Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2018 Jan 29;7:e31130. doi: 10.7554/eLife.31130

The modulation of neural gain facilitates a transition between functional segregation and integration in the brain

James M Shine 1,2,, Matthew J Aburn 3, Michael Breakspear 3,4, Russell A Poldrack 1,
Editor: Gustavo Deco5
PMCID: PMC5818252  PMID: 29376825

Abstract

Cognitive function relies on a dynamic, context-sensitive balance between functional integration and segregation in the brain. Previous work has proposed that this balance is mediated by global fluctuations in neural gain by projections from ascending neuromodulatory nuclei. To test this hypothesis in silico, we studied the effects of neural gain on network dynamics in a model of large-scale neuronal dynamics. We found that increases in neural gain directed the network through an abrupt dynamical transition, leading to an integrated network topology that was maximal in frontoparietal ‘rich club’ regions. This gain-mediated transition was also associated with increased topological complexity, as well as increased variability in time-resolved topological structure, further highlighting the potential computational benefits of the gain-mediated network transition. These results support the hypothesis that neural gain modulation has the computational capacity to mediate the balance between integration and segregation in the brain.

Research organism: None

Introduction

The function of complex networks such as the human brain requires a trade-off between functional specialization and global communication (Deco et al., 2015a; Park and Friston, 2013; Tononi et al., 1994). Contemporary models of brain function suggest that this balance is manifest through dynamically changing patterns of correlated activity, constrained by the brains’ structural backbone (Deco et al., 2013; Honey et al., 2007; Varela et al., 2001). This in turn allows exploration of a repertoire of cortical states that balance the opposing topological properties of segregation (i.e. modular architectures with high functional specialization) and integration (i.e. inter-connection between specialist regions [Deco et al., 2015b; Ghosh et al., 2008]).

Recent work has demonstrated that the extent of integration in the brain is important for a range of cognitive functions, including effective task performance (Bassett et al., 2015; Shine et al., 2016a), episodic memory retrieval (Westphal et al., 2017) and conscious awareness (Barttfeld et al., 2015; Godwin et al., 2015). Furthermore, the topological properties of functional brain networks have been shown to fluctuate over time (Chang and Glover, 2010; Hutchison et al., 2013), both within individual neuroimaging sessions (Shine et al., 2016a; Zalesky et al., 2014) and over the course of weeks to months (Shine et al., 2016b). While the extent of integration in the brain may relate to more effective inter-regional communication, perhaps via synchronous oscillatory activity (Fries, 2015; Lisman and Jensen, 2013; Varela et al., 2001), there are also benefits related to a relatively segregated network architecture, including lower metabolic costs (Bullmore and Sporns, 2012; Zalesky et al., 2014) and effective performance as a function of learning (Bassett et al., 2015). However, despite these insights, the biological mechanisms responsible for driving fluctuations between integration and segregation remain unclear.

A candidate mechanism underlying flexible brain network dynamics is the global alteration in neural gain mediated by ascending neuromodulatory nuclei such as the locus coeruleus (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005; Sara, 2009). This small pontine nucleus projects diffusely throughout the brain and releases noradrenaline, a potent modulatory neurotransmitter that alters the precision and responsivity of targeted neurons (Waterhouse et al., 1988). Alterations in this system are known to play a crucial role in cognition, as there is evidence for a nonlinear (inverted-U shaped) relationship between noradrenaline concentration and cognitive performance (Robbins and Arnsten, 2009; Figure 1a).

Figure 1. Manipulating neural gain.

Figure 1.

(a) the Yerkes-Dodson relationship linking activity in the locus coeruleus nucleus to cognitive performance; (b) neural gain is modeled by a parameter (σ) that increases the maximum slope of the transfer function between incoming and outgoing activity within a brain region; (c) excitability is modeled by a parameter (γ) that amplifies the level of output; (d) the approach presently used to estimate network topology from the biophysical model.

Mechanistically, the noradrenergic system has been shown to alter neural gain (Servan-Schreiber et al., 1990) Figure 1b), increasing the signal to noise ratio of afferent input onto regions targeted by projections from the locus coeruleus. A crucial question is how these local changes in neural gain influence the configuration of the brain at the network level. Recent work has linked fluctuations in network topology to changes in pupil diameter (Eldar et al., 2013; Shine et al., 2016a; Shine et al., 2018), an indirect measure of locus coeruleus activity (Joshi et al., 2016; Murphy et al., 2014; Reimer et al., 2014, 2016), providing evidence for a link between the noradrenergic system and network-level topology. However, despite these insights, the mechanisms through which alterations in neural gain mediate fluctuations in global network topology are poorly understood.

Biophysical models of large-scale neuronal activity have yielded numerous insights into the dynamics of brain function, both during the resting state as well as in the context of task-driven brain function (Deco et al., 2009; Honey et al., 2007); for review, see Breakspear, 2017. Whereas prior research in this area has examined the influence of local dynamics, coupling strength, structural network topology and stochastic fluctuations on functional network topology (Deco et al., 2015b; Deco and Jirsa, 2012; Deco et al., 2017; Gollo et al., 2015; Woolrich and Stephan, 2013), the direct influence of neural gain has not been studied. Here, we used a combination of biophysical modeling and graph theoretical analyses (Sporns, 2013) to characterize the effect of neural gain on emergent network topology. Based on previous work (Shine et al., 2016a; Shine et al., 2018), we hypothesized that manipulations of neural gain would modulate the extent of integration in time-averaged patterns of functional connectivity.

Results

To test this hypothesis, we implemented a generic 2-dimensional neuronal oscillator model (Fitzhugh, 1961; Stefanescu and Jirsa, 2011) within the Virtual Brain toolbox (Jirsa et al., 2010; Sanz Leon et al., 2013) to generate regional time series that were constrained by a directed white matter connectome derived from the CoCoMac database (Kötter, 2004) Figure 1d). The simulated neuronal time series were passed through a Balloon-Windkessel model to simulate realistic BOLD data. Graph theoretical analyses were then applied to time-averaged correlations of regional BOLD data to estimate the functional topological signatures of network fluctuations (see Materials and methods for further details).

To simulate the effect of ascending neuromodulatory effects on inter-regional dynamics, we systematically manipulated neural gain (σ; Figure 1b) and excitability (γ; Figure 1c). These two parameters alter different aspects of a sigmoidal transfer function, which models the nonlinear relationship between presynaptic afferent inputs and local firing rates (Freeman, 1979). When the σ and γ parameters are both low, fluctuations in regional activity arise mainly due to noise and local feedback. As the σ and γ parameters increase, the influence of activity communicated from connected input regions also increases, leading to non-linear cross-talk and hence, changes in global brain topology and dynamics. Here, we investigated the topological signature of simulated BOLD time series across a parameter space spanned by σ and γ in order to understand the combined effect of neural gain and excitability on global brain network dynamics.

Neural gain and excitability modulate network-level topological integration

We simulated BOLD time series data across a range of σ (0–1) and γ (0–1) and then subjected the time series from our simulation to graph theoretical analyses (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). This allowed us to estimate the amount of integration in the time-averaged functional connectivity matrix across the parameter space (Figure 2a). Specifically, we used the mean participation coefficient (BA) of the time-averaged connectivity matrix at each combination of σ and γ. High values of mean BA suggest a relative increase in inter-modular connectivity, thus promoting the diversity of connections between modules (Bertolero et al., 2017) and increasing the integrative signature of the network (Shine et al., 2016a). The converse situation (i.e., segregation) can thus be indexed by low mean BA scores, or alternatively by the modularity statistic, Q. We observed a complex relationship between σ, γ and BA, such that maximal integration occurred at high levels of σ but with intermediate values of γ. Outside of this zone, the time-averaged connectome was markedly less integrated. Similar patterns were observed for other topological measures of integration, such as the inverse modularity (Q−1) and global efficiency (Figure 2—figure supplement 1).

Figure 2. Network Integration and Phase Synchrony.

(a) mean participation as a function of σ and γ; (b) phase synchrony (ρ) as a function of σ and γ; (c) mean participation (BA) aligned to the critical point (represented here as a dotted line) as a function of increasing σ; (d) BA aligned to the critical point as a function of increasing γ – the left and right dotted lines depicts the synchrony change at low and high γ, respectively. The y-axis in (c) and (d) represents the distance in parameter space aligned to the critical point/bifurcation for either σ (ΔσCB; mean across 0.2 ≤ γ ≤0.6) or γ (ΔγCB; mean across 0.3 ≤ σ ≤1.0). Lines are colored according to the state of phase synchrony on either side of the bifurcation (blue: low synchrony; yellow: high synchrony).

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Relationship between phase regimen boundary and alternative measures of network integration.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(a-c) the inverse modularity (Q−1) was maximal following the σ boundary (ΔσCP; mean across 0.2 ≤ γ ≤0.6) and the immediately prior to the abrupt phase transition at high γ (ΔγCP; mean across 0.3 ≤ σ ≤1.0); (d-f) global efficiency (G.E.) was maximally variable with increasing σ and across the critical boundary at high γ.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Standard deviation of the order parameter across the parameter space.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(a) standard deviation of order parameter across the parameter space; (b) fluctuation scaling pre-boundary (σ = 0.375 and γ = 0.50); and (c) post-boundary (σ = 0.50 and γ = 0.575) – the thin blue line denotes a Pareto (i.e., power law) scaling effect.
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Transition to self-sustained oscillations in a single brain region.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

For the generic 2D oscillator model this shows the real parts of eigenvalues at equilibrium as the level of input (Iapp) to a region is increased. A transition to self-sustained oscillations in a local region occurs where this curve crosses zero. That regime is bounded by supercritical Hopf bifurcations at Iapp = 2.0 and Iapp = 14.
Figure 2—figure supplement 4. Average time-averaged connectivity matrix in regions of the parameter space associated with high (yellow) or low (blue) ordered phase synchrony.

Figure 2—figure supplement 4.

Neural gain transitions the network across a critical boundary

The relative simplicity of our local neural model allows formal quantification of the inter-regional phase relationships that characterize the underlying neuronal dynamics. These fast neuronal phase dynamics compliment the view given by the slow BOLD amplitude fluctuations and give insight into their fundamental dynamic causes. We employed a phase order parameter, that quantifies the extent to which regions within the network align their oscillatory phase – high values on this scale reflect highly ordered synchronous oscillations across the network, whereas low values reflect a relatively asynchronous system (Breakspear et al., 2010; Kuramoto, 1984).

Across the parameter space, we observed two clear states (Figure 2b): one associated with high (ρ ≥0.5; yellow) and one with low (ρ <0.5; blue) mean synchrony, with a clear critical boundary demarcating the two states (dotted white line in Figure 2a/b) that was associated with a relative increase in the standard deviation of the order parameter (Figure 2—figure supplement 2a). This strong demarcation between states is a known signature of critical behavior (Chialvo, 2010), which can occur at both the regional and network level. We observed evidence for both regional and network criticality in our simulation, whereby small changes in parameters (here, σ and γ) facilitated an abrupt transition between qualitatively distinct states. At the regional level, this pattern is observed as a transition from input-driven fluctuations about a stable equilibrium to self-sustained oscillations (Figure 2—figure supplement 3). At the network level, the combined influence of increased gain and structural connections manifest as a transition to high amplitude, inter-regional phase synchrony (Figure 2—figure supplement 2b).

To further disambiguate the system-level dynamics, we studied the probability distribution of the fluctuations in the order parameter. Close to the boundary, we observed a truncated Pareto (i.e., power law) scaling regime, spanning up to two orders of magnitude (Figure 2—figure supplement 2b). This pattern is consistent with a critical bifurcation within a complex system consisting of many components (see Cocchi et al., 2017 and Heitmann and Breakspear, 2017Heitman and Breakspear, 2017 for further discussion). After crossing the boundary, this relationship develops a ‘knee’ above the power-law scaling (Figure 2—figure supplement 2b), consistent with the emergence of a characteristic temporal scale in a super-critical system (Roberts et al., 2015). These observations suggest that the system undergoes a bifurcation across a critical boundary as the synchronization manifold loses stability.

A host of contemporary neuroscientific theories hypothesize that temporal phase synchrony between regions underlies effective communication between neural regions (Fries, 2015; Lisman and Jensen, 2013; Varela et al., 2001), which would otherwise remain isolated if not brought into temporal lockstep with one another. As such, we might expect that the changes in neural gain that integrate the brain might do so through the modulation of inter-regional phase synchrony. Our results were consistent with this hypothesis. By aligning changes in the topological signature of the network to the critical point delineating the two states, we were able to demonstrate a significant increase in integration (mean BA; T798 = 2.57; p=0.01) and decrease in segregation (Q; T798 = −17.44; p<0.001) of network-level BOLD fluctuations in the highly phase synchronous state. Specifically, global integration demonstrated a sharp increase in the zone associated with the high amplitude synchronous oscillations, particularly for intermediate values of γ (Figure 2c). In contrast, the transitions associated with manipulating γ (particularly at high values of σ) led to an inverse U-shaped relationship: the network was relatively segregated at high and low levels of γ, but integrated at intermediate values of γ, albeit with a monotonic relationship when increasing σ for low levels of γ (Figure 2d). In addition, increases in between-hemisphere connectivity were more pronounced than within-hemisphere connectivity in the ordered state (within: 0.010 ± 0.017; between: 0.014 ± 0.013; T2,848 = 7.104; p=10−12; see Figure 2—figure supplement 4). Together, these results suggest that neural gain and excitability act together to traverse a transition in network dynamics, maximizing inter-regional phase synchrony and integrating the functional connectome.

Neural gain increases topological complexity and temporal variability

Having identified a relationship between neural gain and network architecture, we next investigated the putative topological benefit of this trade-off. A measure that characterizes the topological balance between integration and segregation is communicability (Estrada and Hatano, 2008), which quantifies the number of short paths that can be traversed between two regions of a network (Mišić et al., 2015). In networks with high communicability, individual regions are able to interact with a large proportion of the network through relatively short paths, which in turn may facilitate effective communication between otherwise segregated regions. In contrast to the relationship observed between neural gain and network integration, communicability was maximal at the critical boundaries between synchronous and asynchronous behavior (Figure 3a–c). Thus, the topological signature of the network was most effectively balanced between integration and segregation as the system transitioned between disorder and order through the modulation of inter-regional synchrony by subtle changes in neural gain.

Figure 3. Topological and temporal relationships with phase regimen boundary.

Figure 3.

(a-c) network communicability was maximal following the σ boundary (ΔσCP; mean across 0.2 ≤ γ ≤0.6) and the immediately prior to the abrupt phase transition at high γ (ΔγCP; mean across 0.3 ≤ σ ≤1.0); (d-f) time-resolved between-module participation (BT) was maximally variable with increasing σ and across the critical boundary at high γ.

Another important signature of complex systems is their flexibility over time. In previous work, we showed that the ‘resting state’ is characterized by significant fluctuations in network topology, in which the brain traverses between states that maximize either integration or segregation (Shine et al., 2016a). This variability was diminished during a cognitively challenging task, and the extent of integration was positively associated with improved task performance (Shine et al., 2016a). To determine whether these alterations in topological variability may have been related to changes in neural gain, we estimated the time-resolved mean participation coefficient (BT) of the simulated BOLD time series and then determined whether the variability of this measure over time changed as a function of σ and γ. We found that the variability of time-resolved integration within each trial was maximized across the critical boundary, as the network switched between disordered and ordered phase synchrony (Figure 3d–f). These results support the hypothesis that changes in neural gain may control the temporal variability of network topology as a function of behavioral state.

Gain-mediated integration is maximal in frontoparietal hub regions

To determine whether the influence of neural gain on network dynamics was related to the underlying structural connectivity of the brain, we estimated the ‘rich club’ architecture of the structural connectome (Figure 4a). Compared to low-degree nodes, rich club regions demonstrated an increase in ‘realized’ mean gain adjacent to the critical boundary (Figure 4b). In short, this means that activity within frontoparietal ‘hub’ regions (red in Figure 4a) was more strongly affected by the interaction between neural gain and network topology than in non-hub regions (blue/green in Figure 4a). Indeed, this result demonstrates that the ‘realized’ gain of individual regions is not simply related to the applied gain (i.e. input from the ascending noradrenergic system; (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005), but also non-linearly depends on afferent activity from topologically connected regions (Figure 4c/d). The observed effect was particularly evident for intermediate values of γ, suggesting that the hub regions were differentially impacted by neural gain at the critical boundary between the asynchronous and synchronous states. Interestingly, similar dissociations were observed when comparing regions with high and low diversity (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), suggesting a role for future experiments to disambiguate the importance of degree and diversity in the mediation of global network topology (Bertolero et al., 2017). However, given the substantial overlap between regions in the ‘rich’ and diverse’ clubs (73% of regions were found in both groups), our results confirm a crucial role for frontoparietal regions in the control of network-level integration as a function of ascending neuromodulatory gain.

Figure 4. Regional clustering results.

(a) regions from the CoCoMac data organized according to rich club (red), feeder (blue) or local (green) status, along with a force-directed plot of the top 10% of connections (aligned by hemisphere), colored according to structural hub connectivity status; (b) the rich club cluster demonstrated an increase in realized mean gain (the relative output as a function of its’ unique topology) at the bifurcation boundary, compared to feeder and local nodes, which showed higher realized gain at high levels of σ and γ; (c) the three clusters of regions also demonstrated differential responses to neural gain; and (d) excitability. The black lines in (c) and (d) denote significant differences in BA between the two groups.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Diverse Club.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(a) regional differences in integration (BA) as a function of changes in neural gain; and (b) excitability, separated into regions within or outside the diverse club.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Clustering coefficient.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(a) clustering coefficient across the parameter space; (b) as a function of changes in neural gain; and (c) excitability.

Discussion

We used a combination of computational modeling and graph theoretical analyses, quantifying the relationship between ascending neuromodulation and network-level integration in order to test a direct prediction from a previous neuroimaging study (Shine et al., 2016a). We found that increasing neural gain transitioned network dynamics across a bifurcation from disordered to ordered phase synchrony (Figure 2b) with a shift from a segregated to integrated neural architecture (Figure 2e and Figure 2—figure supplement 1). The critical boundary between these two states was associated with maximal communicability and temporal topological variability (Figure 3). Finally, the effect of neural gain was felt most prominently in high-degree frontoparietal network hubs (Figure 4 and Figure 4—figure supplement 2). Together, these results confirm our prior hypothesis and complement an emerging view of the brain that highlights a mechanistic bridge between ascending arousal systems and cognition (Shine et al., 2016a), providing a potential mechanistic explanation for the long-standing notion that noradrenergic activity demonstrates an inverted U-shaped curve with cognitive performance (Robbins and Arnsten, 2009, Figure 1a).

The major result from our study is that network-level fluctuations between segregation and integration in functional (BOLD) networks reflect an underlying transition in synchrony of faster neuronal oscillations, thus providing a previously unknown link between temporal scales in the brain (Figure 2b). At low levels of γ and σ, the governing equations are strongly stable (damped), so that all excursions from equilibrium must be driven by local noise – that is, regions are relatively insensitive to incoming inputs (Figure 1b/c). As γ and σ increase, local activity approaches an instability, and consequently incoming activity is able to substantially influence activity in target regions. This causes changes in the emergent whole-brain dynamics evident at both the short time scale of brain oscillations and the long time scale of BOLD correlation. A stark transition occurs at a critical point in the parameter space (denoted by the boundary between blue and yellow in Figure 2b), whereby small increases in σ lead to substantial alterations in the phase relationships between regions. Specifically, the network abruptly shifts from stable equilibrium to high-amplitude synchronized oscillation, facilitating an increase in effective communication between otherwise topologically distant regions (Fries, 2005; Varela et al., 2001). This same transition point is associated with a peak in informational complexity (Figure 3), further suggesting the importance of criticality in maximizing the information processing capacity of global network topology. Notably, the transition is also accompanied by a peak in the topological variability over time: hence a dynamic instability amongst fast neuronal oscillations yields increased network fluctuations at very slow time scales, again highlighting the crucial role of criticality to multi-scale neural phenomena (Cocchi et al., 2017).

The effect of neural gain on topology was greatest in a bilateral network of high-degree frontoparietal cortical regions (Figure 4). This suggests that the recruitment of these hub regions at intermediate levels of excitability and neural gain shifts collective network dynamics across a bifurcation, increasing effective interactions between otherwise segregated regions. This result underlines the effective influence of the structural ‘rich club’ (Figure 4), which in addition to providing topological support to the structural connectome (van den Heuvel and Sporns, 2013), may also facilitate the transition between distinct topological states. This relationship has been demonstrated previously in other studies, either by manipulating the excitability parameter alone (Deco et al., 2017; Zamora-López et al., 2016), or through the alteration of the intrinsic dynamics of the 2d oscillator model (Curto et al., 2009; Safaai et al., 2015), thus providing a strong conceptual link between structural topology and emergent dynamics. Crucially, the integrated states facilitated by gain-mediated hub recruitment have been shown to underlie effective cognitive performance (Shine et al., 2016a), episodic memory retrieval (Westphal et al., 2017) and conscious awareness (Barttfeld et al., 2015; Godwin et al., 2015), confirming the importance of ascending neuromodulatory systems for a suite of higher-level behavioral capacities.

Overall, our findings broadly support the predictions of the neural gain hypothesis of noradrenergic function (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005). For instance, manipulating neural gain, a plausible instantiation of the effects of ascending noradrenergic tone in the brain (Servan-Schreiber et al., 1990), led to marked alterations in network topology. Given the demonstrated links between network topology and cognitive function (Cohen and D'Esposito, 2016; Hearne et al., 2017; Shine et al., 2016a; Shine and Poldrack, 2017), our work thus provides a plausible mechanistic account of the long-standing notion of a nonlinear relationship between catecholamine levels and effective cognitive performance (Robbins and Arnsten, 2009; Shine et al., 2016a; Figure 1a). However, it bears mention that our model highlighted a relationship between neural gain, excitability and network topology, in which there was an inverted-U shaped relationship observed between excitability and integration that was related to two separate bifurcations (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). In contrast, the effect of neural gain on topology was demonstrably more linear, particularly at intermediate levels of γ (Figure 2). Importantly, although noradrenaline has been directly linked to alterations in gain (Servan-Schreiber et al., 1990), there is also reason to believe that noradrenergic tone should have a demonstrable effect on excitability (Curto et al., 2009; Safaai et al., 2015; Stringer et al., 2016). Combined with our observation of the importance of the interaction between neural gain and high-degree (Figure 4), diverse (Figure 4—figure supplement 1) hub regions, our results thus represent an extension of the neural gain hypothesis that integrates the ascending arousal system with the constraints imposed by multiple order parameters and structural network topology.

In addition, our results also align with previous hypotheses that highlighted the importance of α2-adrenoreceptor mediated hub recruitment with increasing concentrations of noradrenaline, particularly in the frontal cortex (Robbins and Arnsten, 2009; Sara, 2009). However, our findings are inconsistent with the hypothesis that neural gain mediates an increase in tightly clustered patterns of neural interactions (Eldar et al., 2013). In contrast to this prediction, our simulations showed that measures that reflect an increase in local clustering, such as modularity and the mean clustering coefficient (Figure 4—figure supplement 2), did not increase as a function of neural gain in the same manner as other measures, such as the mean participation coefficient. Therefore, our results suggest that an increase in functional integration (and hence, a concomitant decrease in local clustering) is a more effective indicator of the topological influence of increasing neural gain. However, it bears mention that the hypothesized relationship between clustering and neural gain was presented in the context of a focused learning paradigm (Eldar et al., 2013), whereas our data were not modeled in an explicit behavioral context. As such, future studies are required to disambiguate the relative relationship between neural gain and network topology as a function of task performance.

Prior computational studies have demonstrated a link between the structural and functional connectome, with the broad repertoire of functional network dynamics bounded by structural constraints imposed by the white-matter backbone of the brain (Deco and Jirsa, 2012; Honey et al., 2007, 2009). While the targeted role of gain modulation on local neuronal dynamics have been studied (Freeman, 1979), the impact of gain on functional network organization has not been pursued. Here, we have demonstrated a putative mechanism by which a known biological system (namely, the ascending noradrenergic system) can mediate structural-functional changes, essentially by navigating the functional connectome across a topological landscape characterized by alterations in oscillatory synchrony. However, the direct relationship between neural gain manipulation and the ascending noradrenergic system is likely to represent an oversimplification. Indeed, given the complexity and hierarchical organization of the brain, it is almost certain that other functional systems, such as the thalamus (Hwang et al., 2017) and fast-spiking interneurons (Stringer et al., 2016), play significant roles in mediating neural gain and hence, the balance between integration and segregation. Further studies are required to interrogate these mechanisms more directly.

A somewhat surprising result of our simulation is the link between phase- and amplitude-related measures of neuronal coupling. It has been known for some time that the BOLD signal is insensitive to the relative phase of underlying neural dynamics (Foster et al., 2016), relating more closely to changes in the local oscillator frequency and fluctuations in the relative amplitude of neural firing. Indeed, each of the model parameters used in our experiment (i.e., gain and coupling) exerts a complex influence on both the oscillator frequencies (and hence, the BOLD activity) and the global synchrony (and hence, the BOLD correlations). Moreover, in coupled oscillator systems such as this, the order parameter acts as a ‘mean field’ that feeds back and influences local dynamics (see e.g. Breakspear et al., 2010). Based on this knowledge, we can infer that estimates of connectivity using BOLD time series relate to covariance in amplitude fluctuations among pairs of regions, rather than alterations in phase synchrony. This clarification is important for modern theories of functional neuroscience, as synchronous relationships between regions in the phase domain have been used to explain effective communication between neural regions (Fries, 2015; Lisman and Jensen, 2013; Siegel et al., 2009), in which the precise timing between spiking populations determines the efficacy of information processing. Our results suggest a surprisingly robust link between these two measures, such that an integrated network with increased inter-modular amplitude correlation coincides with a peak in ordered phase synchrony between regions. In our model, the peak of network variability occurs at the critical transition between disordered and ordered phases, where the local dynamic states shows the most variability and where fast stochastic perturbations are most able to influence slow amplitude fluctuations. However, while our model provides evidence linking neural gain to functional integration, advanced models that display a broader variety of non-linear dynamics (Breakspear, 2017) are required to test these hypotheses more directly.

Together, our results suggest that the balance between integration and segregation relates to alterations in neural gain that exist within a ‘zone’ of maximal communicability and temporal variability. Our findings thus highlight important constraints on contemporary models of brain function, while also providing crucial implications for understanding effective brain function during task performance or as a function of neurodegenerative or psychiatric disease.

Materials and methods

Dynamical network modeling

The Virtual Brain software (Sanz Leon et al., 2013) was used to simulate neural activity across a lattice of parameter points in which we manipulated the inter-regional coupling between regions using both a gain parameter and an excitability parameter. Specifically, we used a generic 2-dimensional oscillator model (Equations 1 and 2) to create time series data that represents neural activity via two variables (the membrane potential and a slow recovery variable). This equation is based upon a modal approximation (Stefanescu and Jirsa, 2008) of a population of Fitzhugh-Nagumo neurons (Izhikevich and FitzHugh, 2006). The neuronal dynamics are given by,

Vi˙(t)= 20(Wi(t)+3Vi(t)2Vi(t)3+γIi)+ξi(t), (1)
W˙i(t)=20(Wi(t)10Vi(t))+ηi(t), (2)

where Vi represents the local mean membrane potential and Wi represents the corresponding slow recovery variable at node i. Stochastic fluctuations are introduced additively through the white noise processes ηi and ξi, drawn independently from Gaussian distributions with zero mean and unit variance. The synaptic current Ii arise from time-delayed input from other regions modulated in strength by the global excitability parameter γ. This input arises after the mean membrane potential V in distant nodes is converted into a firing rate via a sigmoid-shaped activation function S, and then transmitted with axonal time delays through the connectivity matrix. Hence the synaptic current at node i is given by,

Ii=jAijSjt-τij (3)

where Aij is the directed connectivity matrix derived from the 76 region CoCoMac connectome (Kötter, 2004), and τij is the corresponding time delay computed from the length of fiber tracts estimated by diffusion spectrum imaging (Sanz Leon et al., 2013). The conversion from regional membrane potential to firing rate is given by a sigmoid-shaped activation function,

Sit=11+e-σVit-m, (4)

where σ is the (global) gain parameter and the sigmoid activation function is shifted to center at m. These equations were integrated using a stochastic Heun method (Rüemelin, 1982).

The simulated neuronal data were fed through a Balloon-Windkessel model to simulate realistic Blood Oxygen Level Dependent signals (Friston et al., 2000). The simulated BOLD time series were band-pass filtered (0.01–0.1 Hz) and the Pearson’s correlation was then computed (and normalized using Fisher’s r-to-Z transformation).

We manipulated the inter-regional neural gain parameter σ and the regional excitability γ through a range of values (between 0–1). After aligning the sensitive region of the sigmoid function with its mean input (m = 1.5). Consistent with the effects of relatively diffuse projections from the locus coeruleus to cortex, all regions were given the same values of the σ and γ parameter for each trial. All code is freely available at https://github.com/macshine/gain_topology (Shine, 2018). A copy is archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/gain_topology.

Integration and segregation

The Louvain modularity algorithm from the Brain Connectivity Toolbox (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010) was used to estimate time-averaged community structure. The Louvain algorithm iteratively maximizes the modularity statistic, Q, for different community assignments until the maximum possible score of Q has been obtained (Equation 5). The modularity estimate for a given network is therefore a quantification of the extent to which the network may be subdivided into communities with stronger within-module than between-module connections. Here, we used the Q parameter to estimate the extent of segregation within each graph,

Q=1v+ijwij+-eij+δMiMj-1v++v-ijwij--eij-δMiMj (5)

where v is the total weight of the network (sum of all negative and positive connections), wij is the weighted and signed connection between regions i and j, eij is the strength of a connection divided by the total weight of the network, and δMiMj is set to one when regions are in the same community and 0 otherwise. ‘+’ and ‘–‘ superscripts denote all positive and negative connections, respectively. Consistent with previous work (Eldar et al., 2013), the mean clustering coefficient, which reflects the proportion of closed ‘triangles’ in the binarized graph, was also used as a measure of segregation (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010).

For each level of neural gain, the community assignment for each region was assessed 100 times and a consensus partition was identified using a fine-tuning algorithm from the Brain Connectivity Toolbox (http://www.brain-connectivity-toolbox.net/). All graph theoretical measures were calculated on weighted and signed connectivity matrices (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010), and weak connections were retained using a consistency thresholding technique that identifies weak, yet consistent connections by identifying edges with minimal variance across multiple iterations (Roberts et al., 2017). In order to assess global, large-scale communities, the resolution parameter was set to 1.0 (higher values tune the algorithm to detect smaller communities, which instead reflect local, rather than global, clustering). This parameter was chosen by calculating the resolution value which maximized the Surprise (Aldecoa and Marín, 2013) between the community structure of the network at each level of gain and resolution and a random network defined using a cumulative hypergeometric distribution (see [Aldecoa and Marín, 2013]).

The participation coefficient, BA (Equation 6) quantifies the extent to which a region connects across all modules (i.e. between-module strength). As such, the mean participation coefficient can be used to estimate the extent of integration within a graph. The participation coefficient, BAi, for a given region i is,

BAi=1-s=1nMκisκi2 (6)

where κis is the strength of the positive connections of region i to regions in module s, and κi is the sum of strengths of all positive connections of region i. The participation coefficient of a region is therefore close to one if its connections are uniformly distributed among all the modules and 0 if all of its links are within its own module. Finally, the global efficiency (mean inverse characteristic path length) and inverse modularity (Q−1) were estimated for each element of the parameter space as adjunct measures of integration.

Phase synchrony order parameter

To estimate the degree of phase synchrony at different points in the parameter space, we extracted the raw signal (Vi) from each region in the simulation and subtracted the least squares linear trend from each channel. We then computed the phase of the analytic signal for each channel using the Hilbert transform and then estimated the phase synchrony order parameter (across all channels), OP, which is given by,

ρ=1Nj=1Neiθj (7)

where i = -1 and θj represents the oscillation phase of the jth region. Large values of ρ denote phase alignment between regions (Breakspear et al., 2010; Kuramoto, 1984). The value of ρ for each parameter combination was subsequently averaged over time and across sessions. By designating each parameter combination as resulting in either a synchronized (ρ ≥0.5) or unsynchronized (ρ <0.5) regime, we were able to determine whether network topology changes as a function of neural gain and excitability estimated from BOLD data coincided with changes of underlying phase synchrony. Specifically, we then separately grouped topological variables and within- and between-hemisphere connectivity according to their underlying ρ value and then estimated an independent-samples t-test between the two groups. The standard deviation of the order parameter, ρ, was also calculated and averaged across sessions. Finally, the dwell times for regional fluctuations were estimated for a number of characteristic parameter choices and analyzed for evidence of Pareto (i.e. power law) scaling.

Communicability

The communicability, C, between a pair of nodes i and j is defined as a weighted sum of the number of all walks connecting the pair of nodes (within weighted connectivity matrix, A) and has been shown to be equivalent to the matrix exponent of a binarized graph, eA (Estrada and Hatano, 2008). For ease of interpretation, we calculated the log10-transformed mean of communicability for each graph across iterations and values of neural gain.

Cij=k=0Akijk!=eA (8)

Topological variability

To estimate time-resolved functional connectivity between the 76 nodal pairs, we used a recently described statistical technique (Multiplication of Temporal Derivatives; (Shine et al., 2015); http://github.com/macshine/coupling), which is computed by calculating the point-wise product of temporal derivative of pairwise time series (Equation 7). To reduce the contamination of high-frequency noise in the time-resolved connectivity data, Mij was averaged over a temporal window (w = 15 time points). Individual functional connectivity matrices were calculated within each temporal window, thus generating an unthresholded (signed and weighted) 3D adjacency matrix (region × region × time) for each participant. These matrices were then subjected to time-resolved topological analyses, which allowed us to estimate the participation coefficient for each region over time (BT). We used the mean regional standard deviation of this measure to estimate time-resolved topological variability in the simulated data.

Mijt=1wtt+wdtit×dtjtσdti×σdtj (9)

for each time point, t, Mij is defined according to Equation 1, where dt is the first temporal derivative of the ith or jth time series at time t, σ is the standard deviation of the temporal derivative time series for region i or j and w is the window length of the simple moving average. This equation can then be calculated over the course of a time series to obtain an estimate of time-resolved connectivity between pairs of regions.

Structural rich club

To test whether changes associated with neural gain were mediated by highly-interconnected high-degree hubs, we identified a set of ‘rich club’ regions using the structural white matter connectome from the CoCoMac database (Kötter, 2004). Briefly, the degree of each node i in the network was determined by calculating the number of links that node i shared with k other nodes in the network. All nodes that showed a number of connections of ≤k were removed from the network. For the remaining network, the rich-club coefficient (Φk) was computed as the ratio of connections present between the remaining nodes and the total number of possible connections that would be present when the set would be fully connected. We then normalized Φk relative to a set of random networks with similar density and connectivity distributions. When ΦZ is greater than 1, the network can be said to display a ‘rich club’ architecture. Individual regions that are interconnected at the value of k at which the network demonstrates a ‘rich club’ architecture are thus designated as ‘rich club’ nodes (n = 22). Any nodes outside of this group but still sharing a connection are labeled as ‘feeder’ nodes (n = 44), and regions disconnected from the rich club are designated as ‘local’ nodes (n = 10). The results were projected onto a standard surface representation of the macaque cortex (Figure 4). After segmenting the network in this fashion, we were able to estimate the realized mean gain and BA across the parameter space for regions according to their structural topology.

Realized neural gain

While the neural gain parameter σ controls the maximum gain in each region within the simulation by setting the maximum slope of the sigmoid, the realized gain (mean ratio of sigmoid output to input) for each brain region depends upon the distribution of its input, and is greater when the input level is concentrated near the center of the sigmoid. We estimated the regional variation in effective or ‘realized’ neural gain by calculating the integral of the instantaneous sigmoid slope over its complete input range, weighted by the probability of each input level. We then compared these values as a function of nodal class (rich club vs other nodes) at each aspect of the parameter space.

Reliability

We ran a number of subsequent tests to ensure that any observed changes in network topology were robust to the processing steps utilized in the analysis. Firstly, we re-analyzed data across a range of network thresholds (1–20%) and observed robust results (i.e. r > 0.75) for Q, mean BA, mean communicability and the standard deviation of BT on graphs estimated between the 9–20% threshold range. Secondly, as the number of modules estimated from graphs can change as a function of network topology, we re-examined the topological characteristics of networks that were matched for the number of modules (N = 4) and found no significant differences to the topological signatures estimated on the whole group.

Acknowledgements

We thank the creators of the Virtual Brain for their open-source software, Peter Bell for helpful comments, Bratislav Misic for sharing code and Joke Durnez for statistical advice.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

James M Shine, Email: mac.shine@sydney.edu.au.

Russell A Poldrack, Email: poldrack@stanford.edu.

Gustavo Deco, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Spain.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grant:

  • National Health and Medical Research Council GNT1072403 to James M Shine.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Resources, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Formal analysis, Supervision, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31130.012

References

  1. Aldecoa R, Marín I. Surprise maximization reveals the community structure of complex networks. Scientific Reports. 2013;3:1060. doi: 10.1038/srep01060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2005;28:403–450. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barttfeld P, Uhrig L, Sitt JD, Sigman M, Jarraya B, Dehaene S. Signature of consciousness in the dynamics of resting-state brain activity. PNAS. 2015;112:887–892. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418031112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bassett DS, Yang M, Wymbs NF, Grafton ST. Learning-induced autonomy of sensorimotor systems. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:744–751. doi: 10.1038/nn.3993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bertolero MA, Yeo BTT, D'Esposito M. The diverse club. Nature Communications. 2017;8:1277. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01189-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Breakspear M, Heitmann S, Daffertshofer A. Generative models of cortical oscillations: neurobiological implications of the kuramoto model. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2010;4:190. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Breakspear M. Dynamic models of large-scale brain activity. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:340–352. doi: 10.1038/nn.4497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bullmore E, Sporns O. The economy of brain network organization. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13:336–349. doi: 10.1038/nrn3214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chang C, Glover GH. Time-frequency dynamics of resting-state brain connectivity measured with fMRI. NeuroImage. 2010;50:81–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chialvo DR. Emergent complex neural dynamics. Nature Physics. 2010;6:744–750. doi: 10.1038/nphys1803. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cocchi L, Gollo LL, Zalesky A, Breakspear M. Criticality in the brain: A synthesis of neurobiology, models and cognition. Progress in Neurobiology. 2017;158:132–152. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2017.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cohen JR, D'Esposito M. The segregation and integration of distinct brain networks and their relationship to cognition. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:12083–12094. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2965-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Curto C, Sakata S, Marguet S, Itskov V, Harris KD. A simple model of cortical dynamics explains variability and state dependence of sensory responses in urethane-anesthetized auditory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:10600–10612. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2053-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Deco G, Jirsa V, McIntosh AR, Sporns O, Kötter R. Key role of coupling, delay, and noise in resting brain fluctuations. PNAS. 2009;106:10302–10307. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901831106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Deco G, Jirsa VK, McIntosh AR. Resting brains never rest: computational insights into potential cognitive architectures. Trends in Neurosciences. 2013;36:268–274. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Deco G, Jirsa VK. Ongoing cortical activity at rest: criticality, multistability, and ghost attractors. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:3366–3375. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2523-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Deco G, Kringelbach ML, Jirsa VK, Ritter P. The dynamics of resting fluctuations in the brain: metastability and its dynamical cortical core. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:3095. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-03073-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Deco G, Tononi G, Boly M, Kringelbach ML. Rethinking segregation and integration: contributions of whole-brain modelling. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2015a;16:430–439. doi: 10.1038/nrn3963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Deco G, Tononi G, Boly M, Kringelbach ML. Rethinking segregation and integration: contributions of whole-brain modelling. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2015b;16:430–439. doi: 10.1038/nrn3963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Eldar E, Cohen JD, Niv Y. The effects of neural gain on attention and learning. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1146–1153. doi: 10.1038/nn.3428. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Estrada E, Hatano N. Communicability in complex networks. Physical Review E. 2008;77:036111. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.77.036111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fitzhugh R. Impulses and physiological states in theoretical models of nerve membrane. Biophysical Journal. 1961;1:445–466. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(61)86902-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Foster BL, He BJ, Honey CJ, Jerbi K, Maier A, Saalmann YB. Spontaneous Neural Dynamics and Multi-scale Network Organization. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2016;10:7. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Freeman WJ. Nonlinear gain mediating cortical stimulus-response relations. Biological Cybernetics. 1979;33:237–247. doi: 10.1007/BF00337412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9:474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fries P. Rhythms for cognition: communication through coherence. Neuron. 2015;88:220–235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Friston KJ, Mechelli A, Turner R, Price CJ. Nonlinear responses in fMRI: the Balloon model, Volterra kernels, and other hemodynamics. NeuroImage. 2000;12:466–477. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0630. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ghosh A, Rho Y, McIntosh AR, Kötter R, Jirsa VK. Noise during rest enables the exploration of the brain's dynamic repertoire. PLoS Computational Biology. 2008;4:e1000196. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Godwin D, Barry RL, Marois R. Breakdown of the brain's functional network modularity with awareness. PNAS. 2015;112:3799–3804. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414466112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gollo LL, Zalesky A, Hutchison RM, van den Heuvel M, Breakspear M. Dwelling quietly in the rich club: brain network determinants of slow cortical fluctuations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2015;370:20140165. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hearne LJ, Cocchi L, Zalesky A, Mattingley JB. Reconfiguration of Brain Network Architectures between Resting-State and Complexity-Dependent Cognitive Reasoning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:0485-17–0488411. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0485-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Heitmann S, Breakspear M. QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute. 2017. Handbook for the Brain Dynamics Toolbox Version 2017c.9781549720703 [Google Scholar]
  33. Honey CJ, Kötter R, Breakspear M, Sporns O. Network structure of cerebral cortex shapes functional connectivity on multiple time scales. PNAS. 2007;104:10240–10245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701519104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Honey CJ, Sporns O, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Thiran JP, Meuli R, Hagmann P. Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity from structural connectivity. PNAS. 2009;106:2035–2040. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811168106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hutchison RM, Womelsdorf T, Allen EA, Bandettini PA, Calhoun VD, Corbetta M, Della Penna S, Duyn JH, Glover GH, Gonzalez-Castillo J, Handwerker DA, Keilholz S, Kiviniemi V, Leopold DA, de Pasquale F, Sporns O, Walter M, Chang C. Dynamic functional connectivity: promise, issues, and interpretations. NeuroImage. 2013;80:360–378. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hwang K, Bertolero MA, Liu WB, D'Esposito M. The human thalamus is an integrative Hub for functional brain networks. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:5594–5607. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0067-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Izhikevich E, FitzHugh R. FitzHugh-Nagumo model. Scholarpedia. 2006;1:1349. doi: 10.4249/scholarpedia.1349. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jirsa VK, Sporns O, Breakspear M, Deco G, McIntosh AR. Towards the virtual brain: network modeling of the intact and the damaged brain. Archives Italiennes De Biologie. 2010;148:189–205. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Joshi S, Li Y, Kalwani RM, Gold JI. Relationships between Pupil Diameter and Neuronal Activity in the Locus Coeruleus, Colliculi, and Cingulate Cortex. Neuron. 2016;89:221–234. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kuramoto Y. Chemical Oscillations, Waves, and Turbulence. 1984. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kötter R. Online retrieval, processing, and visualization of primate connectivity data from the CoCoMac database. Neuroinformatics. 2004;2:127–144. doi: 10.1385/NI:2:2:127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lisman JE, Jensen O. The θ-γ neural code. Neuron. 2013;77:1002–1016. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mišić B, Betzel RF, Nematzadeh A, Goñi J, Griffa A, Hagmann P, Flammini A, Ahn YY, Sporns O. Cooperative and competitive spreading dynamics on the human connectome. Neuron. 2015;86:1518–1529. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Murphy PR, O'Connell RG, O'Sullivan M, Robertson IH, Balsters JH. Pupil diameter covaries with BOLD activity in human locus coeruleus. Human Brain Mapping. 2014;35:4140–4154. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Park HJ, Friston K. Structural and functional brain networks: from connections to cognition. Science. 2013;342:1238411. doi: 10.1126/science.1238411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Reimer J, Froudarakis E, Cadwell CR, Yatsenko D, Denfield GH, Tolias AS. Pupil fluctuations track fast switching of cortical states during quiet wakefulness. Neuron. 2014;84:355–362. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Reimer J, McGinley MJ, Liu Y, Rodenkirch C, Wang Q, McCormick DA, Tolias AS. Pupil fluctuations track rapid changes in adrenergic and cholinergic activity in cortex. Nature Communications. 2016;7:13289. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Robbins TW, Arnsten AF. The neuropsychopharmacology of fronto-executive function: monoaminergic modulation. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2009;32:267–287. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Roberts JA, Boonstra TW, Breakspear M. The heavy tail of the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2015;31:164–172. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Roberts JA, Perry A, Roberts G, Mitchell PB, Breakspear M. Consistency-based thresholding of the human connectome. NeuroImage. 2017;145:118–129. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. NeuroImage. 2010;52:1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Rüemelin W. Numerical Treatment of Stochastic Differential Equations. SIAM Journal on Numerical Analysis. 1982;19:604–613. doi: 10.1137/0719041. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Safaai H, Neves R, Eschenko O, Logothetis NK, Panzeri S. Modeling the effect of locus coeruleus firing on cortical state dynamics and single-trial sensory processing. PNAS. 2015;112:12834–12839. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1516539112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Sanz Leon P, Knock SA, Woodman MM, Domide L, Mersmann J, McIntosh AR, Jirsa V. The Virtual Brain: a simulator of primate brain network dynamics. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics. 2013;7:10. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2013.00010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Sara SJ. The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:211–223. doi: 10.1038/nrn2573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Servan-Schreiber D, Printz H, Cohen JD. A network model of catecholamine effects: gain, signal-to-noise ratio, and behavior. Science. 1990;249:892–895. doi: 10.1126/science.2392679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Shine JM, Bissett PG, Bell PT, Koyejo O, Balsters JH, Gorgolewski KJ, Moodie CA, Poldrack RA. The dynamics of functional brain networks: integrated network states during cognitive task performance. Neuron. 2016a;92:544–554. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Shine JM, Koyejo O, Bell PT, Gorgolewski KJ, Gilat M, Poldrack RA. Estimation of dynamic functional connectivity using Multiplication of Temporal Derivatives. NeuroImage. 2015;122:399–407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Shine JM, Koyejo O, Poldrack RA. Temporal metastates are associated with differential patterns of time-resolved connectivity, network topology, and attention. PNAS. 2016b;113:9888–9891. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1604898113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Shine JM, Poldrack RA. Principles of dynamic network reconfiguration across diverse brain states. NeuroImage. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Shine JM, van den Brink RL, Hernaus D, Nieuwenhuis S, Poldrack RA. Catecholaminergic manipulation alters dynamic network topology across cognitive states. Network Neuroscience. 2018 doi: 10.1162/netn_a_00042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Shine JM. Gain_topology. Github; 2018. https://github.com/macshine/ gain_topology [Google Scholar]
  63. Siegel M, Warden MR, Miller EK. Phase-dependent neuronal coding of objects in short-term memory. PNAS. 2009;106:21341–21346. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908193106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sporns O. The human connectome: origins and challenges. NeuroImage. 2013;80:53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Stefanescu RA, Jirsa VK. A low dimensional description of globally coupled heterogeneous neural networks of excitatory and inhibitory neurons. PLoS Computational Biology. 2008;4:e1000219. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Stefanescu RA, Jirsa VK. Reduced representations of heterogeneous mixed neural networks with synaptic coupling. Physical Review E. 2011;83:026204. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.83.026204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Stringer C, Pachitariu M, Steinmetz NA, Okun M, Bartho P, Harris KD, Sahani M, Lesica NA. Inhibitory control of correlated intrinsic variability in cortical networks. eLife. 2016;5:e19695. doi: 10.7554/eLife.19695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Tononi G, Sporns O, Edelman GM. A measure for brain complexity: relating functional segregation and integration in the nervous system. PNAS. 1994;91:5033–5037. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.11.5033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O. An anatomical substrate for integration among functional networks in human cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:14489–14500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2128-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Varela F, Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2001;2:229–239. doi: 10.1038/35067550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Waterhouse BD, Sessler FM, Cheng JT, Woodward DJ, Azizi SA, Moises HC, Jung-Tung C. New evidence for a gating action of norepinephrine in central neuronal circuits of mammalian brain. Brain Research Bulletin. 1988;21:425–432. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(88)90154-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Westphal AJ, Wang S, Rissman J. Episodic memory retrieval benefits from a less modular brain network organization. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:3523–3531. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2509-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Woolrich MW, Stephan KE. Biophysical network models and the human connectome. NeuroImage. 2013;80:330–338. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Zalesky A, Fornito A, Cocchi L, Gollo LL, Breakspear M. Time-resolved resting-state brain networks. PNAS. 2014;111:10341–10346. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1400181111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Zamora-López G, Chen Y, Deco G, Kringelbach ML, Zhou C. Functional complexity emerging from anatomical constraints in the brain: the significance of network modularity and rich-clubs. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:38424. doi: 10.1038/srep38424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Gustavo Deco1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "The modulation of neural gain facilitates a transition between functional segregation and integration in the brain" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by a Senior Editor and three reviewers, one of whom, Gustavo Deco (Reviewer #1), is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Tobias H Donner (Reviewer #2); Maxwell Bertolero (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Your paper presents a very useful integration a modeling framework and graph theoretical analysis, providing a mechanistic explanation of how neuromodulation balances the equilibrium between segregation and integration of functional information. All reviewers found the paper to be well written and the results to be relevant for the broader neuroscience community. All reviewers support the publication of the paper in eLife, provided that you address the following issues in your revision.

1) Additional analyses:

1a) The participation coefficient is a great measure of integration, but it needs to be supplemented with other measures. You should test the integration hypotheses with the participation coefficient, Q (inverse), and efficiency, and more carefully lay out why these measures make sense for testing the hypotheses regarding integration and segregation.

Moreover, the participation coefficient is a nodal measure, but you only report the mean – are all nodes equally increasing their participation coefficients? Are low or high nodes increasing their participation coefficients? These analyses will give more insight into exactly what is occurring in the network as a result of neural gain.

Rationale behind this request: The participation coefficient is a relative measure of the diversity a node's connections across communities. Thus, consider a node with strong connectivity to community A and weak connectivity to communities B-F. If the node simply decreases connectivity to community A, its participation coefficient increases. While this is likely rare, it can occur, and this might not capture the intuition of increased integration. On the other hand, perhaps it does, as the node is interacting with communities in a more equal fashion. Moreover, if other nodes from community A shift to community B, the participation coefficient of the node will increase, even if none of its connections did. Because of these ambiguities, supplementing the mean participation coefficient with Q and efficiency (the inverse of the sum of shortest paths) would be ideal. While Q is used as a measure of segregation, which is appropriate, the reasoning behind this is not laid out. It should be.

1b) You show that the profile for global phase synchrony (computed from the regional membrane potentials) is very similar to the profiles for the graph-theoretical measures that are based on the correlations of (much slower) local BOLD time series (Figure 2A and B). This observation is non-trivial. It should be complemented by an analysis of a measure of the dynamics of global phase synchrony – for instance, the standard deviation of global phase synchrony, which has been used to quantify metastability. Does this measure exhibit the same profile as those in Figure 2A and B? Along that line, please discuss why the similarity between BOLD correlation topology and global phase synchrony emerges. Do you have an intuition for this?

1c) The "Gain mediated integration is maximal in frontoparietal hub regions" findings should be complemented by analyzing the "diverse club" or the regions with high participation coefficients (Bertolero et al., 2017), which was found to be more highly interconnected than the rich club in the macaque.

2) Discussion, embedding of the present results into the existing literature.

2a) The Discussion should elaborate on the facts that (i) several of the results are motivated and expected from previous work, and (ii) the excitability parameter in your model is analogous to the global coupling, which has been used as control parameter in previous studies. The main new contribution here seems to be the addition of a second control parameter, the global gain.

Please note that, all reviewers appreciate the need of implementing the effects of neuromodulation effect explicitly, but they feel that the readership would benefit from a more balanced discussion of the context.

Rationale behind this request: Several previous studies have shown that networks of oscillators reach a balance between integration and segregation, a maximal complexity, and the largest variability of temporal networks at the critical point separating the asynchronous and synchronous phases (e.g., Schmidt et al., 2015; Zamora-Lopez et al. 2016; Deco et al. 2017). These studies also showed that rich clubs have a leading role concerning integration/segregation and complexity. The network can be displaced from one phase to the other by changing a control parameter, which is usually chosen to be the global coupling or connectivity strength – analogous to the excitability parameter used in your model. In addition, you manipulate a second control parameter, namely the slope of the transfer function that converts inputs into firing rates. Increasing the slope brings the system from asynchronous state to the synchronous state, and, as shown in Equation [3], has the same effect than a global coupling.

2b) Changes in both parameters (gain and excitability) are necessary for producing the effects of interest in your model. In particular, the graph-theoretical measures show a non-monotonic (inverted U) dependency on excitability, but not on gain. Still, the title and much of your Discussion focusses on gain only, drawing firm links to the Astone-Jones and Cohen Adaptive Gain Theory. Please discuss more explicitly the interesting effects of excitability. Specifically: How can you reconcile the predominance of inverted U patterns for excitability but not gain, with the Aston-Jones and Cohen framework.

2c) Previous work by the labs of Ken Harris and Stefano Panzeri has also used the FitzHugh-Nagumo model to characterise the effects of state/neuromodulation on cortical dynamics – but by modulating the dynamics of the 2D-oscillator itself (Curto et al., 2009; Safaai et al., PNAS, 2015). This work should be discussed and related to your current approach.

3) Clarifications

3a) In general, all reviewers appreciate that the paper was written for a general audience. However, please include all equations and note in the main text where they exist. For example, please include the equation for Communicability and expand the explanation – is it all walks, or all walks that are part of all shortest walks between all nodes? It is not clear in the Results or Materials and methods section.

3b) Please unpack the rationale behind using the correspondence between structural and functional connectivity as criterion for delineating the "plausible" sub-space of parameter combinations. This is important because work into neuromodulation in small circuits shows that neuromodulation can reconfigure circuits, thus overriding the structural connectome (Marder, Neuron, 2012).

3c) It seems that Vi is used for the computation of phase synchrony (i.e., not the neural activity passed through the hemodynamics). This should be made explicit in the Materials and methods. (Terms like "raw signal" or "neural data" are ambiguous.)

3d) Figure 2D is described as inverted U relationship, a description that glosses over the left flank of this plot. The relationship clearly is monotonic, but not an inverted U.

3e) Figure 2—figure supplement 3 is missing axis labels and won't be understandable for a broad audience. Please explain what it shows and what that means.

eLife. 2018 Jan 29;7:e31130. doi: 10.7554/eLife.31130.017

Author response


[…] All reviewers support the publication of the paper in eLife, provided that you address the following issues in your revision.

1) Additional analyses:

1a) The participation coefficient is a great measure of integration, but it needs to be supplemented with other measures. You should test the integration hypotheses with the participation coefficient, Q (inverse), and efficiency, and more carefully lay out why these measures make sense for testing the hypotheses regarding integration and segregation.

We thank the reviewers for their thoughtful comments regarding the potential limitations associated with the interpretation of the participation coefficient as a unitary measure to estimate topological integration. As requested, we have calculated the inverse of the modularity statistic (Q-1), taking care to iterate the algorithm a number of times due to stochasticity, and the global efficiency (GE) for the time-averaged connectivity matrix associated with each element of the parameter space. The expected result was apparent for both variables – i.e., increased Q-1 and GE as a function of increasing α and intermediate γ. We have added these two results to the manuscript, and included a separate figure, Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

The following text was added to the manuscript:

“Similar patterns were observed for other topological measures of integration, such as the inverse modularity (Q-1) and global efficiency (Figure 2—figure supplement 1).”

Moreover, the participation coefficient is a nodal measure, but you only report the mean – are all nodes equally increasing their participation coefficients? Are low or high nodes increasing their participation coefficients? These analyses will give more insight into exactly what is occurring in the network as a result of neural gain.

We thank the reviewers for this comment, and have conducted an adjunct analysis (similar to the experiment conducted for the ‘rich club’ regions) for the so-called ‘diverse club’ – i.e., regions with elevated participation. Specifically, we calculated the mean BA across the parameter space and then identified the top 30% of nodes (to match the number of identified rich club regions) – 16/22 regions (~73%) were present in both the ‘diverse’ and ‘rich’ club. We then estimated the mean BA across the parameter space for regions within the ‘diverse club’ and contrasted their gain- and excitability-mediated alterations in BA. The results (presented in Figure 4—figure supplement 1) were comparable to those observed in the rich club analysis, suggesting a need for future experiments to delineate the precise topological role of degree and diversity in mediating the functional signature of brain activity over time.

The following text was added to the manuscript:

“Interestingly, similar dissociations were observed when comparing regions with high and low diversity (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), suggesting a role for future experiments to disambiguate the importance of degree and diversity in the mediation of global network topology (Bertolero et al., 2017). However, given the substantial overlap between regions in the ‘rich’ and diverse’ clubs (73% of regions were found in both groups), our results confirm a crucial role for frontoparietal regions in the control of network-level integration as a function of ascending neuromodulatory gain.”

Rationale behind this request: The participation coefficient is a relative measure of the diversity a node's connections across communities. Thus, consider a node with strong connectivity to community A and weak connectivity to communities B-F. If the node simply decreases connectivity to community A, its participation coefficient increases. While this is likely rare, it can occur, and this might not capture the intuition of increased integration. On the other hand, perhaps it does, as the node is interacting with communities in a more equal fashion. Moreover, if other nodes from community A shift to community B, the participation coefficient of the node will increase, even if none of its connections did. Because of these ambiguities, supplementing the mean participation coefficient with Q and efficiency (the inverse of the sum of shortest paths) would be ideal. While Q is used as a measure of segregation, which is appropriate, the reasoning behind this is not laid out. It should be.

We thank the reviewers for their thoughtful comments and have added a more explicit justification of the utilization of the participation coefficient and modularity statistic as indices of integration and segregation, respectively.

The following text was added to the manuscript:

“This allowed us to estimate the amount of integration in the time-averaged functional connectivity matrix across the parameter space (Figure 2A[…] The converse situation (i.e., segregation) can thus be indexed by low mean BA scores, or alternatively by the modularity statistic, Q.”

1b) You show that the profile for global phase synchrony (computed from the regional membrane potentials) is very similar to the profiles for the graph-theoretical measures that are based on the correlations of (much slower) local BOLD time series (Figure 2A and B). This observation is non-trivial. It should be complemented by an analysis of a measure of the dynamics of global phase synchrony – for instance, the standard deviation of global phase synchrony, which has been used to quantify metastability. Does this measure exhibit the same profile as those in Figure 2A and B?

We appreciate the constructive suggestion to better quantify the nature of the dynamics at the critical boundary and, as suggested, have now estimated the standard deviation of the order parameter as a function of neural gain and excitability.

The following text was added to the manuscript:

“Across the parameter space, we observed two clear states (Figure 2B): one associated with high (ρ ≥ 0.5; yellow) and one with low (ρ < 0.5; blue) mean synchrony, with a clear critical boundary demarcating the two states (dotted white line in Figure 2A/B) that was associated with a relative increase in the standard deviation of the order parameter (Figure 2—figure supplement 2A).”

We next undertook additional analyses to better quantify the nature of the dynamics at the critical boundary. We first observed that the standard deviation of the order parameter increased around the boundary delineating the low and high ordered states (Figure 2—figure supplement 2B). This increase in fluctuation amplitude suggests the presence of complex dynamics, such as criticality, metastability or multistability (see Cocchi et al., 2017 and Heitman and Breakspear, 2017 for further discussion). To disambiguate these possibilities, we next studied the probability distribution of the fluctuations in the order parameter (Figure 2—figure supplement 2 and Roberts et al., 2015). Close to the boundary, we observed a truncated Pareto (power law) scaling regime spanning up to two orders of magnitude (Figure Y2—figure supplement 2B), consistent with a critical bifurcation in a complex, many-bodied system. After crossing the boundary, this relationship develops a ‘knee’ above the power-law scaling (Figure 2—figure supplement 2B), consistent with the emergence of a characteristic temporal scale in a super-critical system (Roberts et al. 2015). These observations suggest that the system undergoes a bifurcation across a critical boundary as the synchronization manifold loses stability.

The following text was added to the manuscript:

“To further disambiguate the system-level dynamics, we studied the probability distribution of the fluctuations in the order parameter. […] These observations suggest that the system undergoes a bifurcation across a critical boundary as the synchronization manifold loses stability.”

The following text was also added to the manuscript:

“The standard deviation of the order parameter, ρ, was also calculated and averaged across sessions. Finally, the dwell times for regional fluctuations were estimated for a number of characteristic parameter choices and analyzed for evidence of Pareto (i.e. power law) scaling.”

Along that line, please discuss why the similarity between BOLD correlation topology and global phase synchrony emerges. Do you have an intuition for this?

The factors mediating the relationship between the BOLD correlation topology and the global phase synchrony are quite complex. Recall that in the present model, the BOLD is simulated by driving the synaptic current through a Balloon-Windkessel model. Therefore, an increase in local neuronal activity (such as by an increase in the local oscillator frequency) will drive an increase in the local BOLD amplitude. Likewise, an increase in inter-system synchrony increases the order parameter. Therefore, to mediate the observed relationship between the BOLD topology and the global phase synchrony, there must exist a relationship between the global phase synchrony and the mean frequency of all oscillators: this is indeed the case (r = 0.21, p = 2.2x10-09) (Author response image 1)

Author response image 1.

Author response image 1.

Moreover, this relationship is mediated in a complex manner (through the neuronal state equation) by dependence of the global synchrony on both coupling (γ) and gain (σ) (Author response image 2).

Author response image 2.

Author response image 2.

In sum, both model parameters (gain and coupling) exert a complex influence on both the oscillator frequencies (and hence, the BOLD activity) and the global synchrony (and hence, the BOLD correlations). Moreover, in coupled oscillator systems such as this, the order parameter acts as a “mean field” that feeds back and influences local dynamics (see e.g. Breakspear et al., 2010).

The following text was added to the manuscript:

“A somewhat surprising result of our simulation is the link between phase- and amplitude-related measures of neuronal coupling. […] Moreover, in coupled oscillator systems such as this, the order parameter acts as a “mean field” that feeds back and influences local dynamics (see e.g. Breakspear et al., 2010).”

1c) The "Gain mediated integration is maximal in frontoparietal hub regions" findings should be complemented by analyzing the "diverse club" or the regions with high participation coefficients (Bertolero et al., 2017), which was found to be more highly interconnected than the rich club in the macaque.

We agree that the relationship between ascending neural gain and the diversity of inter-regional connectivity is likely to be important for global integration. As such, we have conducted an adjunct analysis (similar to the experiment conducted for the ‘rich club’ regions) for the so-called ‘diverse club’ – i.e., regions with elevated participation. Specifically, we calculated the mean BA across the parameter space and then identified the top 30% of nodes (to match the number of identified rich club regions) – 16/22 regions (~73%) were present in both the ‘diverse’ and ‘rich’ club. We then estimated the mean BA across the parameter space for regions within the ‘diverse club’ and contrasted their gain- and excitability-mediated alterations in BA. The results (presented in Figure 4—figure supplement 1) were comparable to those observed in the rich club analysis, suggesting a need for future experiments to delineate the precise topological role of degree and diversity in mediating the functional signature of brain activity over time.

The following text was added to the manuscript:

“Interestingly, similar dissociations were observed when comparing regions with high and low diversity (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), suggesting a role for future experiments to disambiguate the importance of degree and diversity in the mediation of global network topology (Bertolero et al., 2017). However, given the substantial overlap between regions in the ‘rich’ and diverse’ clubs (73% of regions were found in both groups), our results confirm a crucial role for frontoparietal regions in the control of network-level integration as a function of ascending neuromodulatory gain.”

2) Discussion, embedding of the present results into the existing literature.

2a) The Discussion should elaborate on the facts that (i) several of the results are motivated and expected from previous work, and (ii) the excitability parameter in your model is analogous to the global coupling, which has been used as control parameter in previous studies. The main new contribution here seems to be the addition of a second control parameter, the global gain.

Please note that, all reviewers appreciate the need of implementing the effects of neuromodulation effect explicitly, but they feel that the readership would benefit from a more balanced discussion of the context.

We thank the reviewers for their comments, and have added a discussion of this work to the manuscript.

The following text was added to the manuscript:

“The effect of neural gain on topology was greatest in a bilateral network of high-degree frontoparietal cortical regions (Figure 4). […] Crucially, the integrated states facilitated by gain-mediated hub recruitment have been shown to underlie effective cognitive performance (Shine et al., 2016a), episodic memory retrieval (Westphal et al., 2017) and conscious awareness (Barttfeld et al., 2015; Godwin et al., 2015), confirming the importance of ascending neuromodulatory systems for a suite of higher-level behavioral capacities.”

Rationale behind this request: Several previous studies have shown that networks of oscillators reach a balance between integration and segregation, a maximal complexity, and the largest variability of temporal networks at the critical point separating the asynchronous and synchronous phases (e.g., Schmidt et al., 2015; Zamora-Lopez et al. 2016; Deco et al. 2017). These studies also showed that rich clubs have a leading role concerning integration/segregation and complexity. The network can be displaced from one phase to the other by changing a control parameter, which is usually chosen to be the global coupling or connectivity strength – analogous to the excitability parameter used in your model. In addition, you manipulate a second control parameter, namely the slope of the transfer function that converts inputs into firing rates. Increasing the slope brings the system from asynchronous state to the synchronous state, and, as shown in Equation [3], has the same effect than a global coupling.

We thank the reviewers for their comments, and have added a discussion of this work to the manuscript.

The following text was added to the manuscript:

“This relationship has been demonstrated previously in other studies, either by manipulating the excitability parameter alone (Zamora-López et al., 2016) (Deco et al., 2017), or through the alteration of the intrinsic dynamics of the 2d oscillator model (Curto et al., 2009; Safaai et al., 2015), thus providing a strong conceptual link between structural topology and emergent dynamics. Crucially, the integrated states facilitated by gain-mediated hub recruitment have been shown to underlie effective cognitive performance (Shine et al., 2016a), episodic memory retrieval (Westphal et al., 2017) and conscious awareness (Barttfeld et al., 2015; Godwin et al., 2015), confirming the importance of ascending neuromodulatory systems for a suite of higher-level behavioral capacities.”

2b) Changes in both parameters (gain and excitability) are necessary for producing the effects of interest in your model. In particular, the graph-theoretical measures show a non-monotonic (inverted U) dependency on excitability, but not on gain. Still, the title and much of your Discussion focusses on gain only, drawing firm links to the Astone-Jones and Cohen Adaptive Gain Theory. Please discuss more explicitly the interesting effects of excitability. Specifically: How can you reconcile the predominance of inverted U patterns for excitability but not gain, with the Aston-Jones and Cohen framework.

We thank the reviewers for their comments, and have added a discussion of this work to the manuscript.

The following text was added to the manuscript:

Overall, our findings broadly support the predictions of the neural gain hypothesis of noradrenergic function (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005b). […] Combined with our observation of the importance of the interaction between neural gain and high-degree (Figure 4), diverse (Figure 4—figure supplement 1) hub regions, our results thus represent an extension of the neural gain hypothesis that integrates the ascending arousal system with the constraints imposed by multiple order parameters and structural network topology.”

2c) Previous work by the labs of Ken Harris and Stefano Panzeri has also used the FitzHugh-Nagumo model to characterise the effects of state/neuromodulation on cortical dynamics – but by modulating the dynamics of the 2D-oscillator itself (Curto et al., 2009; Safaai et al., PNAS, 2015). This work should be discussed and related to your current approach.

We thank the reviewers for their comments, and have added a discussion of this work to the manuscript.

The following text was added to the manuscript:

“This relationship has been demonstrated previously in other studies, either by manipulating the excitability parameter alone (Zamora-López et al., 2016) (Deco et al., 2017), or through the alteration of the intrinsic dynamics of the 2d oscillator model (Curto et al., 2009; Safaai et al., 2015), thus providing a strong conceptual link between structural topology and emergent dynamics. Crucially, the integrated states facilitated by gain-mediated hub recruitment have been shown to underlie effective cognitive performance (Shine et al., 2016a), episodic memory retrieval (Westphal et al., 2017) and conscious awareness (Barttfeld et al., 2015; Godwin et al., 2015), confirming the importance of ascending neuromodulatory systems for a suite of higher-level behavioral capacities.”

3) Clarifications

3a) In general, all reviewers appreciate that the paper was written for a general audience. However, please include all equations and note in the main text where they exist. For example, please include the equation for Communicability and expand the explanation – is it all walks, or all walks that are part of all shortest walks between all nodes? It is not clear in the Results or Materials and methods section.

We apologize for the lack of clarity. Communicability was calculated on all walks. This information (along with the equation defining this metric) has been added to the Materials and methods section.

The following text was added to the Materials and methods section:

“The communicability, C, between a pair of nodes i and j is defined as a weighted sum of the number of all walks connecting the pair of nodes (within weighted connectivity matrix, A) and has been shown to be equivalent to the matrix exponent of a binarized graph, eA (Estrada and Hatano, 2008). For ease of interpretation, we calculated the log10-transformed mean of communicability for each graph across iterations and values of neural gain.”

Cij=∑k=0∞Akijk!=eA[8]

3b) Please unpack the rationale behind using the correspondence between structural and functional connectivity as criterion for delineating the "plausible" sub-space of parameter combinations. This is important because work into neuromodulation in small circuits shows that neuromodulation can reconfigure circuits, thus overriding the structural connectome (Marder, Neuron, 2012).

In light of the referenced paper, we have opted to remove the constraint that the structural connectome should be similar to the functional connectome. Importantly, this does not change the interpretation of our results.

3c) It seems that Vi is used for the computation of phase synchrony (i.e., not the neural activity passed through the hemodynamics). This should be made explicit in the Materials and methods. (Terms like "raw signal" or "neural data" are ambiguous.)

We have amended each instance of these terms in the manuscript to improve clarity.

3d) Figure 2D is described as inverted U relationship, a description that glosses over the left flank of this plot. The relationship clearly is monotonic, but not an inverted U.

We have added a statement to this effect in the manuscript.

3e) Figure 2—figure supplement 3 is missing axis labels and won't be understandable for a broad audience. Please explain what it shows and what that means.

The figure legends for Figure 2—figure supplement 3 have been amended in the updated version.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31130.012

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES