Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2018 Feb 27;7:e31670. doi: 10.7554/eLife.31670

Early alterations of social brain networks in young children with autism

Holger Franz Sperdin 1,†,, Ana Coito 2,, Nada Kojovic 1, Tonia Anahi Rihs 2, Reem Kais Jan 2,3, Martina Franchini 1, Gijs Plomp 4, Serge Vulliemoz 5, Stephan Eliez 1, Christoph Martin Michel 2, Marie Schaer 1
Editor: Jan-Marino Ramirez6
PMCID: PMC5828667  PMID: 29482718

Abstract

Social impairments are a hallmark of Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD), but empirical evidence for early brain network alterations in response to social stimuli is scant in ASD. We recorded the gaze patterns and brain activity of toddlers with ASD and their typically developing peers while they explored dynamic social scenes. Directed functional connectivity analyses based on electrical source imaging revealed frequency specific network atypicalities in the theta and alpha frequency bands, manifesting as alterations in both the driving and the connections from key nodes of the social brain associated with autism. Analyses of brain-behavioural relationships within the ASD group suggested that compensatory mechanisms from dorsomedial frontal, inferior temporal and insular cortical regions were associated with less atypical gaze patterns and lower clinical impairment. Our results provide strong evidence that directed functional connectivity alterations of social brain networks is a core component of atypical brain development at early stages of ASD.

Research organism: Human

eLife digest

Newborns are attracted to voices, faces and social gestures. Paying attention to these social cues in everyday life helps infants and young children learn how to interact with others. During this period of development, a network of connections forms between different parts of the brain that helps children to understand other people’s social behaviors.

During their first year of life, infants who later develop autism spectrum disorders (ASD) pay less attention to social cues. This early indifference to these important signals leads to social deficits in children with ASD. They are less able to understand other people’s behaviors or engage in typical social interactions. It’s not yet clear why children with ASD are less attuned to social cues. But is likely that the development of brain networks essential for understanding social behavior suffers as a result. Studying how such networks develop in typical very young children and those with ASD may help scientist learn more.

Now, Sperdin et al. confirm there are differences in the social brain-networks of very young children with ASD compared with their typical peers. In the experiment, 3-year-old children with ASD and without watched videos of other children playing, while Sperdin et al. recorded what they looked at and what happened in their brains. Eyemovements were measured with a tracker, and the brain activity was recorded using an electroencephalogram (EEG), which uses sensors placed on the scalp to measure electrical signals.

What children with ASD looked at was different than their typical peers, and these differences corresponded with alterations in the brain networks that process social information. Children with ASD who had less severe symptoms had stronger activity in these brain networks. What they looked at also was more similar to typical children. This suggests less severely affected children with ASD may be able to compensate that way.

Identifying ASD-like behaviors and brain differences early in life may help scientists to better understand what causes the condition. It may also help clinicians provide more individualized therapies early in life when the brain is most adaptable. Long-term studies of these brain-network differences in children with ASD are necessary to better understand how therapies can influence these changes.

Introduction

Early preferential attention to social cues is a fundamental mechanism that facilitates interactions with other human beings. During the third trimester of gestation, the human foetus is already sensitive to both voices (DeCasper and Spence, 1986) and face-like stimuli (Reid et al., 2017). Newborns orient to biological motion (Simion et al., 2008) and prefer their mothers’ voices to those of other females (DeCasper and Fifer, 1980). Infants as young as two weeks imitate faces and human gestures (Meltzoff and Moore, 1977). The orientation to and interaction with social cues during infancy drives the later acquisition of social communication skills during toddler and preschool years. As function of experience, the repeated exposure leads to the progressive emergence of adaptive interactions with conspecifics. Alongside, the brain develops a network of cerebral regions specialized in understanding the social behaviours of others. This network includes the orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortices, the superior temporal cortex, the temporal poles, the amygdala, the precuneus, the temporo-parietal junction, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the insula (Brothers, 1990; Frith and Frith, 2010; Adolphs, 2009; Blakemore, 2008). Collectively, these areas form the social brain and are all implicated to some extent in processing social cues and encoding human social behaviours (Brothers, 1990; Frith and Frith, 2010; Adolphs, 2009; Blakemore, 2008).

Autism is a life-long lasting, highly prevalent neurodevelopmental disorder that affects core areas of cognitive and adaptive function, communication and social interactions (Christensen et al., 2016). A common observation in infants later diagnosed with ASD is the presence of less sensitivity and diminished preferential attention to social cues during the first year of life (Osterling and Dawson, 1994). Toddlers with ASD orient preferentially to non-social contingencies (Klin et al., 2009). Indifference to voices (Sperdin and Schaer, 2016) and faces (Grelotti et al., 2002) in ASD leads to deficits in the development of adapted social interactions with others and to difficulties in understanding human behaviours. It is not established why children with ASD show insensitivity to stimuli with social contingencies at early developmental stages, but this apparent indifference to social cues ultimately hinders the normal development of the social brain network or parts thereof (Pelphrey et al., 2011; Gotts et al., 2012). Some authors propose that deficits in the development of social cognition (such as learning to attribute mental states to others, 'theory of mind' [Frith, 1989]) and/or in sensory processing (Dinstein et al., 2012) prevent children with ASD to actively and appropriately engage with social stimuli. Another hypothesis suggests that they have difficulties building up stimulus-reward contingencies for social stimuli, due to a reduced motivation to attend and engage with them. Regardless of the reasons behind reduced social orienting, diminished interaction and exposure to social stimuli may in turn impede the development of the social brain at early developmental stages in ASD (Chevallier et al., 2012; Dawson et al., 2004).

Evidence remains limited for brain network alterations in response to socially meaningful stimuli in ASD during the period spanning the toddler to preschool years, partly because the acquisition of data during that age period is extremely challenging (Raschle et al., 2012). However, studying very young children with ASD, closer to their diagnosis, is all the more important when recent findings suggest the presence of major developmental changes in large-scale brain networks between adults and younger individuals with ASD (Nomi and Uddin, 2015). Currently, it remains unclear how autism affects the development of the functional brain networks implicated in the processing of socially meaningful information at early developmental stages. A better delineation of the timing and nature of the neurodevelopmental alterations associated with core social deficits in autism may in turn help to improve therapeutic strategies.

Electroencephalography (EEG) is as a powerful non-invasive method to study atypical brain responses to social stimuli in clinical paediatric populations with ASD. For example, surface-based experiments have reported aberrant evoked potentials in response to dynamic eye gaze in infants at high-risk for ASD (Elsabbagh et al., 2012) or to speech stimuli in toddlers with ASD (Kuhl et al., 2013) with differences in resting EEG power in infants at high-risk for ASD (Tierney et al., 2012). Whilst useful, most of the EEG experiments performed on very young children with ASD (younger than four years) have been done with few electrodes only and the analysis restricted to the sensor space. Therefore, hypothetical alterations in the functional brain networks underlying the processing of social stimuli remain to be determined for that age period in ASD.

Here, we recorded high-density EEG and high resolution eye-tracking in toddlers and preschoolers with ASD and their TD peers as they watched naturalistic and ecologically valid dynamic social movies. Using data-driven methods, we first investigated whether the visual exploration behaviour was atypical in toddlers and preschoolers with ASD using kernel density distribution estimations. Then, we explored whether their ongoing source-space directed functional connectivity was altered compared to their TD peers using Granger-causal modelling applied to the EEG source signals. This method estimates brain connectivity in the frequency domain. It identifies which brain regions are the key drivers of information flow in a brain network and directional relationships between brain regions that belong to a network (Coito et al., 2016b). This approach has been applied to study connectivity alterations using intra-cranial recordings (Wilke et al., 2009; van Mierlo et al., 2013; van Mierlo et al., 2011) as well as source imaging based on EEG recordings in clinical populations (Ding et al., 2007; Coito et al., 2016a; Coito et al., 2015; Coito et al., 2016b) and in healthy human participants (Astolfi et al., 2007; Hu et al., 2012; Plomp et al., 2015b). Finally, we looked for relationships between directed functional connectivity measures, visual exploration behaviour and clinical phenotype. As toddlers with ASD have less preferential attention for social cues, we hypothesized that they would show both a different visual exploration behaviour of the dynamic social images and altered directed functional connectivity patterns in brain regions involved in processing social information compared to their TD peers.

Results

Summed outflow

The summed outflow (i.e.- the amount of information transfer) is a measure that reflects the importance (i.e.-the amount of driving) of a given region of interest (ROI) in the network (see Materials and methods section). To understand the functional wiring and the dynamic flow underlying the processing of the dynamic social stimuli, we used a data-driven method to explore in which frequency band the highest summed outflow occurred in 82 ROIs across the whole brain. A ROI with a strong summed outflow has a key role in directing the activity towards other ROIs in the network. The strongest summed outflow across the whole brain occurred in the theta band (4–7 Hz) in both groups. The summed outflow of the largest drivers across frequencies is illustrated for each group in Figure 1a. As can be seen, the largest peaks of activity are present in the theta band range (4–7 Hz) in both groups followed by peaks of activity in the alpha band range (8–12 Hz). The global driving in the theta and alpha bands did not differ between groups (theta : df = 34, t = 0.6201, p = 0.536; alpha : df = 34, t = 0.1736, p = 0.8632). Driving in the theta band was higher compared to the driving in the alpha band in both groups (ASD: df = 17, t = 11.86, p < 0.0001; TD:df = 17, t = 8.025, p < 0.0001). Several regions common to both groups showed a large driving (summed outflow) in both frequency bands, and notably the bilateral medial frontal and superior orbitofrontal regions, the bilateral hippocampi, the bilateral ACC and the right amygdala (Figure 1b).

Figure 1. Summed outflow of the largest drivers across frequencies.

Figure 1.

(a) The summed outflow of the largest drivers across frequencies is illustrated for each group (TD, Left; ASD, Right). (b) Regions consistently showing large driving in both groups for theta and alpha. Summed outflows are represented as spheres: the larger the sphere, the higher the summed outflow. ROIs are displayed on an ICBM Average Brain, axial top view. See acronyms list in Table 2.

Thereafter, we characterized the differences in the summed outflow across all brain regions between the groups in the theta band and in the alpha band, separately. For the theta band, we identified six ROIs that showed a statistically higher driving (stronger summed outflow) in the ASD group in comparison to the TD group (Mann-Whitney-Wilcoxon test, two-tailed, p<0.05): the right orbital part of the superior frontal gyrus (Ws = 267, z = −2.088. p=0.037, r = −0.348), the bilateral orbital parts of the middle frontal gyri (Left: Ws = 259, z = −2.341, p=0.019, r = −0.39; Right: Ws = 252, z = −2.563, p=0.01, r = −0.427), the right middle cingulate gyrus (Ws = 259, z = −2.341, p=0.019, r = −0.390), the left superior occipital gyrus (Ws = 270, z = - 1.993, p=0.047, r = −0.332), and the left superior temporal gyrus (STG) (Ws = 255, z = −2.468, p=0.013, r = −0.411) (Figure 2a). This indicates the presence of a stronger driving from these regions in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD compared to their TD peers when viewing the dynamic social images. For the alpha band, we identified three ROIs that had a different driving in the ASD group in comparison to the TD group (Mann-Whitney-Wilcoxon test, two-tailed, p<0.05). The the right orbital part of the middle frontal gyrus (Ws = 262, z = −2.246, p=0.024, r = −0.374) and the left cuneus (Ws = 265, z = −2.151, p=0.031, r = −0.358) had a higher driving and the right STG had a weaker driving (Ws = 265, z = −2.151, p=0.031, r = −0.358) (Figure 2a). The boxplots with the summed outflow values for each group and for each of the significant ROIs are displayed in Figure 2b for theta and Figure 2c for alpha.

Figure 2. Summed outflow differences in the ASD group compared to their typically developing peers.

Figure 2.

(a) On the left, the 6 ROIs with a statistically significant different summed outflow in the ASD group compared to their TD peers for the theta band. On the right, the 3 ROIs with a statistically significant different summed outflow in the ASD group compared to their TD peers for the alpha band. A red nod indicates increased driving, a blue nod indicates decreased driving. Corresponding ROIs are displayed on an ICBM Average Brain, with sagital, axial and corronal views. (b) Boxplots with the summed outflow values comparing each group for each significant ROI in the theta band. (c) Boxplots with the summed outflow values comparing each group for each significant ROI in the alpha band. The boxplots display the full range of variation of the summed outflows (from min to max), rectangles span the interquartile range and the median. See acronyms list in Table 2.

Region-to-region directed functional connectivity

We looked for differences in the region-to region directed functional connectivity using Granger-causal modelling (see Materials and methods section) from each of the six nodes for the theta band, and from each of the three nodes in the alpha band separately in both groups. In the theta band, all the connections from the six ROIs in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD were stronger than the strongest connections in the TD participants (Mann − Whitney − Wilcoxon, two − tailed, p < 0.05, Benjamini − Hochberg = 0.05). This suggests the presence of hyper-connectivity in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD in theta. The region-to-region directed functional connectivity from the six ROIs in theta is illustrated in Figure 3. The estimation of the region-to-region directed connectivity (i.e. to which other ROIs the activity was directed) also revealed different network patterns for all the six ROIs in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD compared to their TD peers. The boxplots of the outflow values of the connections from the right orbital part of the superior frontal gyrus seed region are provided in Figure 3—figure supplement 2 for the ASD group and Figure 3—figure supplement 2 for the TD group. In the alpha band, the region-to-region directed functional connectivity analysis revealed stronger connections from the right orbital part of the middle frontal gyrus and the left cuneus whereas the right STG had weaker connection in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD compared to their TD peers (Mann-Whitney-Wilcoxon, two-tailed, p<0.05, Benjamini-Hochberg = 0.05). Similarly to what we found in the theta band, all three significant ROIs in the alpha band had different network patterns in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD compared to their TD peers (Figure 4).

Figure 3. Region-to-region directed functional connectivity for the theta band (4–7 Hz) from each of the six significant ROIs represented as large red spheres.

Outflows are represented as arrows: the larger the arrow, the stronger the outflow. ROIs and connections are displayed on an ICBM Average Brain, axial and coronal views. See acronyms list in Table 2.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. ORBsup.R seed (large red sphere) for the ASD group in theta band.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Boxplots with the outflow values comparing each group for each connection. Outflows are represented as arrows: the larger the arrow, the stronger the outflow. The boxplots display the full range of variation (from min to max) of the outflow values, rectangles span the interquartile range and the median. See acronyms list in Table 2.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. ORBsup.R seed for the (large red sphere) for the TD group in theta band.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

Boxplots with the outflow values comparing each group for each connection. Outflows are represented as arrows: the larger the arrow, the stronger the outflow. The boxplots display the full range of variation (from min to max) of the outflow values, rectangles span the interquartile range and the median. See acronyms list in Table 2.

Figure 4. Region-to-region directed functional connectivity for the alpha band (8–12 Hz) from each of the three significant ROIs, represented as large red spheres.

Figure 4.

Outflows are represented as arrows: the larger the arrow, the stronger the outflow. ROIs and connections are displayed on an ICBM Average Brain, axial and coronal views. See acronyms list in Table 2.

Correlations with ADOS-2, PEP-3, VABS-II and gaze Proximity Index

We further explored associations between the summed outflow in the theta and alpha bands from the ROIs and clinical and behavioural phenotypes (Spearman − rho, two − tailed, p <0.05, Benjamini − Hochberg = 0.05). None of the correlations between the summed outflow and ADOS-2 severity scores survived False discovery rate (FDR) correction for either bands (Benjamini − Hochberg = 0.05). For the summed outflow in the theta band, we found strong positive correlations between the summed outflow in the right lingual area and standard scores from the socialization domain (rs = 0.751, N = 18, p=0.0003, two-tailed, <0.05; Benjamini-Hochberg = 0.05) as well as with standard scores from the leisure and play skills subdomain of the VABS-II (rs = 0.802, N = 18, p=0.0001, two-tailed, <0.05; Benjamini-Hochberg = 0.05). None of the correlations between the summed outflow and VABS-II standard scores survived FDR correction for the alpha band (Benjamini-Hochberg = 0.05). Higher summed outflow within the left Heschl area near the posterior convolutions of the insula and the left rolandic operculum near the circular sulcus of the insula rostrally were positively related to better fine (rs = 0.745, N = 18, p=0.0004, two-tailed, <0.05; Benjamini-Hochberg = 0.05) and gross motor skills (rs = 0.744, N = 18, p=0.0004, two-tailed, <0.05; Benjamini-Hochberg = 0.05) as measured by the PEP-3. For the alpha band, higher summed outflow within the left hippocampus and the left rolandic operculum were positively related to better fine (rs = 0.736, N = 18, p=0.0005, two-tailed, <0.05; Benjamini-Hochberg = 0.05) and gross motor skills (rs = 0.737, N = 18, p=0.0005, two-tailed, <0.05; Benjamini-Hochberg = 0.05) as measured by the PEP-3. The toddlers and preschoolers with ASD with a gaze pattern similar to their TD peers showed an increased driving in theta within the left middle cingulate cortex (rs = 0.726, N = 18, p=0.0007, two-tailed, <0.05; Benjamini-Hochberg = 0.05) and the right paracentral lobule (rs = 0.738, p = 0.0005, two − tailed, <0.05; Benjamini − Hochberg = 0.05). There was no significant relationship between the Proximity Index (see Materials and methods section) and the summed outflow in the alpha band after FDR correction. The significant correlations between the summed outflows and the Proximity Index, VABS-II standard scores and PEP-3 standard scores for each frequency band are displayed in Figure 5. Finally, we explored associations between gaze performance with developmental scores obtained from the PEP-3 and with adaptive scores obtained from the VABS-II (D'Agostino-Pearson omnibus normality test, K2, p<0.05; Pearson’s r, two-tailed, p<0.05). We didn't find any significant correlations between the Proximity Index and the global level of autistic severity as measured with the calibrated ADOS-2 severity score. However, we found that the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD with a better gaze performance had better global adaptive functioning as measured by the VABS-II (K2 = 3.339, p=0.1883; r = 0.578, p=0.012), which was driven by better global (K2 = 0.8179, p=0.6643; r = 0.575, p=0.013) and fine (K2 = 0.5438, p=0.7619; r = 0.509, p=0.031) motor skills, and better development of interpersonal relationships (K2 = 5.308, p=0.0704; r = 0.581, p=0.011). We also found that these children with a better gaze performance had better visual motor imitation skills (K2 = 2.671, p=0.263; r = 0.534, p=0.022) as measured by the PEP-3.

Figure 5. Significant correlations after FDR correction between the summed outflow in theta for the ASD participants (represented as blue dots) and (a) standardized VABS-II scores, (b) PEP-3 standardized scores and (c) Proximity Index.

Figure 5.

(d) Significant correlations after FDR correction between the summed outflow in alpha and PEP-3 standardized scores. TD summed outflow values are plotted on the Y axis in red. Corresponding ROI are displayed on an ICBM Average Brain, axial top view.

Discussion

Abnormal processing of social cues is a hallmark of ASD (Chevallier et al., 2012; Dawson et al., 2004; Dichter et al., 2009; Elsabbagh et al., 2012; Gotts et al., 2012; Greene et al., 2011; Klin et al., 2009; Pelphrey et al., 2011). However, evidence for alterations of social brain networks at early stages of ASD is scant. Using data-driven methods, we observed aberrant gaze patterns together with frequency specific alterations in the directed functional connectivity in toddlers and preschoolers with ASD when exploring dynamic social stimuli compared to their TD peers. These differences manifested as increased driving and hyper-connectivity in the theta frequency band from nodes that include the right orbital part of the superior frontal gyrus, the bilateral orbital parts of the middle frontal gyri, the right middle cingulate gyrus, the left superior occipital gyrus and the left STG. For the alpha band, we found increased driving from the right orbital part of the middle frontal gyrus and the left cuneus and decreased connectivity from the right STG. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first evidence indicating concomitant alterations in the visual exploration of dynamic social images and in the directed functional connectivity involving key nodes of the social brain (Brothers, 1990; Frith and Frith, 2010; Adolphs, 2009; Blakemore, 2008) at early stages of ASD.

The results indicate that the highest information transfer (i.e. summed outflow) occurs at the global brain level in the theta band (4–7 Hz) followed by the alpha frequency band (8–12 Hz). As such, our data-driven approach reveals high information transfer in physiologically relevant frequency bands given the young age of our participants. These are, namely, prominent brain rhythms during infancy and toddlerhood (Saby and Marshall, 2012; Orekhova et al., 2006). Throughout development, slow waves modulate attentional brain states, encode specific information and ease communication between neuronal populations (Lopes da Silva, 2013). Theta and alpha bands are thought to underlie different cerebral functions, but are closely related (Klimesch, 1999). During infancy and early childhood, modulations in alpha band activity have been associated with the progressive development of visual attentional networks and inhibition of task-irrelevant brain areas (Orekhova et al., 2001; Stroganova et al., 1999), while theta is thought to play a functional role in memory formation, emotional and cognitive functioning (Saby and Marshall, 2012; Orekhova et al., 2006).

In our experiment, the highest information transfer occurred in the theta band. In very young children, theta modulations have been related to the development of the social brain. For example, surface-based EEG experiments in TD infants report enhanced theta power to social versus non-social stimuli at 12 months (Jones et al., 2015). Theta increases during attention to social stimulation in infants and preschool aged children (Orekhova et al., 2006). Hence, social contingencies modulate theta band activity. Similarly, our results show high information transfer in theta when toddlers and preschoolers visually explore dynamic social stimuli.

The development of attentional processes in young children has been associated with modulations in alpha band activity (Orekhova et al., 2001). Conversely, our young participants had to deploy their attentional focus to the dynamic social stimuli. This would explain why high information transfer was also found in the alpha frequency band.

EEG experiments in individuals with ASD show a reduction or an increase in coherence patterns in the theta and/or alpha frequency bands compared to their TD peers at various ages and under different experimental conditions (Schwartz et al., 2017). However, most of the available EEG experiments performed on very young children with ASD and analysis thereof were so far restricted to the scalp surface. As a result, information remains limited regarding the presence of frequency specific alterations within brain regions when young children are exposed to social stimuli. Here, our data-driven source-space approach revealed not only high information transfer in the theta and alpha frequency bands, but also, the involvement of the bilateral medial and the superior orbital frontal regions, the bilateral hippocampi, the bilateral ACC and right amygdala. These areas are implicated in processing social cues and encoding human social behaviours (Brothers, 1990; Frith and Frith, 2010; Adolphs, 2009; Blakemore, 2008).

Our results further indicate the presence of frequency specific alterations in the driving from several brain areas in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD compared to their TD peers. In the theta band, we found an overall dominant higher driving within several frontal and the cingulate regions. In TD individuals, theta generates within the frontal cortices or the ACC (Asada et al., 1999). In comparison to their TD peers, both these areas develop differently in young toddlers later diagnosed with ASD (Schumann et al., 2010). Accordingly, our results raise the possibility that the brain regions generating theta also follow a different development in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD. For the alpha band, alteration in the driving was evident from the right orbital part of the middle frontal gyrus. Although experiments during the first three years of life are currently sparse, increased alpha-range EEG connectivity over frontal and central electrodes has recently been reported in high-risk infants who were diagnosed with ASD at 36 month (Orekhova et al., 2014). A magnetoencephalography (MEG) study performed at rest using a source-space approach reported lower coherence in the theta and alpha bands within parietal and occipital regions but their ASD group only included adolescents (Ye et al., 2014). The differences between this specific study and our could stem from either variations in the age groups (adolescents versus toddlers and preschoolers), the stimuli used (grey cross inside a white circle versus dynamic biological visual stimuli) or the methods. More generally, several factors explain discrepancies in brain connectivity results between studies. The type of connectivity measures applied, the approach (surface versus source based), the brain regions analysed and frequency bands examined are variables that influence the results or the age of the participants (Mohammad-Rezazadeh et al., 2016).

Frontal and cingulate areas have been implicated in various complex aspects of social cognition, social reward, social perception and social behaviour (Jonker et al., 2015; Apps et al., 2013). Metabolic changes within the medial prefrontal cortex and the cingulate cortex are correlated with social interaction impairments in childhood ASD (Ohnishi et al., 2000). Several experiments report structural (Patriquin et al., 2016) and functional (Gotts et al., 2012; Patriquin et al., 2016; Greene et al., 2011; Cheng et al., 2015) alterations within these brain areas in school aged children, adolescents and adults with ASD when compared to their TD peers. A recent study described hyper-connectivity within the ACC and bilateral insular cortices in a sample including children aged between seven to 12 years (Uddin et al., 2013a). Some authors propose that the two together form the salience network, whose role is to direct attention to behaviourally-relevant stimuli (Menon and Uddin, 2010). Although we didn’t find differences in the driving from the insula compared to the TD peers, there is an increasing number of evidence showing an abnormal development of the salience network or components thereof in ASD (Uddin, 2015), which may partially explain the limited interest for and engagement with social stimuli that is often observed in individuals with ASD and that constitutes a hallmark of the disorder (Klin et al., 2009; Pelphrey et al., 2011). Accordingly, the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD had a different visual exploration behaviour of the dynamic social stimuli raising the possibility of a reduced interest to visually engage with them. Alternatively, alterations in driving from these regions could partially reflect a reduced motivation to attend and engage with the dynamic social stimuli (Chevallier et al., 2012; Dawson et al., 2004). The alterations in the driving in the alpha band we found here, could also be related to the presence of developmental impairments in attentional networks and/or inhibitory functions (Keehn et al., 2013).

We found higher driving in theta from the left superior temporal and occipital gyri. In the alpha band, we found alterations in the driving from a node in the right STG and the left cuneus in the occipital lobe. Those brain areas are implicated in the processing of biological motion, in analysing the intentions of other people’s actions and self-reflection (Pelphrey and Carter, 2008; Pelphrey et al., 2005; Pelphrey and Morris, 2006; Pelphrey et al., 2004). Our result would suggest that the exploration of the dynamic social visual stimuli that contained biological movements led to altered driving from these brain areas in both frequency bands in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD compared to their TD peers.

Overall hyper-connectivity seems prevalent in younger populations (that is, infants at high-risk for ASD, toddlers and preschoolers with ASD) while hypo-connectivity is more observed during adolescence and adulthood in ASD (Uddin et al., 2013b). Conversely, a developmental shift occurs in brain growth with an initial period of early brain overgrowth followed by normalization sometime during adolescence (Courchesne et al., 2011). Accordingly, structural white matter connectivity studies also highlight this shift from higher structural connectivity in very young children with ASD to lower connectivity in older children with ASD (Hoppenbrouwers et al., 2014; Conti et al., 2015). Therefore, a global higher-driving and hyper-connectivity from key nodes of the social brain in the theta frequency band in our ASD group is consistent with reports in the literature when considering the very young age of our participants (around 3 years of age on average). For the alpha frequency band, we found alterations in the driving manifesting as both hyper-connectivity from a frontal and an occipital area and under-connectivity from a node in the superior temporal pole. Hence, we found frequency-specific network alterations with distinct patterns of directed functional connectivity in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD compared to their TD peers. This result is in line with recent experiments indicating the presence of distinct patterns of hyper- and hypo-connectivity between brain regions functionally defined by neural oscillatory activity in children and adolescents with ASD (Ye et al., 2014; Kitzbichler et al., 2015; Datko et al., 2016).

We further explored associations between the driving in the nodes of the network (that is, the summed outflow) and clinical and behavioural phenotypes for each frequency band. We didn’t observe any significant relationships between summed outflow and the ADOS severity scores after FDR correction in either frequency bands. In theta, we observed an increased driving from the median cingulate cortex and the paracentral lobule in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD who had a more similar visual exploration pattern to their TD peers. Thus, an improved visual exploration pattern of the dynamic social images was related to increased summed outflow in theta from these regions. Moreover, higher summed outflow from the right lingual area was related to better socialization behaviour and leisure and play skills as measured by the VABS-II. Higher summed outflow from the left Heschl's area near the posterior convolutions of the insula and the left rolandic operculum near the circular sulcus of the insula rostrally were positively related to better fine and gross motor skills as measured by the PEP-3. Finally, for the alpha band, we found that higher driving within the left hippocampal area and the left rolandic operculum were positively related to better fine and gross motor skills as measured by the PEP-3. As such, overall increased activity in the theta band within dorsomedial frontal, inferior temporal and insular cortical regions were associated with lower clinical impairment and less atypical gaze patterns whereas increased driving in the alpha band was selectively associated with better motor performance. The presence of hyperactivity within relevant brain region has been interpreted as a possible compensatory mechanism when performing a social target detection task, in adults with ASD at least (Dichter et al., 2009). While to the best of our knowledge, there is currently no other relevant experimental data that addresses this question, we speculate that the overall hyper-driving from these relevant brain regions might be a mechanism to compensate for atypical development of the brain's circuitry over time as higher directed functional connectivity was related improved socialization, motor behaviours and better visual exploration of dynamic social images. However, longitudinal measurements are necessary to fully confirm this interpretation. De facto, the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD who had better gaze performance had better adaptive behaviour, improved global and fine motor skills and enriched interpersonal relationships as measured by the VABS-II and better visual motor imitation skills as measured by the PEP-3. They were also those who had higher summed outflow in several relevant brain areas.

Beyond functional and structural brain alterations reported elsewhere in older children and adults with ASD, our results suggest for the first time, the presence of frequency specific alterations in the driving of information flow from brain areas implicated in social information processing during the viewing of naturalistic dynamic social images in toddlers and preschool with ASD. Furthermore, we show that these frequency specific directed functional connectivity network alterations within regions of the social brain are present at early stages of ASD, justifying further investigation into how early therapeutic interventions targeting social orienting skills may help to remediate social brain development during this critical age period when plasticity is still possible. Longitudinal experiments on very young children with ASD are critically needed to better delineate modulations in brain patterns at the time of diagnosis, and how these alterations are influenced by therapeutic intervention. The present experiment is a first step towards that direction.

Materials and methods

Participants

Recruitment of toddlers and preschoolers with ASD was achieved via clinical centres specialized in ASD and French-speaking parent associations. TD toddlers and preschoolers were recruited via announcements in the Geneva community. Prior to the experiments, all the procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Geneva Hospital in accordance with the ethical standards proclaimed in the Declaration of Helsinki. For all participants, an interview over the phone and a medical developmental history questionnaire were completed before their initial visit. All participants' parents gave their informed consent prior to inclusion in the study. 120 participants were recruited for the experiment. We did not manage to put the EEG cap on the head of 23 ASD and 7 TD participants. We managed to put the cap on 90 participants. Out of those, we excluded 28 ASD and 26 TD participants because of too many movement-related artefacts, unrepairable noisy signal, lack of interest, or insufficient amounts of epochs available for subsequent analysis. This was to be expected given the extremely sensitive population at study here. As a result, 36 participants were included: 18 young children with ASD (2 females; mean age 3.1 years ± 0.8, range 2.2–4.4) and 18 age matched (df = 34, t = 2.72, p=0.852) TD peers (5 females; mean age 3.1 years ± 0.9, range 2.0–4.8). All participants with ASD included in the study received a clinical diagnosis prior to their inclusion in the research protocol. Diagnosis of ASD was rigorously verified and confirmed with either the Autism Diagnosis Observation Schedule-Generic (Lord et al., 2000) or the Autism Diagnosis Observation Schedule, second edition (ADOS-2) (Luyster et al., 2009). The latter contains a toddler module that defines concern for ASD. ADOS assessments were administered and scored by experienced clinicians working at the institution and specialized in ASD identification. In order to compare scores from different modules, we transformed the ADOS-G scores into Calibrated Severity Scores (ADOS-CSS) (Gotham et al., 2009). For the participants that underwent the ADOS-2-toddler module, we calibrated the scores into Severity Scores (Esler et al., 2015). Five children under 30 months of age performed the toddler module of the ADOS-2. All scored in the moderate to severe range of concern for ASD. For all the participants younger than 3 years of age (n = 10) at the EEG acquisition, clinical diagnosis was confirmed after one year by a clinician specialized in ASD identification using the ADOS-G or ADOS-2. The mean global ADOS-CSS for the entire group of patients with ASD was 7.9 (SD = 1.6). The assessment of the participants with ASD also included the administration of additional clinical standardized tests. Adaptive behaviour was assessed using the Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scale-II (VABS-II) (Sparrow et al., 2005), a standardized parent report interview. Developmental level was assessed with the Psycho-educational Profile Third Edition (PEP-3) (Lansing et al., 2005). See Table 1 for characteristics of study participants. Prior to their inclusion in our research protocol, potential TD participants were initially screened for neurological/psychiatric problems and learning disabilities using a medical and developmental history questionnaire before their visit. Moreover, they underwent ADOS-G or ADOS-2 evaluations to exclude any ASD symptomatology. Fourteen controls were tested with Modules 1 or 2 and four underwent the toddler module of the ADOS-2. All TD participants had a minimal severity score of 1, except one child who had a score of 3.

Table 1. Characteristics of Study Participants.

Characteristic Autism spectrum disorder Typically developing
Gender ratio (M/F) 16/2 13/5
Mean, SD, N Mean, SD, N T value df P value
Age in years 3.1, 0.8, 18 3.1, 0.9, 18 0.165 34 0.87
ADOS CSS 7.9, 1.6, 18 1.1, 0.47, 18 17.87 34 0.000
PEP-Cognitive verbal/pre-verbal 67.78, 18.85, 18 95.81, 7, 16 −5.87 32 0.000
PEP-Expressive language 50.28, 27.49, 18 92.94, 8.61, 16 −6.24 32 0.000
PEP-Receptive language 60.06, 23.93, 18 96.19, 6.17, 16 −6.17 32 0.000
PEP-Fine motor 61.83, 23.59, 18 88.81, 16.01, 16 −3.85 32 0.000
PEP-Gross motor 59.33, 27.86, 18 90.56, 7.66, 16 −4.56 32 0.000
PEP-Visual Motor Imitation 56.11, 25.29, 18 93.69, 6.93, 16 −6.05 32 0.000
VABS-II-Adaptive Behaviour Composite 75.5, 10.73, 18 105.28, 10.19, 18 −8.53 34 0.000
VABS-II-Communication 76.5, 12.59, 18 107.28, 8.16, 18 −8.7 34 0.000
VABS-II-Daily living skills 79.94, 11.28, 18 103.56, 9.28, 18 −6.85 34 0.000
VABS-II-Socialization 74.67, 11.26, 18 102.89, 6.98, 18 −9.03 34 0.000
VABS-II-Motor Skills 83.56, 10.8, 18 101.44, 12.15, 18 −4.66 34 0.000
VABS-II-receptive language 10, 2.45, 18 16.89, 2.32, 18 −8.65 34 0.000
VABS-II-expressive language 10.11, 2.4, 18 16.56, 1.5, 18 −9.65 34 0.000
VABS-II-gross motor skills 13.83, 5.53, 18 14.89, 1.78, 18 −0.77 34 0.449
VABS-II-fine motor skills 12.06, 2.58, 18 15.61, 2.45, 18 −4.23 34 0.000
VABS-II-interpersonal relationships 10.06, 2.6, 18 15.83, 2.26, 18 −7.12 34 0.000
VABS-II-play and leisure time 10.33, 2.03, 18 17, 1.68, 18 −10.73 34 0.000

Table 2. Acronyms Table.

1 PreCG.L Precentral Gyrus Left
2 PreCG.R Precentral Gyrus Right
3 SFGdor.L Frontal Superior Left
4 SFGdor.R Frontal Superior Right
5 ORBsup.L Frontal Superior Orbital Left
6 ORBsup.R Frontal Superior Orbital Right
7 MFG.L Frontal Middle Left
8 MFG.R Frontal Middle Right
9 ORBmid.L Frontal Middle Orbital Left
10 ORBmid.R Frontal Middle Orbital Right
11 IFGoperc.L Frontal Inferior Operculum Left
12 IFGoperc.R Frontal Inferior Operculum Right
13 IFGtriang.L Frontal Inferior Triangularis Left
14 IFGtriang.R Frontal Inferior Triangularis Right
15 ORBinf.L Frontal Inferior Orbital Left
16 ORBinf.R Frontal Inferior Orbital Right
17 ROL.L Rolandic Operculum Left
18 ROL.R Rolandic Operculum Right
19 SMA.L Supplementary Motor Area Left
20 SMA.R Supplementary Motor Area Left
21 OLF.L Olfactory Left
22 OLF.R Olfactory Right
23 SFGmed.L Frontal Superior Medial Left
24 SFGmed.R Frontal Superior Medial Right
25 ORBsupmed.L Frontal Medial Orbital Left
26 ORBsupmed.R Frontal Medial Orbital Right
27 REC.L Rectus Left
28 REC.R Rectus Right
29 INS.L Insula Left
30 INS.R Insula Right
31 ACG.L Cingulum Anterior Left
32 ACG.R Cingulum Anterior Right
33 DCG.L Cingulum Middle Left
34 DCG.R Cingulum Middle Right
35 PCG.L Cingulum Posterior Left
36 PCG.R Cingulum Posterior Right
37 HIP.L Hippocampus Left
38 HIP.R Hippocampus Right
39 PHG.L ParaHippocampal Left
40 PHG.R ParaHippocampal Right
41 AMYG.L Amygdala Left
42 AMYG.R Amygdala Right
43 CAL.L Calcarine Left
44 CAL.R Calcarine Right
45 CUN.L Cuneus Left
46 CUN.R Cuneus Right
47 LING.L Lingual Left
48 LING.R Lingual Right
49 SOG.L Occipital Superior Left
50 SOG.R Occipital Superior Right
51 MOG.L Occipital Middle Left
52 MOG.R Occipital Middle Right
53 IOG.L Occipital Inferior Left
54 IOG.R Occipital Inferior Right
55 FFG.L Fusiform Left
56 FFG.R Fusiform Right
57 PoCG.L Postcentral Left
58 PoCG.R Postcentral Right
59 SPG.L Parietal Superior Left
60 SPG.R Parietal Superior Right
61 IPL.L Parietal Inferior Left
62 IPL.R Parietal Inferior Right
63 SMG.L SupraMarginal Left
64 SMG.R SupraMarginal Right
65 ANG.L Angular Left
66 ANG.R Angular Right
67 PCUN.L Precuneus Left
68 PCUN.R Precuneus Right
69 PCL.L Paracentral Lobule Left
70 PCL.R Paracentral Lobule Right
71 HES.L Heschl Left
72 HES.R Heschl Right
73 STG.L Temporal Superior Left
74 STG.R Temporal Superior Right
75 TPOsup.L Temporal Pole Superior Left
76 TPOsup.R Temporal Pole Superior Right
77 MTG.L Temporal Middle Left
78 MTG.R Temporal Middle Right
79 TPOmid.L Temporal Pole Middle Left
80 TPOmid.R Temporal Pole Middle Right
81 ITG.L Temporal Inferior Left
82 ITG.R Temporal Inferior Right

Stimuli

Stimuli consisted of two video sequences of dynamic social images without audio information of approximatively two minutes each. These videos included ecologically valid and complex naturalistic dynamic images where young children practised yoga alone, imitated animal-like behaviours (behaving like a monkey or jumping like a frog), waived their arms, struck a pose, jumped, made faces or whistled (Yoga Kids 3 ; Gaiam, Boulder, Colorado, http://www.gaiam.com, created by Marsha Wenig, http://yogakids.com/). Presentation and timing of stimuli were controlled by Tobii Studio software (Sweden, http://www.tobii.com).

Procedure and task

The experiment was conducted in a lit room at the office Médico-Pédagogique in Geneva. To familiarize the child with the procedure, the families received a kit containing a custom-made EEG replica cap and pictures illustrating the protocol two weeks prior to their first visit. Participants were seated on their parents lap in order to make them feel as secure as possible and to minimize head and body movements or alone. Once seated, the experimenter measured the circumference of the head and placed the corresponding cap on the participant's head. A couple of minutes were taken in order to allow the participants to settle into the experiment's environment and get used to the cap before starting the experiment. Following this, a five point eye-tracking calibration procedure was initiated using the Tobii system (Sweden, http://www.tobii.com). An attractive colourful object (either a kitten, a bus, a duck, a dog or a toy) was presented together with its corresponding sound on a white background and the participants had to follow the object visually. The recording and presentation of the visual stimuli started when a minimum of four calibration points were acquired for each eye. To best capture the child’s attention, we first showed them an age-appropriate animated cartoon, followed by some fractals and another animated cartoon. The block ended with a film containing dynamic social images, the condition of interest in the present experiment. All participants were presented with the same visual stimuli in the same order. Following the first block, impedances were rechecked and electrodes were readjusted where needed to maintain them below 40 kOhm. A second block was then acquired (animated cartoon; animated fractals; animated cartoon; second condition of interest: dynamic social images). The experimenter continuously monitored the eye-tracking to ensure children were looking at the screen. The whole experiment lasted about half an hour. We used stringent criteria and only participants with the highest data quality were kept for subsequent analysis.

Eye-tracking measurements

Eye-tracking data were recorded with the TX300 Tobii eye-tracking system (sampling rate resolution of 300 Hz). In order to analyse and quantify differences in visual exploration between our groups, we developed a data-driven method to define dynamic norms of the exploration of the visual scenes (Kojovic et al., in preparation). First, we applied a kernel density distribution estimation (Botev et al., 2010) on the eye-tracking data recorded from the TD group at each time frame of the films containing dynamic complex social images to compute a normative gaze distribution pattern. Then, for each of the participants with ASD individually, we computed a deviation index from this normative gaze distribution, and this, for each single time frame separately (Figure 6). We averaged these values across the two films to obtain a mean Proximity Index (PI) value. This index describes for a given ASD participant, his distance from the normative gaze distribution pattern calculated on the TD group. A high index value indicates a visual behaviour approaching the visual exploration of the TD participants (more similarity), while a low index indicates a visual behaviour deviating from the TD group (more dissimilarity).

Figure 6. Exemplar single time frame of the normative gaze pattern for each group on one random time frame.

Figure 6.

Each dot represents the gaze position for an individual participant. The face has been blurred on purpose to preserve anonymity but was fully visible for the participants during the experiment.

EEG acquisition and preprocessing

The EEG was acquired with a Hydrocel Geodesic Sensor Net (HCGSN, Electrical Geodesics, USA) with 129 scalp electrodes at a sampling frequency of 1000Hz. On-line recording was band-pass filtered at 0−100Hz using the vertex as reference. Data pre-processing was done using Matlab (Natick, MA) and Cartool (http://sites.google.com/site/cartoolcommunity/). We down-sampled the montage to a 111-channel electrode array to exclude electrodes on the cheek and the neck since those are often contaminated with artefacts. Data were filtered between 1 and 40Hz (using non-causal filtering) and a 50Hz notch filter was applied. Each file was then visually inspected by one of the three EEG experts (HFS, TAR, and RKJ) to exclude periods of movements artefacts. Periods where subjects were not looking at the screen were excluded. Independent component analysis (ICA) was performed on the data to identify and remove the components related to eye movement artefacts (eye blinks, saccades). Subsequently, channels with substantial noise were interpolated using spherical spline interpolation for each recording. Finally, the cleaned data were down-sampled to 125Hz, recalculated against the average reference and inspected by two EEG experts (HFS and AC) to ensure that no artefacts had been missed. One hundred and twenty artefact-free epochs of 1 second per participant were included for further analysis and were considered as a minimum to ensure high enough data quality.

Electrical Source Imaging and selection of regions of interest

The general analysis strategy is summarized in Figure 7. Electrical source imaging (ESI) was performed to reconstruct the sources of brain activity that gave rise to the scalp EEG field. For this, we used a toddler template head model (33–44 month) (using the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) brain) with consideration of skull thickness (Locally Spherical Model with Anatomical Constraints, LSMAC). 4159 solution points were equally distributed in the grey matter. We used a distributed linear inverse solution (Low Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography, LORETA [Pascual-Marqui et al., 1994]) to compute the 3-dimensional (3D) current source densities. We then projected this 3D dipole time-series onto the predominant dipole direction of each region of interest (ROI) across time and epochs, therefore obtaining a scalar time-series (Coito et al., 2016a; Coito et al., 2015; Plomp et al., 2015a; Coito et al., 2016b). We parcelled the grey matter in 82 ROIs based on the automated anatomical labelling (AAL) digital atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002), after normalization to the MNI space using SPM8 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, UK, www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). In order to reduce the dimensionality of the solution space, we considered the solution point closest to the centroid of each ROI as representative of the source activity in that ROI for further analysis. This allowed to obtain the source activity across time of 82 solution points, representative of 82 ROIs (Coito et al., 2016b).

Figure 7. The general analysis strategy.

Figure 7.

Directed functional connectivity using Granger-causality

Directed functional connectivity estimates the influence that one signal exerts onto another, facilitating the study of directional relationships between brain regions. It is commonly assessed using the concept of Granger-causality: given two signals in a process, if the knowledge of the past of one allows a better prediction of the presence of the other signal in the process, then the former signal is said to Granger-cause the latter signal (Granger, 1969).

Granger-causal modelling is a well-validated statistical method (Bressler and Seth, 2011) that has been successfully applied to estimate the strength of directed interactions between brain regions in rats using epicranial EEG (Plomp et al., 2014) and in non-human primates using intracranial recordings (Brovelli et al., 2004; Saalmann et al., 2012). It has also efficiently been used to study connectivity patterns in healthy humans with source imaging using EEG (Astolfi et al., 2007; Hu et al., 2012; Plomp et al., 2015b) and MEG (Michalareas et al., 2016). This approach has also effectively been applied in clinical populations to study network alterations in patients with focal epilepsy using intra-cranial recordings (Wilke et al., 2009; van Mierlo et al., 2013; van Mierlo et al., 2011) as well as electrical source imaging (Ding et al., 2007; Coito et al., 2016a; Coito et al., 2015; Coito et al., 2016b).

In order to have interpretable results, Granger-causality analysis should be performed using electrical source imaging rather than on electrodes measured at the scalp surface (Schoffelen and Gross, 2009; Bastos et al., 2015). Therefore, in order to estimate the directional relationships in our data, we computed the weighted Partial Directed Coherence (wPDC) (Baccalá and Sameshima, 2001; Astolfi et al., 2006; Plomp et al., 2014) using the 82 source signals. PDC is a multivariate approach, which considers all signals simultaneously in the same model and estimates brain connectivity in the frequency domain. It is computed using multivariate autoregressive models of a certain model order. Here, we used a model order of 5, corresponding to 40ms. The wPDC was computed for each subject and epoch and then, the average of the PDC values within subjects was taken (Coito et al., 2016b). The average PDC was subsequently scaled (0 − 1) across ROIs and frequencies (1 − 40 Hz) by subtracting the minimum power and dividing by the range. In order to weight the PDC by the spectral power (SP) of each source signal, while avoiding frequency doubling, we computed the Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) for each electrode, applied ESI to the real and imaginary part of the FFT separately and then combined them (Coito et al., 2016a; Coito et al., 2015; Plomp et al., 2015a; Yuan et al., 2008). The mean SP was obtained for each subject and scaled (that is 0–1, in the same way as PDC). For further details on the methodological approach to compute directed functional connectivity from electrical-source imaging signals, we refer the reader to (Coito et al., 2016b). For each subject, we obtained a 3D connectivity matrix (ROIs x ROIs x frequency), representing the outflow from one ROI to another for each frequency. For further analysis, we reduced the connectivity matrix to 3 frequency bands: theta (4 − 7Hz), alpha (8 − 12Hz) and beta (13 − 30Hz), by calculating the mean connectivity value in each band. For each subject and frequency band, we computed the summed outflow as the sum of wPDC values from a given ROI to all the others. This reflects the driving importance of this ROI in the network: ROIs with high summed outflow strongly drive the activity of other ROIs. We identified the highest information transfer (summed outflow) in the theta band followed by the alpha band. Therefore, we focused our subsequent analysis on these two frequency bands. We carried out statistical comparisons of the summed outflows between subject groups using a non-parametrical statistical test (Mann − Whitney − Wilcoxon, two − tailed, p < 0.05). We then investigated the outflows from the ROIs that showed statistically significant summed outflow between groups to the whole brain (remaining 81 ROIs) and carried out a statistical comparison of these outflows between groups (Mann − Whitney − Wilcoxon, two − tailed, p < 0.05, Benjamini − Hochberg = 0.05). We correlated (Spearman − rho, two − tailed, p < 0.05) the summed outflow results obtained in each of the 82 ROIs with ADOS-CSS scores, with developmental scores obtained from the PEP-3, with adaptive scores obtained from VABS-II and with the PI values obtained from the eye-tracking data. In all cases, correlation p-values were Benjamini-Hochberg-corrected for multiple testing with p = 0.05. Connectivity computations were performed in Matlab. Figures 1, 2 and 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 2, Figure 3—figure supplement 2, Figures 4 and 5 were produced using the BrainNet Viewer toolbox (Xia et al., 2013).

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to express their gratitude to all the families who took part in this research. This research is supported by a grant from the National Centre of Competence in Research (NCCR) 'SYNAPSY-The Synaptic Bases of Mental Diseases’ financed by the Swiss National Science Foundation (number: 51AU40-125759) and by private funding by the Fondation Pole Autisme (http://www.pole-autisme.ch). This work was further supported by a SNSF grant to MS. (number: 163859), as well as SNSF grants number 320030–159705 to CMM and number 169198 and CRSII5170873 to SV. RKJ received individual support from a Marie Curie fellowship, which received funding from the European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP7:2007–2013) under grant agreement number 267171.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Holger Franz Sperdin, Email: holger.sperdin@unige.ch.

Jan-Marino Ramirez, Seattle Children's Research Institute and University of Washington, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Centre of Competence in Research 51AU40-125759 to Stephan Eliez, Christoph M Michel, Marie Schaer.

  • Swiss National Science Foundation 163859 to Marie Schaer.

  • Swiss National Science Foundation 320030-159705 to Christoph M Michel.

  • Swiss National Science Foundation 169198 to Serge Vulliemoz.

  • Swiss National Science Foundation CRSII5170873 to Serge Vulliemoz.

  • Marie Curie Fellowship 267171 to Reem Kais Jan.

  • Seventh Framework Programme 267171 to Reem Kais Jan.

  • Fondation Pôle Autisme to Stephan Eliez, Marie Schaer.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization.

Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Software, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing—review and editing.

Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition.

Conceptualization, Resources, Software, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Human subjects: Prior to the experiments, all the procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Geneva Hospital in accordance with the ethical standards proclaimed in the Declaration of Helsinki. For all participants, an interview over the phone and a medical developmental history questionnaire were completed before their initial visit. All participants' parents gave their informed consent prior to inclusion in the study.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

References

  1. Adolphs R. The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:693–716. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Apps MA, Lockwood PL, Balsters JH. The role of the midcingulate cortex in monitoring others' decisions. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2013;7:251. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Asada H, Fukuda Y, Tsunoda S, Yamaguchi M, Tonoike M. Frontal midline theta rhythms reflect alternative activation of prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex in humans. Neuroscience Letters. 1999;274:29–32. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00679-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Astolfi L, Cincotti F, Mattia D, Marciani MG, Baccala LA, de Vico Fallani F, Salinari S, Ursino M, Zavaglia M, Ding L, Edgar JC, Miller GA, He B, Babiloni F. Comparison of different cortical connectivity estimators for high-resolution EEG recordings. Human Brain Mapping. 2007;28:143–157. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Astolfi L, Cincotti F, Mattia D, Marciani MG, Baccalà LA, de Vico Fallani F, Salinari S, Ursino M, Zavaglia M, Babiloni F. Assessing cortical functional connectivity by partial directed coherence: simulations and application to real data. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering. 2006;53:1802–1812. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2006.873692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baccalá LA, Sameshima K. Partial directed coherence: a new concept in neural structure determination. Biological Cybernetics. 2001;84:463–474. doi: 10.1007/PL00007990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bastos AM, Vezoli J, Bosman CA, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Dowdall JR, De Weerd P, Kennedy H, Fries P. Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels. Neuron. 2015;85:390–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:267–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn2353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Botev ZI, Grotowski JF, Kroese DP. Kernel density estimation via diffusion. The Annals of Statistics. 2010;38:2916–2957. doi: 10.1214/10-AOS799. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bressler SL, Seth AK. Wiener-Granger causality: a well established methodology. NeuroImage. 2011;58:323–329. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brothers L. The social brain: a project for integrating primate behavior and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts Neurosci. 1990;1:27–61. [Google Scholar]
  12. Brovelli A, Ding M, Ledberg A, Chen Y, Nakamura R, Bressler SL. Beta oscillations in a large-scale sensorimotor cortical network: directional influences revealed by Granger causality. PNAS. 2004;101:9849–9854. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308538101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cheng W, Rolls ET, Gu H, Zhang J, Feng J. Autism: reduced connectivity between cortical areas involved in face expression, theory of mind, and the sense of self. Brain. 2015;138:1382–1393. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chevallier C, Kohls G, Troiani V, Brodkin ES, Schultz RT. The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16:231–239. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Christensen DL, Baio J, Van Naarden Braun K, Bilder D, Charles J, Constantino JN, Daniels J, Durkin MS, Fitzgerald RT, Kurzius-Spencer M, Lee LC, Pettygrove S, Robinson C, Schulz E, Wells C, Wingate MS, Zahorodny W, Yeargin-Allsopp M, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years--autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, united states, 2012. MMWR. Surveillance Summaries. 2016;65:1–23. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6503a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Coito A, Genetti M, Pittau F, Iannotti GR, Thomschewski A, Höller Y, Trinka E, Wiest R, Seeck M, Michel CM, Plomp G, Vulliemoz S. Altered directed functional connectivity in temporal lobe epilepsy in the absence of interictal spikes: A high density EEG study. Epilepsia. 2016a;57:402–411. doi: 10.1111/epi.13308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Coito A, Michel CM, van Mierlo P, Vulliemoz S, Plomp G. Directed functional brain connectivity based on eeg source imaging: methodology and application to temporal lobe epilepsy. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering. 2016b;63:2619–2628. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2016.2619665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Coito A, Plomp G, Genetti M, Abela E, Wiest R, Seeck M, Michel CM, Vulliemoz S. Dynamic directed interictal connectivity in left and right temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2015;56:207–217. doi: 10.1111/epi.12904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Conti E, Calderoni S, Marchi V, Muratori F, Cioni G, Guzzetta A. The first 1000 days of the autistic brain: a systematic review of diffusion imaging studies. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2015;9:159. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Courchesne E, Campbell K, Solso S. Brain growth across the life span in autism: age-specific changes in anatomical pathology. Brain Research. 2011;1380:138–145. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.09.101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Datko M, Gougelet R, Huang MX, Pineda JA. Resting state functional connectivity mri among spectral meg current sources in children on the autism spectrum. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2016;10:258. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dawson G, Toth K, Abbott R, Osterling J, Munson J, Estes A, Liaw J. Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40:271–283. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. DeCasper AJ, Fifer WP. Of human bonding: newborns prefer their mothers' voices. Science. 1980;208:1174–1176. doi: 10.1126/science.7375928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. DeCasper AJ, Spence MJ. Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns' perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development. 1986;9:133–150. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(86)90025-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dichter GS, Felder JN, Bodfish JW. Autism is characterized by dorsal anterior cingulate hyperactivation during social target detection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2009;4:215–226. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ding L, Worrell GA, Lagerlund TD, He B. Ictal source analysis: localization and imaging of causal interactions in humans. NeuroImage. 2007;34:575–586. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dinstein I, Heeger DJ, Lorenzi L, Minshew NJ, Malach R, Behrmann M. Unreliable evoked responses in autism. Neuron. 2012;75:981–991. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Elsabbagh M, Mercure E, Hudry K, Chandler S, Pasco G, Charman T, Pickles A, Baron-Cohen S, Bolton P, Johnson MH, Team B, BASIS Team Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic eye gaze is associated with later emerging autism. Current Biology. 2012;22:338–342. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Esler AN, Bal VH, Guthrie W, Wetherby A, Ellis Weismer S, Lord C. The autism diagnostic observation schedule, toddler module: standardized severity scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2015;45:2704–2720. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2432-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Frith U, Frith C. The social brain: allowing humans to boldly go where no other species has been. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2010;365:165–176. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Frith U. A new look at language and communication in autism. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 1989;24:123–150. doi: 10.3109/13682828909011952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gotham K, Pickles A, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2009;39:693–705. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gotts SJ, Simmons WK, Milbury LA, Wallace GL, Cox RW, Martin A. Fractionation of social brain circuits in autism spectrum disorders. Brain. 2012;135:2711–2725. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Granger CWJ. Investigating causal relations by econometric models and cross-spectral methods. Econometrica. 1969;37:424–438. doi: 10.2307/1912791. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Greene DJ, Colich N, Iacoboni M, Zaidel E, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M. Atypical neural networks for social orienting in autism spectrum disorders. NeuroImage. 2011;56:354–362. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Grelotti DJ, Gauthier I, Schultz RT. Social interest and the development of cortical face specialization: what autism teaches us about face processing. Developmental Psychobiology. 2002;40:213–225. doi: 10.1002/dev.10028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hoppenbrouwers M, Vandermosten M, Boets B. Autism as a disconnection syndrome: A qualitative and quantitative review of diffusion tensor imaging studies. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2014;8:387–412. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2013.12.018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hu L, Zhang ZG, Hu Y. A time-varying source connectivity approach to reveal human somatosensory information processing. NeuroImage. 2012;62:217–228. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Jones EJ, Venema K, Lowy R, Earl RK, Webb SJ. Developmental changes in infant brain activity during naturalistic social experiences. Developmental Psychobiology. 2015;57:842–853. doi: 10.1002/dev.21336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jonker FA, Jonker C, Scheltens P, Scherder EJ. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in cognition and behavior. Reviews in the Neurosciences. 2015;26:1–11. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2014-0043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Keehn B, Müller RA, Townsend J. Atypical attentional networks and the emergence of autism. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;37:164–183. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kitzbichler MG, Khan S, Ganesan S, Vangel MG, Herbert MR, Hämäläinen MS, Kenet T. Altered development and multifaceted band-specific abnormalities of resting state networks in autism. Biological Psychiatry. 2015;77:794–804. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Klimesch W. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews. 1999;29:169–195. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Klin A, Lin DJ, Gorrindo P, Ramsay G, Jones W. Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature. 2009;459:257–261. doi: 10.1038/nature07868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kuhl PK, Coffey-Corina S, Padden D, Munson J, Estes A, Dawson G. Brain responses to words in 2-year-olds with autism predict developmental outcomes at age 6. PLoS One. 2013;8:e64967. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lopes da Silva F. EEG and MEG: relevance to neuroscience. Neuron. 2013;80:1112–1128. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A, Rutter M. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2000;30:205–223. doi: 10.1023/A:1005592401947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Luyster R, Gotham K, Guthrie W, Coffing M, Petrak R, Pierce K, Bishop S, Esler A, Hus V, Oti R, Richler J, Risi S, Lord C. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-toddler module: a new module of a standardized diagnostic measure for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2009;39:1305–1320. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0746-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Meltzoff AN, Moore MK. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science. 1977;198:74–78. doi: 10.1126/science.897687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Structure and Function. 2010;214:655–667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Michalareas G, Vezoli J, van Pelt S, Schoffelen JM, Kennedy H, Fries P. Alpha-Beta and gamma rhythms subserve feedback and feedforward influences among human visual cortical areas. Neuron. 2016;89:384–397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mohammad-Rezazadeh I, Frohlich J, Loo SK, Jeste SS. Brain connectivity in autism spectrum disorder. Current Opinion in Neurology. 2016;29:137–147. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Nomi JS, Uddin LQ. Developmental changes in large-scale network connectivity in autism. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2015;7:732–741. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.02.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ohnishi T, Matsuda H, Hashimoto T, Kunihiro T, Nishikawa M, Uema T, Sasaki M. Abnormal regional cerebral blood flow in childhood autism. Brain. 2000;123:1838–1844. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.9.1838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Orekhova EV, Elsabbagh M, Jones EJ, Dawson G, Charman T, Johnson MH, Team B, BASIS Team EEG hyper-connectivity in high-risk infants is associated with later autism. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2014;6:40. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-6-40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Orekhova EV, Stroganova TA, Posikera IN, Elam M. EEG theta rhythm in infants and preschool children. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2006;117:1047–1062. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Orekhova EV, Stroganova TA, Posikera IN. Alpha activity as an index of cortical inhibition during sustained internally controlled attention in infants. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2001;112:740–749. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00502-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Osterling J, Dawson G. Early recognition of children with autism: a study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1994;24:247–257. doi: 10.1007/BF02172225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Pascual-Marqui RD, Michel CM, Lehmann D. Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: a new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1994;18:49–65. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(84)90014-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Patriquin MA, DeRamus T, Libero LE, Laird A, Kana RK. Neuroanatomical and neurofunctional markers of social cognition in autism spectrum disorder. Human Brain Mapping. 2016;37:3957–3978. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pelphrey KA, Carter EJ. Charting the typical and atypical development of the social brain. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:1081–1102. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Pelphrey KA, Morris JP, McCarthy G. Grasping the intentions of others: the perceived intentionality of an action influences activity in the superior temporal sulcus during social perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16:1706–1716. doi: 10.1162/0898929042947900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Pelphrey KA, Morris JP, Michelich CR, Allison T, McCarthy G. Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: an FMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15:1866–1876. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Pelphrey KA, Morris JP. Brain mechanisms for interpreting the actions of others from biological-motion cues. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2006;15:136–140. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00423.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Pelphrey KA, Shultz S, Hudac CM, Vander Wyk BC. Research review: Constraining heterogeneity: the social brain and its development in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:631–644. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02349.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Plomp G, Astolfi L, Coito A, Michel CM. Spectrally weighted Granger-causal modeling: Motivation and applications to data from animal models and epileptic patients. Conference proceedings : Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. Annual Conference. 2015a;2015:5392–5395. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2015.7319610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Plomp G, Hervais-Adelman A, Astolfi L, Michel CM. Early recurrence and ongoing parietal driving during elementary visual processing. Scientific Reports. 2015b;5:18733. doi: 10.1038/srep18733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Plomp G, Quairiaux C, Michel CM, Astolfi L. The physiological plausibility of time-varying Granger-causal modeling: normalization and weighting by spectral power. NeuroImage. 2014;97:206–216. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Raschle N, Zuk J, Ortiz-Mantilla S, Sliva DD, Franceschi A, Grant PE, Benasich AA, Gaab N. Pediatric neuroimaging in early childhood and infancy: challenges and practical guidelines. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2012;1252:43–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06457.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Reid VM, Dunn K, Young RJ, Amu J, Donovan T, Reissland N. The human fetus preferentially engages with face-like visual stimuli. Current Biology. 2017;27:2052. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Saalmann YB, Pinsk MA, Wang L, Li X, Kastner S. The pulvinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on attention demands. Science. 2012;337:753–756. doi: 10.1126/science.1223082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Saby JN, Marshall PJ. The utility of EEG band power analysis in the study of infancy and early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2012;37:253–273. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2011.614663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Schoffelen JM, Gross J. Source connectivity analysis with MEG and EEG. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30:1857–1865. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Schopler E, Lansing MD, Reichler RJ, Marcus LM. PEP-3 Psychoeducational Profile. third edition. Austin: Pro-Ed; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  75. Schumann CM, Bloss CS, Barnes CC, Wideman GM, Carper RA, Akshoomoff N, Pierce K, Hagler D, Schork N, Lord C, Courchesne E. Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study of cortical development through early childhood in autism. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:4419–4427. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5714-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Schwartz S, Kessler R, Gaughan T, Buckley AW. Electroencephalogram Coherence Patterns in Autism: An Updated Review. Pediatric Neurology. 2017;67:7–22. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2016.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Simion F, Regolin L, Bulf H. A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. PNAS. 2008;105:809–813. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707021105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sparrow SS, Cicchetti VD, Balla AD. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, United States: American Guidance Service; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  79. Sperdin HF, Schaer M. Aberrant development of speech processing in young children with autism: new insights from neuroimaging biomarkers. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2016;10:393. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Stroganova TA, Orekhova EV, Posikera IN. EEG alpha rhythm in infants. Clinical Neurophysiology. 1999;110:997–1012. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(98)00009-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Tierney AL, Gabard-Durnam L, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Developmental trajectories of resting EEG power: an endophenotype of autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One. 2012;7:e39127. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, Mazoyer B, Joliot M. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage. 2002;15:273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Lynch CJ, Khouzam A, Phillips J, Feinstein C, Ryali S, Menon V. Salience network-based classification and prediction of symptom severity in children with autism. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013a;70:869–879. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Menon V. Reconceptualizing functional brain connectivity in autism from a developmental perspective. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013b;7:458. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Uddin LQ. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2015;16:55–61. doi: 10.1038/nrn3857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. van Mierlo P, Carrette E, Hallez H, Raedt R, Meurs A, Vandenberghe S, Van Roost D, Boon P, Staelens S, Vonck K. Ictal-onset localization through connectivity analysis of intracranial EEG signals in patients with refractory epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2013;54:1409–1418. doi: 10.1111/epi.12206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. van Mierlo P, Carrette E, Hallez H, Vonck K, Van Roost D, Boon P, Staelens S. Accurate epileptogenic focus localization through time-variant functional connectivity analysis of intracranial electroencephalographic signals. NeuroImage. 2011;56:1122–1133. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Wilke C, van Drongelen W, Kohrman M, He B. Identification of epileptogenic foci from causal analysis of ECoG interictal spike activity. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2009;120:1449–1456. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.04.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Xia M, Wang J, He Y. BrainNet Viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS One. 2013;8:e68910. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Ye AX, Leung RC, Schäfer CB, Taylor MJ, Doesburg SM. Atypical resting synchrony in autism spectrum disorder. Human Brain Mapping. 2014;35:6049–6066. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Yuan H, Doud A, Gururajan A, He B. Cortical imaging of event-related (de)synchronization during online control of brain-computer interface using minimum-norm estimates in frequency domain. IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering. 2008;16:425–431. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2008.2003384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Jan-Marino Ramirez1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Early alterations of social brain networks in young children with autism" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by Sabine Kastner (Senior Editor) and three reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

This manuscript used multisite EEG, source localization, and functional connectivity analyses to detect differences in brain network activity in 3-year old children with autism viewing a social video, providing, according to the authors, the first functional connectivity analysis of the toddler brain activated by social stimuli.

Characterizing EEG activity in autistic children is critical for understanding the underlying cause. Ultimately it is the electrical brain activity that can provide the important mechanistic insights that lead to the observable behavioral phenotype. Thus, this study has the potential to become an important contribution to the field of autism.

Indeed, the reviewers congratulate the authors for the novelty of this approach and bringing it to the study of humans with neurodevelopmental disorders. The reviewers suggest that the work could somehow be re-framed to bridge the gap between Astolfi et al. (2006) with Coito (2016).

Essential revisions:

1) While the reviewers admire the work performed in this study, and agree that EEG-based directional functional connectivity measures constitute an important and promising approach to study autism, the sole reliance on the Granger method raises concerns. This skepticism is based on the lack of prior literature, and the findings presented here – chiefly much connectivity activity seen at the hippocampus, a structure that generates little to the EEG. Probably the majority of EEG researchers, the reviewers among them, who have attempted directed functional connectivity estimation at the scalp level find the method spurious. For example, connectivity at electrode pairs can always be computed, but validation of the direction and weight seems unreliable, for example estimated at nearest neighbor electrodes, yield irregular directions and weights.

We agree, that intracranial EEG and LFP in animals show interpretable Granger causality and there are many fine publications on this. However, to the best of our knowledge there are no comparable papers based on scalp EEG. The signal may be too noisy for the Granger method, and this is why it has not been widely applied. We understand that the authors are not examining EEG at electrodes, but they examine connectivity in source space. Methodology here appears conservative with LORETA technique and appropriate MNI template model. This suggests the accuracy of source reconstruction is probably good – to the extent that reconstruction will be good in three-year-children.

As for the revision: To increase the confidence in the results presented in this study, additional work would have to be done to demonstrate stable Granger patterns. This could be done in any number of ways, and we leave it up to the authors to find the best way: One possible approach might be done at the voxel level. Neighboring voxels should show similar directed functional connectivity. This could be demonstrated and Granger details documented in revision, such that future research can replicate the study.

2) Analyses were restricted to theta due to the finding that less "summed outflow" occurred in the α and β bands. Although I recognize that the choice was guided by practicalities of analysis as well as the understanding that theta relates to general activation and memory formation, the figures seem to indicate in some sites less α activity with ASD, relating perhaps to a less relaxed mental state. Whether this interpretation of the α data is correct or not, this comment gets to the issue of whether the paper achieves its greatest potential by restricting analysis to theta. The paper could have a much greater impact if there was a contrast between directed coherence in the theta and α bands.

3) Contrasting α and theta could help with a significant limitation of the current analysis which produces a list of region-to-region changes with ASD which, in toto, demonstrate details of what is already known, namely that brain activity is different between ASD and TD children. The α and theta contrast might provide functional meaning to the changes, which otherwise are difficult to interpret.

4) As an example of a difficult-to-interpret finding using theta alone, there is a strong correlation between output of the left Heschl's gyrus and fine motor skills. This would seem to imply an auditory receptive language component in motor skill learning, not necessarily performance as suggested, and could be a general property upstream of motor function. Certainly, α band activity in motor cortex has been related to motor performance per se.

5) A compelling argument is made that greater summed outflow (re: TD) in some brain areas for some ASD individuals relates to better gaze performance, and thus may be a compensatory mechanism for ASD. This seems to be the strongest aspect of this manuscript and deserves greater attention. It would be interesting to report whether the individuals that compensated in this way had lower ADOS scores, or better socialization, as compared to those that did not, and, more importantly, whether (or not) such compensations were uniform across functional domains. Does each individual that shows such compensation have an overall similar (or different) compensatory architecture in either their summed outflow from other ROIs or their overall functional connectivity? If so, what are the implications for therapy?

eLife. 2018 Feb 27;7:e31670. doi: 10.7554/eLife.31670.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) While the reviewers admire the work performed in this study, and agree that EEG-based directional functional connectivity measures constitute an important and promising approach to study autism, the sole reliance on the Granger method raises concerns. This skepticism is based on the lack of prior literature, and the findings presented here – chiefly much connectivity activity seen at the hippocampus, a structure that generates little to the EEG. Probably the majority of EEG researchers, the reviewers among them, who have attempted directed functional connectivity estimation at the scalp level find the method spurious. For example, connectivity at electrode pairs can always be computed, but validation of the direction and weight seems unreliable, for example estimated at nearest neighbor electrodes, yield irregular directions and weights.

We agree, that intracranial EEG and LFP in animals show interpretable Granger causality and there are many fine publications on this. However, to the best of our knowledge there are no comparable papers based on scalp EEG. The signal may be too noisy for the Granger method, and this is why it has not been widely applied. We understand that the authors are not examining EEG at electrodes, but they examine connectivity in source space. Methodology here appears conservative with LORETA technique and appropriate MNI template model. This suggests the accuracy of source reconstruction is probably good – to the extent that reconstruction will be good in three-year-children.

We would like to gratefully thank the reviewers for their summary and positive appraisal of our manuscript. As we understand, the first concern is that the sole reliance on the Granger method raises concern because of the lack of prior literature. As the reviewers further mention, there are several publications based on intracranial EEG data and LFP in animals showing interpretable Granger causality but no comparable papers based on scalp EEG. In our experience too, the SNR is higher in recordings from animal models, and this certainly helps for inferring directed functional connections. We would hasten to note, however, that comparable papers using source imaging based on EEG recordings in both clinical and healthy populations have recently been published, by our group as well as others. We now make this point more clear in the manuscript and have added the following parts in the Introduction and in the Materials and method sections:

Introduction: “This approach is being increasingly and successfully applied to study connectivity alterations using intra-cranial recordings (Wilke, van Drongelen et al. 2009, van Mierlo, Carrette et al. 2011, van Mierlo, Carrette et al. 2013) as well as source imaging based on EEG recordings in clinical populations (Ding, Worrell et al. 2007, Coito, Plomp et al. 2015, Coito, Genetti et al. 2016, Coito, Michel et al. 2016) and in healthy controls (Astolfi, Cincotti et al. 2007, Hu, Zhang et al. 2012, Plomp, Hervais-Adelman et al. 2015).”

Materials and methods: “Directed functional connectivity estimates the influence that one signal exerts onto another, facilitating the study of directional relationships between brain regions. […] In order to have interpretable results, Granger-causality analysis should be performed using electrical source imaging rather than on electrodes measured at the scalp surface (Schoffelen and Gross 2009, Bastos, Vezoli et al. 2015).”

As for the revision: To increase the confidence in the results presented in this study, additional work would have to be done to demonstrate stable Granger patterns. This could be done in any number of ways, and we leave it up to the authors to find the best way: One possible approach might be done at the voxel level. Neighboring voxels should show similar directed functional connectivity. This could be demonstrated and Granger details documented in revision, such that future research can replicate the study.

We thank the reviewers for highlighting this second concern about the stability of the Granger Causality patterns and appreciate the ideas offered for improving the manuscript.

As the reviewers mention, Granger-causality should be performed using electrical source imaging as we did in the present experiment, rather than on electrodes measured at the scalp surface. We adopted a rather conservative methodology with LORETA technique and appropriate MNI template model suggesting accurate source reconstruction. Furthermore, the results have been corrected for multiple testing using the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure (BH step-up procedure) to guard against spurious effects. Also, the data-driven approach reveals high information transfer in physiologically relevant frequency bands given the young age of our participants. The suggestion as proposed by the reviewers here, namely to demonstrate whether neighboring voxels (i.e. solutions points) show similar Granger patterns would require doing a very high resolution reanalysis for each of the solution points (4159 solution points), which is not possible with the multivariate methods we use. Moreover, in order to reduce the dimensionality of the solution space, we considered the solution point closest to the centroid of each ROI. Thus, for a single ROI only a single solution point is representative of the source activity in this ROI (82 solution points, representative of the 82 ROIs). However, in order to increase the confidence in the results, we now include boxplots with summed outflow values for each of the significant ROIs comparing our two groups and this for all the significant ROIs (see new Figure 2B for the theta band and Figure 2C for α band). As can be seen from these boxplots, systematic differences between the groups are present for all the significant ROIs and for both frequency bands suggesting consistent (stable) Granger patterns across participants. If Granger patterns were spurious (that is random), then these systematic differences between the groups would not be obtained. We have also added boxplots for the ORBsup.R seed regions in the theta band showing outflow values toward each of the selected target regions for the ASD group and for the TD group as Figure 3—figure supplement 1 and Figure 3—figure supplement 2, respectively.

2) Analyses were restricted to theta due to the finding that less "summed outflow" occurred in the α and β bands. Although I recognize that the choice was guided by practicalities of analysis as well as the understanding that theta relates to general activation and memory formation, the figures seem to indicate in some sites less α activity with ASD, relating perhaps to a less relaxed mental state. Whether this interpretation of the α data is correct or not, this comment gets to the issue of whether the paper achieves its greatest potential by restricting analysis to theta. The paper could have a much greater impact if there was a contrast between directed coherence in the theta and α bands.

We thank the reviewer for this observation. We have now performed the analysis also for the α band (8-12Hz), which we now include in the revised manuscript. The new analyses revealed α specific network atypicalities, which we now include and discuss in detail in the manuscript. Please see also our reply in points (3) and (4) below for further details.

3) Contrasting α and theta could help with a significant limitation of the current analysis which produces a list of region-to-region changes with ASD which, in toto, demonstrate details of what is already known, namely that brain activity is different between ASD and TD children. The α and theta contrast might provide functional meaning to the changes, which otherwise are difficult to interpret.

We now contrast theta and α. The global driving in the theta and α bands did not differ between groups (theta: df=34, t=0.6201, p = 0.536; α: df=34, t=0.1736, p = 0.8632). Driving in the theta band was higher compared to the driving in the α band in both groups (ASD: df=17, t=11.86, p < 0.0001; TD: df=17, t=8.025, p < 0.0001) (see new Figure 1B). While we found six ROIs in the theta band with an altered driving, in the α band we found three ROIs that had a different driving in the ASD group compared to the TD group. The right orbital part of the middle frontal gyrus (Ws = 262, z = -2.246, p= 0.024, r = -0.374) and the left cuneus (Ws = 265, z = -2.151, p= 0.031, r = -0.358) had a higher driving and the right STG had a weaker driving (Ws = 265, z = -2.151, p= 0.031, r = -0.358) (see new Figure 2A). The region-to-region directed functional connectivity analysis for the α band revealed stronger connections from the right orbital part of the middle frontal gyrus and the left cuneus whereas the right STG had weaker connection in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD compared to their TD peers (Mann-Whitney-Wilcoxon, two-tailed, p<0.05, Benjamini-Hochberg = 0.05). All three significant ROIs in α had different network patterns in the toddlers and preschoolers with ASD compared to their TD peer (see new Figure 4). We now display the connections from all the seed regions in both bands and for both groups in new Figure 3 (theta) and new Figure 4 (α) (see Results and Discussion sections in the manuscript).

4) As an example of a difficult-to-interpret finding using theta alone, there is a strong correlation between output of the left Heschl's gyrus and fine motor skills. This would seem to imply an auditory receptive language component in motor skill learning, not necessarily performance as suggested, and could be a general property upstream of motor function. Certainly, α band activity in motor cortex has been related to motor performance per se.

We thank the reviewer for this observation. We have now explored associations between the summed outflow in α band with gaze performance and clinical and behavioral phenotype as we did for the theta band. For the α band, we only found two significant correlations after FDR correction: higher summed outflow within the left hippocampus and the left rolandic operculum were positively related to better fine (rs = 0.736,N = 18, p = 0.0005, two-tailed, <0.05; Benjamini-Hochberg = 0.05) and gross motor skills (rs = 0.737,N = 18, p = 0.0005, two-tailed, <0.05; Benjamini-Hochberg = 0.05) as measured by the PEP-3 (see new Figure 5). Thus, higher summed outflow in α band within these areas was related to better motor performance per se. We didn’t find any other relationships between the summed outflow in α and gaze performance, the ADOS-2 severity scores, adaptive behavior scores using the VABS-II or items form the developmental level assessed with the PEP-3 suggesting that increased driving in the α band was confined to better motor performance (see Results and Discussion sections in the manuscript).

5) A compelling argument is made that greater summed outflow (re: TD) in some brain areas for some ASD individuals relates to better gaze performance, and thus may be a compensatory mechanism for ASD. This seems to be the strongest aspect of this manuscript and deserves greater attention. It would be interesting to report whether the individuals that compensated in this way had lower ADOS scores, or better socialization, as compared to those that did not, and, more importantly, whether (or not) such compensations were uniform across functional domains. Does each individual that shows such compensation have an overall similar (or different) compensatory architecture in either their summed outflow from other ROIs or their overall functional connectivity? If so, what are the implications for therapy?

Following the reviewer’s remark, we examined the possibility that toddlers with ASD with better gaze performance are the ones with lower autistic severity, better adaptive functioning and higher cognitive developmental scores. To this purpose, we correlated the Proximity Index (i.e. gaze performance) with the global severity of autistic symptoms as measured by the ADOS-2 calibrated severity score, the PEP-3 scores in the developmental areas of cognitive verbal/preverbal, expressive and receptive language, fine and gross motor skills and visual motor imitation skills and with VABS-II standardized Adaptive Behavior Composite scores as well as the four domain scores: Communication, Daily Living Skills, Socialization, and Motor Skills (see Table 1). We didn’t find any significant correlation between the PI and the overall severity of autistic symptoms as measured with the ADOS-2 severity score. However, we found that the toddlers with ASD with a better gaze performance had better adaptive functioning as measured with the parent-reported scale of the VABS-II (total score), that was driven by improved global and fine motor skills and better development of interpersonal relationships. In terms of cognitive development, the children with better gaze performances also showed better visual motor imitation skills as measured by the PEP-3. This results reflect the fact that the toddlers with ASD who have better gaze performance, are the ones who have both less clinical impairments and greater summed outflow. Whether or not this reflects a compensatory mechanism remains an important question to be solved. Our laboratory has indeed been interested over the last years by the potential to use gaze performance measured with eye-tracking to reflect clinical phenotype or help predict clinical outcome over time, so that the above-reported results that children with better gaze performance are the ones with better adaptive and cognitive functioning comes as no surprise to us. In a first cross-sectional study, similarly to the correlation between gaze performance and clinical severity reported above, we found that toddlers with ASD who spent more time exploring dynamic social images compared to geometrical forms were those who had more joint attention behaviors and improved communication skills (Franchini, Glaser et al., 2017). In a second longitudinal eye-tracking experiment, we found that toddlers with ASD who preferred social stimuli in comparison to geometric shapes at the age of 3 developed better outcome in terms of decreased symptom severity a year later (Franchini, de Wilde et al., 2016). The present study suggests that different patterns of brain activation might be associated with different clinical outcomes, but a longitudinal design with EEG and clinical assessments at both time points would be needed to fully to answer this question.

Regarding the implications for therapy, the reviewer’s question is important, but again we think that the current results might be too preliminary to support changes in existing therapeutic interventions. Nowadays, state of the art therapies for children with autism are early and intensive behavioral therapies that focus on the development of reciprocal interactions through play-based exchanges that increase the motivation of the child to orient to social cues. One of such approach is the Early Start Denver Model (Dawson et al., 2010) which is entirely based on the idea that intervening early, helping children orient to social stimuli, may partially restore aberrant trajectories of brain development in affected children. In that sense, we would expect that better gaze performance, which could be the results of therapeutic interventions or compensation by the child himself, is reflected by changes in brain activity such as greater summed outflow. Again, only a proper longitudinal design would allow to fully answer this question, and no published study to date has combined eye-tracking and EEG in a longitudinal design. The present experiment is a first step towards that direction.

To address the reviewer’s questions, we have edited the manuscript in the Results and Discussion sections. We now write:

Results: “Finally, we explored associations between gaze performance with developmental scores obtained from the PEP-3 and with adaptive scores obtained from the VABS-II (D’Agostino-Pearson omnibus normality test, K2, p<0.05; Pearson's r, two-tailed, p<0.05). […] We also found that these children with a better gaze performance had better visual motor imitation skills (K2 = 2.671, p = 0.263; r =.534, p = 0.022) as measured by the PEP-3.”

Discussion: “While to the best of our knowledge, there is currently no other relevant experimental data that addresses this question, we speculate that the overall hyper-driving from these relevant brain regions might be a mechanism to compensate for atypical development of the brain’s circuitry over time as higher directed functional connectivity was related improved socialization, motor behaviours and better visual exploration of dynamic social images. […] The present experiment is a first step towards that direction.”

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES