Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2018 Jan-Mar;32(1):1–9. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000237

Population Neuroscience: Dementia Epidemiology serving Precision Medicine and Population Health

Mary Ganguli 1, Emiliano Albanese 2, Sudha Seshadri 3, David A Bennett 4, Constantine Lyketsos 5, Walter A Kukull 6, Ingmar Skoog 7, Hugh C Hendrie 8
PMCID: PMC5821530  NIHMSID: NIHMS923850  PMID: 29319603

Abstract

Over recent decades, epidemiology has made significant contributions to our understanding of dementia, translating scientific discoveries into population health. Here, we propose reframing dementia epidemiology as “population neuroscience,” blending techniques and models from contemporary neuroscience with those of epidemiology and biostatistics. Based on emerging evidence and newer paradigms and methods, population neuroscience will minimize the bias typical of traditional clinical research, identify the relatively homogeneous subgroups that comprise the general population, and investigate broader and denser phenotypes of dementia and cognitive impairment. Long-term followup of sufficiently large study cohorts will allow the identification of cohort effects and critical windows of exposure. Molecular epidemiology and omics will allow us to unravel the key distinctions within and among subgroups and better understand individuals’ risk profiles. Interventional epidemiology will allow us to identify the different subgroups that respond to different treatment/prevention strategies. These strategies will inform precision medicine. Additionally, insights into interactions between disease biology, personal and environmental factors, and social determinants of health will allow us to measure and track disease in communities and improve population health. By placing neuroscience within a real-world context, population neuroscience can fulfill its potential to serve both precision medicine and population health.

Keywords: Aging, risk factors, protective factors, trends, cohort effects, life course epidemiology, molecular epidemiology, translational epidemiology

How will epidemiology contribute to our understanding of dementia over the coming decades?

The classic “seven uses” of epidemiology, proposed in the 1950s,1 include completing the clinical picture of disease, community diagnosis, delineating new syndromes, computing individual morbid risk, charting historical trends, evaluating health services in action, and identifying causal/risk factors. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) offers one of the best examples of epidemiology “completing the clinical picture.” In 1906, Professor Alois Alzheimer reported a single case study of Auguste D, a 51-year old woman brought to his expert attention at the Frankfurt Asylum, suffering from the disease later named for him.2 Based on his work, for the next several decades the condition was assumed to be a rare disease of middle-aged people. Not until 1964, when Professor Martin Roth and colleagues reported a population study of individuals aged 65 years and older in Newcastle-upon-Tyne, England, was it understood that it was also a common disease of older people, increasing in frequency with age.3

In dementia research, the classic “uses” have been accomplished through approaches generally categorized as descriptive epidemiology (completing the clinical picture, community diagnosis, charting historical trends, delineating new syndromes such as late-onset AD), and analytic epidemiology (computing individual risk, identifying risk factors). Experimental or interventional epidemiology (such as embedded randomized controlled trials and population-level field trials) is only now beginning to emerge, as we will describe later. The term translational epidemiology has been used to broadly summarize the role played by epidemiology in translating scientific discoveries into population health impact and in the synthesis of knowledge.4 Building on the classic “uses” and study results to date, we offer a perspective on emerging directions in dementia epidemiology, leveraging new opportunities and resources than were unimaginable a decade ago, let alone half a century ago. We will show why contemporary dementia epidemiology is better characterized as population neuroscience, with implications for both population health and precision medicine. Although we will focus broadly on dementia, rather than on subtypes such as AD dementia, we will also discuss studies that investigated dementia subtypes using evolving research diagnostic criteria based on disease biology.

For the types of epidemiological investigations discussed here, sources of study cohorts have ranged from population-based, such as a census of a region, or a health insurance database where there is universal health care, to non-geographical community-based sources, such as members of religious orders or occupational groups such as nurses or military veterans. Their key characteristic is that they are not composed of patients seeking care for dementia, or research volunteers responding to advertisements. The cohort must be recruited in as unbiased a manner as possible from the source, to optimize its external validity, and should have minimal attrition once it is recruited, to optimize its internal validity.5 In these cohorts, the vast majority of individuals are free of dementia at the time of recruitment; they are thoroughly characterized at study entry and then followed prospectively to determine who eventually develops dementia and other outcomes of interest.

For readers unfamiliar with epidemiological terminology, prevalence is the proportion of a defined population that has the condition of interest at a given point in time; incidence is the rate at which new cases of the condition develop within a defined population; risk (and protective) factors are variables associated with subsequently higher (and lower) incidence rates, respectively; exposures are variables which might turn out to be risk or protective factors.

Recent epidemiological reports have dramatically changed longstanding concepts in the science of dementia (Table 1). For example, the age-specific incidence of dementia may be declining in high-income countries; this decline is not entirely explained by declining cardiovascular risk factors or increasing education.6, 17, 18 Also, most dementia at the population level is not due to any single pathological process, but, rather, represents the sum of two or more pathological processes arising in brains with varying degrees of resilience against disease.9 In contrast, many individuals with only one pathology remain free of dementia. In a study comparing the brains of individuals with clinically probable AD dementia from research clinics vs. community samples, the community cases were older, had less severe AD pathology, and were more likely to have infarcts and mixed pathologies, than the clinic cases.19 Clinical research studies are understandably skewed towards younger individuals with earlier onset of disease, who are more likely than older persons to seek services and participate in clinical research. Regrettably, as a result, clinical research is deprived of the opportunity to consider the many characteristics of AD that change with aging, including clinical presentation and course, comorbidity, underlying pathology, and mortality risk. Further, incidence continues to rise exponentially into the tenth decade of life, but many of the known risk factors are associated only with dementia onset before age 85 or so, and not with dementia of later onset.20 Community-based brain donor programs have been able to associate long-term lifestyle and risk factor data (such as diet) both with specific neuropathology and with apparent resistance to pathology.21, 22 With aging, neuropathology becomes almost ubiquitous and dementia risk increases dramatically, and yet most older people do not develop dementia.23 Since healthy seniors do not seek psychiatric and neurologic services, factors associated with healthy cognitive aging, like dietary patterns or education, can only be investigated at the population, and possibly primary care, levels.24, 25 Such insights have clear implications for prevention and treatment strategies in dementia, and are transforming basic research and clinical trial design in AD.

Table 1.

Some Examples of Recent Contributions from Dementia Epidemiology

Temporal trends. Discovering a trend towards decreasing age-specific incidence of dementia in high-income countries; factors underlying these trends could shed light on disease mechanisms and inform preventive strategies.6, 7, 8
Full spectrum of pathology. Community-based brain autopsy studies showing that neuropathologies are common in people without dementia, and that most dementia, particularly in the eighth and later decades of life, is due to multiple etiologies; this knowledge broadens the potential range of preventive and therapeutic targets.9
Life-course impact of risk factors. Demonstrating critical periods during which different risk factors exert their influences, identifying the windows during which preventive strategies are likely to be effective.10, 11, 12
Modifiable environmental risk factors, such as air pollution, shown to be associated with dementia.13, 14
Modifiable protective factors, such as cognitive and physical activity, shown to predict healthy cognitive aging.15, 16

Over the past decade, epidemiological or population work in dementia has become increasingly allied with clinical and basic neuroscience, including biofluid and imaging biomarkers and genomics. More recently, molecular genomics has been incorporated into community-based studies of aging and dementia. This interdisciplinary approach is consistent with the relatively recent concept of “Population Neuroscience” which, in essence, is the study of the full range of brain disease, risk factors, and underlying biological pathways as they present in the population at large.26, 27 Neuroscience has made great strides in social, cognitive, clinical, affective, economic, developmental, and communication areas; however, most of its findings are based on studies of convenience samples which differ systematically from the populations from which they were drawn. These systematic differences can bias the observed associations and thus distort our inferences about brain-behavior relationships.28 Meanwhile, traditional population studies, including epidemiologic and social science research, have made major contributions towards understanding the associations of behavior with a range of external factors; however, these studies have treated the brain like the proverbial “black box.” By marrying the two approaches, population neuroscience leverages interdisciplinary expertise and emphasizes interactional models which explore moderators of brain-behavior links and predictors of relevant outcomes.26 We can use not only the tools of traditional epidemiology and biostatistics, but also detailed structural, functional and chemical brain imaging, electronic monitoring (i.e., wearable technologies), cognitive neuroscience, and various molecular genomic techniques using both ante-mortem and post-mortem biofluids and biospecimens including brain samples. It is becoming possible to integrate multi-level brain omics (genome, epigenome, transcriptome, proteome, lipideome, metabolome29) using systems biology and data mining techniques, at the population level.30

We therefore propose that the broad concept of “population neuroscience” supplant the traditional models of “neuroepidemiology” and “psychiatric epidemiology,” in dementia research. Although a modest paradigm shift, it highlights the translational aspects that are informing the molecular epidemiology of AD and other dementias at the population level, and more clearly establishes the epidemiology of brain dysfunction as a branch of neuroscience.

One example could be integrating brain imaging, genetic, and biomarker studies to identify the biologically relevant subgroups in the population which best respond to a preventive or therapeutic intervention. Another might be devising methods to scale up imaging and biomarker studies to larger and more representative samples of the population. A third example could be using multi-level brain omics to identify novel therapeutic targets, including those related to resilience. Thus, population neuroscience can serve as a bi-directional bridge between clinical and basic research, develop frameworks and models to generate and test hypotheses across disciplines, and provide tools for translational medicine.31 Table 2 shows some emerging directions that we believe represent the future of population neuroscience over the coming decades; in essence, the new avatars of the classic “seven uses of epidemiology.” The proposed directions are complementary and have some overlap since they represent different approaches towards the same broad scientific goals.

Table 2.

Future directions for population neuroscience of dementia

1. Investigate expanded phenotypes of dementia and mild cognitive impairment.
2. Investigate links between disease patterns and the modifiable macro-environment.
3. Further develop molecular epidemiology incorporating omics.
4. Investigate epidemiology of disease over the life course.
5. Examine trends in disease and risk factors across birth cohorts.
6. Employ novel approaches to study aging and disease in new generations.
7. Expand the study of inadequately studied populations where novel risk and protective factors may exist.
8. Expand the scope of interventional epidemiology.
9. Appropriately use Big Data approaches.
10. Use judicious pooled and coordinated analyses

1. Expanding the phenotypes of dementia and mild cognitive impairment

1a. Characterize and investigate deeper and denser phenotypes

We must take greater advantage of growing opportunities to combine standard cognitive and other clinical measures with biofluid and neuroimaging biomarkers, in longitudinal population-based studies. The increasing use of relatively affordable structural and resting state functional MRI in population studies is an example of moderate progress along this path. Practically, large numbers of frail elderly individuals in the general aging population cannot be imaged at academic medical centers; more neuroimaging research must be conducted at community hospitals and freestanding imaging centers, introducing new challenges which we must meet. Statistical methods have been developed, and more are under development, to correct for the biases that are inevitably introduced by multiple scanners, and by the non-randomness of the sample that agrees and is eligible to be neuroimaged.32 Some groups in Europe and the US are incorporating CSF assays33, 34 and amyloid and tau PET imaging35 into subsets of population-based cohort studies. As in vivo methods for characterizing underlying pathologies become more reliable, affordable, and commonplace, we will be better able to observe the incidence and progression of biomarkers at the population level. We will also be able to measure their interactions with (or independence from) risk factors in contributing to cognitive dysfunction. Population studies employing such markers will further “complete the clinical picture of disease” in the community. By helping to identify the boundaries of neighboring syndromes and disease entities, they will improve the utility and prognostic value of our diagnostic groupings at the population level. These advances will help us hone in on the mechanisms by which risk factors lead to disease. Population studies are also needed to determine the external validity (“generalizability”) of the substantial proportions of cognitively intact individuals with biomarker-positive preclinical AD or cerebrovascular disease, in both clinical and community studies35, 36, 37, 34

Through deep phenomics, population studies are broadening our understanding of links among organ systems, and establishing pathophysiological links among various previously siloed traits and conditions. Examples include insulin resistance in diabetes and Alzheimer disease,38 and inflammation in visceral fat and brain glia.39 Changes in glucose,40 blood pressure,41 and body mass index42 have been reported to occur in the period preceding the emergence of dementia. There are also links with sleep and circadian rhythms,43 pulmonary function,44 chronic kidney disease,45 and anemia.46, 47

Clearly, these relationships cannot be investigated in samples composed of the dementia patients typically recruited in clinical research settings. Further, since the ideal approach to chronic disease is prevention, it is only in people free of disease, at the community level, that we can apply preventive strategies. Studying deeper phenotypes at the community level will allow us to investigate emerging conditions that precede or coincide with preclinical (presymptomatic) disease, possibly in midlife; better understand the roles of aging and genetics in the above; and examine a wider range of relevant exposures than has been possible to date, for example, the role of stress.48 Identifying the subgroups in which these factors influence the trajectory of cognitive decline, and the circumstances under which they do, will help target molecular analyses and bring us closer to precision medicine.49 The intensive study of population-based samples can also help contextualize detailed phenotyping of volunteer and clinical samples. The aforementioned diversity of dementia in the population at large can be harnessed by identifying (for example) AD subtypes with different characteristics and outcomes, with implications for population health.

1b. Broaden the cognitive phenotype of dementia and mild cognitive impairment to include non-amnestic syndromes

Expanding the conventional dementia phenotype to include cognitive domains other than memory will allow population studies to capture atypical AD cases as well as non-AD dementias.50 Non-amnestic mild cognitive impairment (MCI) also progresses to dementia, although at a slower rate than amnestic MCI.51, 52 Susceptibility to delirium (e.g., post-operatively) in seemingly normal elderly is a potential preclinical marker of impending dementia.53 Social cognition,54 thus far primarily studied in clinical samples of younger people with conditions such as schizophrenia, autism, Huntington’s disease, and frontotemporal dementia, is now recognized as a cognitive domain that can also be impaired in other forms of late-life dementia.50, 55

1c. Broaden the dementia phenotype to include neuropsychiatric and motor manifestations

Behavioral and psychological symptoms are integral parts of the phenotype, and not mere epiphenomena, of dementia and mild cognitive impairment. They often precede, and are more distressing and burdensome than, the cognitive symptoms; they are frequently the first reason for which services are sought. The unfortunate Auguste was not taken to the psychiatric hospital to see Dr. Alzheimer because she was forgetful. Families report that personality change precedes the development of cognitive decline,56 although they are likely referring to behavioral changes such as apathy, rather than personality traits such as neuroticism (which appear stable).57 Population studies have also identified apathy as a common feature of mild cognitive impairment and a budding prodrome of dementia.58 Work has begun to characterize a primarily behavioral phenotype of the dementia prodrome (currently labeled “Mild Behavioral Impairment”) that may precede or coexist with the more familiar cognitive phenotype of MCI.59, 60 Similar to the application of amyloid PET imaging to study memory loss, studies could perhaps be undertaken with dopamine PET to investigate apathy. Such investigations would be best initiated in clinical epidemiology studies, to clearly describe the phenotype in well-characterized patients before moving to detect preclinical and subclinical phenotypes in the population at large.

In addition, changes in mobility61 and motor function, such as extrapyramidal signs,62 are harbingers of subsequent cognitive decline and dementia including AD. These changes are related to the pathology of AD as well as Lewy Body disease and cerebrovascular disease.63

2. Explore links between disease patterns and the modifiable macro-environment

We still know relatively little about environmental influences on AD pathology, in contrast to what we know about Parkinson’s disease (e.g., the pesticide paraquat).64 Longitudinal epidemiological studies are the only feasible approach to investigate the impact of environmental factors such as lead or mercury exposure,65, 21 air pollution,66, 67 and effects of the built, social, and economic environments on heart and brain disease.68, 69 A recently coined term, the ‘exposome,’ refers collectively not only to these various externally experienced factors, but also to the exposures that individuals may generate or experience within themselves (such as variability in blood pressure, physical activity, diet, gut microbiome). Traditionally, epidemiology has partnered with public health to identify a few high impact risk factors that could be modified to drastically reduce individual and population risk; for instance, the impact of smoking on lung cancer, or possibly of early education on resilience to clinical dementia in the face of neurodegenerative pathology. However, for the complex condition of aging-related dementia, it appears more likely that a large number of interacting factors exert joint impacts. Different factors may contribute in different clusters of individuals; their investigation will require the development of new analytic and computational tools.70 This is a potentially high-impact target for public health and public policy interventions.

3. Molecular epidemiology and Omics

It has been suggested that an optimistic outlook for epidemiology is being offered by the “convergence of striking developments in biotechnology, the increasing availability of biobanked samples, and advances in biostatistics and bioinformatics.”71 We need to move these rapidly evolving investigations out of case-control studies within specialty clinics and into the larger community where AD cases can potentially be identified at all disease stages including preclinical disease, and with all the common comorbidities with which therapeutics must contend. Omics provides an opportunity for precision medicine,72 in that it can identify subgroups with different pathways defined, for example, by different polygenic scores. Expanding the procurement of brain autopsies in community/population studies will help this cause. Beyond the genome, multi-level brain omics must be derived from the biospecimens of interest. For cognition, this is the brain; however, in Lewy Body disease there is interest in examining the autonomic nervous system where peripheral specimens such as cutaneous nerves and parasympathetic ganglia may be usefully examined.73

4. Life Course Epidemiology

The population neuroscience of dementia has to begin in early life. Signals from highly selected clinical samples with autosomal dominant AD suggest that the neuropathology of AD begins to develop decades before the onset of symptoms or detectable deficits.7476 Links between developmental and aging processes have been identified;7779 in-utero conditions may influence the risk of age-related diseases through epigenetic changes.80, 81 Replicating these findings in very long-term population studies, starting in early adulthood, will allow us to identify antecedents and true risk factors for the more common, non-dominant, late-onset AD; these data will have direct implications for prevention trials. Longitudinal population studies reveal that cognitive change is not linear, and that different aspects of these non-linear trajectories are associated with different risk factors82, 83 and brain pathology.84 These studies not only shed light on disease biology but also reveal the critical windows over the life course during which the different exposures exert their effects on risk on late-life dementia85 and on the underlying neuropathologies.86, 87 This approach can identify risk factors that play out over many years, exerting small but cumulative effects that are far below the resolution of shorter-duration randomized clinical trials. Following cognitively intact individuals in the population longitudinally, as they age, allows us to detect the earliest changes and manifestations that occur well before patients appear in the clinical setting seeking services. To accomplish this goal cost-effectively, we can extend the study of “aging” backwards into earlier periods of life; leverage existing birth cohorts and cohorts of young adults, and follow them longitudinally throughout their lives, periodically measuring putative biomarkers where feasible. Well-established life-course studies, starting with the 1946 British Birth Cohort study88 and including several others in Northern Europe48, 89, 90 and North America,9196 and existing longitudinal studies that have followed young children through early adulthood,97 all provide the opportunity to continue following these well-characterized cohorts with a change in research focus (and funding agency). These studies could delineate the long-term trajectories of cognitive and behavioral change, and also the evolution of biomarkers over time, thus helping elucidate the dynamics of the long induction and latency phases of degenerative dementias.

5. Cohort Effects

There is growing evidence that rates of dementia incidence,6, 7, 17, 98 stroke incidence,99 and cognitive decline100 may have been dropping over recent decades in the high income countries.101 The same may not be uniformly true across various ethnicities within those countries.102 Intriguingly, a rare autopsy study suggested that amyloid deposition appeared to have declined in aging brains over recent decades.103 However, given rising life expectancy and duration of survival with dementia, prevalence is unlikely to decline over the next few decades, although one national US study has reported a decline in prevalence.104 Potential explanations for declining disease incidence include better early nutrition and control of infectious disease, better education, less exposure to environmental toxins such as lead, and lower rates of smoking, in more recently born cohorts. Plausible and testable hypotheses would relate to the downstream effects on brain function and dementia incidence of advances in cardiovascular disease management; specifically, diastolic blood pressure in the 1960s, systolic blood pressure and statins in the 1990s, and tissue plasminogen activator for acute stroke in the 2000s.18 None of these developments have as yet been shown to explain declining dementia incidence.6 Meanwhile, rising rates of obesity12 and diabetes105 may counteract the favorable trends by increasing dementia risk. Investigating such trends requires very long-term studies examining cohort effects, i.e. variations based on the year or decade of birth. Different exposures or levels of exposure in different birth cohorts may well lead to different rates of outcomes in those cohorts. Age-specific incidence may still be rising in low- and middle-income countries such as Brazil, India and China; incidence dropped among African-Americans but not among Africans (Nigerian Yoruba) between 1992 and 2001.106 Generational or birth cohort effects will vary across regions and in accordance with locally applicable historical events and exposures, such as wars and famines. Caution should of course be exercised to ensure that apparent trends are not artifacts of shifts in study design, diagnostic criteria, population characteristics, or response rates over time.107

6. New generations

In addition to birth cohorts which have already reached the age of risk, coming generations will experience a range of different relevant exposures than preceding ones (e.g., nutrition, immunizations, transportation, electronic media and digital devices). Potential new risk factors could include the more pervasive sleep disturbances108 and increased stress experiences that result from stagnant wages, multiple jobs, intrusion of work into all hours of the day,109 increased jet travel,110 increased sedentary time,111 and other lifestyle changes.112 In mouse models, a “modern-life-like stress paradigm” has been found to exacerbate Aβ pathology and increase synapse loss.113, 114

New generations (e.g., “millennials”) may be easier to recruit using social media, capitalizing on their increased propensity to share information. Social media are being found to be of value in infectious disease epidemiology, for disease surveillance and outbreak management,115 and for pharmacovigilance of adverse drug reactions.116 Twitter has been used to identify stigmatizing statements about AD;117 potentially, ways can be found to use social media for the recruitment of community-based samples for longitudinal studies. Secure web-based data collection, m-health applications, and increasing comfort with allowing data to be collected passively, should make participation easier and less time-consuming.

7. Inadequately studied populations

Populations of low- and middle-income countries, and also ethnic/racial/social/geographic minorities in the high-income countries, may have different rates of both exposures and outcomes, and different secular trends. Epigenetics may also vary across populations.118 Given increasing travel and migration, local factors from environmental pollution (e.g., lead-tainted paint and water)65 to infectious diseases (e.g., zika)119 can have global impacts. Previous cross-national studies of dementia106, 120, 121 have made substantial contributions to our understanding of varying prevalence and incidence across the globe, and of similarities and differences in traditional risk factors. However, newer studies are needed that are of suitable scale, reflect current thinking, and harness current technologies, to collect data and appropriate biological specimens and explore unique exposures and modifiers/moderators in the local context.

8. Interventional Epidemiology

Interventional epidemiology can involve the application of epidemiologic principles to design and recruit for trials, to analyze the data, and to interpret the results.122 Additionally, trials can be totally embedded in representative population-based cohorts,123 or in distinct targeted subgroups of the cohort. Within heterogeneous general populations it is usually possible to identify latent homogeneous subgroups, among which associations and even mechanisms can potentially vary. Epidemiology can enhance trials by identifying different homogeneous subgroups in which different approaches may be beneficial. This, in fact, offers an approach to precision medicine, allowing us to understand how different individuals and groups arrive at dementia along different albeit not exclusive paths.

However, it is sometimes necessary to challenge the conventional hierarchy of evidence. Some exposures like smoking and head trauma cannot be randomized in clinical trials, and sufficiently long randomized clinical trials are not practical for midlife risk and protective factors. In these cases, causal inferences must be drawn from observational data. Steps can be taken to enhance the reliability and validity of non-randomized study designs, e.g., Mendelian randomization, in which genetic polymorphisms associated with modifiable exposures are used to strengthen the robustness of causal inference in observational studies.124 Their use may increase, thanks to the expanding availability of genetic data in large, representative samples. Observational epidemiologic studies often not only mirror findings of randomized clinical trials but also provide long-term data that cannot be obtained from trials.125127 “Evidence must be translated, whether or not complete.”128

9. Big Data

This term has been used in various ways to refer to databases that are large in volume (for example data on hundreds of thousands of metabolites, gut microbes, or genetic variants), velocity of accrual (such as minute to minute data on a person’s movements collected by an accelerometer) or variety (different types of data on diet or pollutants). Here, we use the term to refer to gigantic databases which were assembled for purposes other than the proposed research. Examples would include electronic health records (EHRs) of large health care systems, the Medicare claims databases in the US, and national health or pharmacy databases in other countries. The increasingly pervasive routine collection of health data will provide unprecedented opportunities to create registries that can be linked to data collected from individuals. On a larger scale, these could be large interlinkable data sets within a single country or networks of multinational databases.129 Done right, judicious mining of these data can narrow the gap between individual health and population health; cost of research data collection and processing will decrease; epidemiology will acquire biological and imaging data that could be difficult to obtain on the same scale in independent population-based research.72, 130 However, these opportunities should be approached with caution, and, the appropriate Big Data set identified for a given research question. Sound epidemiological principles and validated methods must guide data mining. The major drawback is uncertain confidence in the clinical phenotype that can be generated by EHR or claims data, which are not collected for research purposes, and could be driven by local vagaries of coding and billing. Thus, this approach may be the best fit for data that are recorded unequivocally, such medication dosages, serious adverse effects, and clinical procedure codes. Pharmaco-epidemiological interrogations of these data could allow post-marketing surveillance to answer questions for which RCTs cannot be employed. Data mining approaches may also be useful for agnostic digital data such as MRI images, actigraphic data, and retinal scans. Here too, it is critical to distinguish disease effects from independent risk factors for disease, and to recognize that patients who are selected to undergo non-routine testing are systematically different from those who are not (“confounding by indication”). A partial solution is to combine direct data collection in a very large sample of the target population with surveillance for events based on EHR. This hybrid approach is being used in the new generation of mega-cohort studies;131134 it works best when embedded in health care systems that maintain detailed records and provide most of the clinical care received by the target populations. Data from EHRs also potentially provide the opportunity for efficient “virtual trials” of interventions analyzed using epidemiologic approaches, again setting the stage for precision medicine and, eventually, population health.

10. Pooled and Coordinated Analyses

We use this term to refer to analyses of appropriately pooled data from multiple epidemiological studies conducted for similar purposes. This approach can increase reach and power to detect modest-sized effects which escape detection in smaller individual study populations. If eventual pooling is a long-term objective, participating studies should use shared core methods without stifling local strengths and innovation. Uniformly collected data and specimens can easily be shared. For post hoc data pooling where data were not uniformly collected,135 sufficient time and effort should be budgeted for appropriate harmonization. Multi-level analyses should account for the clustered nature of the pooled data, and account for within-group as well as between-group effects with appropriate weighting, as is typically done in meta-analysis. For balance, it should be asked whether there is a reasonable upper limit to sample size. Is bigger always better? What is the smallest effect that is useful to detect? Are data quality and integrity more important than number of studies included or total sample size? How will we recognize spurious findings? Cross-validation approaches and appropriate simulations should be planned. Other cautions abound. The harmonizing of measurements has its rational limits; cohorts studied during different eras at different sites will have unique characteristics which cannot be harmonized or statistically adjusted; in many long-term studies, participation (response/refusal) rates are declining, and mortality/attrition rates will vary across studies, introducing different levels of potential selection bias and survival bias. These issues can lead to under- or over-estimates of incidents, and of risk estimates, when pooling data across cohorts.

In summary, we have proposed some 21st century extensions of Morris’ classic seven uses of epidemiology. Descriptive and analytic epidemiology will allow us to identify and study longitudinally the relatively homogeneous subgroups within populations and identify potential therapeutic and prevention targets. Molecular epidemiology and omics will allow us to “unravel the key distinctions within and among subgroups” and “point the way toward better-targeted, safer, and more effective treatments, based on a deeper understanding of individual patients’ risk profiles.”130, 136 Interventional epidemiology will allow us to identify the different subgroups that respond to different treatment/prevention strategies. These strategies will inform precision medicine, which can be applied at the community level – and only at the community level - to prevent disease. Insights into interactions between disease biology, personal and environmental factors, and social determinants of health, will allow us to measure and track disease in communities and improve population health, a translational approach that has been termed precision public health.137, 138 Thus, by placing neuroscience within a real-world context, population neuroscience can fulfill its potential to serve both precision medicine and population health.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dallas Anderson, PhD, NIA Division of Neuroscience, for encouraging the writing of this editorial.

The authors thank Erin Jacobsen MS for assistance with manuscript preparation.

This work was supported in part by the following grants K07AG044395; U01AG016976; P30AG10133; R01AG045350; P50AG005146; R01AG054076; P30AG10161, and R01AG17917 from the National Institute on Aging, US DHHS; and grant 2015-02830 from Vetenskapsrådet, Sweden.

References

  • 1.Morris J. Uses of epidemiology. British Medical Journal. 1955;2(4936):395. doi: 10.1136/bmj.2.4936.395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Alzheimer A. Über eigenartige Krankheitsfälle des späteren Alters. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie. 1911;4(1):356. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kay D, Beamish P, Roth M. Old age mental disorders in Newcastle upon Tyne. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 1964;110(468):668–682. doi: 10.1192/bjp.110.468.668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Khoury MJ, Gwinn M, Ioannidis JP. The emergence of translational epidemiology: from scientific discovery to population health impact. Am J Epidemiol. 2010;172(5):517–24. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwq211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kukull WA, Ganguli M. Generalizability: the trees, the forest, and the low-hanging fruit. Neurology. 2012;78(23):1886–91. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318258f812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Satizabal CL, Beiser AS, Chouraki V, et al. Incidence of dementia over three decades in the Framingham Heart Study. New England Journal of Medicine. 2016;374(6):523–532. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1504327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Prince M, Ali GC, Guerchet M, et al. Recent global trends in the prevalence and incidence of dementia, and survival with dementia. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy. 2016;8(1):23. doi: 10.1186/s13195-016-0188-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wu YT, Beiser AS, Breteler MMB, et al. The changing prevalence and incidence of dementia over time - current evidence. Nat Rev Neurol. 2017;13(6):327–339. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2017.63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, Bang W, et al. Mixed brain pathologies account for most dementia cases in community-dwelling older persons. Neurology. 2007;69(24):2197–2204. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000271090.28148.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gottesman RF, Schneider AL, Zhou Y, et al. Association between midlife vascular risk factors and estimated brain amyloid deposition. JAMA. 2017;317(14):1443–1450. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.3090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gottesman RF, Albert MS, Alonso A, et al. Associations Between Midlife Vascular Risk Factors and 25-Year Incident Dementia in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Cohort. JAMA Neurol. 2017;74(10):1246–1254. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.1658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Albanese E, Launer LJ, Egger M, et al. Body mass index in midlife and dementia: Systematic review and meta-regression analysis of 589,649 men and women followed in longitudinal studies. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring. 2017;8:165–178. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2017.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Schikowski T, Vossoughi M, Vierkötter A, et al. Association of air pollution with cognitive functions and its modification by APOE gene variants in elderly women. Environmental Research. 2015;142:10–16. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2015.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wilker EH, Preis SR, Beiser AS, et al. Long-term exposure to fine particulate matter, residential proximity to major roads and measures of brain structure. Stroke. 2015;46(5):1161–1166. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.008348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2011;108(7):3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Weuve J, Barnes LL, de Leon CFM, et al. Cognitive aging in black and white Americans: Cognition, cognitive decline, and incidence of Alzheimer disease dementia. Epidemiology. 2017 doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Matthews F, Stephan B, Robinson L, et al. A two decade dementia incidence comparison from the Cognitive Function and Ageing Studies I and II. Nature Communications. 2016;7 doi: 10.1038/ncomms11398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Skoog I. Dementia: Dementia incidence–the times, they are a-changing. Nature Reviews Neurology. 2016;12(6):316. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2016.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Schneider JA, Aggarwal NT, Barnes L, et al. The neuropathology of older persons with and without dementia from community versus clinic cohorts. J Alzheimers Dis. 2009;18(3):691–701. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2009-1227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ganguli M, Lee CW, Snitz BE, et al. Rates and risk factors for progression to incident dementia vary by age in a population cohort. Neurology. 2015;84(1):72–80. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Morris MC, Brockman J, Schneider JA, et al. Association of seafood consumption, brain mercury level, and APOE ε4 status with brain neuropathology in older adults. JAMA. 2016;315(5):489–497. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.19451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bennett DA, Arnold SE, Valenzuela MJ, et al. Cognitive and social lifestyle: links with neuropathology and cognition in late life. Acta Neuropathol. 2014;127(1):137–50. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1226-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Jack CR, Jr, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, et al. Age-specific population frequencies of cerebral beta-amyloidosis and neurodegeneration among people with normal cognitive function aged 50–89 years: a cross-sectional study. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(10):997–1005. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70194-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Tangney CC, Kwasny MJ, Li H, et al. Adherence to a Mediterranean-type dietary pattern and cognitive decline in a community population. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;93(3):601–7. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.007369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Van Dijk KR, Van Gerven PW, Van Boxtel MP, et al. No protective effects of education during normal cognitive aging: results from the 6-year follow-up of the Maastricht Aging Study. Psychol Aging. 2008;23(1):119–30. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Falk EB, Hyde LW, Mitchell C, et al. What is a representative brain? Neuroscience meets population science. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(44):17615–22. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1310134110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Paus T. Population neuroscience: why and how. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(6):891–903. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.LeWinn KZ, Sheridan MA, Keyes KM, et al. Sample composition alters associations between age and brain structure. Nat Commun. 2017;8(1):874. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-00908-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Shoemark DK, Allen SJ. The microbiome and disease: reviewing the links between the oral microbiome, aging, and Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;43(3):725–38. doi: 10.3233/JAD-141170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hodes RJ, Buckholtz N. Accelerating Medicines Partnership: Alzheimer’s Disease (AMP-AD) Knowledge Portal Aids Alzheimer’s Drug Discovery through Open Data Sharing. Expert Opin Ther Targets. 2016;20(4):389–91. doi: 10.1517/14728222.2016.1135132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Callahan CM, Foroud T, Saykin AJ, et al. Translational research on aging: clinical epidemiology as a bridge between the sciences. Transl Res. 2014;163(5):439–45. doi: 10.1016/j.trsl.2013.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ganguli M, Lee CW, Hughes T, et al. Who wants a free brain scan? Assessing and correcting for recruitment biases in a population-based sMRI pilot study. Brain Imaging Behav. 2015;9(2):204–12. doi: 10.1007/s11682-014-9297-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Fagan AM. CSF biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease: impact on disease concept, diagnosis, and clinical trial design. Advances in Geriatrics. 2014;2014 http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2014/302712. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Höglund K, Kern S, Zettergren A, et al. Preclinical amyloid pathology biomarker positivity: effects on tau pathology and neurodegeneration. Translational Psychiatry. 2017;7(1):e995. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Jack CR, Jr, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, et al. Age-specific and sex-specific prevalence of cerebral beta-amyloidosis, tauopathy, and neurodegeneration in cognitively unimpaired individuals aged 50–95 years: a cross-sectional study. Lancet Neurol. 2017;16(6):435–444. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30077-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Aizenstein HJ, Nebes RD, Saxton JA, et al. Frequent amyloid deposition without significant cognitive impairment among the elderly. Archives of Neurology. 2008;65(11):1509–1517. doi: 10.1001/archneur.65.11.1509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Vos SJ, Xiong C, Visser PJ, et al. Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and its outcome: a longitudinal cohort study. The Lancet Neurology. 2013;12(10):957–965. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70194-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Luchsinger JA. Adiposity, hyperinsulinemia, diabetes and Alzheimer’s disease: an epidemiological perspective. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2008;585(1):119–129. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2008.02.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Farooqui AA, Horrocks LA, Farooqui T. Modulation of inflammation in brain: a matter of fat. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2007;101(3):577–599. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.04371.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hendrie HC, Zheng M, Li W, et al. Glucose level decline precedes dementia in elderly African Americans with diabetes. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2017;13(2):111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Stewart R, Xue QL, Masaki K, et al. Change in blood pressure and incident dementia. Hypertension. 2009;54(2):233–240. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.109.128744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Singh-Manoux A, Dugravot A, Shipley M, et al. Obesity trajectories and risk of dementia: 28 years of follow-up in the Whitehall II Study. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.06.2637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Spira AP, Chen-Edinboro LP, Wu MN, et al. Impact of sleep on the risk of cognitive decline and dementia. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2014;27(6):478–83. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Buchman AS, Yu L, Wilson RS, et al. Post-mortem brain pathology is related to declining respiratory function in community-dwelling older adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7197 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Buchman AS, Tanne D, Boyle PA, et al. Kidney function is associated with the rate of cognitive decline in the elderly. Neurology. 2009;73(12):920–7. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b72629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Trevisan C, Veronese N, Bolzetta F, et al. Low Hemoglobin Levels and the Onset of Cognitive Impairment in Older People: The PRO.V.A. Study. Rejuvenation Res. 2016;19(6):447–455. doi: 10.1089/rej.2015.1768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Pandav RS, Chandra V, Dodge HH, et al. Hemoglobin levels and Alzheimer disease: an epidemiologic study in India. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2004;12(5):523–6. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajgp.12.5.523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Johansson L, Guo X, Waern M, et al. Midlife psychological stress and risk of dementia: a 35-year longitudinal population study. Brain. 2010;133(8):2217–2224. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Cholerton B, Larson EB, Quinn JF, et al. Precision medicine: clarity for the complexity of dementia. The American journal of pathology. 2016;186(3):500–506. doi: 10.1016/j.ajpath.2015.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®) American Psychiatric Pub.; Washington, DC: 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Ganguli M, Snitz BE, Saxton JA, et al. Outcomes of mild cognitive impairment by definition: a population study. Arch Neurol. 2011;68(6):761–7. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Au B, Dale-McGrath S, Tierney MC. Sex differences in the prevalence and incidence of mild cognitive impairment: A meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. 2017;35:176–199. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Fong TG, Davis D, Growdon ME, et al. The Interface of Delirium and Dementia in Older Persons. The Lancet Neurology. 2015;14(8):823–832. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00101-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Henry JD, Von Hippel W, Molenberghs P, et al. Clinical assessment of social cognitive function in neurological disorders. Nature Reviews Neurology. 2016;12(1):28. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2015.229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Sachdev PS, Blacker D, Blazer DG, et al. Classifying neurocognitive disorders: the DSM-5 approach. Nature Reviews Neurology. 2014;10(11):634–642. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2014.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Smith-Gamble V, Baiyewu O, Perkins AJ, et al. Informant reports of changes in personality predict dementia in a population-based study of elderly african americans and yoruba. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2002;10(6):724–32. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Terracciano A, An Y, Sutin AR, et al. Personality Change in the Preclinical Phase of Alzheimer Disease. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017 doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Onyike CU, Sheppard JM, Tschanz JT, et al. Epidemiology of apathy in older adults: the Cache County Study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007;15(5):365–75. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000235689.42910.0d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Taragano FE, Allegri RF, Krupitzki H, et al. Mild behavioral impairment and risk of dementia. The Journal of clinical psychiatry. 2009;70(4):584. doi: 10.4088/jcp.08m04181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Ismail Z, Smith EE, Geda Y, et al. Neuropsychiatric symptoms as early manifestations of emergent dementia: Provisional diagnostic criteria for mild behavioral impairment. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12(2):195–202. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sorond FA, Cruz-Almeida Y, Clark DJ, et al. Aging, the central nervous system, and mobility in older adults: neural mechanisms of mobility impairment. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2015;70(12):1526–1532. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glv130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Louis ED, Tang MX, Mayeux R. Parkinsonian signs in older people in a community-based study: risk of incident dementia. Archives of Neurology. 2004;61(8):1273–1276. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.8.1273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Buchman AS, Leurgans SE, Nag S, et al. Cerebrovascular disease pathology and parkinsonian signs in old age. Stroke. 2011;42(11):3183–3189. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.623462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Goldman SM. Environmental toxins and Parkinson’s disease. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 2014;54:141–64. doi: 10.1146/annurev-pharmtox-011613-135937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Power MC, Korrick S, Tchetgen Tchetgen EJ, et al. Lead exposure and rate of change in cognitive function in older women. Environmental Research. 2014;129:69–75. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2013.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Wilker EH, Preis SR, Beiser AS, et al. Long-term exposure to fine particulate matter, residential proximity to major roads and measures of brain structure. Stroke; a Journal of Cerebral Circulation. 2015;46(5):1161–1166. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.008348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Oudin A, Forsberg B, Adolfsson AN, et al. Traffic-Related Air Pollution and Dementia Incidence in Northern Sweden: A Longitudinal Study. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2016;124(3):306–312. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1408322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Freedman VA, I, Grafova B, Rogowski J. Neighborhoods and chronic disease onset in later life. Am J Public Health. 2011;101(1):79–86. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.178640. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Sabariego C, Coenen M, Ballert C, et al. Determinants of Psychosocial Difficulties Experienced by Persons with Brain Disorders: Towards a ‘Horizontal Epidemiology’ Approach. PLoS One. 2015;10(12):e0141322. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Lynch SM, Moore JH. A call for biological data mining approaches in epidemiology. BioData Min. 2016;9:1. doi: 10.1186/s13040-015-0079-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Bonassi S, Taioli E, Vermeulen R. Omics in population studies: a molecular epidemiology perspective. Environ Mol Mutagen. 2013;54(7):455–60. doi: 10.1002/em.21805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. Trans-Omics for Precision Medicine (TOPMed) Program. 2016 doi: 10.1093/aje/kwab115. Accessed: Nov 6, 2017. Available from: https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/research/resources/nhlbi-precision-medicine-initiative/topmed. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 73.Donadio V, Incensi A, Rizzo G, et al. A new potential biomarker for dementia with Lewy bodies: Skin nerve alpha-synuclein deposits. Neurology. 2017;89(4):318–326. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Bateman RJ, Xiong C, Benzinger TLS, et al. Clinical and Biomarker Changes in Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer’s Disease. New England Journal of Medicine. 2012;367(9):795–804. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1202753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Fleisher AS, Chen K, Quiroz YT, et al. Associations between biomarkers and age in the presenilin 1 E280A autosomal dominant Alzheimer disease kindred: a cross-sectional study. JAMA Neurol. 2015;72(3):316–24. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.3314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Reiman EM, Quiroz YT, Fleisher AS, et al. Brain imaging and fluid biomarker analysis in young adults at genetic risk for autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease in the presenilin 1 E280A kindred: a case-control study. The Lancet Neurology. 2012;11(12):1048–1056. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70228-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Taal HR, St Pourcain B, Thiering E, et al. Common variants at 12q15 and 12q24 are associated with infant head circumference. Nat Genet. 2012;44(5):532–538. doi: 10.1038/ng.2238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Identification of additional risk loci for stroke and small vessel disease: a meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies. The Lancet Neurology. 2016;15(7):695–707. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(16)00102-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Ikram MA, Fornage M, Smith AV, et al. Common variants at 6q22 and 17q21 are associated with intracranial volume. Nat Genet. 2012;44(5):539–44. doi: 10.1038/ng.2245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Doehner W, Prasse L, Wolpers J, et al. Transgenerational transmission of an anticholinergic endophenotype with memory dysfunction. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;51:19–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.11.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Maloney B, Lahiri DK. Epigenetics of dementia: understanding the disease as a transformation rather than a state. Lancet Neurol. 2016;15(7):760–774. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(16)00065-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Yu L, Boyle P, Wilson RS, et al. A random change point model for cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neuroepidemiology. 2012;39(2):73–83. doi: 10.1159/000339365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Cloutier S, Chertkow H, Kergoat MJ, et al. Patterns of Cognitive Decline Prior to Dementia in Persons with Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;47(4):901–13. doi: 10.3233/JAD-142910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Boyle PA, Yang J, Yu L, et al. Varied effects of age-related neuropathologies on the trajectory of late life cognitive decline. Brain. 2017;140(3):804–812. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Vos SJ, van Boxtel MP, Schiepers OJ, et al. Modifiable Risk Factors for Prevention of Dementia in Midlife, Late Life and the Oldest-Old: Validation of the LIBRA Index. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2017;58(2):537–547. doi: 10.3233/JAD-161208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Contador I, Bermejo-Pareja F, Puertas-Martin V, et al. Childhood and Adulthood Rural Residence Increases the Risk of Dementia: NEDICES Study. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2015;12(4):350–7. doi: 10.2174/1567205012666150324181327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Hall KS, Gao S, Unverzagt FW, et al. Low education and childhood rural residence: risk for Alzheimer’s disease in African Americans. Neurology. 2000;54(1):95–9. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.1.95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Kuh D, Pierce M, Adams J, et al. Cohort profile: updating the cohort profile for the MRC National Survey of Health and Development: a new clinic-based data collection for ageing research. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2011;40(1):e1–e9. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Kivipelto M, Ngandu T, Fratiglioni L, et al. Obesity and vascular risk factors at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology. 2005;62(10):1556–1560. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.10.1556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Harris TB, Launer LJ, Eiriksdottir G, et al. Age, gene/environment susceptibility–Reykjavik Study: multidisciplinary applied phenomics. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;165(9):1076–1087. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwk115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Schmidt R, Schmidt H, Curb JD, et al. Early inflammation and dementia: A 25‐year follow‐up of the Honolulu‐Asia aging study. Annals of Neurology. 2002;52(2):168–174. doi: 10.1002/ana.10265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Investigators A. The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study: design and objectives. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1989;129(4):687–702. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Friedman GD, Cutter GR, Donahue RP, et al. CARDIA: study design, recruitment, and some characteristics of the examined subjects. J Clin Epidemiol. 1988;41(11):1105–16. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(88)90080-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Dawber TR, Meadors GF, Moore FE., Jr Epidemiological approaches to heart disease: the Framingham Study. Am J Public Health Nation’s Health. 1951;41(3):279–81. doi: 10.2105/ajph.41.3.279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Splansky GL, Corey D, Yang Q, et al. The Third Generation Cohort of the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute’s Framingham Heart Study: design, recruitment, and initial examination. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;165(11):1328–35. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Feinleib M, Kannel WB, Garrison RJ, et al. The Framingham Offspring Study. Design and preliminary data. Prev Med. 1975;4(4):518–25. doi: 10.1016/0091-7435(75)90037-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Freedman DS, Goodman A, Contreras OA, et al. Secular trends in BMI and blood pressure among children and adolescents: the Bogalusa Heart Study. Pediatrics. 2012;130(1):e159–66. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-3302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Kosteniuk JG, Morgan DG, O’Connell ME, et al. Simultaneous temporal trends in dementia incidence and prevalence, 2005–2013: a population-based retrospective cohort study in Saskatchewan, Canada. International Psychogeriatrics. 2016;28(10):1643–1658. doi: 10.1017/S1041610216000818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Sivenius J, Tuomilehto J, Immonen-Raiha P, et al. Continuous 15-year decrease in incidence and mortality of stroke in Finland: the FINSTROKE study. Stroke. 2004;35(2):420–5. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000110220.63212.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Dodge HH, Zhu J, Lee CW, et al. Cohort effects in age-associated cognitive trajectories. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69(6):687–94. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Ganguli M. The times they are a-changin’: cohort effects in aging, cognition, and dementia. International Psychogeriatrics. 2017;29(3):353–355. doi: 10.1017/S1041610216001927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Rocca WA, Petersen RC, Knopman DS, et al. Trends in the incidence and prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease, dementia, and cognitive impairment in the United States. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2011;7(1):80–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2010.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Kövari E, Herrmann FR, Bouras C, et al. Amyloid deposition is decreasing in aging brains An autopsy study of 1,599 older people. Neurology. 2014;82(4):326–331. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Langa KM, Larson EB, Crimmins EM, et al. A Comparison of the Prevalence of Dementia in the United States in 2000 and 2012. JAMA Intern Med. 2017;177(1):51–58. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.6807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Chatterjee S, Peters SA, Woodward M, et al. Type 2 Diabetes as a Risk Factor for Dementia in Women Compared With Men: A Pooled Analysis of 2.3 Million People Comprising More Than 100,000 Cases of Dementia. Diabetes Care. 2016;39(2):300–7. doi: 10.2337/dc15-1588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Gao S, Ogunniyi A, Hall KS, et al. Dementia incidence declined in African-Americans but not in Yoruba. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12(3):244–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.06.1894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Czajka JL, Beyler A. Background Paper: Declining Response Rates in Federal Surveys: Trends and Implications Final Report, Vol 1 M.P Report. 2016
  • 108.Shi L, Chen SJ, Ma MY, et al. Sleep disturbances increase the risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2017.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Janicki-Deverts D, Cohen S, Matthews KA, et al. History of unemployment predicts future elevations in C-reactive protein among male participants in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2008;36(2):176–185. doi: 10.1007/s12160-008-9056-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Tranah GJ, Blackwell T, Stone KL, et al. Circadian activity rhythms and risk of incident dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older women. Annals of Neurology. 2011;70(5):722–732. doi: 10.1002/ana.22468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Radak Z, Hart N, Sarga L, et al. Exercise plays a preventive role against Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2010;20(3):777–783. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-091531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Stephan B, Wells J, Brayne C, et al. Increased fructose intake as a risk factor for dementia. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2010;65(8):809–814. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Baglietto-Vargas D, Chen Y, Suh D, et al. Short-term modern life-like stress exacerbates Aβ-pathology and synapse loss in 3xTg-AD mice. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2015;134(5):915–926. doi: 10.1111/jnc.13195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Marcello E, Gardoni F, Di Luca M. Alzheimer’s disease and modern lifestyle: what is the role of stress? J Neurochem. 2015;134(5):795–8. doi: 10.1111/jnc.13210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Charles-Smith LE, Reynolds TL, Cameron MA, et al. Using Social Media for Actionable Disease Surveillance and Outbreak Management: A Systematic Literature Review. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(10):e0139701. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0139701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Sloane R, Osanlou O, Lewis D, et al. Social media and pharmacovigilance: A review of the opportunities and challenges. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology. 2015;80(4):910–920. doi: 10.1111/bcp.12717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Oscar N, Fox PA, Croucher R, et al. Machine Learning, Sentiment Analysis, and Tweets: An Examination of Alzheimer’s Disease Stigma on Twitter. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2017;72(5):742–751. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbx014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Richards EJ. Population epigenetics. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development. 2008;18(2):221–226. doi: 10.1016/j.gde.2008.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Ribeiro BG, Werner H, Lopes F, et al. Central Nervous System Effects of Intrauterine Zika Virus Infection: A Pictorial Review. Radiographics. 2017;37(6):1840–1850. doi: 10.1148/rg.2017170023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Ganguli M, Chandra V, Kamboh MI, et al. Apolipoprotein E polymorphism and Alzheimer disease: The Indo-US Cross-National Dementia Study. Arch Neurol. 2000;57(6):824–30. doi: 10.1001/archneur.57.6.824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Borenstein AR, Wu Y, Bowen JD, et al. Incidence rates of dementia, Alzheimer disease, and vascular dementia in the Japanese American population in Seattle, WA: the Kame Project. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2014;28(1):23–9. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3182a2e32f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Moll van Charante EP, Richard E, Eurelings LS, et al. Effectiveness of a 6-year multidomain vascular care intervention to prevent dementia (preDIVA): a cluster-randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2016;388(10046):797–805. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)30950-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Kivipelto M, Solomon A, Ahtiluoto S, et al. The Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability (FINGER): study design and progress. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9(6):657–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Davey Smith G, Hemani G. Mendelian randomization: genetic anchors for causal inference in epidemiological studies. Human Molecular Genetics. 2014;23(R1):R89–R98. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddu328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Zandi PP, Carlson MC, Plassman BL, et al. Hormone replacement therapy and incidence of Alzheimer disease in older women: the Cache County Study. JAMA. 2002;288(17):2123–9. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.17.2123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Shumaker SA, Legault C, Rapp SR, et al. Estrogen plus progestin and the incidence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2003;289(20):2651–62. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.20.2651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Shao H, Breitner JC, Whitmer RA, et al. Hormone therapy and Alzheimer disease dementia: new findings from the Cache County Study. Neurology. 2012;79(18):1846–52. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318271f823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Dacks PA, Bennett DA, Fillit HM. Evidence needs to be translated, whether or not it is complete. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(2):137–8. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.5376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Ehrenstein V, Nielsen H, Pedersen AB, et al. Clinical epidemiology in the era of big data: new opportunities, familiar challenges. Clin Epidemiol. 2017;9:245–250. doi: 10.2147/CLEP.S129779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Collins FS, Varmus H. A new initiative on precision medicine. New England Journal of Medicine. 2015;372(9):793–795. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1500523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.National Institutes of Health. All of Us Research Program. 2017 Accessed: Nov 6, 2017. Available from: https://allofus.nih.gov/
  • 132.Bakrania K, Edwardson CL, Khunti K, et al. Associations between sedentary behaviours and cognitive function: cross-sectional and prospective findings from the UK Biobank. Am J Epidemiol. 2017 doi: 10.1093/aje/kwx273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Gaziano JM, Concato J, Brophy M, et al. Million Veteran Program: A mega-biobank to study genetic influences on health and disease. J Clin Epidemiol. 2016;70:214–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2015.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Gilsanz P, Mayeda ER, Glymour MM, et al. Association Between Birth in a High Stroke Mortality State, Race, and Risk of Dementia. JAMA Neurol. 2017;74(9):1056–1062. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.1553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Sachdev PS, Lipnicki DM, Kochan NA, et al. COSMIC (Cohort Studies of Memory in an International Consortium): an international consortium to identify risk and protective factors and biomarkers of cognitive ageing and dementia in diverse ethnic and sociocultural groups. BMC Neurol. 2013;13:165. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-13-165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.American Heart Association. Professional Heart Daily. 2017 Accessed: Nov 6, 2017. Available from: https://professional.heart.org/professional/ResearchPrograms/InstituteforPrecisionCardiovascularMedicine/UCM_461668_Information-on-the-Institute-for-Precision-Cardiovascular-Medicine.jsp.
  • 137.Khoury M. Genomics and Health Impact Blog. 2016 Available from: https://blogs.cdc.gov/genomics/2016/04/21/shift/https://blogs.cdc.gov/genomics/2016/04/21/shift/
  • 138.Braunwald E. Cardiovascular medicine at the turn of the millennium: triumphs, concerns, and opportunities. New England Journal of Medicine. 1997;337(19):1360–1369. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199711063371906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES