Skip to main content
The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity logoLink to The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity
. 2018 Feb 27;15:22. doi: 10.1186/s12966-018-0654-8

The effects of acceptance and commitment therapy on eating behavior and diet delivered through face-to-face contact and a mobile app: a randomized controlled trial

Elina Järvelä-Reijonen 1,, Leila Karhunen 1,2, Essi Sairanen 3,4, Joona Muotka 3, Sanni Lindroos 5, Jaana Laitinen 6, Sampsa Puttonen 6, Katri Peuhkuri 5, Maarit Hallikainen 1, Jussi Pihlajamäki 1,2, Riitta Korpela 5, Miikka Ermes 7, Raimo Lappalainen 3, Marjukka Kolehmainen 1,2
PMCID: PMC5828146  PMID: 29482636

Abstract

Background

Internal motivation and good psychological capabilities are important factors in successful eating-related behavior change. Thus, we investigated whether general acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) affects reported eating behavior and diet quality and whether baseline perceived stress moderates the intervention effects.

Methods

Secondary analysis of unblinded randomized controlled trial in three Finnish cities. Working-aged adults with psychological distress and overweight or obesity in three parallel groups: (1) ACT-based Face-to-face (n = 70; six group sessions led by a psychologist), (2) ACT-based Mobile (n = 78; one group session and mobile app), and (3) Control (n = 71; only the measurements). At baseline, the participants’ (n = 219, 85% females) mean body mass index was 31.3 kg/m2 (SD = 2.9), and mean age was 49.5 years (SD = 7.4). The measurements conducted before the 8-week intervention period (baseline), 10 weeks after the baseline (post-intervention), and 36 weeks after the baseline (follow-up) included clinical measurements, questionnaires of eating behavior (IES-1, TFEQ-R18, HTAS, ecSI 2.0, REBS), diet quality (IDQ), alcohol consumption (AUDIT-C), perceived stress (PSS), and 48-h dietary recall. Hierarchical linear modeling (Wald test) was used to analyze the differences in changes between groups.

Results

Group x time interactions showed that the subcomponent of intuitive eating (IES-1), i.e., Eating for physical rather than emotional reasons, increased in both ACT-based groups (p = .019); the subcomponent of TFEQ-R18, i.e., Uncontrolled eating, decreased in the Face-to-face group (p = .020); the subcomponent of health and taste attitudes (HTAS), i.e., Using food as a reward, decreased in the Mobile group (p = .048); and both subcomponent of eating competence (ecSI 2.0), i.e., Food acceptance (p = .048), and two subcomponents of regulation of eating behavior (REBS), i.e., Integrated and Identified regulation (p = .003, p = .023, respectively), increased in the Face-to-face group. Baseline perceived stress did not moderate effects on these particular features of eating behavior from baseline to follow-up. No statistically significant effects were found for dietary measures.

Conclusions

ACT-based interventions, delivered in group sessions or by mobile app, showed beneficial effects on reported eating behavior. Beneficial effects on eating behavior were, however, not accompanied by parallel changes in diet, which suggests that ACT-based interventions should include nutritional counseling if changes in diet are targeted.

Trial registration

ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT01738256), registered 17 August, 2012.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (10.1186/s12966-018-0654-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Keywords: ACT, Behavior change, Mindfulness, Mindful eating, Intuitive eating, Dietary intake, Regulation of eating behavior, Overweight, Obesity, mHealth

Background

Making long-term eating-related behavioral changes to promote health is difficult. We need to find ways to support people in making the changes [1]. Long-term changes seem to be associated, for example, with supporting individual’s autonomy and internal motivation [2]. Internal motivation for regulating eating means that one is engaged in health-related behavior for one’s own sake and free will and that one’s action is congruent with own values and goals [3].

Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) is one promising method in changing behavior towards a person’s own values and goals. ACT consists of six interrelated core processes: (1) clarification of own values, (2) commitment to act based on those values, (3) being in contact with the present moment (i.e., mindfulness), (4) having self as context (i.e., being aware of thoughts, feelings, etc. without attaching to them), (5) defusion (i.e., altering the way to interact with or relate to thoughts, feelings, etc.), and (6) acceptance [4]. Thus, ACT aims to strengthen positive psychological processes related to commitment, behavior change, mindfulness, and acceptance [4], which can be applied to promote healthy behavioral patterns [5]. Using ACT is supported by the promising results of ACT-based interventions on food cravings [6] and weight loss [713].

Furthermore, deficiency of one of the core processes of ACT, namely, mindfulness, during eating can lead to overeating or eating without physical hunger [14, 15]. Mindfulness training, instead, has reduced impulsive eating and binge eating in adults with overweight and obesity [16], has reduced energy intake in experimental settings [17, 18], and may thus increase consciousness of one’s eating behavior and its regulation.

One aim in mindful eating training is to increase awareness of bodily hunger and satiety cues and to eat according to them [19, 20]. The emphasis on bodily hunger and satiety cues is also included in two concepts of eating behavior: intuitive eating (i.e., having unconditional permission to eat whatever desired, and eating relying on hunger and satiety cues and not on emotions) [21, 22] and eating competence (i.e., having positive attitudes about eating and food, accepting and eating an ever-increasing variety of foods, eating according to internal hunger and satiety signals, and having skills and resources for managing daily meals) [23]. Both intuitive eating and eating competence have been associated with better diet quality [24, 25] and lower BMI [22, 2628]. However, no previous ACT or mindfulness intervention studies have been targeted on eating competence, and those reporting effects on intuitive eating have had strong emphasis on intuitive eating approach in the intervention [29, 30]. Previous mindfulness-based interventions have been shown to decrease [31, 32] or to have no effect [9, 33] on emotional eating (i.e., eating based on negative emotions [34]). Thus, more research on the effects of ACT and mindfulness on eating behavior is needed.

There is also a need for new methods that are cost-effective to health care systems and easily accessible to the people who need support. New technology, such as mobile apps, have gained wide interest recently [3538], and there is already some evidence that mobile apps can be effective in improving health-related behavior [39].

The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of ACT intervention delivered in two different ways, i.e., via face-to-face group sessions and via mobile app, on reported eating behavior and diet quality among adults with psychological distress and overweight or obesity. Because there is some evidence that human support enhances technology-based interventions’ effects [4042], we hypothesized that the effects of an independently used mobile app ACT would be more modest than the effects of face-to-face ACT. The ACT intervention was not designed to specifically target eating behavior. However, several hypothesized effects are presented in Fig. 1. We found previously in this study population [43] that higher perceived stress is associated with unfavorable features of eating behavior: having less intuitive eating, eating competence and cognitive restraint, and more uncontrolled and emotional eating. Therefore, we also investigated whether baseline perceived stress moderates the effects of ACT on eating behavior.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Theoretical model. The hypothesized effects of the core processes of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) on the reported features of eating behavior

Methods

Study design

The present study is a secondary analysis of the parallel-arm Elixir randomized controlled trial in which three different psychological interventions were studied [44]. The present study focuses on the effects of the two intervention arms based on ACT. See Lappalainen et al. [44] for the study protocol and participant flow chart.

The study participants were recruited by advertisements in local newspapers and screened for eligibility via telephone and on-line questionnaire from August 2012 until January 2013. The participants had to be 25–60 years old and have a self-reported body mass index (BMI) 27–34.9 kg/m2. The participants also had to be psychologically distressed (≥3/12 points from the General Health Questionnaire, GHQ-12 [45]) and have computer and Internet access. There were several exclusion criteria, such as diagnosed severe chronic illness including eating disorder, disabilities/illnesses affecting substantially physiological or mental health, pregnancy or breastfeeding within the past 6 months, psychotherapy or other psychological or mental treatment at least twice a month, and participation in other intervention studies during the present study. The multicenter study was conducted in three cities in Finland (Jyväskylä, Kuopio, and Helsinki) in two phases. The first phase started in autumn and the second phase in spring. The participants filled in electronic questionnaires, visited the local study center for clinical and biochemical measurements, and reported their food consumption in a 48-h dietary recall by telephone. Measurements were conducted before the intervention (baseline, study week 00), after the 8-week intensive intervention period (post-intervention, study week 10), and 36 weeks after baseline measurements (follow-up, study week 36). The measurements were collected from August 2012 until December 2013.

The sample size of the current study is based on the power calculation (for depression symptoms) of the Elixir randomized controlled trial [44], resulting a sample size of n = 80–85 per group.

Ethics, consent and permissions

The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Central Finland Health Care District (reference number 7 U/2012) and was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The participants gave their written informed consent before participating. The study was registered with ClinicalTrials.gov with the identifier NCT01738256.

Participants

Of the 254 individuals randomized to the Face-to-face, Mobile or Control groups, 219 participated in baseline measurements. At baseline, the participants’ (n = 219, 85% females) mean BMI was 31.3 kg/m2 (SD = 2.9), and their mean age was 49.5 years (SD = 7.4). The baseline demographic and clinical characteristics did not differ among the three study groups (Table 1). The number of participants at baseline, post-intervention, and follow-up were as follows: Face-to-face group—70, 62, and 60; Mobile group—78, 75, and 73; and Control group—71, 68, and 67, respectively. Thus, 89%, 96% and 96% of the Face-to-face, Mobile and Control group participants completed the post-intervention measurements, and 86%, 94% and 94% completed the follow-up measurements, respectively.

Table 1.

Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics of each group

Face-to-face Mobile Control pa
Number of participants (n) 70 78 71
Starting time of the study (n) .642
 Autumn 35 37 30
 Spring 35 41 41
Study center (n) .970
 Jyväskylä 20 22 17
 Kuopio 22 25 23
 Helsinki 28 31 31
Gender (n) .670
 Female 61 66 58
 Male 9 12 13
Age (years) 50.3 ± 7.2 49.1 ± 7.7 49.2 ± 7.4 .575
Weight (kg) 86.1 ± 10.3 88.4 ± 10.4 88.3 ± 11.5 .342
BMI (kg/m2) 31.0 ± 3.1 31.6 ± 2.7 31.2 ± 2.8 .423
Psychological distress (GHQ-12 score) 7.2 ± 3.0 6.8 ± 2.8 7.4 ± 2.7 .408b
Perceived stress (PSS score) 25.8 ± 8.0 26.9 ± 7.8 26.9 ± 7.6 .597

Values are n / mean ± SD; Autumn = September – October 2012; Spring = January – February 2013; BMI body mass index, GHQ-12 General Health Questionnaire-12, PSS Perceived Stress Scale

ap-value for differences between the study groups (Pearson chi-square for categorical variables and one-way ANOVA for continuous variables unless other noted)

bNon-parametric Kruskal-Wallis test

Study groups

The Face-to-face and Mobile interventions were based on the same ACT program constructed by the same research group. Thus, only the delivery method of the intervention differed. The two interventions included the following main components: value clarification, acting according to own values, mindfulness skills, the observing self (e.g., observing thoughts without being caught up in them), and acceptance skills (e.g., making room for unpleasant feelings and urges allowing them to come and go). The main focus was on ACT skills but minor parts of mindful eating, relaxation, and everyday physical activity were also included. Mindful eating was the topic of one group session in the Face-to-face intervention group and of one section in the Mobile group’s app. The mindful eating component of the intervention consisted of learning to be present while eating; observe eating-related thoughts and feelings; observe and trust hunger and satiety cues; notice challenges for eating based on physical cues; be aware of the effects of not eating mindfully; recognize individual needs and feelings related to meal rhythm; and practicing mindful grocery shopping. Intervention did not include nutrition education. Only a hyperlink to a public nutritional web site was provided to the participants in intervention groups, which was to be utilized if the dietary changes were according to one’s values. See Lappalainen et al. [44] for a more detailed description of the intervention.

The Face-to-face group had six group sessions led by a psychologist during the 8-week intervention period. Each session took approximately 90 min, and each group consisted of 6–12 participants. The sessions included exercises, pair and group discussions, and homework for which the participants received a workbook.

The mobile group had one group session in which participants learned of the principles of ACT and received smartphones with the pre-installed Oiva mobile app [46]. The Oiva app contains 46 exercises in text and audio formats and introduction videos about the ACT skills. The user experience results of the app were positive [46]. The participants were free to choose exercises and videos in any order and to do them as many times as the participants wanted during the 8-week intervention period. The participants returned the smartphones during the post-intervention laboratory study visit. The participants’ usage of the mobile app is reported in detail by Mattila et al. [47].

Participants randomized to the Control group participated in all of the measurements and did not receive any intervention. After the follow-up measurements, the participants in the Control group had an opportunity to attend one group session in which principles of ACT were presented and to utilize the Internet-based lifestyle coaching program.

Measures

Background characteristics

Weight and height were measured with calibrated instruments at each study center in the morning after a 12-h overnight fast [44]. BMI was calculated from the measured weight and height as kilograms per meters squared. The demographic information was collected using a questionnaire. The 12-item General Health Questionnaire, GHQ-12 [45], was used to screen the volunteers for psychological distress. The GHQ-12 has been found to be a valid screening tool for common mental health problems in the Finnish population [48]. The respondents were asked, considering the past few weeks, to answer questions such as “Have you recently felt capable of making decisions about things?” Bimodal scoring was used: “not at all” (0 points); “same as usual” (0); “rather more than usual” (1); and “much more than usual” (1), with the total sum score ranging from 0 to 12. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.72.

Outcome measures

Eating behavior

The Intuitive Eating Scale, IES [22], consists of 21 items with subcategories of intuitive eating: (a) Unconditional Permission to Eat (9 items, e.g., “If I am craving a certain food, I allow myself to have it.”), (b) Eating for Physical Rather Than Emotional Reasons (6 items, e.g., reversely scored “I find myself eating when I am bored, even when I’m not physically hungry.”), and (c) Reliance on Internal Hunger/Satiety Cues (6 items, e.g., “I trust my body to tell me when to eat.”). The statements are answered with a 5-point Likert scale. The scores are averaged; thus, the possible ranges of the IES total score and its subscales are 1–5. Cronbach’s alpha at baseline was 0.79 for the entire scale and 0.66, 0.84, and 0.77 for the subscales Unconditional Permission to Eat, Eating for Physical Rather Than Emotional Reasons, and Reliance on Internal Hunger/Satiety Cues, respectively. The questionnaire had been validated among college women in the USA [22].

The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire, TFEQ-R18 [34], was used to measure (a) Cognitive Restraint (6 items, e.g., “I deliberately take small helpings as a means of controlling my weight.”), (b) Uncontrolled Eating (9 items, e.g., “Sometimes when I start eating, I just can’t seem to stop.”), and (c) Emotional Eating (3 items, e.g., “When I feel blue, I often overeat.”). The answers are given by 4-point Likert scale, except for one item, which is answered using an 8-point Likert scale. The possible range of the total scores was 0–100. Cronbach’s alphas were 0.71, 0.88, and 0.89 for the scales Cognitive Restraint, Uncontrolled Eating, and Emotional Eating, respectively. The Finnish version of the questionnaire had been validated in young, mostly normal weight, females and showed good structural validity [49].

Of the Health and Taste Attitude Scales, HTAS [50], subcategories (a) Pleasure (6 items, e.g., “When I eat, I concentrate on enjoying the taste of food.”) and (b) Using Food as a Reward (6 items, e.g., “I reward myself by buying something really tasty.”) were used. The statements were answered using a 7-point Likert scale. The scores were averaged; thus, the possible ranges were 1–7. Cronbach’s alphas were 0.71 and 0.81 for the subcategories Pleasure and Using Food as a Reward, respectively. The questionnaire developed in Finland had been validated among several general Finnish adult samples [5052].

Eating competence was measured using a preliminary Finnish translation of ecSatter Inventory 2.0, ecSI 2.0 [28, 53, 54]. The definition of eating competence consisted of four components, which also constituted the 16-item questionnaire’s subcategories: (a) Eating Attitudes (5 items, e.g., “I am relaxed about eating.”), (b) Food Acceptance (3 items, e.g., “I experiment with new food and learn to like it.”), (c) Internal Regulation (3 items, e.g., “I eat as much as I am hungry for.”), and (d) Contextual Skills (5 items, e.g., “I generally plan for feeding myself. I don’t just grab food when I get hungry.”). The statements were answered: “always” (3 points), “often” (2), “sometimes” (1), “rarely” (0), or “never” (0). The possible ranges of the sum scores were as follows: Eating Competence total score, 0–48; Eating Attitudes and Contextual Skills, 0–15, and Food Acceptance and Internal Regulation, 0–9. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.76 for the whole scale and 0.58, 0.68, 0.59, and 0.75 for the subscales Eating Attitudes, Food Acceptance, Internal Regulation, and Contextual Skills, respectively. The questionnaire had been validated among mostly female, overweight and educated adult sample [26], low-income females [28, 53] and parents of preschool-age children [54] in the USA.

The motivation for eating behavior regulation was measured using the 24-item Regulation of Eating Behavior Scale, REBS [3]. The participants were asked to answer the question “Why are you regulating your eating behaviors?” with a 7-point scale ranging from “Does not correspond at all” (1) to “Corresponds exactly” (7). The scale measured autonomous forms of motivation: (a) Intrinsic motivation (e.g., “It is fun to create meals that are good for my health”), (b) Integrated regulation (e.g., “Eating healthy is an integral part of my life”), and (c) Identified regulation (e.g., “It is a good idea to try to regulate my eating behaviors”). In addition, there were controlled forms of motivation: (d) Introjected regulation (e.g., “I don’t want to be ashamed of how I look.”), (e) External regulation (e.g., “People around me nag me to do it.”), and (f) Amotivation (e.g., “I can’t really see what I’m getting out of it.”). Each category (a–f) included four items. The scores were averaged; thus, the possible ranges were 1–7. Cronbach’s alphas were 0.86, 0.89, 0.75, 0.60, 0.89, and 0.71 for a, b, c, d, e, and f, respectively. The questionnaire had been validated among female university students in Canada [3]. The Finnish version used in this study had been pilot-tested among a general adult sample (n = 37).

Food consumption and nutrient intake

A concise measure of food consumption, the Index of Diet Quality (IDQ) [55], consisted of 18 questions about frequency, portion size, and/or type of certain foods and drinks consumed during the previous month to evaluate adherence to Nordic and Finnish nutrition recommendations. The questions involved whole-grain products, fat-containing foods, liquid dairy products, vegetables, fruits and berries, sugary products, and the regularity of meal pattern. The answers were scored as either reflecting health-promoting diet (1 point) or not (0 points). Part of the questions (regarding both frequency and portion of the food or drink) were combined for the scoring, and thus the possible IDQ total score was 0–15. Points below 10 indicated non-adherence, and points from 10 to 15 indicated adherence to the health-promoting diet [55]. In this study, answers that seemed possibly unrealistic or outliers (e.g., 27 slices of bread per day) were confirmed with the participant, and corrections were made when needed. Answers that remained unverified (n = 1 at baseline, n = 2 at post-intervention) were coded as missing. The IDQ had been developed and validated among Finnish healthy, mostly normal weight, adult females using a seven-day food record [55].

Alcohol consumption during the previous six months was measured using the Finnish version of the questionnaire Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test Consumption, AUDIT-C [56]. This questionnaire had been shown to have strong correlation to alcohol consumption in a general Finnish population [57]. The questionnaire contained three questions regarding the frequency and amounts of alcohol usage. For the questions concerning the amount of drinks consumed, a list of typical Finnish serving sizes and their corresponding amounts as standard drinks (e.g., 33 cl bottle of beer is one drink) were provided. The responses were scored from 0 to 4 and summed, and the possible total score was from 0 to 12. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.66.

The 48-h dietary recall was conducted to collect information on nutrient intake. The participants were asked to describe all of the foods and drinks consumed during the previous full 48 h (beginning at midnight and ending at midnight over two consecutive 24 h periods). The interview was conducted by trained nutritionists by telephone at a pre-scheduled time. The participants were told that the interview considered diet, but anything regarding 48-h recall was not mentioned beforehand. An electronic picture book [58] was used to help to describe portion sizes. The interviews were performed from Tuesday to Friday. The nutrient intake was calculated using AivoDiet software version 2.0.2.2 (Aivo Ltd., Turku, Finland) and the Fineli® Finnish Food Composition Database (National Institute for Health and Welfare, Nutrition Unit, Helsinki, Finland). The interview protocol of the 48-h dietary recall was created based on the face-to-face 48-h dietary recall conducted in the national FINDIET 2012 survey [59]. The 48-h dietary recall protocol of the Elixir study was designed by the three nutritionists who also conducted the interviews. The participants were encouraged to be truthful in the 48-h dietary recall and were told that the interviewer would not assess or comment on their eating and drinking or give any dietary counseling. The foods and beverages consumed during the 48 h were repeated at the end, and the interviewer encouraged the participant to make additions or modifications while repeating the course of the days’ events.

Moderator

Perceived stress

The Perceived Stress Scale, PSS [60], is a 14-item measure for assessing the degree to which a person perceives life as stressful. The questionnaire has demonstrated acceptable psychometric properties worldwide [61]. Questions concern how often a person has experienced certain feelings and thoughts during the previous month, e.g., “In the last month, how often have you found that you could not cope with all the things that you had to do?” The 5-point Likert scale from “never” (0) to “very often” (4) is summed for the total score (possible range 0–56). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.88.

Statistical methods

The statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics version 21 and Mplus version 7.3. Pearson chi-square test, one-way ANOVA, and the Kruskal-Wallis test were used to test whether baseline demographic and clinical characteristics differed between the study groups.

Hierarchical linear modeling (HLM, Wald test) was used to analyze the group x time interaction, i.e., whether the three study groups changed differently between the measured time points (study weeks 00, 10, and 36). If there was a difference, post hoc tests were conducted to determine between the three study groups whether the difference was during the intensive intervention period (from study week 00 to 10) or after the intensive intervention period (from study week 10 to 36). HLM accounts for missing values at random (MAR) and includes all of the available data. The parameters were estimated using the full-information maximum likelihood method (MLR estimation in Mplus). The analyses were adjusted for study center and starting time of the study. Emotional eating, External regulation, and intake of monounsaturated fat (E%) differed significantly between the groups at baseline, and these analyses were conducted also adjusting for the baseline value. Exact p-values of Wald tests are shown in Table 2 and Additional file 1, whereas statistically significant p-values of the post hoc analyses are presented in the text.

Table 2.

The effects of ACT-based Face-to-face and Mobile interventions on eating behavior

Face-to-face Mobile Control pa db
0 wk 10 wk 36 wk dc 0 wk 10 wk 36 wk dc 0 wk 10 wk 36 wk dc
IES total score 2.9 ± 0.4 3.0 ± 0.5 3.1 ± 0.4 0.45 2.9 ± 0.5 3.0 ± 0.4 3.1 ± 0.5 0.29 3.0 ± 0.5 3.0 ± 0.5 3.0 ± 0.5 0.16 .090 0.27
0.13
 Unconditional Permission to Eat 3.0 ± 0.5 3.0 ± 0.5 3.1 ± 0.5 0.10 3.1 ± 0.6 3.1 ± 0.6 3.1 ± 0.7 − 0.05 3.1 ± 0.6 3.0 ± 0.7 3.1 ± 0.6 − 0.01 .277 0.11
− 0.05
 Eating for Physical Rather Than Emotional Reasons 2.4 ± 0.8 2.6 ± 0.7 2.8 ± 0.8 0.50 2.4 ± 0.8 2.6 ± 0.8 2.7 ± 0.8 0.44 2.6 ± 0.9 2.6 ± 0.8 2.7 ± 0.8 0.10 .019 0.40
0.33
 Reliance on Internal Hunger/Satiety Cues 3.2 ± 0.6 3.3 ± 0.7 3.4 ± 0.6 0.36 3.2 ± 0.7 3.3 ± 0.6 3.4 ± 0.6 0.29 3.2 ± 0.7 3.2 ± 0.6 3.4 ± 0.6 0.27 .967 0.04
− 0.01
TFEQ-R18
 Cognitive Restraint 43.1 ± 16.6 49.4 ± 14.4 51.5 ± 17.0 0.47 45.2 ± 16.2 47.6 ± 17.6 48.7 ± 15.3 0.26 45.8 ± 15.3 48.4 ± 15.3 47.7 ± 16.2 0.11 .252 0.37
0.15
 Uncontrolled Eating 49.3 ± 18.3 44.7 ± 20.1 39.5 ± 20.5 − 0.46 49.4 ± 20.1 44.6 ± 19.2 43.9 ± 20.3 − 0.30 50.2 ± 20.9 48.4 ± 21.0 47.7 ± 19.0 − 0.11 .020 − 0.34
− 0.20
 Emotional Eating 64.9 ± 25.3 57.3 ± 24.6 54.6 ± 25.6 − 0.40 62.4 ± 27.5 56.3 ± 26.0 52.8 ± 25.8 − 0.36 55.9 ± 27.9 54.4 ± 28.9 53.8 ± 25.1 − 0.08 .083d − 0.31
− 0.27
HTAS
 Pleasure 4.7 ± 0.9 4.8 ± 0.8 4.8 ± 0.9 0.16 4.9 ± 1.0 4.8 ± 0.9 4.7 ± 1.0 − 0.16 4.7 ± 1.0 4.7 ± 1.1 4.8 ± 1.0 0.15 .066 − 0.01
− 0.30
 Using Food as a Reward 4.3 ± 1.2 4.2 ± 1.2 4.0 ± 1.2 − 0.21 4.6 ± 1.1 4.2 ± 1.2 4.1 ± 1.2 − 0.39 4.3 ± 1.2 4.2 ± 1.1 4.2 ± 1.2 − 0.10 .048 −0.11
− 0.29
ecSI 2.0 total score 26.2 ± 6.0 26.6 ± 6.3 28.1 ± 6.6 0.22 26.3 ± 5.7 26.8 ± 6.2 26.2 ± 6.2 − 0.04 25.8 ± 6.4 25.8 ± 5.9 26.5 ± 6.4 0.07 .164 0.14
−0.11
 Eating Attitudes 10.0 ± 2.1 9.7 ± 2.1 10.0 ± 2.5 −0.03 9.7 ± 2.2 9.8 ± 1.9 9.4 ± 2.4 −0.12 9.7 ± 2.6 9.5 ± 2.6 9.7 ± 2.2 −0.03 .144 0.00
−0.09
 Food Acceptance 4.9 ± 2.0 4.9 ± 1.9 5.5 ± 1.6 0.25 5.2 ± 1.9 5.1 ± 2.0 5.0 ± 2.0 −0.09 4.9 ± 1.8 4.9 ± 1.9 4.8 ± 1.9 −0.04 .048 0.31
−0.06
 Internal Regulation 4.7 ± 1.9 4.8 ± 1.8 5.1 ± 1.7 0.22 5.0 ± 1.6 5.1 ± 1.6 4.7 ± 1.8 −0.17 4.8 ± 1.8 5.0 ± 1.5 4.9 ± 1.8 0.02 .077 0.20
− 0.18
 Contextual Skills 6.7 ± 3.1 7.3 ± 3.1 7.6 ± 3.1 0.24 6.5 ± 3.0 6.8 ± 3.0 7.1 ± 3.2 0.18 6.4 ± 3.1 6.4 ± 2.6 7.1 ± 2.9 0.20 .720 0.05
− 0.02
REBS
 Intrinsic motivation 5.1 ± 1.2 5.3 ± 1.2 5.4 ± 1.2 0.19 5.1 ± 1.3 5.2 ± 1.2 5.2 ± 1.4 0.05 4.9 ± 1.4 4.9 ± 1.3 5.0 ± 1.4 0.09 .831 0.09
−0.04
 Integrated regulation 3.9 ± 1.3 4.5 ± 1.3 4.7 ± 1.4 0.55 4.3 ± 1.3 4.4 ± 1.1 4.3 ± 1.4 0.09 4.1 ± 1.3 4.0 ± 1.3 4.2 ± 1.4 0.12 .003 0.41
− 0.04
 Identified regulation 5.8 ± 0.9 6.0 ± 0.8 6.0 ± 0.8 0.17 5.9 ± 0.9 5.7 ± 1.0 5.7 ± 0.9 − 0.20 5.7 ± 0.9 5.6 ± 1.1 5.7 ± 0.9 0.02 .023 0.15
−0.21
 Introjected regulation 4.2 ± 1.1 4.0 ± 1.2 4.0 ± 1.2 −0.13 4.2 ± 1.2 4.0 ± 1.2 4.1 ± 1.3 −0.09 4.3 ± 1.2 4.1 ± 1.2 4.1 ± 1.3 −0.19 .955 0.08
0.10
 External regulation 3.0 ± 1.6 3.0 ± 1.6 2.9 ± 1.8 0.04 3.5 ± 1.6 3.5 ± 1.5 3.3 ± 1.6 −0.11 3.7 ± 1.7 3.5 ± 1.8 3.6 ± 1.7 −0.04 .489e 0.10
−0.07
 Amotivation 2.1 ± 1.0 1.8 ± 0.9 1.7 ± 0.8 −0.39 2.1 ± 1.0 2.1 ± 1.0 2.1 ± 1.0 −0.00 2.1 ± 1.0 2.2 ± 1.0 2.1 ± 1.1 −0.00 .059 −0.36
0.00

The values are unestimated means ± SD. IES Intuitive Eating Scale, TFEQ-R18 The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire-R18, HTAS Health and Taste Attitude Scales, ecSI 2.0 preliminary Finnish translation of Satter Eating Competence Inventory 2.0, REBS Regulation of Eating Behavior Scale. Higher scores represent higher amount of the feature in all of the scales. There were missing values of one (n = 1) participant in the Mobile group at week 36 and of three participants (n = 3) in the Control group at weeks 10 and 36

ap-value for differences in changes between the three study groups using all measured time points (study weeks 00, 10, and 36) adjusted for study center and starting time using estimated parameters (hierarchical linear model, Wald test). Bold text indicates significant p-value < 0.05

bCohen’s d from baseline to follow-up between the Face-to-face and Control groups (above) and between the Mobile and Control groups (below) using estimated parameters

cCohen’s d from baseline to follow-up within the group using estimated parameters

dAfter adding the baseline value to the adjustments, p = 0.088

eAfter adding the baseline value to the adjustments, p = 0.569

Cohen’s d was calculated from baseline to follow-up (Δ 36 weeks) within- and corrected between-groups to estimate effect sizes using the estimated values. A within-group effect size of 0.5 is considered small, 0.8 medium, and 1.1 large, and a corrected between-group effect size of 0.2 is considered small, 0.5 medium, and 0.8 large [62].

Baseline perceived stress was tested mean-centered as a moderator of the intervention effects on change in eating behavior from baseline to follow-up (Δ 36 weeks). Each outcome variable was tested separately in a single, saturated, moderation model in which the intervention groups were compared separately to the Control group using Mplus software. Maximum Likelihood (MLR) estimation was used.

Results

Treatment adherence

Of the data included in the analyses, most of the participants in the Face-to-face group attended either all six group sessions (n = 16, 23%) or five group sessions (n = 31, 44%). One participant did not attend any group sessions (n = 1, 1%) or attended only one (n = 1, 1%) or two group sessions (n = 1, 1%). The participants attended on average 4.7 group sessions. In the Mobile group, the median number of usage sessions of the mobile app was 21 (range 4–91, interquartile range IQR 11–33), according to the usage log files of the smartphones. The median number of usage days was 15 (range 4–59, IQR 8–23). The median total duration of use was 274 min (range 43–2001, IQR 181–421).

Intervention effects on reported eating behavior

Group x time interactions were found among the three study groups during the entire study period (study weeks 00, 10, and 36) in the following subcomponents: the subcomponent of intuitive eating (IES), i.e., Eating for physical rather than emotional reasons; the subcomponent of TFEQ-R18, i.e., Uncontrolled eating; the subcomponent of health and taste attitudes (HTAS), i.e., Using food as a reward; the subcomponent of eating competence (ecSI 2.0), i.e., Food acceptance; and two subcomponents of regulation of eating behavior (REBS), i.e., Integrated and Identified regulation, with small or small-to-medium effect sizes (p < 0.050) (Table 2). These differences are presented in more detail in Fig. 2 and in the following.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

The statistically significant intervention effects. The measurements were conducted before the intervention (baseline, study week 00), after the 8-week intervention period (study week 10), and 36 weeks after the baseline measurements (study week 36). Face-to-face = Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT)-based intervention, six group sessions led by a psychologist; Mobile = ACT-based intervention, one group session and mobile app; Control = only the measurements. The values are unestimated means ± SD. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01 adjusted for study center and starting time of the study. A blue asterisk (*) represents a difference between the Face-to-face group and Control, an orange asterisk (*) represents a difference between the Mobile group and Control, and a black asterisk (*) represents a difference between the Face-to-face and Mobile groups. IES = Intuitive Eating Scale; REBS = Regulation of Eating Behavior Scale; HTAS = Health and Taste Attitude Scales; TFEQ = The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire-R18; ecSI = preliminary Finnish translation of Satter Eating Competence Inventory 2.0. Higher scores represent higher amount of the feature in all of the scales

Changes from baseline to post-intervention (study weeks 00–10)

There were improvements in the subcomponents of intuitive eating (IES), regulation of eating behavior (REBS), and health and taste attitudes (HTAS) (Fig. 2a–d). Eating for physical rather than emotional reasons increased in both the Face-to-face and Mobile groups compared to the Control group (p = 0.007 and p = 0.006, respectively). Integrated regulation increased in the Face-to-face group compared to both the Control group and Mobile group (p = 0.001 and p = 0.027, respectively). Similarly, Identified regulation increased in the Face-to-face group compared to both the Control group and Mobile group (p = 0.033 and p = 0.004, respectively). Using food as a reward decreased in the Mobile group compared to the Face-to-face group (p = 0.027).

Changes from post-intervention to follow-up (study weeks 10–36)

There were improvements in the subcomponents of TFEQ-R18 and eating competence (ecSI 2.0) (Fig. 2e, f). Uncontrolled eating decreased in the Face-to-face group compared to the Control group (p = 0.014). Food acceptance increased in the Face-to-face group compared to both the Control group and Mobile group (p = 0.007 and p = 0.011, respectively).

Moderating effect of perceived stress

Baseline perceived stress did not moderate effects on the abovementioned features of eating behavior from baseline to follow-up (Additional file 2).

Intervention effects on reported diet quality

There were no statistically significant differences in the changes in diet quality between the groups. The mean values, Cohen’s d, and p-values for the differences in changes between the three groups are presented in Additional file 1.

Discussion

This study investigated the effects of ACT interventions that were delivered in group sessions or by mobile app on reported eating behavior and diet quality among adults with psychological distress and overweight or obesity. The ACT-based interventions showed beneficial effects on eating behavior with no parallel changes in diet. Our results suggest that ACT was able to change the reasons for eating from emotional or environmental triggers towards hunger and satiety cues, increase the acceptance of a variety of foods, and help the individual to perceive healthy eating more consistently with his or her own values and goals. The results are consistent with the ACT theory and related to all of the core processes of ACT. The effects were more pronounced in the Face-to-face group than in the Mobile group, although both showed positive changes. A subcomponent of Intuitive eating, Eating for physical rather than emotional reasons, increased in both ACT groups, and Using food as a reward decreased in the Mobile ACT-group during the intervention. Furthermore, internal (Integrated and Identified) motivation for regulating eating behavior increased in the Face-to-face ACT-group during the intervention. Uncontrolled eating decreased and Food acceptance increased in the Face-to-face ACT-group during the follow-up. The baseline perceived stress did not moderate the intervention effects on changes in these features of eating behavior from baseline to follow-up.

In the previous ACT or mindfulness intervention studies, effects have been shown on all subcomponents of Intuitive eating [29, 30], whereas in the present study, only the scores of subcomponent Eating for physical rather than emotional reasons increased. This result may be explained by different intervention contents because in previous studies, intuitive eating was included in all of the intervention sessions [29, 30], whereas our intervention consisted of general ACT with minor mindful eating component and no other eating-specific content. Our previous findings show that weight-related psychological flexibility seems to particularly mediate the effects of ACT on intuitive eating [63].

Although the ACT intervention increased Eating for physical rather than emotional reasons compared to the control, Emotional eating (measured by TFEQ-R18) did not change at a statistically significant level. The lack of an intervention effect compared to the control is in line with previous studies [9, 33]. However, our data showed a trend for a decrease in Emotional eating during the study period in both intervention groups, with small between-group effect sizes compared to the Control group. In line with this, decreased emotional eating compared to waitlist [31] and treatment as usual [13] have been reported. Considering the trend for decreased Emotional eating, the increased Eating for physical rather than emotional reasons, and the decrease in Using food as a reward, our results suggest that ACT can decrease eating for emotional reasons.

The effect of ACT on eating competence has not been studied previously. The ACT intervention in the present study with minor mindful eating component did not have an effect on the total score or subscales Eating attitudes, Internal regulation, or Contextual skills. Nevertheless, the Food acceptance subscale (e.g., “I experiment with new food and learn to like it.”) increased in the Face-to-face group compared to other study groups after the intensive intervention period, which suggests that the participants may have focused on learning general acceptance skills during the intensive intervention period and applied them to eating behavior later during the follow-up.

To the best of our knowledge, there are no previous studies on the effects of ACT on forms of motivation for eating behavior regulation. Integrated and Identified regulation (e.g., “Eating healthy is an integral part of my life”, “It is a good idea to try to regulate my eating behaviors”, respectively) increased in the Face-to-face group. This result is in line with the theory of ACT because Integrated and Identified regulation of behavior include acting consistently with one’s values [3]. Our results indicate that ACT can increase eating behavior based on personal values which in turn has predicted making healthier choices in the long term [3]. However, parallel changes in diet were not observed in our study.

Although the ACT had effects on reported eating behavior that have been associated with health-beneficial dietary intake, no effects were found on the index of diet quality, alcohol consumption or energy nutrient intake compared to the control. The lack of intervention effects on dietary measures may be due to several reasons. First, the ACT intervention did not include nutrition education, and only a hyperlink to a public nutritional web site was provided. Previous mindfulness-based interventions without strong or any emphasis on diet or eating have showed similar results [32, 64], whereas mindfulness-based interventions including also dietary information have shown improvement in diet [33, 65, 66]. Second, because ACT concentrates on psychological processes and overall behavioral change, the primary focus of the participants may not have been on dietary changes, and therefore these changes may have needed more time to occur. Furthermore, the scores of the index of diet quality indicate that, in general, the participants’ diet was health-promoting [55] already at the baseline, and thus there was no room for drastic changes. In the future, it would be interesting to study whether the changes in eating behavior mediate changes in dietary intake in the long term.

We found previously in the current study population that perceived stress was associated with several features of unbeneficial eating behavior reflecting less intuitive eating, less eating competence and less cognitive restraint, and more uncontrolled and emotional-based eating [43]. Of those features, ACT intervention improved three, namely, Eating for physical rather than emotional reasons, Uncontrolled eating, and Using food as a reward. More importantly, according to the moderation analyses, intervention effects on these features of eating behavior occurred regardless of the baseline perceived stress level.

ACT is usually studied delivered in group sessions, and this is the first time that its effects on reported eating behavior and diet quality delivered via mobile app have also been studied. Mobile-based solutions are seen as promising because they may save time and costs in health care and be easily accessible to patients [3538]. The way that the two ACT interventions were delivered in our study seemed to impact eating behavior somewhat differently. The impact of the Face-to-face intervention seemed to be larger than what was observed in the Mobile intervention. In addition, all of the effects in the Mobile group occurred during the intensive intervention period, which suggested that there was an effect when the app was in active use [47]. The usage of mobile app was completely on the participant’s own responsibility, willingness, and remembrance. Thus, it may be possible that although the content of the ACT interventions were similar, the participants may have applied them differently because participating in the group sessions demanded intensive attention to the intervention contents. Furthermore, technology alone may not be as effective as intervention including human interaction [67]. Although the median duration of the mobile app usage was rather high, four and a half hours, the participants in Face-to-face group were more exposed to the treatment (on average seven hours). It is noteworthy that the mobile app was well accepted, e.g., the minimum number of usage days was four, and the median was fifteen.

Strengths and limitations

The present study is unique in several ways. First, the ACT intervention was delivered in two different ways: face-to-face in group sessions and individually via mobile app. Second, the study examined the effects of general ACT, which included minor mindful eating component but no nutrition education. Third, the effects of ACT on this wide variety of eating behavior and diet quality measures have not been reported previously. Fourth, the study population consisting of working-aged adults with psychological distress and overweight or obesity without serious medical conditions is unique compared to the study populations of previous studies. A large sample size and multicenter design, representing three areas in Finland, is also a strength of this study. The participants were likely to be interested in lifestyle changes because they had all enrolled in the Elixir lifestyle intervention study voluntarily and thus represented a possible target group of ACT group treatment or Oiva mobile app users.

There were also some limitations in terms of generalizability and methodology. The generalization of the study results may be limited because most of the participants were female, and due to the exclusion of, for example, individuals with severe chronic illness, the study population does not represent all treatment-seeking individuals of the community. Furthermore, although the internal consistency reliability was high in most of the scales, two subscales of the ecSatter Inventory had rather low Cronbach’s coefficient alphas (< 0.6), which may reflect the small number of items in the subscales [68] or suggest that these were not reliable measures to use in this population. In addition, although all of the questionnaires had been validated in their original language, all of the Finnish translations had not been validated, especially among adults with overweight or obesity. The 48-h dietary recall telephone interviews were conducted instead of using food records to diminish the burden on the participants [69]. This retrospective method that considered a rather long time period could also be regarded as a limitation in our study. The outcome depends on participants’ memory, although this limitation was addressed in our interview protocol. The validity of the 48-h recall has rarely been studied, and the results have been partly controversial [7072]. However, compared to a single 24-h recall, a 48-h recall is found to be superior [71]. The 48-h dietary recalls were performed from Tuesday to Friday, so Fridays and Saturdays are missing from the dietary intake data, which may have influenced our results because energy intake typically increases on weekends [73, 74]. It is also important to notice that, at baseline, the participants were unaware beforehand about the pre-scheduled telephone interview’s content. However, at post-intervention and follow-up, the participants have quite likely guessed what the scheduled telephone interview would involve, and they have had the possibility to change their eating to be able to report it as more socially desirable (more healthy food items and less unhealthier food items). Another consideration is related to the possibly increased attention towards eating in intervention groups because of the ACT skills and its effect on reporting food intake or eating behavior more accurately at post-intervention and follow-up.

Conclusions

ACT-based interventions delivered in the Face-to-face group sessions or by the Mobile app showed beneficial effects on several aspects of reported eating behavior and were most pronounced in the Face-to-face group. However, the current general ACT intervention including only a minor mindful eating component is not enough to promote dietary changes. Thus, to affect diet, adding nutritional counseling to this form of therapy is suggested. Further studies on the effects of ACT-based skills that specifically target diet quality are needed. The ACT-based intervention could be a useful approach for people with overweight or obesity and difficulties in eating behavior. It is important to determine which populations would benefit most from face-to-face and mobile app interventions because both interventions could also be used in health care settings.

Additional files

Additional file 1: (26.8KB, pdf)

Table S1. The effects of ACT-based Face-to-face and Mobile interventions on diet quality. (PDF 26 kb)

Additional file 2: (27KB, pdf)

Table S2. Standardized estimates (standard error) and p-values for moderated intervention effects. (PDF 27 kb)

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all of the study participants for their valuable voluntary contribution. We would like to thank Urho Kujala, Sara Mutikainen and Tiina Föhr (Faculty of Sport and Health Sciences, University of Jyväskylä), Tero Myllymäki (Department of Psychology, University of Jyväskylä), Elina Mattila (VTT Technical Research Centre of Finland, Tampere, Finland), and Kristiina Juvonen (Institute of Public Health and Clinical Nutrition, Clinical Nutrition, University of Eastern Finland) who contributed to the Elixir study design and data collection; Eeva Lajunen, Erja Kinnunen, and Maritta Putkiranta for skillful assistance in managing the study visits; Iina Grönberg, Saara Vähäkylä (formerly Leino), Siniriikka Ventelä, and Raisa Harjula for skillful participation in conducting the practical period and handling dietary data; Sini Lähteenmäki for data handling and revision, as well as Marianna Koivisto, Heidi Kinnunen (formerly Aaltonen) and Saara Peuhkuri for handling dietary data. We would also like to thank Salla Muuraiskangas for managing the electronic questionnaires of the Elixir study and Anni Helldán (The National Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland) for giving advice on the 48-h dietary recall protocol development.

Funding

The Elixir study was supported by the SalWe Research Program for Mind and Body (Tekes – the Finnish Funding Agency for Technology and Innovation grant 1104/10). The preparation of this paper was also supported by grant from the Business Finland (no. 2726/31/2014), by grant from the Academy of Finland (no. 286028), by a personal grant (EJ-R) of The Diabetes Research Foundation, by a personal grant (EJ-R) of Finnish Cultural Foundation, North Savo Regional fund, and by the PhD student position (EJ-R) in the Doctoral Programme in Nutrition of the Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Eastern Finland. The funding sources did not have a role in the study design, data collection, data analysis, interpretation of the findings, writing of the article, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Availability of data and materials

Please contact the corresponding author to discuss availability of the datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study.

Abbreviations

ACT

Acceptance and commitment therapy

AUDIT-C

Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test Consumption questionnaire

BMI

Body mass index

E%

Percentage of energy

ecSI 2.0

preliminary Finnish translation of Satter Eating Competence Inventory 2.0

GHQ-12

12-item General Health Questionnaire

HLM

Hierarchical linear modeling

HTAS

Health and Taste Attitude Scales

IDQ

Index of Diet Quality

IES-1

Intuitive Eating Scale

MAR

missing at random

PSS

Perceived Stress Scale

REBS

Regulation of Eating Behavior Scale

TFEQ-R18

The 18-item Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire

Authors’ contributions

MK, RL, ME, RK, LK, JL, SP, KP and JP designed the trial. EJ-R, ES, SL and MH acquired the data. EJ-R and JM analyzed the data. EJ-R drafted the manuscript. MK and LK supervised drafting the manuscript. MK, RL, ME, RK, JL and SP were the principal investigators of the Elixir study. All of the authors participated in the interpretation of the findings and critically reviewed the manuscript. All of the authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Central Finland Health Care District (reference number 7 U/2012) and was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The participants gave their written informed consent before participating.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (10.1186/s12966-018-0654-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Contributor Information

Elina Järvelä-Reijonen, Email: elina.jarvela-reijonen@uef.fi.

Leila Karhunen, Email: leila.karhunen@uef.fi.

Essi Sairanen, Email: essi.sairanen@kau.se.

Joona Muotka, Email: joona.s.muotka@jyu.fi.

Sanni Lindroos, Email: sanni.m.lindroos@gmail.com.

Jaana Laitinen, Email: jaana.laitinen@ttl.fi.

Sampsa Puttonen, Email: sampsa.puttonen@ttl.fi.

Katri Peuhkuri, Email: katri.peuhkuri@netnut.fi.

Maarit Hallikainen, Email: maarit.hallikainen@uef.fi.

Jussi Pihlajamäki, Email: jussi.pihlajamaki@uef.fi.

Riitta Korpela, Email: riitta.korpela@helsinki.fi.

Miikka Ermes, Email: miikka.ermes@gmail.com.

Raimo Lappalainen, Email: raimo.lappalainen@jyu.fi.

Marjukka Kolehmainen, Email: marjukka.kolehmainen@uef.fi.

References

  • 1.Kelly MP, Barker M. Why is changing health-related behaviour so difficult? Public Health. 2016;136:109–116. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2016.03.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Samdal GB, Eide GE, Barth T, Williams G, Meland E. Effective behaviour change techniques for physical activity and healthy eating in overweight and obese adults; systematic review and meta-regression analyses. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2017;14:42. doi: 10.1186/s12966-017-0494-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Pelletier LG, Dion SC, Slovinec-D’Angelo M, Reid R. Why do you regulate what you eat? Relationships between forms of regulation, eating behaviors, sustained dietary behavior change, and psychological adjustment. Motiv Emot. 2004;28:245–277. doi: 10.1023/B:MOEM.0000040154.40922.14. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hayes SC, Luoma JB, Bond FW, Masuda A, Lillis J. Acceptance and commitment therapy: model, processes and outcomes. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44:1–25. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lillis J, Kendra KE. Acceptance and commitment therapy for weight control: model, evidence, and future directions. J Contextual Behav Sci. 2014;3:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2013.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Forman EM, Hoffman KL, McGrath KB, Herbert JD, Brandsma LL, Lowe MR. A comparison of acceptance- and control-based strategies for coping with food cravings: an analog study. Behav Res Ther. 2007;45:2372–2386. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Forman EM, Butryn ML, Hoffman KL, Herbert JD. An open trial of an acceptance-based behavioral intervention for weight loss. Cogn Behav Pract. 2009;16:223–235. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2008.09.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lillis J, Hayes SC, Bunting K, Masuda A. Teaching acceptance and mindfulness to improve the lives of the obese: a preliminary test of a theoretical model. Ann Behav Med. 2009;37:58–69. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9083-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Tapper K, Shaw C, Ilsley J, Hill AJ, Bond FW, Moore L. Exploratory randomised controlled trial of a mindfulness-based weight loss intervention for women. Appetite. 2009;52:396–404. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Forman EM, Butryn ML, Juarascio AS, Bradley LE, Lowe MR, Herbert JD, et al. The mind your health project: a randomized controlled trial of an innovative behavioral treatment for obesity. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2013;21:1119–1126. doi: 10.1002/oby.20169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Forman EM, Butryn ML, Manasse SM, Crosby RD, Goldstein SP, Wyckoff EP, et al. Acceptance-based versus standard behavioral treatment for obesity: results from the mind your health randomized controlled trial. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2016;24:2050–2056. doi: 10.1002/oby.21601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lillis J, Niemeier HM, Thomas JG, Unick J, Ross KM, Leahey TM, et al. A randomized trial of an acceptance-based behavioral intervention for weight loss in people with high internal disinhibition. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2016;24:2509–2514. doi: 10.1002/oby.21680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Palmeira L, Pinto-Gouveia J, Cunha M. Exploring the efficacy of an acceptance, mindfulness & compassionate-based group intervention for women struggling with their weight (kg-free): a randomized controlled trial. Appetite. 2017;112:107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.01.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wansink B. Environmental factors that increase the food intake and consumption volume of unknowing consumers. Annu Rev Nutr. 2004;24:455–479. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nutr.24.012003.132140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cohen DA. Neurophysiological pathways to obesity: below awareness and beyond individual control. Diabetes. 2008;57:1768–1773. doi: 10.2337/db08-0163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ruffault A, Czernichow S, Hagger MS, Ferrand M, Erichot N, Carette C, et al. The effects of mindfulness training on weight-loss and health-related behaviours in adults with overweight and obesity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Res Clin Pract. 2017;11(Suppl 1):90-111. 10.1016/j.orcp.2016.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 17.Jordan CH, Wang W, Donatoni L, Meier BP. Mindful eating: trait and state mindfulness predict healthier eating behavior. Personal Individ Differ. 2014;68:107–111. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.04.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Arch JJ, Brown KW, Goodman RJ, Della Porta MD, Kiken LG, Tillman S. Enjoying food without caloric cost: the impact of brief mindfulness on laboratory eating outcomes. Behav Res Ther. 2016;79:23–34. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kristeller JL, Wolever RQ. Mindfulness-based eating awareness training for treating binge eating disorder: the conceptual foundation. Eat Disord. 2011;19:49–61. doi: 10.1080/10640266.2011.533605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Fung TT, Long MW, Hung P, Cheung LW. An expanded model for mindful eating for health promotion and sustainability: issues and challenges for dietetics practice. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2016;116:1081–1086. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2016.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Tribole E, Resch E. Intuitive eating: a recovery book for the chronic dieter. New York: St. Martin’s Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Tylka TL. Development and psychometric evaluation of a measure of intuitive eating. J Couns Psychol. 2006;53:226–240. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.53.2.226. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Satter E. Eating competence: definition and evidence for the Satter eating competence model. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2007;39(Suppl 5):142–153. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2007.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lohse B, Bailey RL, Krall JS, Wall DE, Mitchell DC. Diet quality is related to eating competence in cross-sectional sample of low-income females surveyed in Pennsylvania. Appetite. 2012;58:645–650. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.11.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Van Dyke N, Drinkwater EJ. Relationships between intuitive eating and health indicators: literature review. Public Health Nutr. 2014;17:1757–1766. doi: 10.1017/S1368980013002139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Lohse B, Satter E, Horacek T, Gebreselassie T, Oakland MJ. Measuring eating competence: psychometric properties and validity of the ecSatter inventory. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2007;39(Suppl 5):154–166. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2007.04.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Herbert BM, Blechert J, Hautzinger M, Matthias E, Herbert C. Intuitive eating is associated with interoceptive sensitivity. Effects on body mass index. Appetite. 2013;70:22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.06.082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Krall JS, Lohse B. Validation of a measure of the Satter eating competence model with low-income females. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2011;8:26. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-8-26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bush HE, Rossy L, Mintz LB, Schopp L. Eat for life: a work site feasibility study of a novel mindfulness-based intuitive eating intervention. Am J Health Promot. 2014;28:380–388. doi: 10.4278/ajhp.120404-QUAN-186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Boucher S, Edwards O, Gray A, Nada-Raja S, Lillis J, Tylka TL, et al. Teaching intuitive eating and acceptance and commitment therapy skills via a web-based intervention: a pilot single-arm intervention study. JMIR Res Protoc. 2016;5:e180. doi: 10.2196/resprot.5861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Alberts HJ, Thewissen R, Raes L. Dealing with problematic eating behaviour. The effects of a mindfulness-based intervention on eating behaviour, food cravings, dichotomous thinking and body image concern. Appetite. 2012;58:847–851. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Daubenmier J, Lin J, Blackburn E, Hecht FM, Kristeller J, Maninger N, et al. Changes in stress, eating, and metabolic factors are related to changes in telomerase activity in a randomized mindfulness intervention pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:917–928. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Timmerman GM, Brown A. The effect of a mindful restaurant eating intervention on weight management in women. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2012;44:22–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2011.03.143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Karlsson J, Persson LO, Sjöström L, Sullivan M. Psychometric properties and factor structure of the three-factor eating questionnaire (TFEQ) in obese men and women. Results from the Swedish obese subjects (SOS) study. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2000;24:1715–1725. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Luxton DD, McCann RA, Bush NE, Mishkind MC, Reger GM. mHealth for mental health: integrating smartphone technology in behavioral healthcare. Prof Psychol Res Pr. 2011;42:505–512. doi: 10.1037/a0024485. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Price M, Yuen EK, Goetter EM, Herbert JD, Forman EM, Acierno R, et al. mHealth: a mechanism to deliver more accessible, more effective mental health care. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2014;21:427–436. doi: 10.1002/cpp.1855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Juarascio AS, Manasse SM, Goldstein SP, Forman EM, Butryn ML. Review of smartphone applications for the treatment of eating disorders. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2015;23:1–11. doi: 10.1002/erv.2327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Vandelanotte C, Müller AM, Short CE, Hingle M, Nathan N, Williams SL, et al. Past, present, and future of eHealth and mHealth research to improve physical activity and dietary behaviors. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2016;48:219–228. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2015.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Schoeppe S, Alley S, Van Lippevelde W, Bray NA, Williams SL, Duncan MJ, et al. Efficacy of interventions that use apps to improve diet, physical activity and sedentary behaviour: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2016;13:127. doi: 10.1186/s12966-016-0454-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Newman MG, Szkodny LE, Llera SJ, Przeworski A. A review of technology-assisted self-help and minimal contact therapies for anxiety and depression: is human contact necessary for therapeutic efficacy? Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31:89–103. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Johansson R, Andersson G. Internet-based psychological treatments for depression. Expert Rev Neurother. 2012;12:861–870. doi: 10.1586/ern.12.63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Richards D, Richardson T. Computer-based psychological treatments for depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2012;32:329–342. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Järvelä-Reijonen E, Karhunen L, Sairanen E, Rantala S, Laitinen J, Puttonen S, et al. High perceived stress is associated with unfavorable eating behavior in overweight and obese Finns of working age. Appetite. 2016;103:249–258. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.04.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lappalainen R, Sairanen E, Järvelä E, Rantala S, Korpela R, Puttonen S, et al. The effectiveness and applicability of different lifestyle interventions for enhancing wellbeing: the study design for a randomized controlled trial for persons with metabolic syndrome risk factors and psychological distress. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:310. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Goldberg D. The detection of psychiatric illness by questionnaire. Maudsley monograph no. 21. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1972. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Ahtinen A, Mattila E, Välkkynen P, Kaipainen K, Vanhala T, Ermes M, et al. Mobile mental wellness training for stress management: feasibility and design implications based on a one-month field study. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. 2013;1:e11. doi: 10.2196/mhealth.2596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Mattila E, Lappalainen R, Välkkynen P, Sairanen E, Lappalainen P, Karhunen L, et al. Usage and dose response of a mobile acceptance and commitment therapy app: secondary analysis of the intervention arm of a randomized controlled trial. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. 2016;4:e90. doi: 10.2196/mhealth.5241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Holi MM, Marttunen M, Aalberg V. Comparison of the GHQ-36, the GHQ-12 and the SCL-90 as psychiatric screening instruments in the Finnish population. Nord J Psychiatry. 2003;57:233–238. doi: 10.1080/08039480310001418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Anglé S, Engblom J, Eriksson T, Kautiainen S, Saha MT, Lindfors P, et al. Three factor eating questionnaire-R18 as a measure of cognitive restraint, uncontrolled eating and emotional eating in a sample of young Finnish females. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2009;6:41. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-6-41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Roininen K, Lähteenmäki L, Tuorila H. Quantification of consumer attitudes to health and hedonic characteristics of foods. Appetite. 1999;33:71–88. doi: 10.1006/appe.1999.0232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Roininen K, Tuorila H. Health and taste attitudes in the prediction of use frequency and choice between less healthy and more healthy snacks. Food Qual Prefer. 1999;10:357–365. doi: 10.1016/S0950-3293(98)00057-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Roininen K, Tuorila H, Zandstra EH, de Graaf C, Vehkalahti K, Stubenitsky K, et al. Differences in health and taste attitudes and reported behaviour among Finnish, Dutch and British consumers: a cross-national validation of the health and taste attitude scales (HTAS) Appetite. 2001;37:33–45. doi: 10.1006/appe.2001.0414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Krall JS, Lohse B. Cognitive testing with female nutrition and education assistance program participants informs validity of the Satter eating competence inventory. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2010;42:277–283. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2009.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Lohse B. The Satter eating competence inventory for low-income persons is a valid measure of eating competence for persons of higher socioeconomic position. Appetite. 2015;87:223–228. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.12.228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Leppälä J, Lagström H, Kaljonen A, Laitinen K. Construction and evaluation of a self-contained index for assessment of diet quality. Scand J Public Health. 2010;38:794–802. doi: 10.1177/1403494810382476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Bush K, Kivlahan DR, McDonell MB, Fihn SD, Bradley KA. The AUDIT alcohol consumption questions (AUDIT-C): an effective brief screening test for problem drinking. Arch Intern Med. 1998;158:1789–1795. doi: 10.1001/archinte.158.16.1789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Aalto M, Alho H, Halme JT, Seppä K. AUDIT and its abbreviated versions in detecting heavy and binge drinking in a general population survey. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;103:25–29. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Paturi M, Nieminen R, Reinivuo H, Ovaskainen M. Ruokien annoskuvakirja. Helsinki: the National Public Health Institute; 2006. p. B11.
  • 59.Helldán A, Raulio S, Kosola M, Tapanainen H, Ovaskainen M, Virtanen S. Finravinto 2012 -tutkimus – The National FINDIET 2012 Survey. THL Report. 2013;16.
  • 60.Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24:385–396. doi: 10.2307/2136404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Lee EH. Review of the psychometric evidence of the perceived stress scale. Asian Nurs Res. 2012;6:121–127. doi: 10.1016/j.anr.2012.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Sairanen E, Tolvanen A, Karhunen L, Kolehmainen M, Järvelä-Reijonen E, Lindroos S, et al. Psychological flexibility mediates change in intuitive eating regulation in acceptance and commitment therapy interventions. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20:1681–1691. doi: 10.1017/S1368980017000441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Kearney DJ, Milton ML, Malte CA, McDermott KA, Martinez M, Simpson TL. Participation in mindfulness-based stress reduction is not associated with reductions in emotional eating or uncontrolled eating. Nutr Res. 2012;32:413–420. doi: 10.1016/j.nutres.2012.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Miller CK, Kristeller JL, Headings A, Nagaraja H, Miser WF. Comparative effectiveness of a mindful eating intervention to a diabetes self-management intervention among adults with type 2 diabetes: a pilot study. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2012;112:1835–1842. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2012.07.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Mason AE, Epel ES, Kristeller J, Moran PJ, Dallman M, Lustig RH, et al. Effects of a mindfulness-based intervention on mindful eating, sweets consumption, and fasting glucose levels in obese adults: data from the SHINE randomized controlled trial. J Behav Med. 2016;39:201–213. doi: 10.1007/s10865-015-9692-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Levine DM, Savarimuthu S, Squires A, Nicholson J, Jay M. Technology-assisted weight loss interventions in primary care: a systematic review. J Gen Intern Med. 2015;30:107–117. doi: 10.1007/s11606-014-2987-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Litwin MS. How to measure survey reliability and validity. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Bingham S, Nelson M, Paul A, Haraldsdottir J, LØken E, van Staveren W. Methods for data collection at an individual level. In: Cameron M, van Staveren W, editors. Manual on methodology for food consumption studies. New York: Oxford University Press; 1988. pp. 53–106. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Männistö S, Ovaskainen M, Valsta L. Finravinto 2002 -tutkimus - The National FINDIET 2002 Study. Kansanterveyslaitoksen julkaisuja. 2003;B3/2003.
  • 71.McNaughton SA, Mishra GD, Bramwell G, Paul AA, Wadsworth MEJ. Comparability of dietary patterns assessed by multiple dietary assessment methods: results from the 1946 British birth cohort. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2005;59:341–352. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Maukonen M, Kanerva N, Partonen T, Kronholm E, Tapanainen H, Kontto J, et al. Chronotype differences in timing of energy and macronutrient intakes: a population-based study in adults. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2017;25:608–615. doi: 10.1002/oby.21747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Ocké MC, Larrañaga N, Grioni S, van den Berg SW, Ferrari P, Salvini S, et al. Energy intake and sources of energy intake in the European prospective investigation into cancer and nutrition. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2009;63(Suppl 4):3–15. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2009.72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Racette SB, Weiss EP, Schechtman KB, Steger-May K, Villareal DT, Obert KA, et al. Influence of weekend lifestyle patterns on body weight. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2008;16:1826–1830. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Additional file 1: (26.8KB, pdf)

Table S1. The effects of ACT-based Face-to-face and Mobile interventions on diet quality. (PDF 26 kb)

Additional file 2: (27KB, pdf)

Table S2. Standardized estimates (standard error) and p-values for moderated intervention effects. (PDF 27 kb)

Data Availability Statement

Please contact the corresponding author to discuss availability of the datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study.


Articles from The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES