nine BD and four HV did not complete this task, which was tested on a separate day.

The robust ANCOVA performed on the latencies of the W and M judgements showed the main effects of the group, F(1) = 5.40, P = 0.022, and condition, F(1) = 18.31, P < 0.001, and an interaction between the group and condition, F(1)=5.73, P = 0.018, after controlling for the effect of age. The age covariate had no relationship with the latencies of the W and M judgements, P > 0.05. Šidák‐corrected post hoc t‐tests showed that BD (M = −53 milliseconds, SD = 85) made the W judgement marginally closer to the actual movement than HV (M = −130 milliseconds, SD = 147), t y(41) = 2.15, d = 0.61, P = 0.074, whereas there were no differences in the latencies of the M judgement (BD: M = −17, SD = 45; HV: M = −8, SD = 42), P > 0.1.

After controlling for the effect of age, the critical difference between W and M judgements (W–M interval) was marginally shorter in the case of BD (M = −36 milliseconds, SD = 90) than in the case of HV (M = −122 milliseconds, SD = 155), F(1) = 4.00, P = 0.051, while the age covariate was not significantly related to the W–M interval, P > 0.1 (Fig. 1f).

Memory

Twenty‐one and 22 BD completed the SWM and PAL tasks, respectively, and 23 HV completed both tasks.

After controlling for the effect of age, there were no statistical differences between the number of errors committed by BD and HV in the SWM task, P > 0.1, but the age covariate was significantly related to the number of errors, F(1) = 6.53, P = 0.014. There were neither differences between the number of errors committed by the groups in the PAL task nor an effect of the age covariate, P > 0.05.

Relationship with clinical variables

On an exploratory basis, we further examined the relationship between the main outcomes and clinical variables. Regarding the Libet clock task, AUDIT score significantly predicted the length of the W–M interval (Fig. 2a), F(1, 47) = 4.96, r lts = 0.31, P = 0.031, with greater alcohol use severity predicting shorter to inverted W–M intervals. AUDIT score significantly predicted the length of the W–M interval in the BD group, F(1, 19) = 4.82, r lm = 0.45, P = 0.041, with greater alcohol use severity predicting shorter to inverted W–M intervals. However, AUDIT score was uncorrelated to W–M length in HV, F(1, 26) = 0.63, r lts = −0.15, P > 0.1. The difference between the correlation coefficients of both groups was also statistically significant, z = 2.14, P = 0.032.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Results of the regression and cluster analyses. (a) Relationship between AUDIT score and W–M difference. Results of the cluster analysis of the (b) weighting parameter (w), (c) model‐free and (d) model‐based scores based on the days since the last binge drinking episode of the binge drinkers (BD). Horizontal lines represent the mean score of each cluster. BD are represented by black triangles and solid lines, healthy volunteers (HV) are represented by grey squares and solid lines and the regression line from the analysis over all subjects is represented by a black dashed line

There was no relationship between AUDIT and either w, model‐free or model‐based scores from the two‐step task, P > 0.1.

Twenty‐two BD responded to the question regarding the days elapsed since their last binge. k‐means clustering showed that subjects that had had the last binge drinking episode under 4 days prior to testing had lower w (M = 0.278, SD = 0.229; Fig. 2b), as well as higher model‐free (M = 0.120, SD = 0.097; Fig. 2c) and lower model‐based (M = 0.025, SD = 0.088; Fig. 2d) scores than those that had had the last episode over 4 days before testing (w: M = 0.483, SD = 0.238; model‐free: M = 0.052, SD = 0.071; model‐based: M = 0.157, SD = 0.096), P = 0.002, P = 0.001 and P = 0.001, respectively.

The time since the last drinking binge had no influence on the W–M interval from Libet's clock task.

Discussion

In the current study, we explore model‐based behaviour and intention awareness in BD and HV. Our BD sample shows particularly severe binge drinking behaviour, as they were only included in the study if they fit National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism criteria and further had been drunk at least once per week for the previous 6 months. The two‐step task (Daw et al. 2011) is used to differentiate between model‐based goal‐directed behaviour and model‐free habitual behaviour. In this task, BD show significantly lower w scores than HV, indicating a bias away from model‐based behaviours towards habitual behaviour. BD also showed lower learning rates than HV in stage 1 of the task, but not in stage 2. Once age was controlled for, we further show that BD have greater perseverative deficits, suggesting not only a tendency towards selecting the same previously rewarded action but also a greater tendency towards selecting the same stage 1 option irrespective of the outcome. The Libet clock task (Libet et al. 1983) is used to discriminate intention from movement awareness. Our findings indicate that, whereas M judgements are comparable between groups, the latency of the W or intention judgement in BD is shifted towards movement onset. Similarly, the W–M interval, an implicit measure of conscious awareness of volitional intention, is significantly reduced in BD than in HV. Crucially, we show that clinical variables appear to influence these measures in BD, with greater alcohol use severity predicting decreased conscious awareness of volitional intention and model‐freeness decreasing with the number of days after a binge episode.

A similar tendency towards habitual learning to the one observed here in BD has been previously observed in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder, methamphetamine addicts and subjects with binge eating disorder (Voon, Derbyshire, et al. 2015b). However, studies with alcohol‐dependent individuals have offered inconsistent results. Sjoerds et al. (2013) reported impaired goal‐directed learning in alcohol‐dependent subjects using an instrumental learning task. Following overtraining on stimulus–response–outcome contingencies, subjects underwent an outcome‐devaluation test. The impairment in goal‐directed learning in alcohol‐dependent subjects was associated with decreased activity in the ventromedial PFC and anterior putamen, and increased activity in posterior putamen (Sjoerds et al. 2013). Using the two‐step task, Sebold et al. (2014) showed impaired model‐based behaviours in alcohol‐dependent subjects tested early in abstinence (2 weeks). However, Voon, Derbyshire, et al. (2015b) did not find differences between alcohol‐dependent subjects and HV, which may be related to the timing of testing, as they showed that w scores increased with abstinence duration (2 weeks to 1 year), suggesting an improvement in goal‐directed behaviour with abstinence. In the current study, we observe that those BD who had been abstinent for longer had higher w and model‐based scores, and lower model‐free scores than the rest of the group. Together, these findings suggest that individuals with current heavy alcohol use and binge drinking behaviours rely less on goal‐directed behaviours. The fact that abstinence and the number of days after a binge episode improve these scores points to a state effect of alcohol use on this measure.

Our BD sample further displayed significantly reduced W–M intervals compared with HV, suggesting an altered experience of volition. Similar results have been described in disorders of the basal ganglia (Moretto et al. 2011; Tabu et al. 2015), stroke lesions of the IPC (Sirigu et al. 2004) and functional neurological disorder (Edwards et al. 2011; Baek et al. in press), but to our knowledge, intention awareness had not yet been explored in relation to alcohol use disorders. Furthermore, our data show that alcohol use severity in BD is associated with poorer intention awareness.

There are several plausible mechanisms by which both of these processes may be impaired. We show that working memory is unlikely to be playing a role. Similarly, non‐specific attentional deficits would be likely to influence attention to both W and M measures, whereas these findings are specific to a shift in W in the BD group. Impairments in inhibitory control are common in substance use disorders and may be relevant across both tasks. The relevance of the interval between the awareness of intention and movement has been highlighted in the veto hypothesis (Libet 1999; Haggard & Libet 2001), which suggests that the 200 milliseconds interval between W and M can be used to evaluate and change or stop the initiated movement. Therefore, the W–M interval allows for the implementation of cognitive control mechanisms, which are required to avoid impulsive or compulsive acts (Bari & Robbins 2013) and execute goal‐directed behaviour. These findings also build on studies focusing on impaired awareness in substance use disorders on multiple levels, including awareness of emotional states, autonomic and bodily signals, error monitoring, awareness of deficits and behavioural choices, and higher‐order metacognitive judgements of confidence. Methamphetamine‐dependent subjects show elevated alexithymia or impaired emotional awareness, with an abnormal relationship with dopamine receptor availability in the anterior insula and anterior cingulate relative to healthy controls (Okita et al. 2016). The role of the anterior insula, implicated in conscious awareness of bodily and autonomic signals, is relevant to addictions (Verdejo‐García, Clark & Dunn 2012; Paulus & Stewart 2014; Stewart et al. 2014) and has been implicated in stroke lesion studies (Naqvi et al. 2007), hypoactivity during cognitive control and negative emotions, and decreased anterior insular volumes (Senatorov et al. 2015). Impairments in self‐monitoring of error awareness have been related to hypoactivity in the anterior cingulate and insula (Hester, Nestor & Garavan 2009). Impairments in higher‐order metacognitive self‐awareness were shown with cocaine use disorder with impairments in the relationship between a visuospatial accuracy task and self‐reported confidence associated with decreased grey matter volume in the rostral anterior cingulate (Moeller et al. 2016). Deficits in self‐awareness of specific behaviours correlated with different brain regions; thus, impaired self‐awareness of apathy was associated with grey matter volume in the dorsal striatum, impulsivity with the orbitofrontal cortex and executive functioning with dorsolateral PFC in addiction (Moreno‐López et al. in press). Similar to our findings, impairments in self‐awareness improve with abstinence, including insight into the concordance between objective and subjective choices of drug‐related and non‐drug‐related cues (Moeller et al. 2010), self‐deception (Martínez‐González et al. 2016) and deficits in self‐awareness of frontal executive function (Verdejo‐García & Pérez‐García 2008).

Research suggests a partial overlap between the networks underlying intention awareness and goal‐directed behaviour, including lateral PFC and IPC (Lau et al. 2004; Sirigu et al. 2004; Gläscher et al. 2010) for goal‐directed behaviour and supplementary motor complex for habitual behaviours (Morris, Kundu, Dowell, et al. 2016b). The decrease in cognitive control implied by our data might be related to frontal anomalies observed in BD (Jacobus et al. 2009; McQueeny et al. 2009; Squeglia et al. 2012; Doallo et al. 2014; Vargas et al. 2014). Although partially reversible (Sullivan et al. 2005; Makris et al. 2008), the pernicious effects of alcohol on these structures are widely acknowledged. Both human and animal models have demonstrated that binge drinking compromises white matter fibre integrity in frontal and temporal regions (Jacobus et al. 2009; McQueeny et al. 2009; Vargas et al. 2014). Moreover, BD have been shown to have increased grey matter volumes in the VS (Howell et al. 2013), influenced by gender (Kvamme et al. 2016), and dlPFC (Doallo et al. 2014), and decreased cortical thickness in cingulate areas (Mashhoon et al. 2014). We have also recently shown that orientation dispersion index, a diffusion measure that putatively marks complexity of dendritic branching, in the same BD population under study demonstrates lower orientation dispersion index in regions implicated in higher‐order processing such as the dlPFC and parietal cortex and higher in the VS (Morris et al. in press). PFC input has been proven essential for both successful goal‐directed behaviour (Valentin et al. 2007; Tanaka et al. 2008; de Wit et al. 2009; Gläscher et al. 2010) and intention awareness (Lau et al. 2004), with model‐based learning further associated with efficient fronto‐striatal connectivity (de Wit et al. 2012; Morris, Kundu, Dowell, et al. 2016b).

From the perspective of action control, similar impairments in motor intention using the Libets clock task have been shown with Parkinson's disease, Tourette's syndrome and functional neurological disorders. These disorders are characterized by impaired self‐agency, or the sense of the loss of control over self‐generated actions. In particular, intriguing similarities may underlie the preceding sensory urge and associated motor tic in Tourette's syndrome and may provide insight into drug cue‐related urges triggered by sensory cue stimuli and subsequent motor (e.g. habitual or motivational) behaviour. The premonitory urge in Tourette's syndrome is involuntary and is commonly experienced as an involuntary sensory phenomenon (Cavanna et al. in press), and the tic itself has features of a voluntary movement in that its timing and decision to perform the tic movement in response to the urge can be controlled. A proportion of tics are preceded by a movement potential, a brain potential seen preceding voluntary movement, suggesting that tics have mixed features of voluntary and involuntary movements. The urge in tic disorders is similarly associated with activity in the anterior insula and anterior cingulate along with the supplementary motor area and parietal cortex (Bohlhalter et al. 2006; Lerner et al. 2007). A study focusing on the temporal generation of the urge demonstrated that the urge was associated with cortical activity at 2 seconds before the tic (supplementary motor area, primary sensory cortex and parietal cortex) and engaging subcortical regions 1 second before the tic (anterior cingulate cortex, putamen, insula, amygdala, cerebellum and occipital cortex) (Neuner et al. 2014). Tourette's syndrome has also been linked with lower gamma‐aminobutyric acid concentration in the sensory motor cortex along with decreased cortical thickness over the sensorimotor, insular and anterior cingulate cortices linked to the strength of premonitory urges (Draganski et al. 2010; Puts et al. 2015; Draper et al. 2016). Tourette's syndrome is also associated with enhanced habit formation correlating with greater structural connectivity of the supplementary motor cortex and sensorimotor putamen (Delorme et al. 2016).

The current study is not without limitations. Further characterization of the BD population in terms of whether they were hungover and further studies on mechanisms that might link the two including attentional mechanisms would be indicated. How these findings might relate to the literature on impaired awareness in substance misuse and the brain regions implicated would be useful. A prospective study using an alcohol challenge may be indicated.

Binge drinking is characterized by longer periods of abstinence between episodes of alcohol consumption (Courtney & Polich 2009). In the current sample of BD, not only was poorer intention awareness associated with alcohol use severity, but also subjects whose last binge drinking episode took place less than 4 days before testing displayed more model‐free habitual and less model‐based goal‐directed behaviour than those whose last binge drinking episode was more distant. In alcohol‐dependent subjects, abstinence has been observed to at least partially reverse some of the effects of alcohol abuse, at both the neuroanatomical and cognitive levels (Petry 2001; Sullivan et al. 2005; Makris et al. 2008; Voon, Derbyshire, et al. 2015b). Moreover, these findings dovetail with prior studies that have shown improvements of model‐based behaviours with abstinence in alcohol‐dependent subjects (Voon, Derbyshire, et al. 2015b). It is particularly interesting that this shift takes place over such a short period of time in BD. However, prior neuroanatomical findings suggest that BD might differ not only from HV but also from alcohol‐dependent subjects, with these differences possibly representing early sequelae, the compensatory effects of repeated binge and withdrawal patterns or even trait effects (Howell et al. 2013). The relationships observed here between intention awareness and alcohol use severity, and between model‐basedness, model‐freeness and abstinence suggest that top‐down volitional control might be ameliorated with alcohol use cessation.

Author Contributions

ND is responsible for the data analysis and interpretation, and drafting of the manuscript and figures; DS, KB and PB performed the data acquisition and drafting of the manuscript; and VV is responsible for the study conception and design, interpretation of the data and drafting of the manuscript. All authors have critically reviewed content and approved final version submitted for publication.

References

  1. Baek K, Doñamayor N, Morris LS, Strelchuk D, Mitchell S, Mikheenko Y, Yeoh SY, Phillips W, Zandi M, Jenaway A, Walsh C, Voon V (in press) Impaired awareness of motor intention in functional neurological disorder: implications for voluntary and functional movement. Psychol Med. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291717000071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bari A, Robbins TW (2013) Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog Neurobiol 108:44–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bauer LO, Ceballos NA (2014) Neural and genetic correlates of binge drinking among college women. Biol Psychol 97:43–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bohlhalter S, Goldfine A, Matteson S, Garraux G, Hanakawa T, Kansaku K, Wurzman R, Hallett M (2006) Neural correlates of tic generation in Tourette syndrome: an event‐related functional MRI study. Brain 129:2029–2037. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cavanna AE, Black KJ, Hallett M, Voon V (in press) Neurobiology of the premonitory urge in Tourette syndrome: pathophysiology and treatment implications. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16070141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Corbit LH, Nie H, Janak PH (2012) Habitual alcohol seeking: time course and the contribution of subregions of the dorsal striatum. Biol Psychiatry 72:389–395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.02.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Courtney KE, Polich J (2009) Binge drinking in young adults: data, definitions, and determinants. Psychol Bull 135:142–156. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Crabbe JC, Harris RA, Koob GF (2011) Preclinical studies of alcohol binge drinking. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1216:24–40. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749‐6632.2010.05895.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Crego A, Rodriguez‐Holguín S, Parada M, Mota N, Corral M, Cadaveira F (2010) Reduced anterior prefrontal cortex activation in young binge drinkers during a visual working memory task. Drug Alcohol Depend 109:45–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Danquah AN, Farrell MJ, O'Boyle DJ (2008) Biases in the subjective timing of perceptual events: Libet et al. (1983) revisited. Conscious Cogn 17:616–627. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Daw ND, Gershman SJ, Seymour B, Dayan P, Dolan RJ (2011) Model‐based influences on humans' choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron 69:1204–1215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. de Wit S, Barker RA, Dickinson AD, Cools R (2011) Habitual versus goal‐directed action control in Parkinson disease. J Cogn Neurosci 23:1218–1229. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. de Wit S, Corlett PR, Aitken MRF, Dickinson A, Fletcher PC (2009) Differential engagement of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex by goal‐directed and habitual behavior toward food pictures in humans. J Neurosci 29:11330–11338. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1639‐09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. de Wit S, Watson P, Harsay HA, Cohen MX, van de Vijver I, Ridderinkhof KR (2012) Corticostriatal connectivity underlies individual differences in the balance between habitual and goal‐directed action control. J Neurosci 32:12066–12075. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1088‐12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Delorme C, Salvador A, Valabrègue R, Roze E, Palminteri S, Vidailhet M, de Wit S, Robbins TW, Hartmann A, Worbe Y (2016) Enhanced habit formation in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Brain 139:605–615. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awv307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Desmurget M, Sirigu A (2012) Conscious motor intention emerges in the inferior parietal lobule. Curr Opin Neurobiol 22:1004–1011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Doallo S, Cadaveira F, Corral M, Mota N, López‐Caneda E, Holguín SR (2014) Larger mid‐dorsolateral prefrontal gray matter volume in young binge drinkers revealed by voxel‐based morphometry. PLoS One 9:e96380. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dolan RJ, Dayan P (2013) Goals and habits in the brain. Neuron 80:312–325. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Draganski B, Martino D, Cavanna AE, Hutton C, Orth M, Robertson MM, Critchley HD, Frackowiak RS (2010) Multispectral brain morphometry in Tourette syndrome persisting into adulthood. Brain 133:3661–3675. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awq300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Draper A, Jackson GM, Morgan PS, Jackson SR (2016) Premonitory urges are associated with decreased grey matter thickness within the insula and sensorimotor cortex in young people with Tourette syndrome. J Neuropsychol 10:143–153. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Edwards MJ, Moretto G, Schwingenschuh P, Katschnig P, Bhatia KP, Haggard P (2011) Abnormal sense of intention preceding voluntary movement in patients with psychogenic tremor. Neuropsychologia 49:2791–2793. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW (2013) From the ventral to the dorsal striatum: devolving views of their roles in drug addiction. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 37:1946–1954. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fried I, Katz A, McCarthy G, Sass KJ, Williamson P, Spencer SS, Spencer DD (1991) Functional organization of human supplementary motor cortex studied by electrical stimulation. J Neurosci 11:3656–3666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fried I, Mukamel R, Kreiman G (2011) Internally generated preactivation of single neurons in human medial frontal cortex predicts volition. Neuron 69:548–562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gillan CM, Morein‐Zamir S, Urcelay GP, Sule A, Voon V, Apergis‐Schoute AM, Fineberg NA, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW (2014) Enhanced avoidance habits in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 75:631–638. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gillan CM, Papmeyer M, Morein‐Zamir S, Sahakian BJ, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, de Wit S (2011) Disruption in the balance between goal‐directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 168:718–726. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10071062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gläscher J, Daw N, Dayan P, O'Doherty JP (2010) States versus rewards: dissociable neural prediction error signals underlying model‐based and model‐free reinforcement learning. Neuron 66:585–595. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gomes G (1998) The timing of conscious experience: a critical review and reinterpretation of Libet's research. Conscious Cogn 7:559–595. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1998.0332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hadj‐Bouziane F, Benatru I, Brovelli A, Klinger H, Thobois S, Broussolle E, Boussaoud D, Meunier M (2013) Advanced Parkinson's disease effect on goal‐directed and habitual processes involved in visuomotor associative learning. Front Hum Neurosci 6 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Haggard P, Libet B (2001) Conscious intention and brain activity. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8:47–64. [Google Scholar]
  31. Hester R, Nestor L, Garavan H (2009) Impaired error awareness and anterior cingulate cortex hypoactivity in chronic cannabis users. Neuropsychopharmacology 34:2450–2458. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hogarth L, Chase HW, Baess K (2012) Impaired goal‐directed behavioural control in human impulsivity. Q J Exp Psychol 65:305–316. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2010.518242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Howell NA, Worbe Y, Lange I, Tait R, Irvine M, Banca P, Harrison NA, Bullmore ET, Hutchison WD, Voon V (2013) Increased ventral striatal volume in college‐aged binge drinkers. PLoS One 8:e74164. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0074164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jacobus J, McQueeny T, Bava S, Schweinsburg BC, Frank LR, Yang TT, Tapert SF (2009) White matter integrity in adolescents with histories of marijuana use and binge drinking. Neurotoxicol Teratol 31:349–355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ntt.2009.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kvamme TL, Schmidt C, Strelchuk D, Chang‐Webb YC, Baek K, Voon V (2016) Sexually dimorphic brain volume interaction in college‐aged binge drinkers. NeuroImage: Clinical 10:310–317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2015.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lau HC, Rogers RD, Haggard P, Passingham RE (2004) Attention to intention. Science 303:1208–1210. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1090973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lerner A, Bagic A, Boudreau EA, Hanakawa T, Pagan F, Mari Z, Bara‐Jimenez W, Aksu M, Garraux G, Simmons JM, Sato S, Murphy DL, Hallett M (2007) Neuroimaging of neuronal circuits involved in tic generation in patients with Tourette syndrome. Neurology 68:1979–1987. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000264417.18604.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Libet B (1999) Do we have free will? Journal of Consciousness Studies 6:47–57. [Google Scholar]
  39. Libet B, Gleason CA, Wright EW, Pearl DK (1983) Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness‐potential). Brain 106:623–642. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/106.3.623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Makris N, Oscar‐Berman M, Jaffin SK, Hodge SM, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS, Marinkovic K, Breiter HC, Gasic GP, Harris GJ (2008) Decreased volume of the brain reward system in alcoholism. Biol Psychiatry 64:192–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Marsh R, Alexander GM, Packard MG, Zhu H, Wingard JC, Quackenbush G, Peterson BS (2004) Habit learning in Tourette syndrome: a translational neuroscience approach to a developmental psychopathology. Arch Gen Psychiatry 61:1259–1268. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.12.1259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Martínez‐González JM, Vilar López R, Becoña Iglesias E, Verdejo‐García A (2016) Self‐deception as a mechanism for the maintenance of drug addiction. Psicothema 28:13–19. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2015.139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mashhoon Y, Czerkawski C, Crowley DJ, Cohen‐Gilbert JE, Sneider JT, Silveri MM (2014) Binge alcohol consumption in emerging adults: anterior cingulate cortical “thinness” is associated with alcohol use patterns. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 38:1955–1964. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.12475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. McQueeny T, Schweinsburg BC, Schweinsburg AD, Jacobus J, Bava S, Frank LR, Tapert SF (2009) Altered white matter integrity in adolescent binge drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 33:1278–1285. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.00953.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Moeller SJ, Fleming SM, Gan G, Zilverstand A, Malaker P, d'Oleire Uquillas F, Schneider KE, Preston‐Campbell RN, Parvaz MA, Maloney T, Alia‐Klein N, Goldstein RZ (2016) Metacognitive impairment in active cocaine use disorder is associated with individual differences in brain structure. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 26:653–662. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Moeller SJ, Maloney T, Parvaz MA, Alia‐Klein N, Woicik PA, Telang F, Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Goldstein RZ (2010) Impaired insight in cocaine addiction: laboratory evidence and effects on cocaine‐seeking behaviour. Brain 133:1484–1493. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awq066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Moreno‐López L, Albein‐Urios N, Martínez‐González JM, Soriano‐Mas C, Verdejo‐García A (in press) Neural correlates of impaired self‐awareness of apathy, disinhibition and dysexecutive deficits in cocaine‐dependent individuals. Addict Biol. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Moretto G, Schwingenschuh P, Katschnig P, Bhatia KP, Haggard P (2011) Delayed experience of volition in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2010.221143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Morris LS, Dowell NG, Cercignani M, Harrison NA, Voon V (in press) Binge drinking differentially affects cortical and subcortical microstructure. Addiction Biology. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Morris LS, Kundu P, Baek K, Irvine MA, Mechelmans DJ, Wood J, Harrison NA, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET, Voon V (2016a) Jumping the gun: mapping neural correlates of waiting impulsivity and relevance across alcohol misuse. Biol Psychiatry 79:499–507. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Morris LS, Kundu P, Dowell N, Mechelmans DJ, Favre P, Irvine MA, Robbins TW, Daw N, Bullmore ET, Harrison NA, Voon V (2016b) Fronto‐striatal organization: defining functional and microstructural substrates of behavioural flexibility. Cortex 74:118–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Naqvi NH, Rudrauf D, Damasio H, Bechara A (2007) Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking. Science 315:531–534. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1135926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism . (2004, Winter). NIAAA council approves definition of binge drinking NIAAA Newsletter (Vol. 3, pp. 3).
  54. Neuner I, Werner CJ, Arrubla J, Stöcker T, Ehlen C, Wegener HP, Schneider F, Shah NJ (2014) Imaging the where and when of tic generation and resting state networks in adult Tourette patients. Front Hum Neurosci 8 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Okita K, Ghahremani DG, Payer DE, Robertson CL, Mandelkern MA, London ED (2016) Relationship of alexithymia ratings to dopamine D2‐type receptors in anterior cingulate and insula of healthy control subjects but not methamphetamine‐dependent individuals. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 19 https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyv129.pyv129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Paulus MP, Stewart JL (2014) Interoception and drug addiction. Neuropharmacology 76:342–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Petry NM (2001) Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 154:243–250. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002130000638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Puts NAJ, Harris AD, Crocetti D, Nettles C, Singer HS, Tommerdahl M, Edden RAE, Mostofsky SH (2015) Reduced GABAergic inhibition and abnormal sensory symptoms in children with Tourette syndrome. J Neurophysiol 114:808 https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00060.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sanchez‐Roige S, Baro V, Trick L, Pena‐Oliver Y, Stephens DN, Duka T (2014) Exaggerated waiting impulsivity associated with human binge drinking, and high alcohol consumption in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 39:2919–2927. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2014.151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sebold M, Deserno L, Nebe S, Schad DJ, Garbusow M, Hägele C, Keller J, Jänger E, Kathmann N, Smolka MN, Rapp MA, Schlagenhauf F, Heinz A, Huys QJM (2014) Model‐based and model‐free decisions in alcohol dependence. Neuropsychobiology 70:122–131. https://doi.org/10.1159/000362840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Senatorov VV, Damadzic R, Mann CL, Schwandt ML, George DT, Hommer DW, Heilig M, Momenan R (2015) Reduced anterior insula, enlarged amygdala in alcoholism and associated depleted von Economo neurons. Brain 138:69–79. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awu305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC (1998) The mini‐international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM‐IV and ICD‐10. J Clin Psychiatry 59:22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sirigu A, Daprati E, Ciancia S, Giraux P, Nighoghossian N, Posada A, Haggard P (2004) Altered awareness of voluntary action after damage to the parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 7:80–84. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sjoerds Z, de Wit S, van den Brink W, Robbins TW, Beekman ATF, Penninx BWJH, Veltman DJ (2013) Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence for overreliance on habit learning in alcohol‐dependent patients. Transl Psychiatry 3:e337. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2013.107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Squeglia L, Sorg S, Schweinsburg A, Wetherill R, Pulido C, Tapert S (2012) Binge drinking differentially affects adolescent male and female brain morphometry. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 220:529–539. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213‐011‐2500‐4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Stewart JL, May AC, Poppa T, Davenport PW, Tapert SF, Paulus MP (2014) You are the danger: attenuated insula response in methamphetamine users during aversive interoceptive decision‐making. Drug & Alcohol Dependence 142:110–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sullivan EV, Deshmukh A, De Rosa E, Rosenbloom MJ, Pfefferbaum A (2005) Striatal and forebrain nuclei volumes: contribution to motor function and working memory deficits in alcoholism. Biol Psychiatry 57:768–776. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Tabu H, Aso T, Matsuhashi M, Ueki Y, Takahashi R, Fukuyama H, Shibasaki H, Mima T (2015) Parkinson's disease patients showed delayed awareness of motor intention. Neurosci Res 95:74–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2015.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Tanaka SC, Balleine BW, O'Doherty JP (2008) Calculating consequences: brain systems that encode the causal effects of actions. J Neurosci 28:6750–6755. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1808‐08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Valentin VV, Dickinson A, O'Doherty JP (2007) Determining the neural substrates of goal‐directed learning in the human brain. J Neurosci 27:4019–4026. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0564‐07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. van der Meer MAA, Redish AD (2010) Expectancies in decision making, reinforcement learning, and ventral striatum. Front Neurosci 3 https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.01.006.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Vargas WM, Bengston L, Gilpin NW, Whitcomb BW, Richardson HN (2014) Alcohol binge drinking during adolescence or dependence during adulthood reduces prefrontal myelin in male rats. J Neurosci 34:14777–14782. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3189‐13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Verdejo‐García A, Clark L, Dunn BD (2012) The role of interoception in addiction: a critical review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 36:1857–1869. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Verdejo‐García A, Pérez‐García M (2008) Substance abusers' self‐awareness of the neurobehavioral consequences of addiction. Psychiatry Res 158:172–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2006.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Vollstädt‐Klein S, Wichert S, Rabinstein J, Bühler M, Klein O, Ende G, Hermann D, Mann K (2010) Initial, habitual and compulsive alcohol use is characterized by a shift of cue processing from ventral to dorsal striatum. Addiction 105:1741–1749. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360‐0443.2010.03022.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Voon V, Baek K, Enander J, Worbe Y, Morris LS, Harrison NA, Robbins TW, Rück C, Daw ND (2015a) Motivation and value influences in the relative balance of goal‐directed and habitual behaviours in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Transl Psychiatry 5:e670. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Voon V, Derbyshire K, Rück C, Irvine MA, Worbe Y, Enander J, Schreiber LRN, Gillan CM, Fineberg NA, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW, Harrison NA, Wood J, Daw ND, Dayan P, Grant JE, Bullmore ET (2015b) Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits. Mol Psychiatry 20:345–352. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2014.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Walsh E, Kühn S, Brass M, Wenke D, Haggard P (2010) EEG activations during intentional inhibition of voluntary action: an electrophysiological correlate of self‐control? Neuropsychologia 48:619–626. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Whelan R, Watts R, Orr CA, Althoff RR, Artiges E, Banaschewski T, Barker GJ, Bokde ALW, Büchel C, Carvalho FM, Conrod PJ, Flor H, Fauth‐Buhler M, Frouin V, Gallinat J, Gan G, Gowland P, Heinz A, Ittermann B, Lawrence C, Mann K, Martinot JL, Nees F, Ortiz N, Paillere‐Martinot ML, Paus T, Pausova Z, Rietschel M, Robbins TW, Smolka MN, Strohle A, Schumann G, Garavan H, the Imagen Consortium (2014) Neuropsychosocial profiles of current and future adolescent alcohol misusers. Nature 512:185–189. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Worbe Y, Irvine M, Lange I, Kundu P, Howell NA, Harrison NA, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, Voon V (2014) Neuronal correlates of risk‐seeking attitudes to anticipated losses in binge drinkers. Biol Psychiatry 76:717–724. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Wylie SA, Claassen DO, Kanoff KE, Ridderinkhof KR, van den Wildenberg WPM (2013) Impaired inhibition of prepotent motor actions in patients with Tourette syndrome. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 38:349–356. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.120138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Acknowledgements

N.D. is a research fellow of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DO1915/1‐1). This study was funded by the Wellcome Trust as part of a Wellcome Trust Intermediate Fellowship to V.V. (983705/Z/10/Z). V.V. is funded by a Medical Research Council Senior Fellowship (MR/P008747/1).

Doñamayor, N. , Strelchuk, D. , Baek, K. , Banca, P. , and Voon, V. (2018) The involuntary nature of binge drinking: goal directedness and awareness of intention. Addiction Biology, 23: 515–526. doi: 10.1111/adb.12505.


Articles from Addiction Biology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES