Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2018 Feb 16;8:3175. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-21428-4

Working Memory And Brain Tissue Microstructure: White Matter Tract Integrity Based On Multi-Shell Diffusion MRI

Sohae Chung 1,2, Els Fieremans 1,2, Nuri E Kucukboyaci 3, Xiuyuan Wang 1,2, Charles J Morton 1,2, Dmitry S Novikov 1,2, Joseph F Rath 4, Yvonne W Lui 1,2,
PMCID: PMC5816650  PMID: 29453439

Abstract

Working memory is a complex cognitive process at the intersection of sensory processing, learning, and short-term memory and also has a general executive attention component. Impaired working memory is associated with a range of neurological and psychiatric disorders, but very little is known about how working memory relates to underlying white matter (WM) microstructure. In this study, we investigate the association between WM microstructure and performance on working memory tasks in healthy adults (right-handed, native English speakers). We combine compartment specific WM tract integrity (WMTI) metrics derived from multi-shell diffusion MRI as well as diffusion tensor/kurtosis imaging (DTI/DKI) metrics with Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) subtests tapping auditory working memory. WMTI is a novel tool that helps us describe the microstructural characteristics in both the intra- and extra-axonal environments of WM such as axonal water fraction (AWF), intra-axonal diffusivity, extra-axonal axial and radial diffusivities, allowing a more biophysical interpretation of WM changes. We demonstrate significant positive correlations between AWF and letter-number sequencing (LNS), suggesting that higher AWF with better performance on complex, more demanding auditory working memory tasks goes along with greater axonal volume and greater myelination in specific regions, causing efficient and faster information process.

Introduction

Working memory is traditionally conceptualized as a hierarchical system with limited capacity and duration at the core of cognition and consciousness. It is composed of three main components: (1) the phonological loop which provides short-term memory traces for sounds by continuously refreshing the information through silent rehearsal; (2) the visual-spatial sketchpad which allows individuals to momentarily create and revisit a mental image that can be manipulated in complex tasks; and (3) the central executive which involves selective attention, inhibition, and shifting between tasks14. Involving both maintenance and manipulation of information, working memory is essential for higher-order functions such as comprehension, learning, reasoning, and decision making2,5. Deficits in working memory are fundamental problems associated with a wide range of progressive and non-progressive conditions including developmental disorders, learning disabilities, traumatic brain injury, stroke, and multiple sclerosis69. In fact, macrostructural alterations of the brain including focal decreases in brain volume10,11, cortical thickness12, and hippocampal volume13 have previously been associated with cognitive disability.

There is specific interest in understanding the microstructural correlates that contribute to such macroscopic changes as well as inform more subtle differences in working memory performance. Several diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) studies of white matter (WM) reveal positive correlations between fractional anisotropy (FA) and performance of a working memory task in fronto-parietal WM in children1416 and adults17; and also in the corpus callosum and posterior temporal WM in younger children18. Positive correlations between FA and verbal working memory performance have also been reported in right precuneal WM in normal young adults19. FA is, however, a nonspecific measure of directional diffusion, affected by a number of biophysical factors such as extracellular water, myelination, axon thickness and density20. In addition, diffusion kurtosis imaging (DKI)21 has been employed to characterize non-Gaussian diffusion and studies show an association between mean kurtosis (MK), a reflection of tissue microstructural complexity, and cognitive deficits in a number of pathologies including multiple sclerosis22, mild cognitive impairment23, Alzheimer’s23, and mild traumatic brain injury24. FA and MK, being empirical measures, intrinsically lack specific biophysical meaning. Thus, it remains unclear what biological structural differences may underlie the observed diffusion signal changes.

Most recently, compartment-specific WM tract integrity (WMTI) metrics derived from a WM modeling of multi-shell diffusion magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) have the potential to disentangle intra- and extra-axonal environments25. WMTI metrics include: 1) axonal water fraction [AWF], 2) intra-axonal diffusivity [Daxon, diffusivity within axons], 3) extra-axonal axial and 4) extra-axonal radial diffusivities [De, and De,, diffusion parallel and perpendicular to the axonal tracts in the extra-axonal space, respectively]. There are promising works showing WMTI metrics to be more specific to underlying tissue microstructure and mechanisms than empirical diffusion measures such as FA: WMTI metrics and tissue microstructure have been studied in several animal validation works, specifically examining demyelination and remyelination2629, as well as in human in vivo studies of neural development and disease. Briefly, alterations in AWF and De, appear to reflect axon density and myelination both in normal development30 and in Alzheimer’s disease31,32; alterations in Daxon have been shown in conditions of axon injury including stroke33 and mild traumatic brain injury34; and De, and De, demonstrate sensitivity to changes in extra-axonal diffusion (such as demyelination, gliosis and astrocytosis, extracellular inflammation) in patients with mild cognitive impairment31. Specifically with regard to working memory and brain microstructure, prior reports show strong association of visual working memory with AWF in healthy subjects35,36.

Here we investigate the relationship between diffusion metrics and performance on Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV)37 subtests tapping auditory working memory, including digit span forward (DSF), backward (DSB), sequencing (DSS), and letter-number sequencing (LNS) in healthy adults. The working memory tasks represent a progression from “simple span” tasks, focusing on short-term storage and deemphasizing the executive component of working memory, to “complex span” tasks which involve both storage and manipulation of information. In this progression, DSF, requiring simple repetition of numbers in the order presented, taps the phonological loop. DSB, which requires that numbers be re-ordered and repeated in reverse, taps both the phonological loop and executive components of working memory. DSS adds an element of semantic processing, because the meaning of the numbers presented must be comprehended in order to repeat them in ascending order. Finally, shifting between letters and numbers as required by LNS involves additional executive demands. LNS has been shown to involve processing speed and visual-spatial working memory components (relating to the strategy of visualizing numbers and letters as they are placed in ascending/alphabetical order) not tapped by DS tasks38. In order to avoid any confounding effects of language and handedness, we included only right-handed, native English speaking individuals in this study.

Results

Length of education of our subjects varied between 12 and 20 years (16 ± 2 years) and the WRAT-4 IQ scores were ranged from 88 to 134 (111 ± 15). The WAIS-IV DS and LNS test scaled scores were ranged from 7 to 19 and age-corrected z-scores were ranged from −1 to 3. They were not significantly correlated with age and length of education, but we found a trend for DSS and LNS with length of education. These are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1.

WAIS-IV working memory subtests scaled/z-scores (N = 15), and their correlation p-values with age and length of education.

Scaled score Z-score Correlation with age (p-value) Correlation with education (p-value)
Mean ± SD Min Max Mean ± SD Min Max
DSF 11.73 ± 3.06 7 16 0.58 ± 1.02 −1 2 0.47 0.56
DSB 12.20 ± 2.65 9 18 0.67 ± 0.87 −0.33 2.67 0.60 0.15
DSS 11.67 ± 3.06 8 17 0.55 ± 1.02 −0.67 2.33 0.43 0.056
LNS 12.60 ± 3.33 9 19 0.87 ± 1.11 −0.33 3 0.44 0.053

From TBSS analysis, there were statistically significant positive correlations of AWF and MK with LNS at the 95% confidence level after multiple comparisons. As shown in Fig. 1A, this significant correlation between AWF and LNS was present mainly in parietal WM, more prominently on the left. Specific areas based on MNI atlas include: right/left parietal WM, left superior and posterior corona radiata, and left body of corpus callosum. A region in the right anterior corona radiata also showed a significant positive correlation between MK and LNS (Fig. 1B). No other diffusion metrics showed area of significant correlations surviving multiple comparison corrections. No significant correlations were found with performance on the DS tasks. We found essentially identical results between using scaled scores and using z-scores.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

(A) Tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) results showing significantly positive correlations between axonal water fraction (AWF) and WAIS-IV letter-number sequencing (LNS) test z-scores. Mean FA skeleton (green) overlaid on the mean FA map. Significantly correlated voxels (corrected p < 0.05) are shown in red-yellow and involve left greater than right parietal white matter (WM) (specifically based on the MNI atlas: right/left parietal WM, left superior and posterior corona radiata, left body of corpus callosum). (B) Significantly correlated voxels between mean kurtosis (MK) and LNS test z-scores are present in the right anterior corona radiata. No negative correlations were found. Identical results were found with WAIS-IV LNS test scaled scores.

Figure 2 presents the scatter plots of each significant metric and LNS z-score, showing that higher AWF (Fig. 2A; r = 0.88) and higher MK (Fig. 2B; r = 0.92) are associated with better performance on the LNS task for those significant voxels on the skeleton in TBSS shown in Fig. 1.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Scatter plots showing significant correlations. (A) Between AWF and LNS z-scores (r = 0.88) and (B) between MK and LNS z-scores (r = 0.92) for voxels on the skeleton with statistically significant association in TBSS (corrected p < 0.05; shown in Fig. 1).

Discussion

This study demonstrates statistically significant WM microstructural associations with performance on a select auditory working memory task, LNS. It is interesting to note that no significant correlations were observed with performance on the three relatively simpler WAIS-IV DS tasks (recalling numbers in order, backwards, and reordering in WAIS-IV DS subtest). However, when subjects were asked to perform LNS, the most complex and demanding WAIS-IV working memory subtest which requires shifting between letters and numbers, significant correlations were observed in AWF as well as MK. While processing speed has been shown to contribute to LNS performance38, past research agrees with the WAIS development team37 that many aspects of crystallized (e.g., high reading level39) and fluid intelligence (e.g., strong visuospatial working memory) may contribute to high scores in this test.

Our findings show WM microstructure to be more closely associated with more complex tasks of working memory that also require efficiencies in processing speed and fluid intelligence. The greater localization to the left hemisphere is consistent with typical lateralization for auditory-verbal working memory40. The correlation shown in a small right frontal region supports virtual visual rehearsal and manipulation that many individuals employ in order to successfully complete such tasks38. Indeed, a prior report shows associations of visual short-term memory with WM microstructure in the superior longitudinal fasciculus and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus within the right hemisphere41. Our findings in the parietal WM regions may also relate to visual rehearsal42.

The correlation coefficients observed in the significant regions are high between AWF and LNS (r = 0.88), and between MK and LNS (r = 0.92). Higher AWF has previously been shown in two conditions; higher axonal density as well as greater myelination28,43. Higher MK reflects greater tissue microstructural complexity29,44. Differences between subjects in terms of axonal density and myelination would certainly be expected to influence neuronal signal transmission45 and could conceivably contribute to greater efficiency in information processing38.

We find no significant correlation between FA and these tests of working memory. The literature is variable with some papers showing positive correlations with FA though in differing brain regions17,19 and others showing no correlation35,46. This may be because FA is inherently a nonspecific empiric measure affected by multiple different microstructural features including myelination, axon density, extracellular fluid content, etc47.

This study includes subjects with age range of 19 to 45 years old. High average reliability of LNS scores (0.86–0.91) in our subjects across age and low (<14%) coefficient of variability48 seen here agree with prior research documenting relative stability of working memory performance in healthy adults, characterized by a slow decline in average scores beginning in middle age4951. In addition, we address any remaining age effects by using age-corrected z-scores derived from a published normative sample (n = 2200) divided into 13 age bands, spanning ages 16 to 9037. This analysis was also cross-checked using the scaled scores of the cognitive tests, which showed no differences in the results. Also, DTI studies52,53 have revealed that this age range is a relatively stable period and possibly contributes to why age-related changes were not observed in this study. Furthermore, all statistical analyses included age and sex as covariates and were not found to have an effect on the findings.

TBSS, used here, is an established methodology, widely used for voxelwise analysis of whole brain WM diffusion measures. Limitations of TBSS include the fact that WM texture is not interrogated using this method as only the maximum value along a line orthogonal to the skeleton is projected onto the skeleton. Other potential problems that have been reported associated with TBSS including effects of partial volume, skeleton shapes, image noise level and registration error54. While TBSS certainly has limitations, TBSS has several strengths which make it an excellent choice of methodology including skeletonization which reduces the need for data smoothing, alleviates residual image misalignment and gains statistical power from reducing dimensionality54. In this study, default parameter settings have been employed and potentially problematic regions (e.g., low FA (<0.2) regions such as fornix, uncinate fasciculus) have not been included in the results, as recommended by Bach et al.54. By using skeletonized WM, TBSS is useful in substantially decreasing the number of comparisons compared with a voxel-by-voxel based approach; however, there remain potential limitations in the methodology applied to small sample size studies55. In particular, variance in diffusion metrics across healthy subjects has been reported, particularly affecting FA55. Variance in FA may reflect why no correlations in FA were detected in our study. Kurtosis measures, on the other hand, show less variance. Based on previously reported intersubject variance in DKI metrics, our study is powered to detect an effect size of 10% at 0.9 statistical power56. In particular, Jensen et al.57 have showed the WMTI model parameters to have deviations being less than 10% at FA threshold ≥0.3 which is much lower as detailed in the De Santis et al. paper55, putting the sample size employed in this work in a reasonable range. Moreover, potential error relating to default FA-based TBSS registration would be further improved by using a tensor-based registration54; however, it was not implemented in this study.

Finally, the WMTI model employed here makes two main assumptions regarding fiber alignment and diffusion compartmentalization: 1) It assumes regions of highly aligned fiber bundles and therefore, in this work, the WM regions were thresholded to FA ≥ 0.4 as has been previously described25,57 (about 30% of total WM) to restrict TBSS analysis to regions of reasonable high directional diffusion for calculation of WMTI metrics; FA threshold at 0.4 conservatively restricts the WM assessed to area where the WMTI model is valid based on Jensen et al.57, and 2) the model assumes Daxon ≤ De,; which is supported by several animal validation studies26,28 though remains a topic of debate5861; Nevertheless, AWF is independent of this latter assumption and therefore the main result of this investigation is not affected.

In summary, our findings support a real potential of diffusion metrics not only to identify WM microstructural associations with working memory but to begin to parse out the meaning behind such relationships, suggesting that higher AWF with better performance on LNS goes along with greater axonal volume and greater myelination in specific regions. Elucidating the links between brain microstructure and working memory has generalizable value across normal and pathologic conditions such as aging, dementia, and abnormalities of attention including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

Methods

Study Population

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at New York University School of Medicine, and all experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. All subjects were prospectively recruited and provided written informed consent before the procedure. We studied 15 healthy individuals (mean age, 31 ± 7 years old; age range, 19–45 years old; 7 males). Exclusion criteria included: 1) reported history of brain disorders, head trauma or psychotic disorders; 2) non-native English speakers and 3) non-right-handed individuals to avoid confounding effects of language and handedness in both WAIS-IV test performance and WM microstructure. Per study procedures, all subjects underwent formal neurocognitive testing using WAIS-IV working memory subtests37 and a brain MRI scan within one day of each other. Wide Range Achievement Test-4th Edition Word Reading subtest (WRAT-4) was also performed to help characterize subjects and the scores were converted to IQ scores for a brief measure of academic achievement.

MRI Acquisition

MR imaging data were acquired on a 3 T MR scanner (Skyra, Siemens Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Germany). Diffusion imaging was performed at multiple shells: 5 b-values (250, 1000, 1500, 2000, 2500 s/mm2) along with 5 diffusion encoding direction schemes (6, 20, 20, 30, 60, respectively) using multiband (factor of two)62 EPI for accelerated acquisitions with anterior-posterior (AP) phase encoding direction. Three non-weighted diffusion images (b = 0 s/mm2) were also acquired. Other imaging parameters were: FOV = 220 mm × 220 mm, resolution = 2.5 × 2.5 × 2.5 mm3, matrix = 88 × 88, slices = 56, TR/TE = 4900/95 ms, bandwidth = 2104 Hz/pixel, a generalized autocalibrating partially parallel acquisitions (GRAPPA) factor of two. For geometric distortion correction, an additional image with b = 0 s/mm2 was acquired with the same imaging parameters, but with reversed (PA) phase encoding direction63. Standard clinical sequences including the MPRAGE (FOV = 256 mm × 256 mm, resolution = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3, matrix = 256 × 256, TR/TE/TI = 2100/3.19/900 ms), FLAIR (FOV = 256 mm × 256 mm, resolution = 0.7 × 0.7 × 5 mm3, matrix = 220 × 220, TR/TE/TI = 9000/90/2500 ms) and SWI (FOV = 256 mm × 256 mm, resolution = 0.7 × 0.7 × 3 mm3, matrix = 220 × 220) were done to assess the presence of any brain abnormalities.

Working Memory Assessment

Working memory was assessed within one day that subjects underwent MRI scanning, using two WAIS-IV subtests37: 1) Digit Span (DS), which includes a) DS Forward (DSF) - examinees repeat back a sequence of numbers read to them, b) DS Backward (DSB) - examinees repeat back a sequence of numbers read to them, in reverse order, c) DS Sequencing (DSS) - examinees repeat back a sequence of numbers read to them, in ascending order, and 2) Letter-Number Sequencing (LNS) - examinees repeat back a sequence of numbers and letters read to them, numbers first in ascending order, followed by letters in alphabetical order. All measures were administered following standardized testing procedures, under the supervision of licensed clinical psychologists blinded to MRI results. Raw scores are converted to scaled scores (defined as mean of 10 with SD = 3, ranging 1–19) with higher scores indicating higher ability. In order to effectively eliminate age as a confounding factor for DS and LNS scores, age-corrected z-scores with a zero mean and a unitary variance were derived from the WAIS-IV normative sample (n = 2200) which was divided into 13 age bands spanning ages 16 to 9037. Both scaled scores and z-scores were used for analysis.

Image Analyses

Diffusion image processing

The pre-processing steps for the diffusion weighted images include Marchenko-Pastur principal component analysis (MP-PCA) denoising64, Gibbs correction65, geometric EPI distortion correction (FSL’s function topup), eddy current distortion and motion correction (FSL’s function eddy), and outlier detection66. In-house image processing software developed in MATLAB R2017a (The Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA) was used to calculate maps of WMTI metrics (AWF, Daxon, De,, De,), as well as both DTI metrics (FA, mean diffusivity [MD], axial diffusivity [AD], radial diffusivity [RD]) and DKI metrics (MK, axial kurtosis [AK], RK).

Tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS)

Voxel-wise analyses were performed to reveal possible correlations between working memory subtest scores and the diffusion parametric maps by using the standard procedure of tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS)67. With TBSS, all subjects’ FA maps were registered to a FA template (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/FMRIB58_FA) and voxel-wise statistics were performed on FA values projected onto the study-specific WM skeleton by looking for maximum local values perpendicular to the skeleton. All other parametric maps underwent the same transformations and processes. The tract skeleton was thresholded at FA of 0.2 for DTI and DKI metrics and at FA of 0.4 to restrict analysis to WM regions consisting of single-fiber orientations for WMTI metrics, as recommended25,57. Statistical tests were conducted using the permutation-based nonparametric analysis routine ‘randomise’ with 10000 iterations; the design matrix was set up for the correlation analysis with covariates (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/GLM). The results were corrected for multiple comparisons using threshold free cluster enhancement (TFCE) in TBSS.

Statistical Analysis

Spearman rank correlation was performed to assess the association of the DS and LNS test scores with age and length of education.

For TBSS analysis, age and sex were included as covariates. Statistical threshold level of p < 0.05 (corrected for multiple correction) was used. Spearman’s partial rank correlation coefficients were also calculated for regions on the skeleton with corrected p < 0.05, adjusted for age and sex using MATLAB R2017a.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported in part by grant funding from the National Institutes of Health (NIH): R01 NS039135-11 and R21 NS090349, National Institute for Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS). This work was also performed under the rubric of the Center for Advanced Imaging Innovation and Research (CAI2R, www.cai2r.net), a NIBIB Biomedical Technology Resource Center (NIH P41 EB017183).

Author Contributions

S.C. and X.W. processed and analyzed MRI data. E.F. and D.S.N. provided a set of diffusion image-processing tools. N.E.K. and J.F.R. performed and oversaw working memory testing, scoring and norming, and wrote parts of the manuscript text. C.J.M. recruited research participants and consent them. S.C. and Y.W.L. wrote the main manuscript text and prepared figures. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Baddeley A. Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:829–839. doi: 10.1038/nrn1201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Conway AR, Kane MJ, Engle RW. Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7:547–552. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Luck SJ, Vogel EK. Visual working memory capacity: from psychophysics and neurobiology to individual differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17:391–400. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Malenka, R., Nestler, E. & Hyman, S. In Molecular Neuropharmacology: A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience 313–321 (McGraw-Hill Medical, 2009).
  • 6.Wingfield A, Stine EAL, Lahar CJ, Aberdeen JS. Does the capacity of working memory change with age. Exp Aging Res. 1988;14:103–107. doi: 10.1080/03610738808259731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Goldman-Rakic PS. Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;6:348–357. doi: 10.1176/jnp.6.4.348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Sandry J. Working memory and memory loss in neurodegenerative disease. Neurodegener Dis Manag. 2015;5:1–4. doi: 10.2217/nmt.14.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sandry, J., Chiou, K. S., DeLuca, J. & Chiaravalloti, N. D. Individual differences in working memory capacity predicts responsiveness to memory rehabilitation after traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil97, 1026–1029 e1021, 10.1016/j.apmr.2015.10.109 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 10.Soria-Pastor S, et al. Decreased regional brain volume and cognitive impairment in preterm children at low risk. Pediatrics. 2009;124:e1161–1170. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-0244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sanfilipo MP, Benedict RHB, Sharma J, Weinstock-Guttman B, Bakshi R. The relationship between whole brain volume and disability in multiple sclerosis: A comparison of normalized gray vs. white matter with misclassification correction. Neuroimage. 2005;26:1068–1077. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wang L, et al. Alterations in cortical thickness and white matter integrity in mild cognitive impairment measured by whole-brain cortical thickness mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. Am J Neuroradiol. 2009;30:893–899. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A1484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Shi F, Liu B, Zhou Y, Yu C, Jiang T. Hippocampal volume and asymmetry in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: Meta-analyses of MRI studies. Hippocampus. 2009;19:1055–1064. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Olesen PJ, Nagy Z, Westerberg H, Klingberg T. Combined analysis of DTI and fMRI data reveals a joint maturation of white and grey matter in a fronto-parietal network. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2003;18:48–57. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Klingberg T. Development of a superior frontal-intraparietal network for visuo-spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:2171–2177. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Darki F, Klingberg T. The role of fronto-parietal and fronto-striatal networks in the development of working memory: a longitudinal study. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25:1587–1595. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Schulze ET, et al. Anatomical correlates of age-related working memory declines. J Aging Res. 2011;2011:606871. doi: 10.4061/2011/606871. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bathelt, J., Gathercole, S. E., Johnson, A. & Astle, D. E. Differences in brain morphology and working memory capacity across childhood. Dev Sci. 10.1111/desc.12579 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 19.Takeuchi H, et al. Verbal working memory performance correlates with regional white matter structures in the frontoparietal regions. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:3466–3473. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.08.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Basser PJ, Pierpaoli C. Microstructural and physiological features of tissues elucidated by quantitative-diffusion-tensor MRI. J Magn Reson B. 1996;111:209–219. doi: 10.1006/jmrb.1996.0086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Jensen JH, Helpern JA. MRI quantification of non-Gaussian water diffusion by kurtosis analysis. NMR Biomed. 2010;23:698–710. doi: 10.1002/nbm.1518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bester M, et al. Non-Gaussian diffusion MRI of gray matter is associated with cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. 2015;21:935–944. doi: 10.1177/1352458514556295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Falangola MF, et al. Non-Gaussian diffusion MRI assessment of brain microstructure in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Magn Reson Imaging. 2013;31:840–846. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2013.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Grossman EJ, et al. Thalamus and cognitive impairment in mild traumatic brain injury: a diffusional kurtosis imaging study. J Neurotrauma. 2012;29:2318–2327. doi: 10.1089/neu.2011.1763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Fieremans E, Jensen JH, Helpern JA. White matter characterization with diffusional kurtosis imaging. Neuroimage. 2011;58:177–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Guglielmetti C, et al. Diffusion kurtosis imaging probes cortical alterations and white matter pathology following cuprizone induced demyelination and spontaneous remyelination. Neuroimage. 2016;125:363–377. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Falangola MF, et al. Histological correlation of diffusional kurtosis and white matter modeling metrics in cuprizone-induced corpus callosum demyelination. NMR Biomed. 2014;27:948–957. doi: 10.1002/nbm.3140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jelescu IO, et al. In vivo quantification of demyelination and recovery using compartment-specific diffusion MRI metrics validated by electron microscopy. Neuroimage. 2016;132:104–114. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kelm ND, et al. Evaluation of diffusion kurtosis imaging in ex vivo hypomyelinated mouse brains. Neuroimage. 2016;124:612–626. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Jelescu IO, et al. One diffusion acquisition and different white matter models: how does microstructure change in human early development based on WMTI and NODDI? Neuroimage. 2015;107:242–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Fieremans E, et al. Novel white matter tract integrity metrics sensitive to Alzheimer disease progression. Am J Neuroradiol. 2013;34:2105–2112. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Benitez A, et al. White matter tract integrity metrics reflect the vulnerability of late-myelinating tracts in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage Clin. 2014;4:64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hui ES, et al. Stroke assessment with diffusional kurtosis imaging. Stroke. 2012;43:2968–2973. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.112.657742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Grossman EJ, et al. N-acetyl-aspartate levels correlate with intra-axonal compartment parameters from diffusion MRI. Neuroimage. 2015;118:334–343. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Golestani A, et al. Constrained by our connections: white matter’s key role in interindividual variability in visual working memory capacity. J Neurosci. 2014;34:14913–14918. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2317-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Lazar M. Working memory: how important is white matter? The Neuroscientist. 2017;23:197–210. doi: 10.1177/1073858416634298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Fourth edn, (Pearson Assessment, 2008).
  • 38.Crowe SF. Does the letter number sequencing task measure anything more than digit span? Assessment. 2000;7:113–117. doi: 10.1177/107319110000700202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Frick A, Wahlin TBR, Pachana NA, Byrne GJ. Relationships between the national adult reading test and memory. Neuropsychology. 2011;25:397–403. doi: 10.1037/a0021988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Thomason ME, et al. Development of spatial and verbal working memory capacity in the human brain. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21:316–332. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Chechlacz M, Gillebert CR, Vangkilde SA, Petersen A, Humphreys GW. Structural variability within frontoparietal networks and individual differences in attentional functions: an approach using the theory of visual attention. J Neurosci. 2015;35:10647–10658. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0210-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Todd JJ, Marois R. Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature. 2004;428:751–754. doi: 10.1038/nature02466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Novikov, D. S. & Fieremans, E. Relating extracellular diffusivity to cell size distribution and packing density as applied to white matter. ISMRM 20th annual meeting & exhibition, Melbourne, Australia. In Proc. Intl. Soc. Mag. Reson. Med. 20. p.1829, (2012).
  • 44.Cheung MM, et al. Does diffusion kurtosis imaging lead to better neural tissue characterization? A rodent brain maturation study. Neuroimage. 2009;45:386–392. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hartline DK, Colman DR. Rapid conduction and the evolution of giant axons and myelinated fibers. Curr Biol. 2007;17:R29–R35. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.11.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Peters BD, et al. Age-related differences in white matter tract microstructure are associated with cognitive performance from childhood to adulthood. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75:248–256. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Jito J, et al. Maturational changes in diffusion anisotropy in the rat corpus callosum: comparison with quantitative histological evaluation. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2008;28:847–854. doi: 10.1002/jmri.21496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wisdom NM, Mignogna J, Collins RL. Variability in Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV subtest performance across age. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2012;27:389–397. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acs041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Kessels RP, Molleman PW, Oosterman JM. Assessment of working-memory deficits in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s dementia using Wechsler’s Working Memory Index. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2011;23:487–490. doi: 10.1007/BF03325245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Economou A. Memory score discrepancies by healthy middle-aged and older individuals: the contributions of age and education. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15:963–972. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709990580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Wang M, et al. Neuronal basis of age-related working memory decline. Nature. 2011;476:210–213. doi: 10.1038/nature10243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Hasan KM, et al. Development and organization of the human brain tissue compartments across the lifespan using diffusion tensor imaging. Neuroreport. 2007;18:1735–1739. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f0d40c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hasan KM, et al. Diffusion tensor quantification of the human midsagittal corpus callosum subdivisions across the lifespan. Brain Res. 2008;1227:52–67. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.06.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Bach M, et al. Methodological considerations on tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) Neuroimage. 2014;100:358–369. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.De Santis S, Drakesmith M, Bells S, Assaf Y, Jones DK. Why diffusion tensor MRI does well only some of the time: Variance and covariance of white matter tissue microstructure attributes in the living human brain. Neuroimage. 2014;89:35–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Szczepankiewicz F, et al. Variability in diffusion kurtosis imaging: impact on study design, statistical power and interpretation. Neuroimage. 2013;76:145–154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Jensen, J. H., McKinnon, E. T., Glenn, G. R. & Helpern, J. A. Evaluating kurtosis-based diffusion MRI tissue models for white matter with fiber ball imaging. NMR Biomed30, 10.1002/nbm.3689 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 58.Novikov, D. S., Veraart, J., Jelescu, I. O. & Fieremans, E. Mapping orientational and microstructural metrics of neuronal integrity with in vivo diffusion MRI. arXiv:1609.09144v1 [physics.bio-ph] (2016).
  • 59.Lampinen B, et al. Neurite density imaging versus imaging of microscopic anisotropy in diffusion MRI: A model comparison using spherical tensor encoding. Neuroimage. 2017;147:517–531. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Reisert M, Kellner E, Dhital B, Hennig J, Kiselev VG. Disentangling micro from mesostructure by diffusion MRI: A Bayesian approach. Neuroimage. 2017;147:964–975. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Veraart, J., Fieremans, E. & Novikov, D. S. Universal power-law scaling of water diffusion in human brain defines what we see with MRI. arXiv:1609.09145v1 [physics.bio-ph] (2016).
  • 62.Setsompop K, et al. Blipped-controlled aliasing in parallel imaging for simultaneous multislice echo planar imaging with reduced g-factor penalty. Magn Reson Med. 2012;67:1210–1224. doi: 10.1002/mrm.23097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Holland D, Kuperman JM, Dale AM. Efficient correction of inhomogeneous static magnetic field-induced distortion in Echo Planar Imaging. Neuroimage. 2010;50:175–183. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Veraart, J., Fieremans, E. & Novikov, D. S. Diffusion MRI noise mapping using random matrix theory. Magn Reson Med 1582–1593, 10.1002/mrm.26059 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 65.Kellner E, Dhital B, Kiselev VG, Reisert M. Gibbs-ringing artifact removal based on local subvoxel-shifts. Magn Reson Med. 2016;76:1574–1581. doi: 10.1002/mrm.26054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Collier Q, Veraart J, Jeurissen B, den Dekker AJ, Sijbers J. Iterative reweighted linear least squares for accurate, fast, and robust estimation of diffusion magnetic resonance parameters. Magn Reson Med. 2015;73:2174–2184. doi: 10.1002/mrm.25351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Smith SM, et al. Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage. 2006;31:1487–1505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES