Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2018 Mar;86:166–175. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.12.001

Verbal learning and hippocampal dysfunction in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis

Mathilde Antoniades a,, Tabea Schoeler a, Joaquim Radua a,b,c, Isabel Valli a, Paul Allen a,d, Matthew J Kempton a,1, Philip McGuire a,1
PMCID: PMC5818020  PMID: 29223768

Highlights

  • Verbal learning is one of the most impaired cognitive functions in schizophrenia.

  • Novel method for including unreported non-significant effects in the meta-analysis.

  • Positive correlations between verbal learning and hippocampal volume in SCZ.

  • Absent correlations in healthy controls.

Keywords: Cognition, Volume, MRI, Memory, Schizophrenia, Medial temporal lobe

Abstract

This meta-analysis summarizes research examining whether deficits in verbal learning are related to bilateral hippocampal volume reductions in patients with or at risk for schizophrenia and in healthy controls. 17 studies with 755 patients with schizophrenia (SCZ), 232 Genetic High Risk (GHR) subjects and 914 healthy controls (HC) were included. Pooled correlation coefficients were calculated between hemisphere (left, right or total) and type of recall (immediate or delayed) for each diagnostic group individually (SCZ, GHR and HC). In SCZ, left and right hippocampal volume positively correlated with immediate (r = 0.256, 0.230) and delayed (r = 0.132, 0.231) verbal recall. There was also a correlation between total hippocampal volume and delayed recall (r = 0.233). None of these correlations were significant in healthy controls. There was however, a positive correlation between left hippocampal volume and immediate recall in the GHR group (r = 0.356). The results suggest that hippocampal volume affects immediate and delayed verbal learning capacity in schizophrenia and provides further evidence of hippocampal dysfunction in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.

1. Introduction

Schizophrenia (SCZ) is a complex disorder with widespread neuroanatomical and neurofunctional deficits. It is associated with a substantial cognitive impairment that affects working memory, processing speed, attention and verbal memory (Heinrichs and Zakzanis, 1998, Dickinson et al., 2007, Fioravanti et al., 2005, Mesholam-Gately et al., 2009). Such cognitive deficits have been shown to be present in children and adolescents who later develop schizophrenia, long before they develop any overt symptoms (Reichenberg et al., 2010). Cognitive deficits have also been detected in the prodromal stage of the illness (Bora and Murray, 2014) and in first-episode patients (Mesholam-Gately et al., 2009). Furthermore, studies have consistently found that verbal learning has shown the greatest impairment throughout the disease stages (Massuda et al., 2013, Mesholam-Gately et al., 2009, Stone et al., 2011, Lencz et al., 2006, Silver and Bilker, 2015).

The most common way to assess verbal learning is through the use of word list learning tasks such as the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task (RAVLT, Lezak et al., 2004), the California Verbal Learning task (Delis et al., 1987), the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (Brandt and Benedict, 2001) or with the Logical Memory Stories test of the Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, 1987). All of these tasks provide a measure of immediate and delayed recall. Factor analysis of the RAVLT revealed three basic factors: acquisition, storage and retrieval (Vakil and Blachstein, 1993). Thus, measures of immediate recall may reflect the cellular process of encoding and delayed recall may reflect the processes of storage and retrieval (Delis et al., 1991).

Verbal learning and memory relies on a network of regions including the prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures, where the hippocampus is important for the acquisition of new information (Simons and Spiers, 2003, Preston and Eichenbaum, 2013). More specifically, studies have shown that the CA2, CA3 and dentate gyrus were activated during encoding of face-name and object-name pairs whereas the subiculum was activated during retrieval (Zeineh et al., 2003, Eldridge et al., 2005). In shorter time periods, the hippocampus is thought to aid retrieval by reactivating neurons that were involved in learning (Tanaka et al., 2014). The combination of these findings and results from many other studies (Kircher et al., 2008, Davachi and Wagner, 2002, Duncan et al., 2014, Hunsaker and Kesner, 2013, Kesner, 2007) highlight the importance of the hippocampus in the encoding, storage and retrieval of memories.

The hippocampus is thought to be one of the key regions implicated in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia (Lodge and Grace, 2011). Studies consistently report hippocampal volume reductions in patients with schizophrenia in comparison to healthy controls (HC, Van Erp et al., 2016, Adriano et al., 2012, Steen et al., 2006); a finding that is also reported in high risk individuals (Wood et al., 2010, Witthaus, 2010; Phillips et al., 2002). Furthermore, an examination into the functioning of the regions involved in verbal learning has shown that patients with schizophrenia (Jessen et al., 2003, Weiss et al., 2004) and those at risk for psychosis (Allen et al., 2011, Allen et al., 2012) have reduced activation in both prefrontal and MTL structures. Thus, abnormalities in hippocampal integrity and function could be related to some of the cognitive deficits observed in patients with or at risk for schizophrenia.

Based on the presence of both memory deficits and hippocampal abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia, many researchers examined the link between verbal learning performance and hippocampal volume. These studies report positive (Herold et al., 2015, Sanfilipo et al., 2002) and negative (Thoma et al., 2009) correlations but the majority report non-significant findings. The aim of this meta-analysis is to pool the results from these studies, which often have small sample sizes, in order to reveal the relationship between verbal learning and hippocampal volume in patients with or at risk for schizophrenia.

Our hypotheses were threefold. First, based on the findings showing that patients with schizophrenia have impairments in verbal learning and that they have hippocampal volume reductions, we hypothesised that there would be a positive correlation between bilateral hippocampal volume and verbal learning performance in patients with schizophrenia. Second, based on the meta-analytic findings that verbal encoding more so than retrieval is lateralised to the left hippocampus (Persson and Soderlund, 2015), we hypothesised that correlations would be between immediate recall (as a measure of encoding) and left hippocampal volume. Third, we hypothesised that positive correlations would also exist in subjects at risk for schizophrenia, mirroring those in the schizophrenia group.

2. Methods

2.1. Search strategy

A systematic literature search strategy was conducted between January 1st 1980 and June 16th 2016 following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Moher et al., 2009). The PubMed database was used to identify relevant publications with the following search terms: “schizophrenia”, “psychosis”, “hippocamp*”, “medial temporal lobe”, “volume”, “verbal”, “neuropsych*”, “learning” and “recall”. All returned articles were screened and all texts other than original articles were excluded (e.g. reviews). The remaining studies were then assessed for eligibility and finally for inclusion criteria. We also examined the bibliographies of the included articles and the main reviews on this topic to identify any additional studies for inclusion. A flow chart of the selection process for this meta-analysis is shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Flow chart of the screening and selection process.

2.2. Selection criteria

Studies were eligible for inclusion if they: 1) were original articles written in English; 2) compared patients with DSM-III (APA, 1980) or later diagnosis of schizophrenia spectrum disorders or clinical (Fusar-Poli et al., 2013) or genetic (Johnstone et al., 2000) high risk state for psychosis with healthy controls; 3) used a region of interest approach with either manual or automatic segmentation of the hippocampus (studies using whole-brain voxel based morphometry methods were excluded); 4) directly correlated hippocampal volume and measures of verbal learning and 5) correlations were assessed in both patient and control groups. Studies were excluded if: 1) the patient group included diagnoses other than schizophrenia (e.g. Bipolar disorder) or 2) there were overlapping samples (i.e. data from the same participants were used in two or more separate studies), in which case the study with the smaller data set was excluded (Herold et al., 2013, Nestor et al., 2007).

2.3. Recorded variables

Simple and partial correlation coefficients between the verbal learning task and hippocampal volume were extracted by one author (M.A) and independently verified by another (T.S). The following variables were also recorded from each article: reference, publication year, sample size, age, percentage of males, task, illness stage, percentage of SCZ sample taking antipsychotic treatment, hippocampal region, method of segmentation, field strength, type of correlation and correlation covariates. Wherever there was missing data, the authors were contacted (10 were contacted and 3 provided missing information).

2.4. Statistical analysis

After contact with the authors, we did not know the specific correlation coefficients of a number of studies but we knew that they had not reached statistical significance. In other words, these correlations were non-statistically significant unreported effects (NSUEs), (SCZ: Haukvik et al., 2015 and HC: Lappin et al., 2014, Guo et al., 2014, Rametti et al., 2007, Karnik-Henry et al., 2012, Kuroki et al., 2006, Sanfilipo et al., 2002). Importantly, studies with NSUEs cannot be excluded as this would bias the meta-analysis towards the probably larger correlations reported in the non-NSUEs studies; note that studies finding larger, statistically-significant effects usually report these effects, whereas studies finding smaller, non-statistically significant effects may not report them. In order to overcome this bias and correctly include studies with NSUEs, we used a novel method called MetaNSUE developed and validated in Radua et al., 2015. Briefly, this method calculates upper and lower bounds where the Fisher z-transformations of the unreported correlations should be according to the alpha levels used in the studies. Then the Fisher z-transformations of the NSUEs are multiply imputed within these thresholds according to the Fisher z-transformation of the most likely correlation coefficient and the most likely between-study heterogeneity. In each set of imputations, a restricted-maximum likelihood random-effects model is used to calculate a meta-analytic Fisher z-transformation, and the Fisher z-transformation of the different imputation sets are finally pooled to obtain a single correlation coefficient (more detail can be found in Radua et al., 2015). Note that random-effects models account for the heterogeneity between studies, e.g. due to the use of different tasks and different segmentation methods; the percentage heterogeneity that is not due to sampling error was measured with the I2 statistic. Potential reporting bias was assessed by meta-regressing the Fisher z-transformations of the correlation coefficients by their standard error to identify funnel plot asymmetry and thus show if small studies may have been published only if they had significant results. This test was only done in meta-analyses with more than 10 studies because otherwise the test does not have sufficient power to differentiate between chance and genuine asymmetry (Sterne et al., 2011). Finally, leave-one-out jackknife sensitivity analyses were conducted to see whether a single study might be driving the results.

Studies that provided correlations in sub-regions of the hippocampus (e.g. anterior and posterior hippocampus) were considered as repeated measures and the weights attributed to them were adjusted accordingly with MetaNSUE. This technique avoids the subjective choice of one sub-region over another which could introduce bias into the meta-analysis. Gur et al., 2000 provided separate correlations for men and women, these were included as independent datasets and their weights were adjusted. These adjustments depend on the expected correlation between the repeated measures, which by default is assumed to be 0.3; thus in order to check whether the meta-analysis results could be influenced by this assumption, we ran a sensitivity analysis with the correlation coefficient set to 0 and 1.

Given that simple and partial correlations (the latter is a correlation between two variables whilst adjusting for a third variable) each have their own advantages and disadvantages in terms of comparability or control of potential confounds, we conducted the meta-analysis three times: a) using a combination of simple and partial correlations, depending on the type reported by the study; b) using simple correlations and discarding those studies from which only partial correlations could be obtained; and c) using partial correlations only.

Similarly, separate meta-analyses were run for left, right and total hippocampal volume and for immediate and delayed recall in SCZ and HC. Details of the studies included in each meta-analysis can be found in Table 1 (methodology details of these studies can be found in Table 1 of the Supplementary Material). For the single study reporting findings using the CVLT (Haukvik et al., 2015), the correlation between hippocampal volume and free recall was used (as opposed to the results with cued recall) because this is more comparable to the method of recall in the other verbal learning tasks. Studies using composite scores (a combination of several different verbal learning tasks) were included in the “delayed” category (Karnik-Henry et al., 2012, Sanfilipo et al., 2002, Sachdev et al., 2000).

Table 1.

Description of the demographic, cognitive and neuroimaging measures and covariates of the samples included in the meta-analysis.

Study
Task
Region
Covariates
Stage
n
Age (mean)
Males (%)
AP Med (%)
n
Age (mean)
Males (%)
Immediate recall Schizophrenia or GHR Healthy Controls
Herold et al. (2015) WMS-R LM I L/R Hipp, Ant & Post Edu SCZ 25 old 55.92 76 88 21 53.67 57.1
23 young 32.78 60.9 100
Guo et al. (2014) HVLT total immediate L/R Hipp Age, sex, Edu, PANSS total FEP 51 22.5 64.7 0 41 22.8 58.5
Francis et al. (2013) WMS-R LM I L/R Sub None GHR 46 25 30.4 0 30 24 43.3
Killgore et al. (2009) WMS-R LM I L/R Hipp None SCZ 19 34.3 73.68 100* 20 27.4 90
Thoma et al. (2009) WMS-R LM I L/R Ant & Post ICV SCZ 24 40.09 83.33 100 24 41.74 66.67
Exner et al., (2008) WMS-R LM I L/R Hipp None SCZ 21 31 66.67 100 21 29 61.9
Seidman et al. (2002) WMS-R LM I L/R Hipp Sex, group, sex*group, Hand, Eth, parental Edu, IQ GHR 28 simplex 41.9 35.7 0 48 40.1 56.3
17 multiplex 38.9 41.2



Delayed recall Schizophrenia or GHR Healthy Controls
Herold et al. (2015) WMS-R LM II L/R Hipp, Ant & Post Edu SCZ 25 old 55.92 76 88 21 53.67 57.1
23 young 32.78 60.9 100
Haukvik et al. (2015) CVLT delayed free recall Total Sub None SCZ 177 31.6 61 84.7 261 36 52
Lappin et al. (2014) RAVLT trial 7 L/R Hipp None FEP 90 83
Francis et al. (2013) HVLT total L/R Sub None GHR 46 25 30.4 0 30 24 43.3
Karnik-Henry et al. (2012) WMS-R LM I, LM II & CVLT composite L Hipp Sex SCZ 36 22.5 84.6 84.6 46 21.1 56.52
GHR 31 22.1 45.5 0 49 20.4 28
Killgore et al.(2009) WMS-R LM II L/R Hipp None SCZ 19 34.3 73.68 100* 20 27.4 90
Thoma et al. (2009) WMS-R LM II L/R Ant & Post ICV SCZ 24 40.09 83.33 100 24 41.74 66.67
Exner et al., 2008 WMS-R LM II L/R and total Hipp None SCZ 21 31 66.67 100 21 29 61.9
Rametti et al. (2007) RAVLT delayed L Ant None SCZ 28 27.7 75 100 33 28.09 63.64
Kuroki et al. (2006) WMS-R LM II L/R Hipp MD SCZ 24 40.3 100 100 31 40.6 100
Toulopoulou et al. (2004) WMS-R LM II L Hipp Age, gender, group SCZ 56 32.8 69.7 100 55 38.8 49.1
GHR 90 50.2 37.8 0
Seidman et al. (2002) WMS-R LM II L/R Hipp Sex, group, sex*group, Hand, Eth, parental Edu, IQ GHR 28 simplex 41.9 35.7 0 48 40.1 56.3
17 multiplex 38.9 41.2
Sanfilipo et al. (2002) WAIS, WMS-R LM I, LM II, Buschke L/R Hipp ICV, age SCZ 62 38.8 100 100 27 35.7 100
O’driscoll et al. (2001) WMS-R LM II L/R and total Am/Hipp None GHR 20 36.2 45 0 14 35.4 36
Gur et al. (2000) WMS-R LM I, LM II & CVLT (1–5) Total Hipp None SCZ 100 29.2 58 61 110 26.1 51
Sachdev et al. (2000) LM1 &2, VR, WAIS, COWAT, WCST L/R Hipp None SCZ 20 64.4 60 100 24 72.67 79

WMS-R: Wechsler memory scale-revised, LM I: logical memory 1 (i.e. immediate recall), LM II: logical memory 2 (i.e. delayed recall), SCZ: schizophrenia; AP Med: antipsychotic medication, GHR: genetic high risk; RAVLT: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task, HVLT: Hopkins verbal learning task, ICV: intracranial volume, L/R: left/right, Sub: subiculum, Hipp: hippocampus, Am: amygdala, Ant: anterior, Post: posterior, Edu: education, Eth: ethnicity, MD: medication dosage, Hand: handedness, * denotes that all patients were medicated but the study does not specify the type of medication.

Although we set out to study the structural-behavioural relationship in individuals at clinical and familial risk of schizophrenia, the search did not reveal any studies examining these correlations in subjects at clinical high risk for schizophrenia. Therefore the meta-analysis was conducted on the five studies that reported correlations in participants with a genetic risk for psychosis (Francis et al., 2013, Toulopoulou et al., 2004, Karnik-Henry et al., 2012).

In order to examine the effect of antipsychotic medication on the correlation coefficients, a meta-regression was conducted using the proportion of each study’s SCZ sample taking antipsychotic medication. A meta-regression was also conducted with age in both SCZ and HC, however there was insufficient data to examine the effect of the number of years spent in education.

Wherever possible, mean left and right hippocampal volume data was extracted for HC, SCZ and genetic high risk (GHR) groups and plotted against mean immediate or delayed memory score (raw scores were used except for Francis et al., 2013) to examine how the relationship between these variables in SCZ and GHR groups compared to that in HC (this data is shown in Fig. 2). The data used to compile the graphs is shown in Table 2 of the Supplementary Material but has been converted so that SCZ and GHR values are a percentage of the HC values. Data was only available for 8 studies.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Graph showing that verbal learning in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives is 55–90% that of healthy controls whereas hippocampal volume is 75–105% that of controls. Memory scores and hippocampal volumes of healthy controls were all set to 100% and are not shown on the graph.

Table 2.

Comparison of MetaNSUE findings using combined, simple and partial correlation coefficients in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls.

Analysis (group, hemisphere, type of recall) Simple and partial correlations combined
Simple correlations only
Partial correlations only
Number of independent studies (number of correlations) Number of NSUEs Total* number of subjects Meta-analysis result: Correlation coefficient (p value) 95% CI Number of independent studies (number of correlations) Number of NSUEs Total* number of subjects Meta-analysis result: Correlation coefficient (p value) 95% CI Number of independent studies (number of correlations) Number of NSUEs Total* number of subjects Meta-analysis result: Correlation coefficient (p value) 95% CI
SCZ left immediate 5 (8) 0 162 0.256 (0.0029) 0.089 to 0.409 3 (4) 0 63 0.146 (0.24) -0.095 to 0.371 3 (6) 0 123 0.242 (0.097) −0.045 to 0.493
SCZ left delayed 11 (15) 0 431 0.131 (0.0038) 0.042 to 0.218 7 (8) 0 229 0.179 (0.0304) 0.017 to 0.332 6 (10) 0 250 0.153 (0.0464) 0.002 to 0.297
SCZ right immediate 5 (8) 0 162 0.230 (0.001) 0.094 to 0.358 3 (4) 0 63 0.174 (0.15) −0.066 to 0.396 3 (6) 0 123 0.128 (0.37) −0.150 to 0.387
SCZ right delayed 8 (12) 0 311 0.234 (<0.0001) 0.135 to 0.329 6 (7) 0 201 0.256 (0.0013) 0.102 to 0.398 4 (8) 0 158 0.227 (0.093) −0.038 to 0.462
HC left immediate 7 (10) 1 204 0.019 (0.87) −0.207 to 0.243 4 (5) 0 95 −0.122 (0.43) −0.402 to 0.178 4 (7) 1 133 0.248 (0.0073) 0.068 to 0.412
HC left delayed 15 (19) 7 519 0.065 (0.26) −0.049 to 0.177 8 (9) 2 243 0.103 (0.24) −0.068 to 0.267 8 (12) 5 302 0.075 (0.32) −0.072 to 0.219
HC right immediate 7 (10) 1 204 0.063 (0.57) −0.152 to 0.272 4 (5) 0 95 −0.081 (0.62) −0.379 to 0.233 4 (7) 1 133 0.297 (0.0007) 0.128 to 0.449
HC right delayed 11 (15) 4 336 0.123 (0.0699) −0.01 to 0.251 7 (8) 1 210 0.095 (0.32) −0.093 to 0.277 5 (9) 3 150 0.207 (0.0235) 0.028 to 0.372

*Total only includes the number of subjects from repeated measures once, NSUE: non-significant unreported effect, SCZ: schizophrenia, HC: healthy control, CI: confidence interval.

All analyses were performed in R version 3.3 with the MetaNSUE package (http://www.metansue.com/).

In order to test the hypothesis that the correlation between immediate recall and left hippocampal volume was stronger than with the right hippocampal volume, correlation coefficients were transformed into z scores (“Fisher’s r to z transformation”) and the z test statistic was used to determine statistical significance.

3. Results

The details of the analyses along with all the results for patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls are shown in Table 2. Forest plots for the main analyses are presented in the Supplementary Material (Figs. 1–8).

3.1. Meta-analyses in patients with schizophrenia

The analyses run using simple and partial correlations (Forest plots for these analyses are in Figs. 1–4 in the Supplementary material) revealed significant positive correlations between left and right hippocampal volume and both immediate and delayed recall (I2 = 26.93%, 0%, 0.8% and 1.04%, respectively). Jackknife analyses were run for all analyses and showed similar correlations when any single study was discarded. Potential reporting bias was only examined for the Left Hippocampus-Delayed Recall analysis but none was observed (P = 0.099). Only the correlation between left and right hippocampal volume and delayed recall were significant in the meta-analysis including simple correlations whereas the meta-analysis including partial correlations only revealed a significant correlation between left hippocampal volume and delayed recall.

The correlation between left hippocampal volume and immediate recall was not significantly greater than the correlation between right hippocampal volume and immediate recall (Ztest = 0.25, P = 0.401).

A meta-analysis was conducted on studies that provided correlations between total hippocampal volume and delayed recall (other correlations were not available). This analysis included 4 correlations from 2 studies with reported correlations (Exner et al., 2008, Gur et al., 2000: separate correlations were available for males and females) and 1 study (Haukvik et al., 2015) with an NSUE (the total sample size was n = 293). There was a significant positive correlation between delayed verbal recall and total hippocampal volume in patients with schizophrenia (r = 0.228; P = 0.0138; 95% CI, 0.047-0.394; I2 = 14.35%). Jackknife analyses revealed a loss of significance when a correlation by (Gur et al., 2000) was discarded (r = 0.163, P = 0.0516). Individual removal of all other studies did not affect the correlations (r range, 0.244–0.297 and P range, 0.005–0.035).

3.2. Meta-analyses in healthy controls

The analyses between left and right hippocampal volume and immediate and delayed recall were all non-significant in healthy controls (I2 = 57.09%, 7.58%, 54.51% and 9.63%, respectively). Jackknife analyses showed similar correlations when any single study was discarded in all analyses except for the Right Hippocampus-Delayed recall. There, they showed a significant correlation when Killgore et al. (2009) was discarded (r = 0.159, P = 0.02), when (Exner et al., 2008) was discarded (r = 0.150, P = 0.03) and when a correlation from (Herold et al., 2015) was discarded (r = 0.129, P = 0.048). No potential reporting bias was observed for the Left or the Right Hippocampus-Delayed Recall analyses (P = 0.54, 0.87; respectively). Forest plots of the analyses using combined simple and partial correlations are shown in Figs. 5 –8 in the Supplementary material. The correlations remained non-significant when only simple correlations were meta-analysed but became significant when only considering the partial correlations.

The meta-analysis between total hippocampal volume and delayed recall included 5 correlations from 4 studies with reported correlations (Haukvik et al., 2015, Exner et al., 2008, O’driscoll et al., 2001, Gur et al., 2000) and one study with an NSUE (Gur et al., 2000), (total sample size, n = 387). There was no correlation between total hippocampal volume and delayed verbal recall in healthy controls (r = 0.104; P > 0.05; 95% CI, −0.065-0.267; I2 = 15.01%). Jackknife analyses showed similar correlations when each study was discarded (r range, 0.016–0.149 and P > 0.05 in all cases).

3.3. Meta-analysis in GHR

The meta-analysis between left hippocampal volume and immediate recall (Francis et al., 2013, Seidman et al., 2002) revealed a significant positive correlation in GHR subjects (n = 89, r = 0.356; P = 0.0009; 95% CI, 0.153-0.531; I2 = 0%), however this analysis has a very small number of studies. Jackknife analyses showed similar correlations when each study was discarded (r range, 0.331–0.398 and P < 0.05 in all cases).

There were no significant correlations between left hippocampal volume and delayed recall (n = 228, r = −0.031; P > 0.05; 95% CI, −0.223-0.163; I2 = 32.17%) or between right hippocampal volume and immediate (n = 89, r = 0.160; P > 0.05; 95% CI, −0.075-0.379; I2 = 10.95%) or delayed recall (n = 107, r = −0.034; P > 0.05; 95% CI, −0.237-0.171; I2 = 4.36%) in GHR subjects. Jackknife analyses showed similar correlations when each study was discarded in each analysis.

The number of studies examining the correlation between total hippocampal volume and measures of recall in GHR subjects was too small to permit a meta-analysis.

3.4. Sensitivity analysis

No changes were observed on any meta-analysis result when the value of the parameter was set to 0 or 1.

3.5. Meta-regression

The meta-regression revealed a significant but very weak negative correlation between correlation coefficients for the Left Hippocampus-Immediate Recall analysis and the proportion of the SCZ sample taking antipsychotic medication (r = −0.004, P = 0.03, n = 5 studies). Data for the proportion of the SCZ sample on antipsychotic medication was unavailable for one study (Lappin et al., 2014) however no significant correlations were found for all other meta-analyses (Left Hippocampus-Delayed Recall: r = −0.009, P > 0.05, n = 10 studies; Right Hippocampus-Immediate Recall: r = 0.001, P > 0.05, n = 5 studies; Right Hippocampus-Delayed Recall: r = −0.016, P > 0.05, n = 7 studies).

Age was not related to correlation coefficients of any of the meta-analyses in patients with schizophrenia (Left Hippocampus-Immediate Recall: r = −0.014, P = 0.07, n = 5 studies; Left Hippocampus-Delayed Recall: r = 0.003, P = 0.619, n = 10 studies; Right Hippocampus-Immediate Recall: r = 0.008, P = 0.286, n = 5 studies; Right Hippocampus-Delayed Recall: r = 0.012, P = 0.126, n = 7 studies) but was significantly correlated with correlation coefficients for Right Hippocampus-Immediate Recall analysis in healthy controls (r = 0.014, P = 0.049, n = 7 studies). All other regressions in healthy controls were non-significant (Left Hippocampus-Immediate Recall: r = 0.014, P = 0.09, n = 7 studies; Left Hippocampus-Delayed Recall: r = 0.004, P = 0.436, n = 13 studies; Right Hippocampus-Delayed Recall: r = 0.005, P = 0.401, n = 10 studies).

4. Discussion

Here we present findings that show a significant positive correlation between bilateral hippocampal volume and the ability to recall verbal information immediately and after a short delay in patients with schizophrenia. Thus, as hippocampal volume decreases, verbal recall performance worsens. Furthermore, contrary to the hypothesis, although the correlation between the left hippocampus and immediate recall is qualitatively larger than the right, there was no statistically significant difference between the two.

There were no correlations between hippocampal volume and verbal learning in healthy controls. However, some correlations in the healthy control group became significant when pooling partial correlations together. Thus, covariates that may affect hippocampal size such as years of education, age, gender or total intracranial volume may have an impact on verbal learning ability in healthy controls but the covariates from the different studies were too varied to derive a meaningful conclusion.

Previous investigations into the structure-function relationship have often revealed positive correlations between hippocampal volume and declarative memory performance only in the presence of pathology (e.g. Bonner-Jackson et al., 2015). However, a meta-analysis in healthy participants revealed a high degree of variability in the relationship between hippocampal volume and episodic memory in healthy participants where the relationship was negative in children, adolescents and young adults and almost absent in older participants (Van Petten, 2004). Thus, the absence of correlation in the current meta-analysis is consistent with previous findings that structure-behaviour relationships occur in the presence of pathology and could be due to the large variability in mean age of the healthy control participants (mean age range in current meta-analysis: 20–73) such that any effect in younger participants could be masked by the absence of effect in older participants. Furthermore, results from the meta-regression suggest that age only had an effect on the relationship between the right hippocampal volume and immediate recall in healthy controls.

In GHR subjects, only the left hippocampal volume was correlated with immediate recall. However, the number of studies in this analysis was very small (n = 2) and should therefore be treated with caution. This finding is consistent with a study showing that verbal learning is lateralised to the left anterior hippocampus (Persson and Soderlund, 2015). Although these participants were not symptomatic at the time of the studies, it is interesting to see that they have a similar relationship between verbal learning and left hippocampal volume to that seen in patients with schizophrenia. This suggests that some genetic factors may be influencing hippocampal volume, which is consistent with a recent study showing an association between hippocampal volume and the Polygenic Schizophrenia-related Risk Score (Harrisberger et al., 2016).

Functional imaging studies in patients with and at risk for schizophrenia consistently report changes in activation during verbal encoding and retrieval in the hippocampus and regions of the prefrontal cortex (Allen et al., 2011, Hofer et al., 2003, Kubicki et al., 2003, Ragland et al., 2001, Zierhut et al., 2010). Further studies also suggest abnormal resting state connectivity between the hippocampus and regions of the prefrontal cortex (Knochel et al., 2014, Kuhn and Gallinat, 2013, Cui et al., 2015, Zhou et al., 2008, Kraguljac et al., 2014, Duan et al., 2015, Tang et al., 2015). The same patients who had abnormal connectivity between the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex also had reduced hippocampal volume and abnormal white matter fibre integrity in the tracts connecting the hippocampus to regions of the frontal lobe (Knochel et al., 2014). Therefore, deficits in verbal memory may be related to a reduced connectivity between the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex secondary to a reduction of hippocampal volume.

Hippocampal volume reductions have been consistently reported in schizophrenia (Van Erp et al., 2016) with volume reductions spreading throughout the subfields as the illness becomes more chronic (Ho et al., 2016). Previous studies suggest that hippocampal atrophy in patients with schizophrenia may arise from an imbalance in excitatory and inhibitory transmission (Schobel et al., 2013) secondary to a reduced functioning of N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptors and a loss of parvalbumin-containing GABAergic interneurons (Braun et al., 2007, Schobel et al., 2013, Lisman et al., 2008). GABAergic interneurons are important for synchronising the activity of glutamatergic pyramidal cells via the production of gamma oscillations (Gonzalez-Burgos and Lewis, 2012), a mechanism by which they help regulate cognitive functions such as learning and memory but may also be important for the retrieval of information (Sejnowski and Paulsen, 2006).

Whilst long-term use of antipsychotic medication has been associated with a decline in verbal learning and memory (Husa et al., 2014), more effective treatments may aim to reduce hippocampal volume loss or normalise hippocampal activity or the connectivity between the hippocampus and frontal lobe regions. Antidepressants are able to stimulate hippocampal neurogenesis and increase GABA levels (Malberg et al., 2000, Simpson et al., 2012) and may enhance long term memory (Bowie et al., 2012). Certain cholinergic agonists have shown efficacy at improving cognition and function in patients with schizophrenia (Keefe et al., 2015) whilst a partial agonist improved verbal learning and working memory (Haig et al., 2016). Other factors that can affect hippocampal volume and are common in patients with schizophrenia are the use of cannabis or the presence of chronic stress (Schlosser et al., 2012, Fusar-Poli et al., 2005, Barha et al., 2011, Demirakca et al., 2011). Therefore, therapies that help manage stress or reduce the intake of cannabis in this population may lead to beneficial improvements in verbal memory.

From the results, it is clear that covariates may influence the relationship between verbal learning and hippocampal volume differently in patients with schizophrenia and in healthy controls and the inability to properly examine the effects of these covariates suggests that the results should be treated with caution. Also, by restricting the focus of this meta-analysis to verbal learning and the hippocampus; it is not possible to know whether hippocampal volume abnormalities are related to other deficits in cognitive abilities or whether verbal learning relates to volume changes in other brain regions.

One of the limitations of this study is the variation in hippocampal segmentation methodologies and thus in the definition of the boundaries of the structure which in turn would affect volume measurements. Furthermore, some studies chose to divide the hippocampus into anterior and posterior regions, which is unlikely to be consistent across studies. Similarly, not all studies used absolute volume in their correlations although some studies added total intracranial volume as a covariate instead. There was also variation in the average age of the patients across the studies (age range 20–65) which itself affects regional volume and cognitive abilities (Driscoll et al., 2003) and may make the studies less comparable. However, studies examining cognitive decline over time have shown that patients with schizophrenia decline at a similar rate to healthy controls (Rannikko et al., 2015, Rajji et al., 2013).

Antipsychotic medication is also known to affect hippocampal volume (Zierhut et al., 2013, Yang et al., 2015) and hippocampal neurogenesis (Newton and Duman, 2007) which contributes to learning and memory (Deng et al., 2010, Lazarov and Hollands, 2016). However, only a proportion of patients from all studies were being treated with antipsychotic medication. Furthermore, the results from the meta-regressions suggest that antipsychotic medication may not be influencing the results of this meta-analysis.

Another limitation arises from the design of the tasks used to measure verbal learning abilities. Delayed recall is only measured after around a 20-min break; therefore such tasks do not allow for the assessment of longitudinal recall of information over months or years. Although the studies are relatively homogeneous in the tasks used, some tasks may involve slightly different cognitive demands. For example, the CVLT and HVLT have groups of words that are semantically organised which may improve recall (Poirier and Saint-Aubin, 1995) and may recruit a larger network of regions (Binder and Desai, 2011) than the RAVLT which contains unrelated words. Therefore, future research may consider the relationship between hippocampal volumes and individual tasks.

Future research should also examine the relationship between hippocampal subfields and different aspects of memory (e.g. encoding vs. retrieval) because there were insufficient studies reporting findings in hippocampal subfields to examine this in this meta-analysis. One could expect a higher correlation between immediate recall and Cornu Ammonis/dentate gyrus regions and between delayed recall and the subiculum (Persson and Soderlund, 2015, Zeineh et al., 2003, Eldridge et al., 2005). Finally, it would be interesting to study these effects in a sample of subjects at ultra-high risk for psychosis as this group has a higher risk of transitioning to frank psychosis compared to GHR subjects (Fusar-Poli et al., 2016).

In conclusion, patients with schizophrenia show variations in bilateral hippocampal volume that are associated with deficits in verbal learning memory. This supports a role for the hippocampus in the development of some of the cognitive symptoms observed in schizophrenia.

Conflict of interest

None.

Disclosure

None.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported in part by an Instituto de Salud Carlos III and FEDER grant (CP14/00041) to JR. The funders had no influence on the study design, collection, analysis and interpretation of the data, writing of the report and in the decision to submit the article for publication. The researchers are independent from the funders. MJK was funded by an MRC Career Development Fellowship (MR/J008915/1).

Footnotes

Appendix A

Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.12.001.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

The following are Supplementary data to this article:

mmc1.docx (21KB, docx)
mmc2.docx (1.2MB, docx)

References

  1. APA, American Psychiatric Association . American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: 1980. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. [Google Scholar]
  2. Adriano F., Caltagirone C., Spalletta G. Hippocampal volume reduction in first-episode and chronic schizophrenia: a review and meta-analysis. Neuroscientist. 2012;18:180–200. doi: 10.1177/1073858410395147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allen P., Seal M.L., Valli I., Fusar-Poli P., Perlini C., Day F., Wood S.J., Williams S.C., Mcguire P.K. Altered prefrontal and hippocampal function during verbal encoding and recognition in people with prodromal symptoms of psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 2011;37:746–756. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Allen P., Luigjes J., Howes O.D., Egerton A., Hirao K., Valli I., Kambeitz J., Fusar-Poli P., Broome M., Mcguire P. Transition to psychosis associated with prefrontal and subcortical dysfunction in ultra high-risk individuals. Schizophr. Bull. 2012;38:1268–1276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barha C.K., Brummelte S., Lieblich S.E., Galea L.A. Chronic restraint stress in adolescence differentially influences hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function and adult hippocampal neurogenesis in male and female rats. Hippocampus. 2011;21:1216–1227. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Binder J.R., Desai R.H. The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011;15:527–536. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bonner-Jackson A., Mahmoud S., Miller J., Banks S.J. Verbal and non-verbal memory and hippocampal volumes in a memory clinic population. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2015;7:61. doi: 10.1186/s13195-015-0147-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bora E., Murray R.M. Meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in ultra-high risk to psychosis and first-episode psychosis: do the cognitive deficits progress over, or after, the onset of psychosis? Schizophr. Bull. 2014;40:744–755. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bowie C.R., McLaughlin D., Carrion R.E., Auther A.M., Cornblatt B.A. Cognitive changes following antidepressant or antipsychotic treatment in adolescents at clinical risk for psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 2012;137:110–117. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brandt J., Benedict R.H.B. Psychology Assessment Resources, Inc.; Lutz, FL: 2001. Hopkins Verbal Learning Test—Revised Professional Manual. [Google Scholar]
  11. Braun I., Genius J., Grunze H., Bender A., Moller H.J., Rujescu D. Alterations of hippocampal and prefrontal GABAergic interneurons in an animal model of psychosis induced by NMDA receptor antagonism. Schizophr. Res. 2007;97:254–263. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cui L.B., Liu J., Wang L.X. Anterior cingulate cortex-related connectivity in first-episode schizophrenia: a spectral dynamic causal modelling study with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015;9:589. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Davachi L., Wagner A.D. Hippocampal contributions to episodic encoding: insights from relational and item-based learning. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;88:982–990. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.2.982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Delis D.C., Kramer J.H., Kaplan E., Ober B.A. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio,Tex: 1987. California Verbal Learning Test: Adult Version. Manual. [Google Scholar]
  15. Delis D.C., Massman P.J., Butters N., Salmon D.P., Cermak L.S., Kramer J.H. Profiles of demented and amnesic patients on the California Verbal Learning Test: implications for the assessment of memory disorders. Psychol. Assess.: J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1991;3:19. [Google Scholar]
  16. Demirakca T., Sartorius A., Ende G., Meyer N., Welzel H., Skopp G., Mann K., Hermann D. Diminished gray matter in the hippocampus of cannabis users: possible protective effects of cannabidiol. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;114:242–245. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Deng W., Aimone J.B., Gage F.H. New neurons and new memories: how does adult hippocampal neurogenesis affect learning and memory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:339–350. doi: 10.1038/nrn2822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dickinson D., Ramsey M.E., Gold J.M. Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2007;64:532–542. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Driscoll I., Hamilton D.A., Petropoulos H., Yeo R.A., Brooks W.M., Baumgartner R.N., Sutherland R.J. The aging hippocampus: cognitive: biochemical and structural findings. Cereb. Cortex. 2003;13:1344–1351. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Duan H.F., Gan J.L., Yang J.M. A longitudinal study on intrinsic connectivity of hippocampus associated with positive symptom in first-episode schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Res. 2015;283:78–86. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Duncan K., Tompary A., Davachi L. Associative encoding and retrieval are predicted by functional connectivity in distinct hippocampal area CA1 pathways. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:11188–11198. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0521-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Eldridge L.L., Engel S.A., Zeineh M.M., Bookheimer S.Y., Knowlton B.J. A dissociation of encoding and retrieval processes in the human hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:3280–3286. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3420-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Exner C., Nehrkorn B., Martin V., Huber M., Shiratori K., Rief W. Sex-dependent hippocampal volume reductions in schizophrenia relate to episodic memory deficits. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2008;20:227–230. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2008.20.2.227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fioravanti M., Carlone O., Vitale B., Cinti M.E., Clare L. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in adults with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2005;15:73–95. doi: 10.1007/s11065-005-6254-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Francis A.N., Seidman L.J., Tandon N., Shenton M.E., Thermenos H.W., Mesholam-Gately R.I., Van Elst L.T., Tuschen-Caffier B., Delisi L.E., Keshavan M.S. Reduced subicular subdivisions of the hippocampal formation and verbal declarative memory impairments in young relatives at risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2013;151:154–157. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fusar-Poli P., Cappucciati M., Borgwardt S., Woods S.W., Addington J., Nelson B., Nieman D.H., Stahl D.R., Rutigliano G., Riecher-Rossler A., Simon A.E., Mizuno M., Lee T.Y., Kwon J.S., Lam M.M., Perez J., Keri S., Amminger P., Metzler S., Kawohl W., Green B., Young R., Kavanagh D. Cannabis use and misuse prevalence among people with psychosis. Br. J. Psychiatry: J. Ment. Sci. 2005;187:306–313. doi: 10.1192/bjp.187.4.306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fusar-Poli P., Borgwardt S., Bechdolf A., Addington J., Riecher-Rossler A., Schultze-Lutter F., Keshavan M., Wood S., Ruhrmann S., Seidman L.J., Valmaggia L., Cannon T., Velthorst E., De Haan L., Cornblatt B., Bonoldi I., Birchwood M., Mcglashan T., Carpenter W., Mcgorry P., Klosterkotter J., Mcguire P., Yung A. The psychosis high-risk state: a comprehensive state-of-the-art review. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:107–120. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fusar-Poli P., Cappucciati M., Borgwardt S., Woods S.W., Addington J., Nelson B., Nieman D.H., Stahl D.R., Rutigliano G., Riecher-Rossler A., Simon A.E., Mizuno M., Lee T.Y., Kwon J.S., Lam M.M., Perez J., Keri S., Amminger P., Metzler S., Kawohl W., Rossler W., Lee J., Labad J., Ziermans T., An S.K., Liu C.C., Woodberry K.A., Braham A., Corcoran C., McGorry P., Yung A.R., McGuire P.K. Heterogeneity of Psychosis Risk Within Individuals at Clinical High Risk: A Meta-analytical Stratification. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73(2):113–120. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gonzalez-Burgos G., Lewis D.A. NMDA receptor hypofunction, parvalbumin-positive neurons, and cortical gamma oscillations in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2012;38:950–957. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Guo X., Li J., Wang J., Fan X., Hu M., Shen Y., Chen H., Zhao J. Hippocampal and orbital inferior frontal gray matter volume abnormalities and cognitive deficit in treatment-naive, first-episode patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2014;152:339–343. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gur R.E., Turetsky B.I., Cowell P.E., Finkelman C., Maany V., Grossman R.I., Arnold S.E., Bilker W.B., Gur R.C. Temporolimbic volume reductions in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2000;57:769–775. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.8.769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Haig G.M.B.E., Robieson W.Z., Baker J.D., Othman A.A. A randomized trial to assess the efficacy and safety of ABT-126, a selective α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor agonist, in the treatment of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2016;173:827–835. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15010093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Harrisberger F., Smieskova R., Vogler C., Egli T., Schmidt A., Lenz C., Simon A.E., Riecher-Rossler A., Papassotiropoulos A., Borgwardt S. Impact of polygenic schizophrenia-related risk and hippocampal volumes on the onset of psychosis. Transl. Psychiatry. 2016;6:e868. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Haukvik U.K., Westlye L.T., Morch-Johnsen L., Jorgensen K.N., Lange E.H., Dale A.M., Melle I., Andreassen O.A., Agartz I. In vivo hippocampal subfield volumes in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. 2015;77:581–588. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.06.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Heinrichs R.W., Zakzanis K.K. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology. 1998;12:426–445. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Herold C.J., Lasser M.M., Schmid L.A., Seidl U., Kong L., Fellhauer I., Thomann P.A., Essig M., Schroder J. Hippocampal volume reduction and autobiographical memory deficits in chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2013;211:189–194. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Herold C.J., Lasser M.M., Schmid L.A., Seidl U., Kong L., Fellhauer I., Thomann P.A., Essig M., Schroder J. Neuropsychology, autobiographical memory, and hippocampal volume in younger and older patients with chronic schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry. 2015;6:53. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Ho N.F., Iglesias J.E., Sum M.Y., Kuswanto C.N., Sitoh Y.Y., De Souza J., Hong Z., Fischl B., Roffman J.L., Zhou J., Sim K., Holt D.J. Progression from selective to general involvement of hippocampal subfields in schizophrenia. Mol. Psychiatry. 2016 doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hofer A., Weiss E.M., Golaszewski S.M., Siedentopf C.M., Brinkhoff C., Kremser C., Felber S., Fleischhacker W.W. Neural correlates of episodic encoding and recognition of words in unmedicated patients during an acute episode of schizophrenia: a functional MRI study. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2003;160:1802–1808. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.10.1802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hunsaker M.R., Kesner R.P. The operation of pattern separation and pattern completion processes associated with different attributes or domains of memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013;37:36–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Husa A.P., Rannikko I., Moilanen J. Lifetime use of antipsychotic medication and its relation to change of verbal learning and memory in midlife schizophrenia – an observational 9-year follow-up study. Schizophr. Res. 2014;158:134–141. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.06.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Jessen F., Scheef L., Germeshausen L., Tawo Y., Kockler M., Kuhn K.U., Maier W., Schild H.H., Heun R. Reduced hippocampal activation during encoding and recognition of words in schizophrenia patients. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2003;160:1305–1312. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.7.1305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Johnstone E.C., Abukmeil S.S., Byrne M., Clafferty R., Grant E., Hodges A., Lawrie S.M., Owens D.G. Edinburgh high risk study–findings after four years: demographic, attainment and psychopathological issues. Schizophr. Res. 2000;46:1–15. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00225-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Karnik-Henry M.S., Wang L., Barch D.M., Harms M.P., Campanella C., Csernansky J.G. Medial temporal lobe structure and cognition in individuals with schizophrenia and in their non-psychotic siblings. Schizophr. Res. 2012;138:128–135. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Keefe R.S., Meltzer H.A., Dgetluck N., Gawryl M., Koenig G., Moebius H.J., Lombardo I., Hilt D.C. Randomized, double-Blind, placebo-Controlled study of encenicline, an alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor agonist, as a treatment for cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:3053–3060. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kesner R.P. Behavioral functions of the CA3 subregion of the hippocampus. Learn. Mem. 2007;14:771–781. doi: 10.1101/lm.688207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Killgore W.D., Rosso I.M., Gruber S.A., Yurgelun-Todd D.A. Amygdala volume and verbal memory performance in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 2009;22:28–37. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e318192cc67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kircher T., Weis S., Leube D., Freymann K., Erb M., Jessen F., Grodd W., Heun R., Krach S. Anterior hippocampus orchestrates successful encoding and retrieval of non-relational memory: an event-related fMRI study. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2008;258:363–372. doi: 10.1007/s00406-008-0805-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Knochel C., Stablein M., Storchak H. Multimodal assessments of the hippocampal formation in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: evidences from neurobehavioral measures and functional and structural MRI. Neuroimage Clin. 2014;6:134–144. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kraguljac N.V., White D.M., Hadley J., Reid M.A., Lahti A.C. Hippocampal-parietal dysconnectivity and glutamate abnormalities in unmedicated patients with schizophrenia. Hippocampus. 2014;24:1524–1532. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kubicki M., McCarley R.W., Nestor P.G., Huh T., Kikinis R., Shenton M.E., Wible C.G. An fMRI study of semantic processing in men with schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 2003;20:1923–1933. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00383-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kuhn S., Gallinat J. Resting-state brain activity in schizophrenia and major depression: a quantitative meta-analysis. Schizophr. Bull. 2013;39:358–365. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kuroki N., Kubicki M., Nestor P.G., Salisbury D.F., Park H.J., Levitt J.J., Woolston S., Frumin M., Niznikiewicz M., Westin C.F., Maier S.E., Mccarley R.W., Shenton M.E. Fornix integrity and hippocampal volume in male schizophrenic patients. Biol. Psychiatry. 2006;60:22–31. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Lappin J.M., Morgan C., Chalavi S., Morgan K.D., Reinders A.A., Fearon P., Heslin M., Zanelli J., Jones P.B., Murray R.M., Dazzan P. Bilateral hippocampal increase following first-episode psychosis is associated with good clinical, functional and cognitive outcomes. Psychol. Med. 2014;44:1279–1291. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Lazarov O., Hollands C. Hippocampal neurogenesis: learning to remember. Prog. Neurobiol. 2016;138–140:1–18. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Lencz T., Smith C.W., Mclaughlin D., Auther A., Nakayama E., Hovey L., Cornblatt B.A. Generalized and specific neurocognitive deficits in prodromal schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2006;59:863–871. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lezak M.D., Howieson D.B., Loring D.W., Hannay H.J., Fisher J.S. University Press; New York, Oxford: 2004. Neuropsychological Assessment. (New York) [Google Scholar]
  58. Lisman J.E., Coyle J.T., Green R.W., Javitt D.C., Benes F.M., Heckers S., Grace A.A. Circuit-based framework for understanding neurotransmitter and risk gene interactions in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31:234–242. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Lodge D.J., Grace A.A. Hippocampal dysregulation of dopamine system function and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 2011;32:507–513. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2011.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Malberg J.E., Eisch A.J., Nestler E.J., Duman R.S. Chronic antidepressant treatment increases neurogenesis in adult rat hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:9104–9110. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-24-09104.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Massuda R., Bucker J., Czepielewski L.S., Narvaez J.C., Pedrini M., Santos B.T., Teixeira A.S., Souza A.L., Vasconcelos-Moreno M.P., Vianna-Sulzbach M., Goi P.D., Belmonte-De-Abreu P., Gama C.S. Verbal memory impairment in healthy siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2013;150:580–582. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Mesholam-Gately R.I., Giuliano A.J., Goff K.P., Faraone S.V., Seidman L.J. Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:315–336. doi: 10.1037/a0014708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Moher D., Liberati A., Tetzlaff J., Altman D.G. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2009;62:1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Nestor P.G., Kubicki M., Kuroki N., Gurrera R.J., Niznikiewicz M., Shenton M.E., Mccarley R.W. Episodic memory and neuroimaging of hippocampus and fornix in chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2007;155:21–28. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Newton S.S., Duman R.S. Neurogenic actions of atypical antipsychotic drugs and therapeutic implications. CNS Drugs. 2007;21:715–725. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200721090-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. O'driscoll G.A., Florencio P.S., Gagnon D., Wolff A.V., Benkelfat C., Mikula L., Lal S., Evans A.C. Amygdala-hippocampal volume and verbal memory in first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry Res. 2001;107:75–85. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(01)00095-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Persson J., Soderlund H. Hippocampal hemispheric and long-axis differentiation of stimulus content during episodic memory encoding and retrieval: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Hippocampus. 2015;25:1614–1631. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Phillips L.J., Velakoulis D., Pantelis C., Wood S., Yuen H.P., Yung A.R., Desmond P., Brewer W., Mcgorry P.D. Non-reduction in hippocampal volume is associated with higher risk of psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 2002;58:145–158. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00392-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Poirier M., Saint-Aubin J. Memory for related and unrelated words: further evidence on the influence of semantic factors in immediate serial recall. Q J. Exp. Psychol. A. 1995;48:384–404. doi: 10.1080/14640749508401396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Preston A.R., Eichenbaum H. Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory. Curr. Biol. 2013;23:R764–73. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Radua J., Schmidt A., Borgwardt S., Heinz A., Schlagenhauf F., Mcguire P., Fusar-Poli P. Ventral striatal activation during reward processing in psychosis: a neurofunctional meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:1243–1251. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Ragland J.D., Gur R.C., Raz J., Schroeder L., Kohler C.G., Smith R.J., Alavi A., Gur R.E. Effect of schizophrenia on frontotemporal activity during word encoding and recognition: a PET cerebral blood flow study. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2001;158:1114–1125. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.7.1114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Rajji T.K., Voineskos A.N., Butters M.A., Miranda D., Arenovich T., Menon M., Ismail Z., Kern R.S., Mulsant B.H. Cognitive performance of individuals with schizophrenia across seven decades: a study using the MATRICS consensus cognitive battery. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2013;21:108–118. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2012.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Rametti G., Segarra N., Junque C., Bargallo N., Caldu X., Ibarretxe N., Bernardo M. Left posterior hippocampal density reduction using VBM and stereological MRI procedures in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2007;96:62–71. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.04.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rannikko I., Haapea M., Miettunen J., Veijola J., Murray G.K., Barnett J.H., Husa A.P., Jones P.B., Isohanni M., Jaaskelainen E. Changes in verbal learning and memory in schizophrenia and non-psychotic controls in midlife: a nine-year follow-up in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort study 1966. Psychiatry Res. 2015;228:671–679. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.04.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Reichenberg A., Caspi A., Harrington H., Houts R., Keefe R.S., Murray R.M., Poulton R., Moffitt T.E. Static and dynamic cognitive deficits in childhood preceding adult schizophrenia: a 30-year study. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2010;167:160–169. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sachdev P., Brodaty H., Cheang D., Cathcart S. Hippocampus and amygdala volumes in elderly schizophrenic patients as assessed by magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2000;54:105–112. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2000.00644.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sanfilipo M., Lafargue T., Rusinek H., Arena L., Loneragan C., Lautin A., Rotrosen J., Wolkin A. Cognitive performance in schizophrenia: relationship to regional brain volumes and psychiatric symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 2002;116:1–23. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(02)00046-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Schlosser D.A., Pearson R., Perez V.B., Loewy R.L. Environmental risk and protective factors and their influence on the emergence of psychosis. Adolesc. Psychiatry (Hilversum) 2012;2:163–171. doi: 10.2174/2210676611202020163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Schobel S.A., Chaudhury N.H., Khan U.A., Paniagua B., Styner M.A., Asllani I., Inbar B.P., Corcoran C.M., Lieberman J.A., Moore H., Small S.A. Imaging patients with psychosis and a mouse model establishes a spreading pattern of hippocampal dysfunction and implicates glutamate as a driver. Neuron. 2013;78:81–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Seidman L.J., Faraone S.V., Goldstein J.M., Kremen W.S., Horton N.J., Makris N., Toomey R., Kennedy D., Caviness V.S., Tsuang M.T. Left hippocampal volume as a vulnerability indicator for schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging morphometric study of nonpsychotic first-degree relatives. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2002;59:839–849. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Sejnowski T.J., Paulsen O. Network oscillations: emerging computational principles. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:1673–1676. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3737-05d.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Silver H., Bilker W.B. Similar verbal memory impairments in schizophrenia and healthy aging. Implications for understanding of neural mechanisms. Psychiatry Res. 2015;226:277–283. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.12.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Simons J.S., Spiers H.J. Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-term memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2003;4:637–648. doi: 10.1038/nrn1178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Simpson S.M., Hickey A.J., Baker G.B., Reynolds J.N., Beninger R.J. The antidepressant phenelzine enhances memory in the double Y-maze and increases GABA levels in the hippocampus and frontal cortex of rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2012;102:109–117. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2012.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Steen R.G., Mull C., Mcclure R., Hamer R.M., Lieberman J.A. Brain volume in first-episode schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2006;188:510–518. doi: 10.1192/bjp.188.6.510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Sterne J.A., Sutton A.J., Ioannidis J.P., Terrin N., Jones D.R., Lau J., Carpenter J., Rucker G., Harbord R.M., Schmid C.H., Tetzlaff J., Deeks J.J., Peters J., Macaskill P., Schwarzer G., Duval S., Altman D.G., Moher D., Higgins J.P. Recommendations for examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials. BMJ. 2011;343:d4002. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d4002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Stone W.S., Giuliano A.J., Tsuang M.T., Braff D.L., Cadenhead K.S., Calkins M.E., Dobie D.J., Faraone S.V., Freedman R., Green M.F., Greenwood T.A., Gur R.E., Gur R.C., Light G.A., Mintz J., Nuechterlein K.H., Olincy A., Radant A.D., Roe A.H., Schork N.J., Siever L.J., Silverman J.M., Swerdlow N.R., Thomas A.R., Tsuang D.W., Turetsky B.I., Seidman L.J. Group and site differences on the California Verbal Learning Test in persons with schizophrenia and their first-degree relatives: findings from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) Schizophr. Res. 2011;128:102–110. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Tanaka K.Z., Pevzner A., Hamidi A.B., Nakazawa Y., Graham J., Wiltgen B.J. Cortical representations are reinstated by the hippocampus during memory retrieval. Neuron. 2014;84(2):347–354. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Tang Y., Chen K., Zhou Y. Neural activity changes in unaffected children of patients with schizophrenia: a resting-state fMRI study. Schizophr. Res. 2015;168:360–365. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.07.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Thoma R.J., Monnig M., Hanlon F.M., Miller G.A., Petropoulos H., Mayer A.R., Yeo R., Euler M., Lysne P., Moses S.N., Canive J.M. Hippocampus volume and episodic memory in schizophrenia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2009;15:182–195. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Toulopoulou T., Grech A., Morris R.G., Schulze K., Mcdonald C., Chapple B., Rabe-Hesketh S., Murray R.M. The relationship between volumetric brain changes and cognitive function: a family study on schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2004;56:447–453. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Vakil E., Blachstein H. Rey auditory-verbal learning test: structure analysis. J. Clin. Psychol. 1993;49:883–890. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199311)49:6<883::aid-jclp2270490616>3.0.co;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Van Erp T.G., Hibar D.P., Rasmussen J.M., Glahn D.C., Pearlson G.D., Andreassen O.A., Agartz I., Westlye L.T., Haukvik U.K., Dale A.M., Melle I., Hartberg C.B., Gruber O., Kraemer B., Zilles D., Donohoe G., Kelly S., Mcdonald C., Morris D.W., Cannon D.M., Corvin A., Machielsen M.W., Koenders L., De Haan L., Veltman D.J., Satterthwaite T.D., Wolf D.H., Gur R.C., Gur R.E., Potkin S.G., Mathalon D.H., Mueller B.A., Preda A., Macciardi F., Ehrlich S., Walton E., Hass J., Calhoun V.D., Bockholt H.J., Sponheim S.R., Shoemaker J.M., Van Haren N.E., Hulshoff Pol H.E., Ophoff R.A., Kahn R.S., Roiz-Santianez R., Crespo-Facorro B., Wang L., Alpert K.I., Jonsson E.G., Dimitrova R., Bois C., Whalley H.C., Mcintosh A.M., Lawrie S.M., Hashimoto R., Thompson P.M., Turner J.A. Subcortical brain volume abnormalities in 2028 individuals with schizophrenia and 2540 healthy controls via the ENIGMA consortium. Mol. Psychiatry. 2016;21:547–553. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Van Petten C. Relationship between hippocampal volume and memory ability in healthy individuals across the lifespan: review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1394–1413. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Wechsler D. Psychological Corp.; San Antonio,Tex: 1987. Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Manual. [Google Scholar]
  97. Weiss A.P., Zalesak M., Dewitt I., Goff D., Kunkel L., Heckers S. Impaired hippocampal function during the detection of novel words in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2004;55:668–675. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Witthaus H. Hippocampal subdivision and amygdalar volumes in patients in an at-risk mental state for schizophrenia. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2010;35:33–40. doi: 10.1503/jpn.090013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Wood S.J., Kennedy D., Phillips L.J., Seal M.L., Yucel M., Nelson B., Yung A.R., Jackson G., Mcgorry P.D., Velakoulis D., Pantelis C. Hippocampal pathology in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis: a multi-modal magnetic resonance study. Neuroimage. 2010;52:62–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Yang C., Wu S., Lu W., Bai Y., Gao H. Brain differences in first-episode schizophrenia treated with quetiapine: a deformation-based morphometric study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2015;232:369–377. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3670-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Zeineh M.M., Engel S.A., Thompson P.M., Bookheimer S.Y. Dynamics of the hippocampus during encoding and retrieval of face-name pairs. Science. 2003;299:577–580. doi: 10.1126/science.1077775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Zhou Y., Shu N., Liu Y. Altered resting-state functional connectivity and anatomical connectivity of hippocampus in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2008;100:120–132. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Zierhut K.B.B., Schott B., Fenker D., Walter M., Albrecht D., Steiner J., Schütze H., Northoff G., Düzel E., Schiltz K. The role of hippocampus dysfunction in deficient memory encoding and positive symptoms in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2010;183:187–194. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Zierhut K.C., Grassmann R., Kaufmann J., Steiner J., Bogerts B., Schiltz K. Hippocampal CA1 deformity is related to symptom severity and antipsychotic dosage in schizophrenia. Brain. 2013;136:804–814. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

mmc1.docx (21KB, docx)
mmc2.docx (1.2MB, docx)

RESOURCES