Abstract
Background
Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) is often associated with attention allocation and emotional regulation difficulties, but the brain dynamics underlying these deficits are unknown. The emotional Stroop task (EST) is an ideal means to monitor these difficulties, because participants are asked to attend to non-emotional aspects of the stimuli. In this study, we used magnetoencephalography (MEG) and the EST to monitor attention allocation and emotional regulation during processing of emotionally-charged stimuli in combat veterans with and without PTSD.
Methods
Thirty-one veterans with PTSD and 20 without PTSD performed the EST during MEG. Three categories of stimuli were used, including combat-related, generally-threatening, and neutral words. MEG data were imaged in the time-frequency domain and the network dynamics were probed for differences in processing threatening and non-threatening words
Results
Behaviorally, veterans with PTSD were significantly slower in responding to combat-related relative to neutral and generally-threatening words. Veterans without PTSD exhibited no significant differences in responding to the three different word-types. Neurophysiologically, we found a significant three-way interaction between group, word-type, and time period across multiple brain regions. Follow-up testing indicated stronger theta-frequency (4–8 Hz) responses in the right ventral prefrontal (0.4 to 0.8 s) and superior temporal cortices (0.6 to 0.8 s) of veterans without PTSD compared to those with PTSD during the processing of combat-related words.
Conclusions
Our data indicated that veterans with PTSD exhibited deficits in attention allocation and emotional regulation when processing trauma cues, while those without PTSD were able to regulate emotion by directing attention away from threat.
Keywords: Attention Allocation, Emotional Regulation, Emotional Stroop Task, MEG, theta, oscillation, Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
Visual stimuli are comprised of many features competing for representational priority given limited attentional resources (e.g., Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Kastner & Ungerleider, 2000). For example, emotionally-salient stimuli are often associated with rapid temporal region activation (Luo et al., 2010). However, if other stimulus features are more task-relevant, those features receive more attentional resources, while emotional aspects are seemingly deemphasized (e.g., Ochsner & Gross, 2005). In other words, visual attention can amplify specific stimulus features, which affects activation across brain areas critical to representing those particular properties. Attention allocation is generally task-driven, and studies show that even emotionally-salient stimuli can be associated with reduced activity in regions like the amygdala if emotional properties of the stimuli are not task-relevant (e.g., Blair et al., 2007; Mitchell et al., 2007).
Many psychological disorders, including posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), are associated with aberrant attention allocation patterns and altered stimulus representations across brain regions (Blair et al., 2013). For example, PTSD is associated with attentional perseverance to threat, which can lead to hyper-processing of emotional aspects of stimuli (e.g., increased activation in the amygdala; e.g., Rauch et al., 2006). The elevated emotional activation noted in PTSD does not appear to dissipate as much as it does in healthy individuals (Blair et al., 2013), instead, emotional representations within the amygdala and similar regions abnormally persist even when attention appears to be allocated to other stimulus features (Luo et al., 2010; Todd et al., 2015). Alternatively, prefrontal areas typically guiding attentional selection and emotional regulation may not be properly engaged in PTSD patients (e.g., McDermott et al. 2016; Aupperle et al., 2012; Leskin & White 2007). Of course, both of these factors may be involved (Pannu Hayes et al. 2009; Cisler et al., 2011). A key barrier to better characterizing these alterations in PTSD is that the neural timing of attentional allocation and emotional regulation during stimulus processing is not understood. For example, individuals with PTSD may have heightened and/or sustained activity within emotional processing regions as compared to those without PTSD. Alternatively, PTSD may be associated with relatively less activity within emotional regulation areas, and/or this activity may not be sustained over time.
The emotional Stroop task (EST) has been used to monitor attention allocation during processing of emotionally-charged stimuli (e.g., Williams et al., 1996), specifically because participants are asked to attend to non-emotional aspects of the stimuli. The EST is a variant of the classic Stroop task in which participants name aloud the ink color of printed words (Stroop 1935). However, in the EST, words vary in emotional salience. For example, words may be neutral (e.g., file) or negative (e.g., bomb). Individuals with attention allocation alterations resulting from anxiety disorders often respond later (i.e., delayed latency) to negative words compared to other words (Williams et al. 1996; McNally 1998; Metzger et al. 1998), and this especially is true when negative words are personally relevant (e.g., combat-related words for veterans with PTSD; Riemann & McNally, 1995; Becker et al., 2001; Phaf & Kan 2007). Metzger and colleagues (1998) suggested that patients’ longer color-naming latencies for threat-related words arise because emotional aspects of words draw attention and dominate representations, even when the emotionality of the words is task-irrelevant. Healthy individuals typically do not produce different color naming latencies for the various EST list types (e.g., Compton et al., 2000), likely indicating they engage emotional regulation areas and focus on task-relevant word dimensions (Phaf & Kan 2007; White et al. 2015). Regarding neural correlates, recent functional neuroimaging studies indicated that the EST elicits activation within the PFC, cingulate areas, emotional processing centers in medial temporal areas, and in other brain regions (e.g., Mitterschiffthaler et al. 2008; Ovaysikia et al. 2011; Dresler et al. 2012; Hwang et al., 2014). However, the temporal dynamics across this circuit have not been determined, and such data is imperative to understanding the precise functional contribution of these brain regions to EST processing, especially in those with PTSD.
Therefore, in the current study, we examined the dynamic time course of neural activity across this circuitry by collecting magnetoencephalography (MEG) data while combat veterans with and without PTSD performed an EST. The spatiotemporal sensitivity of MEG makes it uniquely suited for probing emotional processing during the EST (Georgopoulos et al. 2010; Engdahl et al. 2010; James et al. 2013; James et al. 2015; Anders et al. 2015; Wilson et al., 2016), because MEG allows the amplitude and duration of neural activity to be precisely quantified during task performance. Our primary goal in this study was to identify the spatiotemporal dynamics of attentional control and emotional regulation during the processing of neutral and emotionally-salient stimuli (both personally-relevant and personally-irrelevant) in combat veterans with and without PTSD. We hypothesized that veterans without PTSD would initially activate emotional representations of threatening stimuli (e.g., Luo et al, 2010; Thomas et al., 2013), but that such activity would dissipate and emerge in executive control regions during early processing, and then be largely sustained until task completion. This overall pattern of activation would be consistent with White et al. (2015), who examined a sample of combat veterans without PTSD using fMRI and a similar task. Thus, we predicted veterans without PTSD would show sustained responses in prefrontal cortices during combat–related and general threat word processing compared to neutral words, mainly reflecting executive control during EST processing. We hypothesized that veterans with PTSD would engage emotional processing areas such as the amygdalae and other medial temporal regions during initial processing of threatening stimuli, and that activity would be sustained in these regions throughout the task. Finally, we hypothesized that veterans with PTSD would show reduced neural activity in prefrontal cortices relative to veterans without PTSD while processing combat-related words, but not general threat or neutral words. Depending on the time course, such findings would indicate aberrant attention allocation and/or emotional regulation in veterans with PTSD.
Materials and Methods
Participants
We recruited male combat veterans from the Omaha area, 31 had PTSD and 20 did not; healthy veterans did not have any psychiatric or neurological condition by history and evaluation with the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI; Sheehan et al. 1998). PTSD was diagnosed or ruled out using the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS; Blake et al. 1995) and the F1/I2 rule (Weathers et al. 1999). For the F1/I2 rule, one is diagnosed with PTSD if they experienced trauma related symptoms on the standard CAPS scale once or more in the past month and the severity of the symptoms were moderate, severe or extreme. Veterans with PTSD were not excluded for depression or anxiety symptoms frequently comorbid with PTSD, but were free of other diagnoses according to the MINI (Sheehan et al., 1998). Participants with and without PTSD were matched on age, ethnicity, education level, and handedness. The two groups were matched on education level, but we did not administer a specific measure of general intelligence. General exclusionary criteria included medical diagnoses affecting CNS function, known brain neoplasm or lesion, history of significant head trauma, and ferromagnetic implants. Written informed consent was obtained and the Institutional Review Board of Creighton University approved the study.
Emotional Stroop Task
Participants completed the EST while seated in the MEG chamber. Task stimuli included three word lists, a combat-related threat list, a general threat list, and a neutral list. Each list contained 30 monosyllabic words. The combat-related threat list was intended to be personally-relevant to combat veterans and was comprised of words related to things encountered in a warzone (e.g., bomb, seize), the general threat list contained words that were negative in valence, but not related to combat (e.g., tax, witch). The neutral list contained words that were non-threatening (e.g., self, flour). We determined list-inclusion based on our judgments, and we asked a recent U.S. military veteran to verify that words were included on the appropriate lists (e.g., that bomb is a combat-related threat word). The three word lists were equated on various lexical features including: length, Hyperspace Analogue to Language (HAL) frequency (Lund & Burgess 1996), orthographic and phonological neighborhood size. We balanced these lexical features across lists based on guidelines provided by a previous meta-analysis of the EST in behavioral studies (Larsen et al. 2006). We used the English Lexicon Project database to determine average naming latency, naming accuracy, lexical decision time, and lexical decision accuracy for each word and equated the word lists (Balota et al. 2007), so that the three lists did not differ from one another (all Fs < 1.09, p > 0.34). However, as expected, the lists did differ according to their emotional arousal and valence ratings (F(2, 89) = 233.406, MSW= 0.111, p < 0.0001; F(2, 89) = 273.218, MSW= 0.283, p < 0.0001), respectively. Arousal and valence ratings used Estes and Adelman’s (2008) normative ratings derived from healthy adults’ ratings of emotional arousal (1 being not arousing to 7 being highly arousing) and emotional valence (1 for the most negative to 7 for the most positive) for individual monosyllabic words. By design, our combat-related threat list words had higher arousal and lower valence ratings than neutral words (both t’s > 20.4 and p’s < 0.0001), but did not differ from the general-threat words in arousal or valence (all t’s < 1.45 and p’s > 0.152). General-threat words also had higher arousal and lower valence ratings than neutral words (both t’s > 19.22 and p’s < 0.0001).
During a single MEG session, each 30-word list was presented three times resulting in 270 total trials (90 neutral, 90 general-threat, 90 combat-related) separated into nine experimental blocks. A blocked design was selected because, as previous research noted, blocked designs are associated with more robust emotional Stroop responses (Cisler et al. 2011). Participants were naïve to the existence of different word lists per block. We randomized word order within each list across presentation blocks. Within each EST trial, participants first viewed a fixation cross for 1 s, then viewed a list item (e.g., bomb) for 2 s. Participants were instructed to vocally respond as soon as possible to the list item (Figure 1). An experimenter scored participant responses as correct (i.e., named the color correctly), incorrect, or as a noise trial (e.g., the participant coughed, etc.) using a keyboard attached to the stimulus presentation computer. Each word was centered on a screen at eye-level approximately 110 cm from the head. Items were presented in red, blue, or green font, and item color was randomly assigned. Reaction times were measured using a dual-plane accelerometer attached to the lower lip and digitized at 1 kHz with the MEG data. Voice onset was determined by a sharp increase in the accelerometer signal amplitude for each person. This approach produces response time accuracy near 1 ms. Total MEG recording time was about 14 minutes per person.
MEG Data Acquisition & Coregistration
With an acquisition bandwidth of 0.1–330 Hz, neuromagnetic responses were sampled continuously at 1 kHz using an Elekta MEG system with 306 magnetic sensors (Elekta, Helsinki, Finland). Using MaxFilter (v2.2; Elekta), MEG data from each participant were individually corrected for head movement and subjected to noise reduction using the signal space separation method with a temporal extension (Taulu & Simola 2006). MEG data were then coregistered with structural T1-weighted MRI data using BESA MRI (V2.0).
MEG Time-Frequency Transformation & Statistics
Cardio-artifacts were removed from the data using signal-space projection, which was accounted for during source reconstruction (Uusitalo & Ilmoniemi 1997). The continuous magnetic time series was divided into epochs of 3.0 s duration (−1.0 to 2.0 s), with 0.0 s being the onset of the word and the baseline being the −0.2 to 0.0 s time bin. Epochs containing artifacts were rejected based on a fixed threshold method, supplemented with visual inspection.
Artifact-free epochs were transformed into the time-frequency domain using complex demodulation (resolution: 2.0 Hz, 25 ms), and the resulting spectral power estimations per sensor were averaged over trials to generate time-frequency plots of mean spectral density. Sensor-level data were normalized by dividing the power value of each time-frequency bin by the respective bin’s baseline power, which was calculated as the mean power during the −0.2 to 0 s time period. We then used a data-driven approach to derive the time-frequency windows of interest. Briefly, windows of interest were determined by statistical analysis of sensor-level spectrograms across the array of gradiometers during the first second of stimulus processing (mean RT: 641 ms). To reduce risk of false positives, while maintaining sensitivity, we followed a two-stage procedure involving nonparametric permutation testing to control for Type 1 error (Ernst 2004; Maris & Oostenveld 2007). This method has been extensively described in previous publications (Wilson et al. 2014; Wilson et al. 2015; Heinrichs-Graham & Wilson 2015). Based on these analyses, the time-frequency windows containing significant oscillatory events across all participants were selected for imaging.
MEG Source Imaging & Statistics
Cortical networks were imaged through an extension of the linearly constrained minimum variance vector beamformer, which employs spatial filters in the frequency domain to calculate source power for the whole brain volume (Gross et al. 2001; Hillebrand et al. 2005). The single images were derived from the cross-spectral densities of all combinations of MEG gradiometers averaged over the time-frequency range of interest, and the solution of the forward problem for each location on a grid specified by input voxel space. Following convention, the power in these images was normalized per participant using a separately averaged pre-stimulus noise period of equal duration and bandwidth (Hillebrand et al., 2005).
Normalized source power was computed for the selected time-frequency bands in each participant at 4.0 × 4.0 × 4.0 mm resolution. Prior to statistical analysis, each participant’s functional images were transformed into standardized space and spatially resampled. The resulting 3D maps of functional brain activity were statistically evaluated using a three-way mixed model ANOVA, with group (2 levels) as a between-subjects factor, and condition (3 levels) and time (4 levels; see below) as within-subjects factors. Follow-up t-tests were conducted on significant interaction effects using a two-stage approach similar to the sensor level analysis to control for Type 1 error. Briefly, two-sample t-tests were used to examine group effects per word list, whereas paired-samples t-tests were conducted to probe word-list effects per group in each time-frequency bin of interest. In the first stage, t-tests were conducted on each voxel and the output was thresholded at (p < 0.05) to create statistical parametric maps (SPMs) showing clusters of potentially significant differences between groups (e.g., PTSD < controls) or conditions (e.g., neutral < general threat). In stage two, a cluster value was derived for each cluster surviving stage one, and permutation testing was used to test the significance of the observed clusters. For each comparison, at least 10,000 permutations were computed to build a distribution of cluster values.
Results
Behavioral Performance
A portion of this EST behavioral performance data was previously reported in a study of how attention training affects EST performance (Khanna et al. 2016). Note that the current study includes 10 participants who were not in the previous study. For the 51 participants included in the current study, the mean age and educational levels did not differ between groups (p = 0.55 and p = 0.49, respectively). Mean and SD data are shown in Table 1. Consistent with enrollment criteria, veterans with PTSD had higher CAPS scores than veterans without PTSD even after controlling for combat exposure, t(38) = 10.95 p <.001.
Table 1.
Group | # of Participants | Age years | Education years | Caps | Combat-related Threat RT | General Threat RT | Neutral RT |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
PTSD | 26 | 33.94 (9.03) |
15.16 (2.40) |
73.81 (15.92) |
762.40 (232.93) |
729.26 (181.77) |
717.90 (176.21) |
Non-PTSD | 16 | 32.45 (7.72) |
14.70 (2.15) |
21.13 (13.77) |
639.07 (94.90) |
638.79 (98.84) |
646.48 (108.61) |
Reaction time data were lost for nine participants (five with PTSD) due to a technical error. For the remaining 42 participants, a mixed-model ANOVA comparing naming latencies for each group across the three lists revealed an interaction, F(2, 80) = 4.18, p = .019. There was no main effect of group or list, although both were suggestive, F(1, 40) = 3.27, p = .078 for group, and F(1, 40) = 2.61, p = .114 for list. To interrogate the interaction effect, we examined color-naming latencies for the three lists using paired-samples t-tests in each group. We found veterans without PTSD showed no difference in color-naming latencies across lists (all p’s > 0.20), while veterans with PTSD, displayed longer color-naming latencies for combat-related lists than neutral (t(25) = 2.58, p = .016) and general threat lists (t(25) = 2.23, p = .035). Color-naming latencies were marginally slower for general threat words as compared to neutral words, t(25) = 1.99, p = .058. The mean color-naming latencies and SD’s per list for each group are reported in Table 1.
MEG Sensor-Level Analysis
Sensor level spectrograms were examined statistically using nonparametric permutation testing to derive precise time-frequency bins for follow-up beamforming analyses. These analyses indicated a significant cluster of sustained theta oscillatory activity (4–8 Hz) that began shortly after onset of the word and continued through the onset of the vocal response (p < 0.001, corrected). To evaluate the dynamics, we split this significant theta response into four non-overlapping time bins of 0.2 s duration (i.e., 0–0.2, 0.2–0.4, 0.4–0.6, and 0.6–0.8 s), and each time window was imaged for statistical analyses.
MEG Imaging Analysis
We initially conducted a 2 × 3 × 4 mixed-model ANOVA, with group as a between-subjects factor and condition and time as within-subjects factors. The results indicated a significant three-way interaction in multiple brain regions (F(6, 588) = 2.12, p < .05; Figure 2), group × condition (F(2, 588) = 3.01, p < .05), group × time (F(3, 588) = 2.62, p < .05), and condition × time (F(6, 588) = 2.12, p < .05) two-way interactions, and main effects of time (F(3, 588) = 2.62, p < .05) and condition (F(2, 588) = 3.01, p < .05). Note that these F-values correspond to the threshold for a significant effect and not the peak voxel or cluster, and that multiple brain regions were significant for each interaction and main effect. To probe interaction effects, two-sample (between-group) and paired-sample (within-group) t-tests were performed, with nonparametric permutation testing used to correct for multiple comparisons.
Between Group Effects
For combat-related words, significant differences between veterans with and without PTSD emerged in the right ventral PFC in the 0.4–0.6 s time period and extended into the 0.6–0.8 s window. Differences in the latter window also included the right superior temporal cortices. In all cases, findings reflected significantly stronger theta activity in veterans without PTSD relative to those with PTSD (p < 0.05, corrected; Figure 3). No other between-group comparisons were significant. To examine the relationship of these data with PTSD symptomatology, we conducted Spearman correlations using the peak voxel value in the right ventral PFC (RVPFC) cluster for the 0.4–0.6 s time window and individual CAPS scores, separately for the PTSD and the non-PTSD groups. Briefly, the strength of theta activity in the RVPFC was marginally correlated with CAPS in the non-PTSD group, r(20) =0.402, (p = .079). There was no correlation in the PTSD group.
Within Group Effects
Given our hypotheses, we were also interested in the neural dynamics serving EST performance in each group, which we examined through paired-samples t-tests using the different words lists (e.g., combat-related threat vs. neutral words) in each group.
General Threat vs. Neutral Words
Veterans without PTSD exhibited stronger left dorsolateral PFC theta activity during general threat words in the 0.2 to 0.4 s time window (p < 0.05, corrected), which spread to include areas of the left inferior frontal, superior and middle temporal, and VPFC, as well as the right supramarginal gyrus during the 0.4–0.6 s time period (Figure 4). Greater theta was sustained in right supramarginal gyrus through the 0.6–0.8 s time bin in veterans without PTSD (p < 0.05, corrected; Figure 4). In contrast, veterans with PTSD exhibited greater theta responses during the 0.6–0.8 s time window for general threat compared to neutral words in the left hippocampus and amygdala, along with the opposite pattern (neutral > general threat) in the left medial prefrontal cortices (Figure 4). No other time period per group contained significant effects.
Combat-related vs. Neutral Words
Veterans without PTSD had stronger theta activity in the left inferior frontal cortices during combat-related relative to neutral word processing from 0.4–0.6 s (p < 0.05, corrected; Figure 4), which dissipated thereafter. In the 0.6–0.8 s time period, theta was stronger for combat words in the RVPFC stretching into right medial temporal structures (hippocampus and amygdala; p < 0.05, corrected; Figure 4). In veterans with PTSD, theta activity was greater for neutral compared to combat words in the parieto-occipital (0.4–0.6 s, Figure 4) and cingulate cortices (0.6–0.8 s; p < 0.05; corrected; Figure 4). No other time periods contained significant effects in either group.
Combat-related vs. General Threat Words
There were no significant differences between these word types in any time bin for veterans without PTSD. In those with PTSD, stronger theta activity was observed for general threat words from 0.4–0.6 s in the cingulate and the RVPFC, extending into the right superior temporal gyrus and into right medial temporal structures (hippocampus/amygdala; p < 0.05, corrected; Figure 4).
Discussion
Our overall pattern of results indicated veterans with and without PTSD process and represent threatening stimuli differently. Specifically, our key results indicated reduced theta activity in the right PFC of veterans with PTSD compared to those without PTSD during the processing of combat-related words, but not other types of words. This finding suggests that veterans with PTSD have impaired attentional control during the processing of personally-relevant emotional stimuli.
Our main findings supported the central hypotheses of the study regarding neural activity in veterans with PTSD during the EST. Specifically, veterans with PTSD exhibited activity within emotional processing medial temporal areas that persisted through the task, while also displaying lower levels of activation within attention and emotional regulation areas of the PFC, especially during combat-related words. This suggests an emotional representation bias for those with PTSD, originating from aberrant attentional processes or dampened emotional regulation. In addition, we observed the predicted pattern of activity for combat-veterans without PTSD. Essentially, healthy veterans showed relatively high levels of sustained activation within prefrontal regions throughout stimuli processing in most conditions, and significantly stronger activity in the PFC relative to those with PTSD during combat-related words. Interestingly, these results support recent findings suggesting that individuals exposed to combat who do not develop PTSD display impressive frontal activity that may reflect emotional regulation in the face of threatening stimuli (e.g., White et al., 2013; Blair et al., 2013; New et al., 2009).
Critically, the most important evidence supporting this model in the current study was revealed by group comparisons of combat-related word processing. Veterans without PTSD had greater theta activity in the RVPFC, which is often implicated in emotional regulation (e.g., Hariri et al. 2003; Ochsner & Gross 2005; Heatherton & Wagner 2011; Veit et al. 2012; Buhle et al. 2014), as it mediates activity in the amygdalae and neighboring medial temporal structures, which are associated with emotional stimuli processing (e.g., LaBar & Cabeza 2006; Kober et al. 2008). Additionally, participants without PTSD displayed greater activation in the hippocampus and amygdala when processing combat-related as compared to neutral words, consistent with the personal relevance of these words. However, this elevated limbic activation was paired with increased RVPFC activation suggesting healthy veterans may engage prefrontal regions to downregulate medial temporal activity, contributing to their healthy adaptation after combat trauma. Perhaps individuals able to recruit these emotional regulation areas develop the balance needed to suppress emotional biases and persevere in the face of trauma, as suggested by New and colleagues (2009).
On the other hand, our neurodynamic findings indicate veterans with PTSD did not show effective prefrontal activation in the face of trauma-related words (across all list-by-list comparisons). In addition, these findings align with those of Thomas and colleagues (2013) in their ERP examination of panic disorder patients who were unable to adequately recruit anterior regions during threatening stimuli processing. These neural findings are also consistent with our behavioral observations that veterans with PTSD displayed longer color-naming latencies for combat-related words compared to other words. Importantly, veterans with PTSD displayed relatively greater theta activity in attention allocation and emotion regulation areas such as the RVPFC, right superior temporal gyrus, and areas of the cingulate while processing generally threatening words as compared to combat-related words. Thus, veterans with combat-related PTSD may be limited in their engagement of attention allocation and emotional regulation areas in the face of combat-related threatening stimuli, but they can engage these executive areas in response to other types of threatening (generally negative) stimuli. This suggests patients with PTSD may not necessarily have a global impediment in attention allocation, response inhibition, and emotional regulation, but that their overall representation of personally threatening stimuli is dominated by the emotional salience of the stimuli, even when task demands should direct attention and representational activity to other features (e.g., ink color) of the stimuli.
These findings suggest both altered attention allocation and perseverance of threat processing in PTSD. Alterations in these mechanisms were suggested in a recent meta-analysis of behavioral EST effects in PTSD (Cisler et al. 2011) and by previous fMRI examinations of veterans with PTSD performing different, but similar, affective tasks (Pannu Hayes et al. 2009; Blair et al., 2013; White et al., 2015). However, our findings extend these data by providing the time course of neural activity in veterans with PTSD, and suggest both hypoactive attentional control and emotional regulation networks, and enhanced threat detection processing. Our results also support models suggested by White et al. (2013) and New et al (2009) for combat-exposed veterans without PTSD, in that resilience may be partly attributable to the ability to recruit attentional control and emotional regulation areas even in the face of personally-relevant threat.
Although our results are compelling, they should be interpreted with caution. First, a larger sample of veterans with and without PTSD would have been ideal. Second, although we equated our participant groups on education level and other demographic factors, directly measuring general intelligence would have enhanced the study. Future work should explore the degree to which personal-relevance mediates the impediment in attention allocation and emotional regulation, perhaps by adding a word list personally-selected by the participant as connected to their traumatic event. Future studies could also test whether computer-mediated attention training such as attention bias modification treatment, and/or attentional control treatment (e.g., Bar-Haim 2010; Schoorl et al. 2013; Schoorl et al. 2014; Kuckertz et al. 2014; Badura-Brack et al. 2015; McDermott et al. 2016b) normalizes these observed neural aberrations associated with PTSD. Recent behavioral findings on a subset of this PTSD group who completed attention training treatment and then a second EST, found symptom suppression and color-naming performance indistinguishable from veterans without PTSD (Khanna et al. 2016); however, it remains unknown if these behavioral changes were linked with modulations in threat detection (e.g. medial temporal) and/or emotional regulation (e.g. ventral PFC) networks. Finally, this work could be improved by a prospective study examining the attention allocation patterns of those who experience a new traumatic event but do not develop PTSD. Perhaps some individuals are resilient to PTSD because they have a superior ability to engage emotion regulation areas in the face of threat, or perhaps some are better able to quickly adapt once traumatic events occur such that when faced with threat, they are able to direct stimuli representations away from emotional dimensions.
Acknowledgments
Financial Support: This research was supported by a grant from the nonprofit organization At Ease, USA (ABB), by a Creighton University College of Arts and Science Summer Undergraduate Research Fellowship (TJM), the Kinman-Oldfield Award from the University of Nebraska Foundation (TWW), grant R01-MH103220 from the National Institutes of Health (TWW), and award #1539067 from the National Science Foundation (TWW). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Previous Presentations: Portions of this work were presented at the Association for Psychological Science (APS) meeting in San Francisco, CA on May 22–25, 2014 and at the Nebraska Neuroscience Symposium in Omaha, NE on September 24, 2014.
Conflict of interest: None
Ethical Standards: The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.
References
- Anders SL, Peterson CK, James LM, Engdahl B, Leuthold AC, Georgopoulos AP. Neural communication in posttraumatic growth. Experimental Brain Research. 2015;233(7):2013–2020. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4272-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aupperle RL, Melrose AJ, Stein MB, Paulus MP. Executive function and PTSD: Disengaging from trauma. Neuropharmacology. 2012;62:686–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badura-Brack AS, Naim R, Ryan TJ, Levy O, Abend R, Khanna MM, McDermott TJ, Pine DS, Bar-Haim Y. Effect of attention training on attention bias variability and PTSD symptoms: Randomized controlled trials in Israeli and U.S. combat veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2015;172(12):1233–1241. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14121578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balota DA, Yap MJ, Cortese MJ, Hutchison KA, Kessler B, Loftis B, Neely JH, Nelson DL, Simpson GB, Treiman R. The English Lexicon Project. Behavior Research Methods. 2007;39:445–459. doi: 10.3758/bf03193014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bar-Haim Y. Research Review: attention bias modification (ABM): a novel treatment for anxiety disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51:859–870. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02251.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker ES, Rinck M, Margraf J, Roth WT. The emotional Stroop effect in anxiety disorders - General emotionality or disorder specificity? Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2001;15:147–159. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6185(01)00055-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair KS, Smith BW, Mitchell DGV, Morton J, Vythilingam M, Pessoa L, Fridberg D, Zametkin A, Nelson EE, Drevets WC, Pine DS, Martin A, Blair RJR. Modulation of emotion by cognition and cognition by emotion. Neuroimage. 2007;35:430–440. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair KS, Vythilingam M, Crowe SL, McCaffrey DE, Ng P, Wu CC, Scaramozza M, Mondillo K, Pine DS, Charney DS, Blair RJR. Cognitive control of attention is differentially affected in trauma-exposed individuals with and without post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychological Medicine. 2013;43:85–95. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712000840. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blake DD, Weathers FW, Nagy LM, Kaloupek DG, Gusman FD, Charney DS, Keane TM. The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1995;8:75–90. doi: 10.1007/BF02105408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, Weber J, Ochsner KN. Cognitive Reappraisal of Emotion: A Meta-Analysis of Human Neuroimaging Studies. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;24:2981–2990. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cisler JM, Wolitzky-Taylor KB, Adams TG, Jr, Babson KA, Badour CL, Willems JL. The emotional Stroop task and posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2011;31:817–828. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compton RJ, Banich MT, Milham MP, Heller W, Miller GA, Scalf PE, Webb A, Cohen NJ, Wszalek T, Kramer A. Paying attention to emotion: An fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2000:54–54. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.2.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual-attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1995;18:193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dresler T, Attar CH, Spitzer C, Lowe B, Deckert J, Buchel C, Ehlis AC, Fallgatter AJ. Neural correlates of the emotional Stroop task in panic disorder patients: An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Psychiatry Research. 2012;46:1627–1634. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engdahl B, Leuthold AC, Tan H-RM, Lewis SM, Winskowski AM, Dikel TN, Georgopoulos AP. Post-traumatic stress disorder: a right temporal lobe syndrome? Journal Neural Engineering. 2010;7(6):066005. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/7/6/066005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ernst MD. Permutation methods: A basis for exact inference. Statistical Science. 2004;19:676–685. [Google Scholar]
- Estes Z, Adelman JS. Automatic vigilance for negative words is categorical and general. Emotion. 2008;8:453–457. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Georgopoulos AP, Tan HRM, Lewis SM, Leuthold AC, Winskowski AM, Lynch JK, Engdahl B. The synchronous neural interactions test as a functional neuromarker for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD): a robust classification method based on the bootstrap. Journal of Neural Engineering. 2010;7(1):16011. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/7/1/016011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross J, Kujala J, Hamalainen M, Timmermann L, Schnitzler A, Salmelin R. Dynamic imaging of coherent sources: Studying neural interactions in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2001;98:694–699. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Fera F, Weinberger DR. Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful stimuli. Biological Psychiatry. 2003;53:494–501. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01786-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heatherton TF, Wagner DD. Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15:132–139. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinrichs-Graham E, Wilson TW. Spatiotemporal oscillatory dynamics during the encoding and maintenance phases of a visual working memory task. Cortex. 2015;69:121–130. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillebrand A, Singh KD, Holliday IE, Furlong PL, Barnes GR. A new approach to neuroimaging with magnetoencephalography. Human Brain Mapping. 2005;25:199–211. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hwang S, White SF, Nolan ZT, Sinclair S, Blair RJR. Neurodevelopmental changes in the responsiveness of systems involved in top down attention and emotional responding. Neuropsychologia. 2014;62:277–285. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James LM, Engdahl BE, Leuthold AC, Lewis SM, Van Kampen E, Georgopoulos AP. Neural network modulation by trauma as a marker of resilience: differences between veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder and resilient controls. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(4):410–418. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James LM, Belitskaya-Lévy I, Lu Y, Wang H, Engdahl BE, Leuthold AC, Georgopoulos AP. Development and application of a diagnostic algorithm for posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Research. 2015;231(1):1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kastner S, Ungerleider LG. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2000;23:315–341. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khanna MM, Badura-Brack AS, McDermott TJ, Shepherd A, Heinrichs-Graham E, Pine DS, Bar-Haim Y, Wilson TW. Attention training normalizes combat-related post-traumatic stress disorder effects on emotional Stroop performance using lexically matched word lists. Cognition and Emotion. 2016;30:1521–1528. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1076769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kober H, Barrett LF, Joseph J, Bliss-Moreau E, Lindquist K, Wager TD. Functional grouping and cortical-subcortical interactions in emotion: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage. 2008;42:998–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuckertz JM, Amir N, Boffa JW, Warren CK, Rindt SEM, Norman S, Ram V, Ziajko L, Webb-Murphy J, McLay R. The effectiveness of an attention bias modification program as an adjunctive treatment for Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2014;63:25–35. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7:54–64. doi: 10.1038/nrn1825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larsen RJ, Mercer KA, Balota DA. Lexical characteristics of words used in emotional Stroop experiments. Emotion. 2006;6:62–72. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leskin LP, White PM. Attentional networks reveal executive function deficits in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychology. 2007;21:275–284. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.3.275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lund K, Burgess C. Hyperspace analogue to language (HAL): A general model semantic representation. Brain and Cognition. 1996;30:5–5. [Google Scholar]
- Luo Q, Holroyd T, Majestic C, Cheng X, Schechter J, Blair RJ. Emotional Automaticity Is a Matter of Timing. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:5825–5829. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.BC-5668-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maris E, Oostenveld R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2007;164:177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDermott TJ, Badura-Brack AS, Becker KM, Ryan TJ, Bar-Haim Y, Pine DS, Khanna MM, Heinrichs-Graham E, Wilson TW. Male Veterans with PTSD exhibit aberrant neural dynamics during working memory encoding. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience. 2016;164:251–260. doi: 10.1503/jpn.150058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDermott TJ, Badura-Brack AS, Becker KM, Ryan TJ, Bar-Haim Y, Pine DS, Khanna MM, Heinrichs-Graham E, Wilson TW. Attention training improves aberrant neural dynamics during working memory processing in veterans with PTSD. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2016b;16:1140–1149. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0459-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNally RJ. Experimental approaches to cognitive abnormality in posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology Review. 1998;18:971–982. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00036-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metzger LJ, Orr SP, Lasko NB, McNally RJ, Pitman RK. Seeking the source of emotional stroop interference effects in PTSD: A study of P3s to traumatic words. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science. 1997;32:43–51. doi: 10.1007/BF02688612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell DGV, Nakic M, Fridberg D, Kamel N, Pine DS, Blair RJR. The impact of processing load on emotion. Neuroimage. 2007;34:1299–1309. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitterschiffthaler MT, Williams SCR, Walsh ND, Cleare AJ, Donaldson C, Scott J, Fu CHY. Neural basis of the emotional Stroop interference effect in major depression. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38:247–256. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- New AS, Fan J, Murrough JW, Liu X, Liebman RE, Guise KG, Tang CY, Charney DS. A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Deliberate Emotion Regulation in Resilience and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66:656–664. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ochsner KN, Gross J. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9:242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ovaysikia S, Tahir KA, Chan JL, DeSouza JFX. Word wins over face: emotional Stroop effect activates the frontal cortical network. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2011;4:234. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pannu Hayes J, Labar KS, Petty CM, McCarthy G, Morey RA. Alterations in the neural circuitry for emotion and attention associated with posttraumatic stress symptomatology. Psychiatry Research. 2009;172:7–15. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phaf RH, Kan KJ. The automaticity of emotional Stroop: A meta-analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2007;38:184–199. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riemann BC, McNally RJ. Cognitive processing of personally relevant information. Cognition & Emotion. 1995;9:325–340. [Google Scholar]
- Schoorl M, Putman P, Van der Does W. Attentional Bias Modification in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 2013;82:99–105. doi: 10.1159/000341920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoorl M, Putman P, Van Der Werff S, Van Der Does AJW. Attentional bias and attentional control in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2014;28:203–210. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1998;59(Suppl 20):22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1935;18:643–662. [Google Scholar]
- Taulu S, Simola J. Spatiotemporal signal space separation method for rejecting nearby interference in MEG measurements. Physics in Medicine and Biology. 2006;51:1759–1768. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/51/7/008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas SJ, Gonsalvez CJ, Johnstone SJ. Neural time course of threat-related attentional bias and interference in panic and obsessive-compulsive disorders. Biological Psychology. 2013;94:116–129. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todd RM, MacDonald MJ, Sedge P, Robertson A, Jetly R, Taylor MJ, Pang EW. Soldiers With Posttraumatic Stress Disorder See a World Full of Threat: Magnetoencephalography Reveals Enhanced Tuning to Combat-Related Cues. Biological Psychiatry. 2015;78:821–829. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uusitalo MA, Ilmoniemi RJ. Signal-space projection method for separating MEG or EEG into components. Medical Biological Engineering & Computing. 1997;35:135–140. doi: 10.1007/BF02534144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veit R, Singh V, Sitaram R, Caria A, Rauss K, Birbaumer N. Using real-time fMRI to learn voluntary regulation of the anterior insula in the presence of threat-related stimuli. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012;7:623–634. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weathers FW, Ruscio AM, Keane TM. Psychometric properties of nine scoring rules for the clinician-administered posttraumatic stress disorder scale. Psychological Assessment. 1999;11:124–133. [Google Scholar]
- White SF, Costanzo ME, Blair JR, Roy MJ. PTSD symptom severity is associated with increased recruitment of top-down attentional control in a trauma-exposed sample. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2015;7:19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams JMG, Mathews A, MacLeod C. The emotional stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin. 1996;120:3–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson TW, Heinrichs-Graham E, Becker KM. Circadian modulation of motor-related beta oscillatory responses. Neuroimage. 2014;102:531–539. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson TW, Heinrichs-Graham E, Becker KM, Aloi J, Robertson KR, Sandkovsky U, White ML, O’Neill J, Knott NL, Fox HS, Swindells S. Multimodal Neuroimaging Evidence of Alterations in Cortical Structure and Function in HIV- Infected Older Adults. Human Brain Mapping. 2015;36:897–910. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson TW, Heinrichs-Graham E, Proskovec AL, McDermott TJ. Neuroimaging with magnetoencephalography: A dynamic view of brain pathophysiology. Translational Research. 2016;175:17–36. doi: 10.1016/j.trsl.2016.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]