Abstract
Effective communication among sympatric species is often instrumental for behavioural isolation, where the failure to successfully discriminate between potential mates could lead to less fit hybrid offspring. Discrimination between con- and heterospecifics tends to occur more often in the sex that invests more in offspring production, i.e. females, but males may also mediate reproductive isolation. In this study, we show that among two Campylomormyrus African weakly electric fish species, males preferentially associate with conspecific females during choice tests using live fish as stimuli, i.e. when all sensory modalities potentially used for communication were present. We then conducted playback experiments to determine whether the species-specific electric organ discharge (EOD) used for electrocommunication serves as the cue for this conspecific association preference. Interestingly, only C. compressirostris males associated significantly more with the conspecific EOD waveform when playback stimuli were provided, while no such association preference was observed in C. tamandua males. Given our results, the EOD appears to serve, in part, as a male-mediated pre-zygotic isolation mechanism among sympatric species. However, the failure of C. tamandua males to discriminate between con- and heterospecific playback discharges suggests that multiple modalities may be necessary for species recognition in some African weakly electric fish species.
Keywords: Campylomormyrus, communication, electric fishes, pre-zygotic isolation, species recognition
1. Background
Many environmental and genetic components may influence the evolution and maintenance of reproductive isolation, including ecological specializations, behavioural mate discrimination and/or less fit hybrid offspring [1,2]. Among sympatric species, reproductive isolation may be further underscored by a given individual's ability to discriminate between con- and heterospecifics and by their strength of preference (SOP) for conspecifics [3]. However, males and females often differ in mate recognition and conspecific preference [4,5]. Reproductive isolation is thereby traditionally attributed to the sex that invests more in offspring production, i.e. females [6]. Male-mediated reproductive isolation has nonetheless been documented in several species, where mate recognition and assessment is facilitated by a wide range of sensory modalities [7–9]. The divergence of those communication cues responsible for species recognition and mate choice often leads to reproductive isolation and speciation [10–13].
African weakly electric fishes (Mormyridae) communicate by means of a species-specific electric organ discharge (EOD) generated with an electric organ [14–16]. Among certain mormyrid lineages, the diversification of this electric discharge has evolved more rapidly than body size, morphology and trophic ecology, suggesting that bioelectrogenesis was an important factor driving the speciation of electrogenic fishes [17]. This is exemplified by the African weakly electric fish genus Campylomormyrus. Endemic to the African freshwater river systems; all but one (C. phantasticus) of the 15 described Campylomormyrus species are found in sympatry in the Congo Basin [18]. Most of the external morphological differences between these species are confined to the trunk-like snout, which coincides with species-specific differences in the EOD waveform [18,19].
There is evidence that the EOD waveform functions in mate choice and species recognition, probably promoting pre-zygotic isolation among sympatric species [20–24]. Disruptive natural selection on characteristics associated with resource acquisition (snout morphology and EOD waveform) and reproductive isolation (EOD waveform) as well as the current sympatric occurrence of most species are consistent with sympatric speciation in this genus [18,25], though secondary syntopy cannot be ruled out with certainty.
Notwithstanding, discrimination based on the EOD waveform in some mormyrid genera appears to be asymmetric [26–29]. For example, female C. compressirostris discriminate between conspecific and C. rhynchophorus EOD waveforms, but do not discriminate between conspecific and C. tamandua EOD waveforms in mate choice tests using live fish and playback stimuli [26]. Given that the C. rhynchophorus EOD waveform is much longer, while the C. tamandua EOD waveform is more similar in duration to C. compressirostris, Feulner et al. [26] hypothesized that EOD duration played a key role in species recognition and discrimination. In this study, we present an alternative hypothesis, that mate choice and/or species recognition may be male-mediated. In doing so, we draw a distinction between mate choice and species recognition, in which both conspecific sexes are recognized as a unified category preferred over heterospecifics regardless of breeding status [30]. Furthermore, we investigate if the EOD waveform alone is indeed sufficient for pre-zygotic reproductive isolation.
Focusing on the preference behaviour of two sympatric African weakly electric fish species, C. compressirostris and C. tamandua, we aimed to address (i) if males discriminate between con- and heterospecifics where females do not, suggesting male-mediated species recognition and (ii) if the EOD mediates this conspecific association preference both during the breeding season and after gonadal regression.
2. Methods
2.1. Study species and experimental set-up
Campylomormyrus compressirostris (n = 17) and C. tamandua (n = 12) individuals were used in this study. The biphasic C. compressirostris EOD starts with a short, head-positive phase that is followed by a second head-negative phase with an average total duration of 164 µs. The triphasic EOD of C. tamandua has an initial small head-negative pre-phase followed by a head-positive and final head-negative phase; the average total duration is 316 µs (see EOD inlays, figure 1) [31]. Previous studies have demonstrated the uniformity of the EOD within single Campylomormyrus species, such that the EOD from any given individual is representative of the EOD produced by the species as a whole (e.g. [18,19]). As shown in other studies, neither under breeding conditions nor after gonadal regression was the EOD waveform sexually dimorphic in either species [31,32].
Figure 1.
Choice tests with live fish as stimuli. Choice test results for C. compressirostris (n = 10) and C. tamandua (n = 9) males during interactions with live female and male con- and heterospecifics. EOD overlays represent the stereotypical waveform of each species. Association behaviour of male focal fish to con- and heterospecific stimuli is displayed as the mean (+s.e.) number of observations per night.
Prior to experiments, fish were kept in communal tanks under 12 L : 12 D cycle and fed daily with red and white mosquito larvae. Focal and stimuli fish came from different aquaria and were thus unfamiliar with each other and the test aquarium. Choice tests were conducted in a test aquarium (160 × 50 × 50 cm) partitioned into three compartments using plastic grids as separators (grid size: 13 × 13 × 13 mm; see electronic supplementary material). The focal fish was placed in the centre compartment (96 × 50 × 50 cm) 4 h prior to testing. Stimuli were located in two identical compartments (32 × 50 × 50 cm) on each side of the centre compartment (one live fish or dipole carbon electrode (for EOD playback) per compartment). The grid partitions separating the focal and stimuli fish allowed visual, olfactory and electric communication but prevented the fish from swimming freely throughout the tank. Two areas of 32 cm adjacent to each stimulus were marked on the exterior of the test aquarium and designated as ‘preference zones’ (see the electronic supplementary material). A plastic tube for shelter was provided in the centre of each compartment.
Breeding conditions were simulated by slowly decreasing water conductivity [32–34]. Fish were then held at a water conductivity of 250 ± 10 µS cm−1 and a temperature of 26 ± 2°C. With the exception of two individuals, all females (C. compressirostris n = 7; C. tamandua n = 3) laid eggs at least once under breeding conditions. All males showed a kink in their anal fin base indicating sex and maturity [26]. Concluding breeding condition experiments, the water conductivity was slowly increased to 700 ± 20 µS cm−1 to induce gonadal regression [32,34].
2.2. Experimental design
During choice tests using live fish as stimuli, male C. compressirostris (n = 10) and C. tamandua (n = 9) were presented with con- and heterospecific females (C. compressirostris n = 7 or C. tamandua n = 3). During a single test, one focal male was given the choice between one con- and one heterospecific female.
To test whether both conspecific sexes are recognized as a unified category preferred over heterospecifics, we then repeated the previous set-up using only male fish; C. compressirostris (n = 6) males were presented the choice between con- and heterospecific male individuals (C. compressirostris n = 5 or C. tamandua n = 6). For this test, we used any individual male either as a focal or as a stimulus fish. Owing to the small number of C. tamandua males (only nine specimens), this would have reduced our sample size to n = 4 (if C. tamandua had also been used as focal males). Therefore, this test was only performed with C. compressirostris as focal males.
Both C. compressirostris and C. tamandua are nocturnal. Male behaviour was therefore recorded for 12 h overnight (Microsoft LifeCam HD-3000; Spike2 video acquisitions, CED) under infrared illumination (880 nm). The video camera for recording was centred approximately 60 cm in front of the test aquarium. All preference tests were repeated with stimuli fish switched between sides to rule out side-bias, which entailed two nights of observations per individual. The overnight recordings were analysed using a custom written Matlab routine (R2015a, MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA), where the focal fish's physical location in the test aquarium (i.e. in a pre-defined preference zone or in the neutral zone; see the electronic supplementary material, figure S1) was scored every 2 min, as in [26].
To test whether the species-specific EOD alone was sufficient to trigger a preference response, the previous set-up was repeated using digitally synthesized playbacks (see the electronic supplementary material). This was done once under breeding conditions and again after five months with the same individuals under non-breeding conditions (i.e. after gonadal regression) to further test if the association behaviour observed was linked to breeding behaviour. During playback, all possible sources of discrimination except EOD waveform were kept constant, including the sequence of pulse intervals (SPI), or temporal pattern of inter-EOD intervals. In contrast to the species-specific EOD, the SPI is variable and modulated by the fish depending on behavioural context (for review, see [35]). Little to no information is available about the function of inter-EOD intervals in socially isolated Campylomormyrus individuals, but natural SPIs have been shown to elicit stronger responses in mormyrids than artificial or scrambled sequences [36]. Additionally, a previous study reported that C. tamandua does not discriminate between the SPIs of con- and heterospecifics [24]. The inter-EOD-interval sequence selected for playback was therefore based on the SPI recorded from a randomly selected C. compressirostris female. We opted to use the same natural SPI in all playbacks to minimize any possible experimental variation/artefacts and to test only the influence of the EOD waveform on association preference. This experimental set-up is in accordance with other behavioural studies on African weakly electric fishes [22,26,29].
During choice tests using playback stimuli, male C. compressirostris (n = 10) and C. tamandua (n = 9) were presented with eight episodes of a 2 min SPI sequence, with a 2 min pause between two subsequent episodes (total playback time of 16 min) [23,26,29]. The orientation of the playback waveform (left versus right) was randomly interchanged after a given episode to control for any side-bias. Recording procedures (i.e. the position of video camera, recording equipment, software, etc.) were the same as in the choice tests using live fish as stimuli. The total time each focal male spent in the pre-defined preference zones was analysed using a custom-written Matlab routine.
The same individuals were used in all experiments, but there was a minimum of 30 days between the choice tests using live fish as stimuli and choice tests using playback stimuli. We also note that while mormyrids can be trained to distinguish minimal differences in individual EOD waveforms [21,22], no reward or punishment was associated with these experiments. The fish, therefore, were and remained untrained.
Experimental set-up and design were purposefully modelled after [26]. All raw data for our experiments are available in Dryad [37].
2.3. Statistics
We scored responses of C. compressirostris and C. tamandua males as the number of observations or total time spent (before and after switching side-assignments) associating with a given stimulus fish or playback electrode within the pre-defined preference zones, respectively. Previous experiments with other fish species, including mosquitofish [38], the redband darter [8] and gobies [39,40], suggest that position and time spent close to potential mates is a good proxy for preference respective mate choice; to the best of our knowledge, no information in this regard is available for mormyrids.
Data were analysed according to [41]. Shortly, the SOP was calculated as ((time spent near conspecific fish or playback stimulus – time spent near heterospecific fish or playback stimulus)/(time spent near conspecific fish or playback stimulus + time spent near heterospecific fish or playback stimulus)), so SOP values could range from −1 (complete avoidance of the conspecific fish or playback stimulus) to 1 (complete preference for conspecific fish or playback stimulus). SOP values were then tested against a random distribution of SOP = 0 using a one-sample t-test. A repeated measures ANOVA was used to compare the SOP of focal fish to playback stimuli during breeding and non-breeding conditions (i.e. after gonadal regression) using the aov function in the built-in R ‘stats’ package [42]. As the same individuals were tested in both conditions, both individual and reproductive stages were included as factors. Significant differences were evaluated with a post hoc Tukey's HSD pairwise comparison with a confidence interval of 95% using the R add-on ‘multcomp’ package [43]. We compared the SOP values of male C. compressirostris to male or female stimuli using a two-tailed t-test. All statistical tests were performed in R using the integrated development environment RStudio [42,44].
2.4. Research ethics and animal treatment
All specimens used in the experiment were imported from Kinshasa (Democratic Republic of the Congo) and are currently maintained at the University of Potsdam in Germany. All experiments were approved by the Deputy for Animal Welfare of the University of Potsdam and are in accordance with national legal requirements.
3. Results
To investigate the potential role of male species recognition and mate choice in pre-zygotic reproductive isolation between C. compressirostris and C. tamandua, we conducted dichotomous choice tests. When male focal fish were given the choice between live con- and heterospecific females, both C. tamandua and C. compressirostris males showed a significant association preference for conspecific females (one-sample t-test; C. tamandua: t = 2.725, d.f. = 8, p = 0.013 and C. compressirostris: t = 2.235, d.f. = 9, p = 0.026; figure 1). When male C. compressirostris focal fish were given the choice between live con- and heterospecific males, C. compressirostris males associated significantly more with conspecific males (one-sample t-test; t = 3.103, d.f. = 5, p = 0.013; figure 1). There was no significant difference in the SOP of C. compressirostris males to either conspecific females or males (two-sided t-test; t = 1.031, d.f. = 5, p-value = 0.35; figure 1).
To further test whether the species-specific EOD waveform was sufficient to elicit differential responses, we tested the preference behaviour of focal males using playback stimuli. Under breeding conditions, only C. compressirostris males associated significantly more with the conspecific playback (one-sample t-test; t = 4.021, d.f. = 9, p = 0.002). Campylomormyrus tamandua males did not discriminate between the two playback stimuli (one-sample t-test; t = 0.200, d.f. = 8, p = 0.423; figure 2). Also under non-breeding conditions (i.e. after gonadal regression), C. compressirostris males associated significantly more with the conspecific playback, whereas C. tamandua males did not (one-sample t-test; C. compressirostris: t = 5.948, d.f. = 9, p < 0.001 and C. tamandua: t = 0.814, d.f. = 8, p = 0.782). We found no significant difference in the SOP between the two trials (i.e. breeding conditions versus gonadal regression) for either C. compressirostris or C. tamandua males (analysis of variance, p = 0.160 and p = 0.558, respectively; figure 2).
Figure 2.
Choice tests with playback stimuli. Choice test results for C. compressirostris (n = 10) and C. tamandua (n = 9) males during interactions with playback stimuli, conducted during the breeding season and after gonadal regression. EOD overlays represent the stereotypical waveform of each species. Association behaviour of male focal fish to con- and heterospecific playback stimuli is displayed as the mean (+s.e.) time in seconds spent near the electrode per night.
4. Discussion
In this study, we investigated the association preference of two sympatric mormyrid weakly electric fish species, Campylomormyrus compressirostris and C. tamandua, and whether or not the EOD waveform facilitates species recognition under breeding and non-breeding conditions.
We show that in both species, males preferentially associate with con- versus heterospecific females in choice tests under breeding conditions using live fish as stimuli, i.e. with all sensory modalities available. Previous research has shown that female C. compressirostris do not discriminate between conspecific and C. tamandua males under comparable conditions [26], suggesting that males may be responsible for species recognition and discrimination. This corresponds with a recent hybrid breeding experiment, where C. compressirostris males and C. tamandua females (and vice versa) were held together in breeding groups; while females spawned eggs several times despite the absence of conspecific males, the eggs were never fertilized by the heterospecific males [45].
Interestingly, this preferential association with conspecifics was not only observed during male–female interactions. Male C. compressirostris also discriminated between male con- and heterospecifics, displaying a significantly stronger association preference for male conspecifics.
In previous studies with mormyrids, the ability to discriminate between con- and heterospecifics has frequently been associated with mate choice. However, a distinction should be made between mate choice and species recognition, another important aspect of behavioural isolation [30]. Given that C. compressirostris focal males respond equally to female and male stimuli, we suggest that the conspecific association preference observed may be male-mediated species recognition (i.e. a general attraction to conspecifics regardless of sex) rather than mate selection. Indiscriminate conspecific association preferences may have arisen as a social or shoaling behaviour, e.g. group foraging, which has been observed in some mormyrid species (Mormyrops anguilloides [46], Marcusenius altisambesi and Mormyrus rume [47]).
The communication cues facilitating species recognition are often fixed and unique to a species, and necessary for attracting a mate [30]. Among Campylomormyrus weakly electric fishes, the EOD is a species-specific but not sexually dimorphic component of electrocommunication [31,32]. We were therefore interested in investigating if the EOD waveform is necessary and sufficient for conspecific association and potentially mate recognition under both breeding and non-breeding (i.e. after gonadal regression) conditions. To do so, we repeated our choice tests with electrical playback stimuli, keeping all possible discrimination cues except the EOD waveform constant (e.g. EOD amplitude, inter-EOD-interval sequence, and electrode position and orientation in the tank). Under the hypothesis that the EOD waveform is sufficient to elicit a differential response, we predicted that both C. compressirostris and C. tamandua males would preferentially associate with the playback sequence pulse coupled with their own species' EOD waveform. While this prediction was supported for C. compressirostris males, C. tamandua males did not show the expected preference. This was true regardless of the conditions under which the experiment was conducted (i.e. under simulated breeding conditions or after induced gonadal regression). While the absence of an association preference does not inherently imply a lack of perceptual discrimination, these results may suggest that in the absence of other cues, the EOD waveform provides sufficient information for species recognition in some but not all African weakly electric fishes. This should not, however, be interpreted to mean that the electric modality is not at all relevant for recognition in these fishes.
In addition to the species-specific EOD waveform, species recognition in mormyrid fishes may be supported by various sensory modalities, including acoustic signals (Pollimyrus [48,49], Brienomyrus [50] and Gnathonemus [51]) and the lateral line or visual cues (Gnathonemus [52,53]). While little information is available about the function of olfactory systems or the production/detection of hormonal pheromones in mormyrids, pheromones are known to mediate species and mate recognition in numerous other teleost fishes [54]. Therefore, we do not exclude the possibility that chemoreception may also act synergistically with the electrosensory system. Indeed, the use of multiple signals for species recognition has been widely studied in various species [55]. Female swordtail fish Xiphophorus pygmaeus successfully discriminate between conspecific and sympatrically occurring heterospecific X. cortezi males only if presented with at least two species-specific cues, i.e. visual and chemical [56]. Among sympatric cricket species, female Gryllodinus kerkennensis identify conspecifics based solely on the continuity of the male song, while Gryllus campestris require both pulse rate and chirp duration for discrimination [57].
The temporal pattern with which the discharge is produced (sequence pulse interval, SPI) may also transmit important information for species recognition. Our study did not specifically focus on the importance of the temporal pattern of EODs for species recognition, but the inter-EOD interval has been linked to species identification (Campylomormyrus [24]), courtship and spawning behaviour (Pollimyrus [58]), and various other social interactions (Gnathonemus [36] and Brienomyrus [59]). Particular patterns of SPIs also appear to correlate with certain social contexts, as observed in the mormyrid genera Marcusenius and Mormyrus [47]. In one African weakly electric fish (Pollimyrus isidori), SPI cues alone were even sufficient for sex recognition and discrimination [48]. As all EOD waveforms were paired with a C. compressirostris SPI in our playback choice tests, we cannot rule out the possibility that C. tamandua males did not discriminate between con- and heterospecific EODs, because they were presented with a heterospecific SPI (i.e. from C. compressirostris). Nonetheless, a previous study found that C. tamandua could not or did not discriminate between con- and heterospecific SPIs [24].
Overall, our results indicate that in at least two African weakly electric fish species, males can discriminate between con- and heterospecific individuals when live fish are provided as stimuli, whereas a previous study demonstrated that females do not exhibit such discrimination [26]. This suggests that male behaviour may be an important component of pre-zygotic isolation and therefore a mechanism of speciation among these sympatric species. We further hypothesize that species recognition rather than mate selection is driving this conspecific association preference. It should be emphasized that both C. compressirostris and C. tamandua share an EOD of similar duration and shape, however with subtle differences in the pre-phase (i.e. regarding the occurrence of a small initial head-negative phase; see EOD inlays, figure 1). While these species-specific EOD waveform characteristics seem to be a strong cue for species recognition, our results show that it is not sufficient in all cases.
It is important to mention the limited sample size of this study, which is most notably restricted by access to only three C. tamandua females for choice tests with live fish as stimuli. The two Campylomormyrus species investigated also limit our results; by including more species, it would be possible to discern, for example, if male-mediated species recognition is prevalent in all 14 sympatrically occurring Campylomormyrus species. Future investigations will also need to clarify which species-specific cues from various other modalities might play a role in mediating successful species recognition.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We kindly thank Tonio Pieterek for the care of the fish and Ingo Schlupp for comments on the experimental design.
Ethics
All experiments were approved by the Deputy for Animal Welfare of the University of Potsdam and are in accordance with the national legal requirements. All fish used in this experiment are currently maintained at the University of Potsdam in Germany.
Data accessibility
The data used in this study are available in the Dryad Digital Repository: (http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.9kv93) [37].
Authors' contributions
R.N. participated in the design of the study, conducted the experiments, analysed the data and drafted the manuscript; V.H. and J.E. participated in the design of the experiment and implemented code for data analysis; F.P. participated in data collection; F.K. and R.T. conceived, participated in the design and coordinated the study. All authors have read and approved the manuscript for publication.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Funding
Funding for this research was provided by the University of Potsdam to R.T.
References
- 1.Butlin R, et al. 2012. What do we need to know about speciation? Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 27–39. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2011.09.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Plath M, et al. 2013. Genetic differentiation and selection against migrants in evolutionarily replicated extreme environments. Evolution 67, 2647–2661. (doi:10.1111/evo.12133) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Pfennig KS. 1998. The evolution of mate choice and the potential for conflict between species and mate-quality recognition. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 1743–1748. (doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0497) [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kozak GM, Reisland M, Boughmann JW. 2009. Sex differences in mate recognition and conspecific preference in species with mutual mate choice. Evolution 63, 353–365. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00564.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Svensson EI, Karlsson K, Friberg M, Eroukhmanoff F. 2007. Gender differences in species recognition and the evolution of asymmetric sexual isolation. Curr. Biol. 17, 1943–1947. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.09.038) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Trivers RL. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. In Sexual selection and the descent of Man 1871–1971 (ed. Campbell B.), pp. 136–207. Chicago, IL: Aldine Publishing Company. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sargent RC, Gross MR, van den Berghe EP. 1986. Male mate choice in fishes. Anim. Behav. 34, 545–550. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80123-3) [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ciccotto PJ, Gumm JM, Mendelson TC. 2013. Male association preference for conspecifics in the redband darter, Etheostoma luteovinctum (Teleostei: Percidae) based on visual cues. Copeia 2013, 154–159. (doi:10.1643/CE-12-056) [Google Scholar]
- 9.Espinedo CM, Gabor CR, Aspbury AS. 2010. Males, but not females, contribute to sexual isolation between two sympatric species of Gambusia. Evol. Ecol. 24, 865–878. (doi:10.1007/s10682-009-9343-z) [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jones G. 1997. Acoustic signals and speciation: the roles of natural and sexual selection in the evolution of cryptic species. Adv. Study Behav. 26, 317–354. (doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60383-6) [Google Scholar]
- 11.Siemers BM, Schnitzler H-U. 2004. Echolocation signals reflect niche differentiation in five sympatric congeneric bat species. Nature 429, 657–661. (doi:10.1038/nature02487.1.) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Braune P, Schmidt S, Zimmermann E. 2008. Acoustic divergence in the communication of cryptic species of nocturnal primates (Microcebus ssp.). BMC Biol. 6, 19 (doi:10.1186/1741-7007-6-19) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Boughman JW. 2002. How sensory drive can promote speciation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 17, 571–577. (doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02595-8) [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hopkins CD. 1981. On the diversity of electric signals in a community of mormyrid electric fish in West Africa. Integr. Comp. Biol. 21, 211–222. (doi:10.1093/icb/21.1.211) [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bell CC, Myers JP, Russell CJ. 1974. Electric organ discharge patterns during dominance related behavioral displays in Gnathonemus petersii (Mormyridae). J. Comp. Physiol. 92, 201–228. (doi:10.1007/BF00694506) [Google Scholar]
- 16.Moller P, Bauer R. 1973. ‘Communication’ in weakly electric fish, Gnathonemus petersii (Mormyridae) II. Interaction of electric organ discharge activities of two fish. Anim. Behav. 21, 501–512. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(73)80010-7) [Google Scholar]
- 17.Arnegard ME, McIntyre PB, Harmon LJ, Zelditch ML, Crampton WGR, Davis JK, Sullivan JP, Lavoué S, Hopkins CD. 2010. Sexual signal evolution outpaces ecological divergence during electric fish species radiation. Am. Nat. 176, 335–356. (doi:10.1086/655221) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Feulner PGD, Kirschbaum F, Mamonekene V, Ketmaier V, Tiedemann R. 2007. Adaptive radiation in African weakly electric fish (Teleostei: Mormyridae: Campylomormyrus): a combined molecular and morphological approach. J. Evol. Biol. 20, 403–414. (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01181.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lamanna F, Kirschbaum F, Ernst ARR, Feulner PGD, Mamonekene V, Paul C, Tiedemann R. 2016. Species delimitation and phylogenetic relationships in a genus of African weakly-electric fishes (Osteoglossiformes, Mormyridae, Campylomormyrus). Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 101, 8–18. (doi:10.1016/j.ympev.2016.04.035) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Moller P, Serrier J. 1986. Species recognition in mormyrid weakly electric fish. Anim. Behav. 34, 333–339. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80101-4) [Google Scholar]
- 21.Graff C, Kramer B. 1992. Trained weakly-electric fishes Pollimyrus isidori and Gnathonemus petersii discriminate between waveforms of electric pulse discharges. Ethology 90, 279–292. (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1992.tb00839.x) [Google Scholar]
- 22.Paintner S, Kramer B. 2003. Electrosensory basis for individual recognition in a weakly electric, mormyrid fish, Pollimyrus adspersus (Günther, 1866). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 55, 197–208. (doi:10.1007/s00265-003-0690-4) [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hopkins CD, Bass AH. 1981. Temprol coding of species recognition signals in an electric fish. Science 212, 85–87. (doi:10.1126/science.7209524) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kramer B, Kuhn B. 1994. Species recognition by the sequence of discharge intervals in weakly electric fishes of the genus Campylomormyrus (Mormyridae, Teleostei). Anim. Behav. 48, 435–445. (doi:10.1006/anbe.1994.1257) [Google Scholar]
- 25.Tiedemann R, Feulner PGD, Kirschbaum F. 2010. Electric organ discharge divergence promotes ecological speciation in sympatrically occurring African weakly electric fish (Campylomormyrus). In Evolution in action (ed. Glaubrecht M.), pp. 307–321. Berlin, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Feulner PGD, Plath M, Engelmann J, Kirschbaum F, Tiedemann R. 2009. Electrifying love: electric fish use species-specific discharge for mate recognition. Biol. Lett. 5, 225–228. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0566) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Markowski B, Baier B, Kramer B. 2008. Differentiation in electrical pulse waveforms in a pair of sibling dwarf stonebashers, Pollimyrus castelnaui and P. marianne: possible mechanisms and functions (Mormyridae, Teleostei). Behaviour 145, 115–135. (doi:10.1163/156853908782687223) [Google Scholar]
- 28.Schmid D, Kramer LB. 2014. Sexual selection by female choice prevents speciation reversal in a hybridizing trio of mormyrid fish in southern Africa: evidence from playback experiments of electric organ discharges. Behaviour 151, 1703–1734. (doi:10.1163/1568539X-00003214) [Google Scholar]
- 29.Arnegard ME, Jackson BS, Hopkins CD. 2006. Time-domain signal divergence and discrimination without receptor modification in sympatric morphs of electric fishes. J. Exp. Biol. 209, 2182–2198. (doi:10.1242/jeb.02239) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mendelson TC, Shaw KL. 2012. The (mis)concept of species recognition. Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 421–427. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2012.04.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Paul C, Mamonekene V, Vater M, Feulner PGD, Engelmann J, Tiedemann R, Kirschbaum F. 2015. Comparative histology of the adult electric organ among four species of the genus Campylomormyrus (Teleostei: Mormyridae). J. Comp. Physiol. A 201, 357–374. (doi:10.1007/s00359-015-0995-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Nguyen L, Paul C, Mamonekene V, Bartsch P, Tiedemann R, Kirschbaum F. 2017. Reproduction and development in some species of the weakly electric genus Campylomormyrus (Mormyridae, Teleostei). Environ. Biol. Fishes 100, 49–68. (doi:10.1007/s10641-016-0554-1) [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kirschbaum F, Schugardt C. 2002. Reproductive strategies and developmental aspects in mormyrid and gymnotiform fishes. J. Physiol. Paris 96, 557–566. (doi:10.1016/S0928-4257(03)00011-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Schugardt C, Kirschbaum F. 2004. Control of gonadal maturation and regression by experimental variation of environmental factors in the mormyrid fish, Mormyrus rume proboscirostris. Environ. Biol. Fishes 70, 227–233. (doi:10.1023/B:EBFI.0000033340.49266.f3) [Google Scholar]
- 35.Carlson BA. 2002. Electric signaling behavior and the mechanisms of electric organ discharge production in mormyrid fish. J. Physiol. Paris 96, 405–419. (doi:10.1016/S0928-4257(03)00019-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Teyssedre C, Serrier J. 1986. Temporal spacing of signals in communication, studied in weakly-electric mormyrid fish (Teleostei, Pisces). Behav. Processes 12, 77–98. (doi:10.1016/0376-6357(86)90073-2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Nagel R, Kirschbaum F, Engelmann J, Hofmann V, Pawelzik F, Tiedemann R. 2018. Data from: Male-mediated species recognition among African weakly electric fishes. Dryad Digital Repository (doi:10.5061/dryad.9kv93) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 38.Bisazza A, Vaccari G, Pilastro A. 2001. Female mate choice in a mating system dominated by male sexual coercion. Behav. Ecol. 12, 59–64. (doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.beheco.a000379) [Google Scholar]
- 39.Forsgren E. 1992. Predation risk affects mate choice in a gobiid fish. Am. Nat. 140, 1041–1049. (doi:10.1086/285455) [Google Scholar]
- 40.Lehtonen TK, Lindström K. 2008. Repeatability of mating preferences in the sand goby. Anim. Behav. 75, 55–61. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.04.011) [Google Scholar]
- 41.Plath M, et al. 2010. Complementary effect of natural and sexual selection against immigrants maintains differentiation between locally adapted fish. Naturwissenschaften 97, 769–774. (doi:10.1007/s00114-010-0691-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.R Core Team. 2014. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. See https://www.R-project.org/.
- 43.Hothorn T, Bretz F, Westfall P. 2008. Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biometrical J. 50, 346–363. (doi:10.1002/bimj.200810425) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.RStudio Team. 2015. RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Boston, MA: RStudio, Inc. See http://www.rstudio.com/.
- 45.Kirschbaum F, et al. 2016. Intragenus (Campylomormyrus) and intergenus hybrids in mormyrid fish: physiological and histological investigations of the electric organ ontogeny. J. Physiol. Paris 110, 281–301. (doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2017.01.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Arnegard ME, Carlson BA. 2005. Electric organ discharge patterns during group hunting by a mormyrid fish. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 1305–1314. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3101) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Gebhardt K, Böhme M, von der Emde G. 2012. Electrocommunication behaviour during social interactions in two species of pulse-type weakly electric fishes (Mormyridae). J. Fish Biol. 81, 2235–2254. (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2012.03448.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Crawford JD. 1991. Sex recognition by electric cues in a sound-producing mormyrid fish, Pollimyrus isidori. Brain Behav. Evol. 38, 20–28. (doi:10.1159/000114377) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Lamml M, Kramer B. 2005. Sound production in the reproductive behaviour of the weakly electric fish Pollimyrus marianne. Bioacoustics 15, 51–78. (doi:10.1080/09524622.2005.9753538) [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kramer B, Tautz J, Markl H. 1981. The EOD sound response in weakly electric fish. J. Comp. Physiol. A 143, 435–441. (doi:10.1007/BF00609910) [Google Scholar]
- 51.Fletcher LB, Crawford JD. 2001. Acoustic detection by sound-producing fishes (Mormyridae): the role of gas-filled tympanic bladders. J. Exp. Biol. 204, 175–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.von der Emde G, Bleckmann H. 1998. Finding food: senses involved in foraging for insect larvae in the electric fish Gnathonemus petersii. J. Exp. Biol. 201, 969–980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Landsberger M, von der Emde G, Haverkate D, Schuster S, Gentsch J, Ulbricht E, Reichenbach A, Makarov F, Wagner HJ. 2008. Dim light vision – morphological and functional adaptations of the eye of the mormyrid fish, Gnathonemus petersii. J. Physiol. Paris 102, 291–303. (doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.10.015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Stacey NE, Sorensen PW. 2008. Hormonally derived sex pheromones in fish. In Fish reproduction (eds Rocha MJ, Arukwe A, Kapoor BG), pp. 201–244. Enfield, NH: Science Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Candolin U. 2003. The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biol. Rev. 78, 575–595. (doi:10.1017/S1464793103006158) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hankison SJ, Morris MR. 2003. Avoiding a compromise between sexual selection and species recognition: female swordtail fish assess multiple species-specific cues. Behav. Ecol. 14, 282–287. (doi:10.1093/beheco/14.2.282) [Google Scholar]
- 57.Popov AV, Shuvalov VF. 1977. Phonotactic behavior of crickets. J. Comp. Physiol. A 119, 111–126. (doi:10.1007/BF00655876) [Google Scholar]
- 58.Bratton BO, Kramer B. 1989. Patterns of the electric organ discharge during courtship and spawning in the mormyrid fish, Pollimyrus isidori. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 24, 349–368. (doi:10.1007/BF00293263) [Google Scholar]
- 59.Moller P, Serrier J, Bowling D. 1989. Electric organ discharge displays during social encounter in the weakly electric fish Brienomyrus niger L. (Mormyridae). Ethology 82, 177–191. (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1989.tb00498.x) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Citations
- Nagel R, Kirschbaum F, Engelmann J, Hofmann V, Pawelzik F, Tiedemann R. 2018. Data from: Male-mediated species recognition among African weakly electric fishes. Dryad Digital Repository (doi:10.5061/dryad.9kv93) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data used in this study are available in the Dryad Digital Repository: (http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.9kv93) [37].