Skip to main content
BMC Pediatrics logoLink to BMC Pediatrics
. 2018 Feb 19;18:68. doi: 10.1186/s12887-018-1017-y

Maternal socio-demographic and psychological predictors for risk of developmental delays among young children in Mongolia

Amarjargal Dagvadorj 1,2,, Duurenbayar Ganbaatar 3, Olukunmi O Balogun 1, Naohiro Yonemoto 4, Bayasgalantai Bavuusuren 3, Kenji Takehara 1, Rintaro Mori 1, Moe Akahira-Azuma 5
PMCID: PMC5817794  PMID: 29458342

Abstract

Background

Factors influencing child development are not well studied in developing settings, and especially in Mongolia. This cohort study examined the relationship between maternal socio-demographic and psychological conditions on risk of young child developmental delay.

Methods

A total of 150 children aged between 13 ~ 24 months old participated in this study. The participants were randomly selected from a pre-existing cohort of 1297 children who were involved in a study on infant bilirubin nomogram development conducted at a tertiary health facility in Mongolia between 2012 and 2013. Child development was evaluated using the Mongolian Rapid Baby Scale (MORBAS), a validated scale for child development. The potential factors for child developmental delay were assessed using a pre-tested questionnaire comprising of 52 questions. Fisher’s exact test and multivariable logistic regression analysis were conducted.

Results

Seventeen (11%) out of the 150 children that participated in the study were at risk of developmental delay. There was a negative association between the risk of child developmental delay and higher maternal education (AOR 0.15, 95% CI: 0.03–0.66). Increasing maternal age (AOR 1.12, 95%CI: 0.98–1.27), maternal depression symptoms (AOR 4.93, 95%CI: 0.93–26.10), child gender being female (AOR 0.25, 95%CI: 0.06–1.00) and being from single mother household (AOR 0.14, 95%CI: 0.01–1.11) were also predictors for risk of developmental delay – although the association was marginal.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that being of underprivileged social status, and poor psychological condition of mothers in Mongolia possibly increases the risk of child developmental delays. Interventions targeting these modifiable predictors are needed to develop prevention strategies for child developmental delay.

Keywords: Child development, Developing country, Maternal education, Mongolia, Risk of child development

Background

Many children in developing countries are exposed to multiple risks which limit their cognitive, motor, and social-emotional development, and research on factors affecting early child development is scarce in many of these countries. An estimated 20–25% of young children in developing countries are known to be experiencing lack of basic needs their normal development such as lack of cognitive stimulation, inadequate nutrition resulting in stunting, iron and iodine deficiency [1]. Various factors contribute to the risk of child developmental delays and can be broardly divided into biological and psychosocial factors [1].

The main biological risk factors that compromise child development include preterm birth [2] and low birthweight (LBW) [1, 3]. As opposed to term babies, lower gestational age at birth babies are reported to have increased likelihood of poorer neurodevelopmental outcomes, especially among infants in low– and middle–income countries [2]. Similarly, being born LBW is known to delay the developmental processes as compared to normal birthweight infants [3]. Specifically, LBW infants are known to have significantly lower mental development and psychomotor development index scores when compared to average birthweight infants [3].

Other biological risk factors include low apgar score and neonatal jaundice. Newborns who have lower apgar scores are reported to be at risk of encephalopathy and other developmental problems and they have adverse outcomes related to asphyxia [4]. A recent review involving studies from both developing and developed countries reported increased risk of developmental delays in children with severe neonatal jaundice [5]. Furthermore, late diagnosis of neonatal jaundice may also cause physical and mental retardation including hearing problems, and visual impairments [5, 6]. These conditions are reported to occur in up to 40% of newborns with moderate hyperbilirubinemia (serum bilirubin concentration 10–20 mg/dl) increasing the risk of other neurodevelopmental problems [7].

Psychosocial risk factors known to hinder child development include maternal depression and child rearing dimensions [8]. Maternal depression may affect child rearing behaviour [9], wherein depressed mothers are more likely to be uninvolved, less sensitive, and negative when interacting with their children [9]. Meanwhile, interventional studies conducted in developing countries showed that cognitive stimulation of young children by their parents promoted higher cognitive functioning in young children significantly [1].

Recent nationwide surveys in Mongolia show that 22.9% of children aged between 2 and 9 years old had mental or physical impairments [10]. However, prevalence of developmental delay and factors that could be putting young children aged between 1 and 2 at risk of developmental delay is not well studied in Mongolia. This area of research is important, as the younger the child, the faster the improvement following an intervention [11]. Reliable and updated information on specific conditions relating to child development would allow us to raise awareness on prevention and intervention strategies for preventing developmental delay in Mongolia. Given that our study was conducted in a developing country, the main conceptual framework is based on the framework proposed by the Walker et al. on child development risk factors in developing countries [1]. This framework includes only modifiable risk factors which can be influenced through interventions or public policy.

Therefore in this study, we aimed to evaluate child development using a validated, country specific scale and determine related predictors in children aged between 13 ~ 24 months old in Mongolia.

Methods

Study population

This is a follow-up study to Akahira-Azuma et al., 2015 that established the hour-specific transcutaenus bilirubin nomogram in Mongolian neonates. Study participants were randomly selected from among 1297 healthy term and late-preterm neonates who participated in the baseline study. All infants born to women enrolled at a tertiary level health facility in Mongolia between October 2012 and September 2013 participated in the baseline study. Details of the setting and participants characteristics have been described previously [12]. This follow-up study was conducted among children aged 13–24 months old following telephone recruitment between October and November, 2014. A total of 150 children were recruited.

Exposure and outcome

According to the framework used in this study [1], risk factors on child development in developing countries can be grouped into four main domains including poverty, socio-cultural factors, biological factors and psychosocial factors. Based on this framework, we made effort to include the main factors from each of the different domains. Poverty level was described by wealth index while for socio-cultural risk factors, we included child gender and maternal education to reflect gender inequity and low maternal education. Biological risks factors included variables representing prenatal and postnatal growth such as delivery mode, gestational age at birth, birthweight, apgar score, transcutaenous bilirubin level, season of birth and exclusive breastfeeding. Biological factors specific to the mother were parity, history of miscarriage, and disease during pregnancy. For psychosocial risks factors, we included environmental and parenting factors such as family crowding, maternal work, single mother household and maternal depression symptoms. Impairment in child development was thereafter assessed in seven child developmental domains.

Baseline characteristics of participants were obtained from the previous study while information regarding the exposures and outcomes were collected during the follow-up survey. The primary outcome in this study was child development. Child development was evaluated using the Mongolian Rapid Baby Scale (MORBAS) [13]. MORBAS is an easy to use rapid screening tool for healthcare providers and parents to evaluate risk of developmental delay in young children. The tool is comprised of seven developmental domains on gross motor, fine motor, cognitive, expressive language, receptive language, social-emotional, and adaptive-behavior. A validation study conducted by comparing the MORBAS with an international gold standard, Bayley-III showed good concurrent, face and content validity [13]. In current study, MORBAS was utilized by healthcare providers at the study hospital to assess risk of developmental delay. Each assessment lasted approximately 15 min.

Information on date of birth, gender, gestational age at birth, birthweight, Apgar score, mode of delivery, maternal age, number of pregnancies, number of deliveries, maternal blood type, jaundice in siblings, feeding at discharge and transcutaenus bilirubin measurement during the first 6 days after birth were obtained from the baseline data [12].

Additional data were collected in the follow-up study using a 52 item pre-tested questionnaire. The questionnaire was divided into two parts and assessed maternal characteristics in terms of socioeconomic status and health (health behavior, history of neonatal jaundice, breastfeeding practices, and disease history) specific to the index child. Information on maternal depression symptoms was obtained using the Self-Reporting Questionnaire (SRQ)-20 [14]. The SRQ-20 has previously been validated among Mongolian women of childbearing age [14] with a cut-off for detecting maternal depression symptoms defined as SRQ-20 score ≥9. Economic status was assessed using data on asset ownership and household characteristics at follow-up and a wealth index was constructed from the data using principal components analysis [15]. Participants were then categorized using this index scores into tertiles according to a 3-point scale ranging from 1 (poor) to 3 (rich).

Given that minimum maternal education attained in our sample was 8 years, maternal education was categorized into two – middle (8–12 years of formal education) and upper (more than 13 years of formal education) levels. Apgar score at 1-min cut-off value was set at 8. Newborns with 1-min Apgar score value lower than 8 were considered to have increased risk of morbidity [16]. Infants were considered small-for-gestational age based on the Mongolian neonate’s values derived from a secondary analysis of the World Health Organization multi-country survey on maternal and newborn health [17]. Seasonal variation at birth is reported to influence hyperbilirubinemia due to the fact that exposure to daylight decreases the level of bilirubin [18, 19]. Given that children’s exposure to daylight dramatically decreases during winter months in Mongolia, we took seasonal variation of birth into account categorizing the season into summer time (from April to September) and winter time (from October to March) [20]. Date of birth provided information regarding seasonal variation at birth.

Statistical analysis

The sample size for this study was estimated using formula below [20] and assuming a MORBAS score standard deviation of 3.0 [21], with significance level of α = 0.05 using two-sided test with power of 80% (β = 0.20) and an effect size of d = 1.5 [13].

n=2SD2Zα/2+Zβ2/d2

Under these assumptions, a total sample size of n = 128 was required to detect differences in the risk of developmental delay. Our final sample size was 150 children anticipating a 15% attrition rate. We randomly selected mother/child dyads from the baseline sample of 1297 women. Telephone calls were made to randomly selected numbers. Of the 1297 eligible women, 344 women could not be reached on phone due to connection error. Additionally, phone numbers provided by 248 women were no longer in use and 53 women declined to participate in the study before we reached the desired sample size.

We used Fisher’s exact test and t-test to determine basic characteristics of study participant on categorical and continuous variables respectively. Multivariable logistic regression analysis was used to find the association between predictor variables and the risk of child developmental delay. Predictors included in the analysis were child gender, maternal age, wealth index, crowding, type of household, maternal education, maternal work, history of miscarriage, disease during pregnancy, maternal depression symptoms, and exclusive breastfeeding. Effect sizes are presented as adjusted odds ratios (AOR) with corresponding confidence intervals (CI).

All analyses were conducted using de-identified data in Stata version 13.0 (StataCorp LP, College Station, Texas, USA).

Results

A total of 150 Mongolian children participated in this study. Basic characteristic of the study children at birth and at baseline are presented in Table 1 and Table 2 respectively. Seventeen (11%) of the 150 children were at risk of developmental delay for at least one of the developmental domains. Specifically, children were at risk of developmental delay in the following domains: gross motor (n = 1), expressive language (n = 9), receptive language (n = 2). One child each was at risk of developmental delay in the other domains except cognitive delay for which no child was at risk.

Table 1.

Characteristics of child and mother at birth n = 150

Baseline data Category Distribution No risk of delay Risk of delay p value
n % n % n %
Sex Male 85 56.7 72 54.1 13 76.4 0.118
Female 65 43.3 61 45.8 4 23.5
Maternal age 17–20 4 2.7 3 2.2 1 5.8 0.502
21–30 81 54 73 54.8 8 47.1
≧31 65 43.3 57 42.8 8 47.1
Maternal age mean(SD) Year 29.8 (5.2) 29.8 (5.2) 30.2 (5.7) 0.111
Delivery mode Vaginal 91 60.7 79 59.4 12 70.5 0.439
Cesarean 59 39.3 54 40.6 5 29.4
Birthweight mean(SD) Gram 3592 (446) 3611 (448) 3441 (412) 0.126
Gestational week mean(SD) Week 38.9 (0.9) 39.0 (0.9) 38.5 (1.3) 0.111
Preterm ≧38 week 143 95.3 128 96.2 15 88.2 0.181
≦37 week 7 4.7 5 3.7 2 11.7
Small for gestational age Yes 4 2.7 4 3 0 0 1
No 146 97.3 129 96.9 17 100
Apgar 1 min ≧8 55 36.7 51 38.3 4 23.5 0.292
≦7 95 63.3 82 61.6 13 76.4
Apgar 1 min mean (SD) Score 7.3 (0.6) 7.3(0.6) 7.2(0.5) 0.51
Hyperbilirubinemia < 10 mg/dL 45 30 38 28.5 7 41.2 0.399
> 10 mg/dL 105 70 95 71.4 10 58.8
Season of birth October–March 73 48.7 63 47.3 10 58.8 0.445
April–September 77 51.3 70 52.6 7 41.2
Parity Primiparous 48 32 42 31.5 6 35.3 0.786
Parous 102 68 91 68.4 11 64.7

Table 2.

Characteristics of child and mother at birth (13–24 months) n = 150

Follow-up data Category Distribution No risk of delay Risk of delay p value
n % n % n %
Mothers
Wealth index Poor 51 34.2 44 33.3 7 41.2 0.852
Middle 49 32.9 44 33.3 5 29.4
Rich 49 32.9 44 33.3 5 29.4
Crowding < 5 96 64.4 86 65.2 10 58.8
≧5 53 35.6 46 34.9 7 41.2
Single mother household Yes 8 5.3 5 3.8 3 17.7 0.048
No 142 94.7 128 96.2 14 82.4
Maternal education Middle 16 10.7 11 8.3 5 29.4 0.021
Upper 134 89.3 122 91.7 12 70.6
Maternal work Yes 75 50.0 68 51.1 7 41.2 0.608
No 75 50.0 65 48.9 10 58.8
History of miscarriage Yes 37 24.7 34 25.6 3 17.6 0.566
No 113 75.3 99 74.4 14 82.4
Alcohol use during pregnancy Yes 12 8.0 11 8.3 1 5.9 1.000
No 138 92.0 122 91.7 16 94.1
Smoking during pregnancy Yes 6 4.0 5 3.8 1 5.9 0.52
No 144 96.0 128 96.2 16 94.1
Disease during pregnancy Yes 81 54.0 75 56.4 6 35.3 0.124
No 69 46.0 58 43.6 11 64.7
Maternal depression symptoms Yes (> 9) 20 13.3 17 12.8 3 17.7 0.703
No (< 8) 130 86.7 116 87.2 14 82.4
Children
Exclusive breastfeeding <  6 month 71 47.3 63 47.4 8 47.1 1.000
≧6 month 79 52.7 70 52.6 9 52.9
Developmental delays in relatives Yes 11 7.3 10 7.5 1 5.9 1.000
no 139 92.7 123 92.5 16 94.1

Among our study participants, 7 (4.7%) children were born preterm, 95 (63.3%) were scored < 7 for 1 min Apgar score, and 105 (70%) children had moderate hyperbilirubinemia (transcutaneous bilirubin level of > 10 mg/dl) at birth (Table 1).

Eight (5.3%) children were from single mother households while 16 (10.6%) children were born to mothers educated below middle school educational level (less than 12 years of formal education). Alcohol and tobacco use during pregnancy was reported by 12 (8%) and 6 (4%) mothers respectively. Twenty (13.3%) mothers had depression symptoms as detected by the SRQ-20 test.

Two multivariable analysis models were constructed to determine predictors of developmental delay at baseline and at follow-up. In the first model, we found no clear association between baseline factors and the risk of developmental delay (Table 3). Further, we constructed another model using the follow-up predictors. We found that higher maternal education was an important protective factor against risk of developmental delay (AOR-0.15; 95%CI [0.03–0.66]). Other predictors associated with risk of developmental delays were gender being female (AOR- 0.25; 95% CI [0.06–1.00]); and increasing maternal age (AOR- 1.12; 95%CI [0.98–1.27]). Additional predictors for risk of developmental delay in the infants were maternal depression symptoms (AOR- 4.93; 95%CI [0.93–26.10]) and being a single mother (AOR-0.14; 95%CI [0.01–1.11]) although the associations were only marginal (Table 4).

Table 3.

A model of predictors of developmental delay at baseline n = 150

Predictors Category OR 95%CI p value AOR 95%CI p value
Child sex Male 1 1
Female 0.36 0.11–1.17 0.09 0.32 0.09–1.15 0.082
Maternal age Per 1 year 1.02 0.92–1.12 0.705 1.03 0.91–1.16 0.604
Delivery mode Vaginal 1 1
Cesarean 0.78 0.45–1.35 0.377 0.61 0.18–2.01 0.410
Gestational week Per 1 week 0.58 0.35–0.97 0.038 0.63 0.35–1.12 0.116
Birthweight Per 100 g 0.91 0.80–1.03 0.14 0.96 0.83–1.11 0.629
Apgar score at 1 min Per score 1 0.8 0.38–1.70 0.24 0.83 0.36–1.92 0.671
Hyperbilirubinemia < 10 mg/dL 1 1
> 10 mg/dL 0.57 0.20–1.61 0.29 0.53 0.16–1.66 0.278
Season of birth October–March 1 1
April–September 0.63 0.22–1.75 0.377 0.51 0.17–1.54 0.237
Parity Primiparous 1 1
Parous 0.84 0.29–2.44 0.757 1.01 0.26–3.72 0.998

Multiple logistic regression analysis adjusted for variables in this table

Table 4.

A model of predictors of developmental delay at follow-up n = 149

Predictors Category OR 95%CI p value AOR 95%CI p value
Child sex Male 1 1
Female 0.36 0.11–1.17 0.09 0.25 0.06–1.00 0.050
Maternal age Per year 1.02 0.93–1.12 0.642 1.12 0.98–1.27 0.073
Wealth index Rich 1 1
Poor 0.83 0.45–1.56 0.578 1.37 0.29–6.45 0.687
Middle 0.62 0.14–2.74 0.536
Crowding < 5 1 1
≧5 1.3 0.46–3.66 0.609 0.68 0.19–2.45 0.565
Single mother household Yes 1 1
No 0.18 0.03–0.84 0.03 0.14 0.01–1.11 0.063
Maternal education Middle 1 1
Upper 0.21 0.06–0.72 0.013 0.15 0.03–0.66 0.012
Maternal work No 1 1
Yes 0.66 0.24–1.86 0.442 0.51 0.15–1.68 0.272
History of miscarriage Yes 1 1
No 1.6 0.43–5.91 0.479 1.81 0.39–8.22 0.441
Disease during pregnancy Yes 1 1
No 2.37 0.82–6.78 0.108 2.61 0.67–10.15 0.165
Maternal depression symptoms No (< 8) 1 1
Yes (> 9) 1.46 0.38–5.62 0.58 4.93 0.93–26.10 0.060
Exclusive breastfeeding <  6 month 1 1
≧ 6 month 1.01 0.36–2.78 0.981 1.42 0.40–5.04 0.579

Multiple logistic regression analysis adjusted for variables in this table

Discussion

The purpose of the current study was to examine major biological and psychosocial factors associated with the risk of young child developmental delay. Eleven percent of the study children were at risk of developmental delay while higher maternal educational level was found to be strongly protective against the risk of child development delay. Although the risk of child developmental delay increased due to other modifiable predictors such as belonging to single mother households and the presence of depression symptoms in mothers, these associations were modest.

The findings from the current study showed that children of less educated mothers were at increased risk of developmental delay compared to children of more educated mothers. Studies from other countries also support our findings [22, 23]. Maternal education has been shown to be associated with many positive aspects of the child development throughout their growth [22]. Moreover, prior studies show that highly educated mothers in developing countries are more likely to seek appropriate care for their children [23]. For example, findings from studies examining the association between increases in maternal education and children’s school readiness show that expressive and receptive language ability, and cognitive test results were better among pre-school children who had mothers with higher education [24]. Similarly, mothers with at least 2 years of college education were shown to have fluent vocabulary and supportive style of child directed speech, which correlated to richer vocabulary of children when compared to less educated mothers [25]. Given that women’s education directly impacts on their autonomy, educated mothers may be more likely to make decision concerning their children’s health condition independently, through perceiving that their behavior is their own responsibility [26].

In our study, maternal depression symptoms was found to be associated with risk of developmental delay although the association was modest. Notwithstanding, this finding is important given that child rearing practices could be affected by maternal depression [9]. Prior studies have shown that depressed mothers are more negatively involved with their children’s daily activities [27] resulting in short and long term consequences. Recently, maternal depression early in a child’s life is reported to be a risk factor for low math score during adolescence [28]. Therefore, screening and implementing preventive interventions for maternal depression may enhance not only improvement in young child development, but also influence school achievement during adolescence. For example, early parenting-programs are reported to improve parental responses [2931] and lead to increase in the abilities of depressed mothers to support their child’s executive functions which controls and regulates a child’s thoughts and behaviors [32, 33].

Furthermore, belonging to a single-mother household was weakly associated with risk of child developmental delay. Prior research shows that transition to single-parent household reduces the child’s well-being, because it can bring emotional and economic losses to children [34]. Particularly, those who experienced parent’s divorce in their earlier pre-school age are reported to have adaptive-behavior problems [34]. In the long-term, children growing up in father-absent households were shown to be more likely to suffer from adolescent social problems, such as school dropout, substance abuse, and juvenile delinquency [35]. This might be because single mothers go through more stress and have higher risk of mental distress than undivorced women [36]. However, children with behavior problems who lived in supportive environments have been shown to have greater chances for advanced development of self-regulatory skills than children who lived in unsupportive family settings [34]. This emphasizes the importance of family member’s support, and careful attention toward children’s conduct to ensure child self-regulation as it is an essential factor for school readiness.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study conducted in Mongolia assessing maternal socio-demographic and psychological predictors related to young child development in Mongolia. We also examined the influence of hyperbilirubinemia on child development using the first country-specific validated screening tool to assess child development. Although the study did not include all the children from the baseline study, our study population was shown to be representative when we compared the characteristic of the study population to the rest of the cohort that did not participate in this study.

Our study has findings with important implications for child development, however, there are several limitations to this study. First, factors that change with family situation such as home environment, changing economic status, cognitive stimulation in the home, and family structure change could not be included in our study. Although we controlled for factors such as type of household i.e. being single mother household or not, such home environment factors may not have been fully accounted for. Second, other potentially relevant variables such as reduced access to services, nutrient deficiencies, environmental toxins, and maternal exposure to violence were not considered. Third, the health condition of the children, including history of jaundice was obtained 1–2 years after birth thus introducing the possibility of recall bias. Fourth, results may not be generalizable to the whole population as the data used in this study is from a cohort of infants born within a single hospital. However, institutional delivery is universal in Mongolia and our study location was a tertiary level national center with more than 10,000 deliveries per annum. Additionally, being a tertiary level care center, mothers come from all over the country for delivery in our study location hospital. Lastly, although the sample size was calculated based on robust statistical methods, we included only a small sample of participants in the study which may have hampered more sophisticated analyses. Given that mothers were randomly selected into this study, we think that our findings may reflect a true situation.

All the predictors described in current study are modifiable, which suggests that women empowerment through education can bring benefit thus maximizing child potential. Maternal education is not only a tool to emerge from poverty, but it also has the capacity to prevent loss of human potential [1].

Conclusions

In conclusion, we found that maternal education plays an important role in reducing the risk of child developmental delay. Furthermore, psychosocial factors such as being from single-mother household and maternal depression symptoms are associated with risk of child developmental delay although this finding was modest. These results suggest that underprivileged social and psychological conditions of mothers in Mongolia contribute to risk of child developmental delays. Thus, our study findings provide valuable contribution towards making appropriate policies, preventive measures and intervention that could be directed toward vulnerable mothers who are less educated, single and have risk of psychological disorder.

Finally, our study emphasizes the need for future studies to highlight the contribution of developmental psychology on the risk of child development.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the participants who took part in the study and Batzaya Batbold, Otgonzaya Baatar, and Solongo Bat who dedicated much of their effort and time to collecting data used in this study. We also thank Haruko Watanabe for her administrative support.

Funding

The study was funded by Grant in-Aid for Research from the National Center for Global Health and Medicine (24A-1).

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviation

AOR

Adjusted Odds Ratio

CI

Confidence Interval

LBW

Low birthweight

MORBAS

Mongolian Rapid Baby Scale

SRQ

Self-Reporting Questionnaire

Authors’ contributions

AD conceptualized and designed the study, conducted the data collection, undertook the statistical analysis and interpretation of the data, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. DG was responsible for the study concept, design, field supervision and data collection. OB was responsible for the study concept, design and interpretation of the data. NY undertook the statistical analysis and interpretation of the data. BB, KT and RM contributed to study concept and design, participated in the design of the questionnaire. MA was responsible for the study concept and design, data collection and interpretation of the data. All authors contributed to the revision of the manuscript and approved the final version for submission.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical review boards of the Mongolian National University of Medical Science, Mongolia and National Center for Child Health and Development in Japan approved the study protocol. All the women who participated in the current study gave written informed consent for their participation as well as on behalf of their children.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Amarjargal Dagvadorj, Phone: +81-3-3416-0181, Email: hiamraa@yahoo.com.

Duurenbayar Ganbaatar, Email: gduurenbayar@yahoo.com.

Olukunmi O. Balogun, Email: omobolanle_balogun@yahoo.com.

Naohiro Yonemoto, Email: nyonemoto@gmail.com.

Bayasgalantai Bavuusuren, Email: bayasgalantai.b@gmail.com.

Kenji Takehara, Email: takehara-k@ncchd.go.jp.

Rintaro Mori, Email: rintaromori@gmail.com.

Moe Akahira-Azuma, Email: nth3son@yahoo.co.jp.

References

  • 1.Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, Lozoff B, Wasserman GA, Pollitt E, Carter JA. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369(9556):145–157. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60076-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gladstone M, Oliver C, Van den Broek N. Survival, morbidity, growth and developmental delay for babies born preterm in low and middle income countries - a systematic review of outcomes measured. PLoS One. 2015;10(3):e0120566. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Howe TH, Sheu CF, Hsu YW, Wang TN, Wang LW. Predicting neurodevelopmental outcomes at preschool age for children with very low birth weight. Res Dev Disabil. 2016;48:231–241. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Levene MI, Sands C, Grindulis H, Moore JR. Comparison of two methods of predicting outcome in perinatal asphyxia. Lancet. 1986;1(8472):67–69. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(86)90718-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Wusthoff CJ, Loe IM. Impact of bilirubin-induced neurologic dysfunction on neurodevelopmental outcomes. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2015;20(1):52–57. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2014.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Vela HC, Martinez MF, Guzman IB. 64. Characteristics of visual evoked potentials in infants who had severe neonatal hyperbilirubinemia. Clin Neurophysiol. 2016;127(9):e317. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.05.339. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Dennery PA, Seidman DS, Stevenson DK. Neonatal hyperbilirubinemia. N Engl J Med. 2001;344(8):581–590. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200102223440807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Caring for children around the world: a view from HOME. Int J Behav Dev. 2005;29(6):468–478. doi: 10.1177/01650250500146925. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Parke RD, Ladd GW. Family-peer relationships: modes of linkage. New York: Routledge; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Statistics Department of the Governor’s Office of Khuvsgul aimag, UNICEF. Khuvsgul Child Development Survey 2012 (MICS), Final Report. Khuvsgul aimag: UNICEF Mongolia; 2014.
  • 11.Sices L, Feudtner C, McLaughlin J, Drotar D, Williams M. How do primary care physicians manage children with possible developmental delays? A national survey with an experimental design. Pediatrics. 2004;113(2):274–282. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.2.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Akahira-Azuma M, Yonemoto N, Mori R, Hosokawa S, Matsushita T, Sukhbat K, et al. An hour-specific transcutaneous bilirubin nomogram for Mongolian neonates. Eur J Pediatr. 2015;174(10):1299–1304. doi: 10.1007/s00431-015-2536-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dagvadorj A, Takehara K, Bavuusuren B, Morisaki N, Gochoo S, Mori R. The quick and easy Mongolian rapid baby scale shows good concurrent validity and sensitivity. Acta Paediatr. 2015;104(3):e94–e99. doi: 10.1111/apa.12884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Pollock JI, Manaseki-Holland S, Patel V. Detection of depression in women of child-bearing age in non-western cultures: a comparison of the Edinburgh postnatal depression scale and the self-reporting Questionnaire-20 in Mongolia. J Affect Disord. 2006;92(2–3):267–271. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vyas S, Kumaranayake L. Constructing socio-economic status indices: how to use principal components analysis. Health Policy Plan. 2006;21(6):459–468. doi: 10.1093/heapol/czl029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Chong DS, Karlberg J. Refining the Apgar score cut-off point for newborns at risk. Acta Paediatr. 2004;93(1):53–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2004.tb00674.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Ota E, Ganchimeg T, Morisaki N, Vogel JP, Pileggi C, Ortiz-Panozo E, Souza JP, Mori R. Risk factors and adverse perinatal outcomes among term and preterm infants born small-for-gestational-age: secondary analyses of the WHO multi-country survey on maternal and newborn health. PLoS One. 2014;9(8):e105155. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bolton P, Pickles A, Harrington R, Macdonald H, Rutter M. Season of birth: issues, approaches and findings for autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992;33(3):509–530. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00888.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Landau EC, Cicchetti DV, Klin A, Volkmar FR. Season of birth in autism: a fiction revisited. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999;29(5):385–393. doi: 10.1023/A:1023030911527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Charan J, Biswas T. How to calculate sample size for different study designs in medical research? Indian J Psychol Med. 2013;35(2):121. doi: 10.4103/0253-7176.116232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Takehara K, Dagvadorj A, Hikita N, Sumya N, Ganhuyag S, Bavuusuren B, et al. Maternal and child health in Mongolia at 3 years after childbirth: a population-based cross-sectional descriptive study. Matern Child Health J. 2015:1–10. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 22.Harding JF, Morris PA, Hughes D. The relationship between maternal education and Children's academic outcomes: a theoretical framework. J Marriage Fam. 2015;77(1):60–76. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12156. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Geldsetzer P, Williams TC, Kirolos A, Mitchell S, Ratcliffe LA, Kohli-Lynch MK, Bischoff EJL, Cameron S, Campbell H. The recognition of and care seeking behaviour for childhood illness in developing countries: a systematic review. PLoS One. 2014;9(4):e93427. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Magnuson KA, Sexton HR, Davis-Kean PE, Huston AC. Increases in maternal education and young children's language skills. Merrill Palmer Q. 2009;55(3):319–350. doi: 10.1353/mpq.0.0024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Hoff-Ginsberg E. Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Dev. 1991;62(4):782–796. doi: 10.2307/1131177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kalipeni E. Determinants of infant mortality in Malawi: a spatial perspective. Soc Sci Med. 1993;37(2):183–198. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(93)90454-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cooper PJ, Tomlinson M, Swartz L, Woolgar M, Murray L, Molteno C. Post-partum depression and the mother-infant relationship in a south African peri-urban settlement. Br J Psychiatry. 1999;175(6):554–558. doi: 10.1192/bjp.175.6.554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Pearson RM, Bornstein MH, Cordero M, Scerif G, Mahedy L, Evans J, Abioye A, Stein A. Maternal perinatal mental health and offspring academic achievement at age 16: the mediating role of childhood executive function. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 29.Jung V, Short R, Letourneau N, Andrews D. Interventions with depressed mothers and their infants: modifying interactive behaviours. J Affect Disord. 2007;98(3):199–205. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Paris R, Bolton RE, Weinberg MK. Postpartum depression, suicidality, and mother-infant interactions. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2009;12(5):309. doi: 10.1007/s00737-009-0105-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Stein A, Woolley H, Senior R, Hertzmann L, Lovel M, Lee J, et al. Treating disturbances in the relationship between mothers with bulimic eating disorders and their infants: a randomized, controlled trial of video feedback. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(5):899–906. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.5.899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Miyake A, Friedman NP. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions four general conclusions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(1):8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ryan RM, Claessens A, Markowitz AJ. Associations between family structure change and child behavior problems: the moderating effect of family income. Child Dev. 2015;86(1):112–127. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.McLoyd VC, Jayaratne TE, Ceballo R, Borquez J. Unemployment and work interruption among African American single mothers: effects on parenting and adolescent socioemotional functioning. Child Dev. 1994;65(2):562–589. doi: 10.2307/1131402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Murry VM, Brody GH. Self-regulation and self-worth of black children reared in economically stressed, rural, single mother-headed families. The Contribution of Risk and Protective Factors J Fam Issues. 1999;20(4):458–484. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from BMC Pediatrics are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES