Abstract
Background
The present study sought to quantify the relationship between alcohol use and alcohol-related consequences in both college student and clinical samples.
Methods
We gathered 33 college student datasets comprising of 15,618 participants and 9 clinical sample datasets comprising of 4,527 participants to determine the effect size of the relationship between alcohol use and alcohol-related consequences. We used random-effects meta-analytic techniques, separately in college and clinical samples, to account for a distribution of true effects and to assess for heterogeneity in effect sizes.
Results
Results demonstrated that the clear majority of the variability in alcohol-related consequences is not explained by alcohol use (i.e., >77% in college samples; >86% in clinical samples), and that there was significant heterogeneity in all effect sizes.
Conclusions
Experiencing alcohol-related consequences results from factors that extend beyond frequency and quantity of alcohol consumed suggesting a need to examine other predictors of alcohol-related consequences beyond alcohol use.
Keywords: Alcohol use, Alcohol related consequences, college students, clinical samples
Introduction
Alcohol use is the main determinant of many chronic diseases (e.g., liver disease, fetal alcohol syndrome, pancreatitis) and incidents of driving while intoxicated (DUI). Chronic diseases and DUI are large contributors to the 5.9% of deaths globally which are attributable to alcohol use1. Beyond these more severe life-threatening consequences of alcohol use, there are other less severe and distinct negative alcohol-related consequences experienced by alcohol users. These consequences include social/interpersonal, academic/occupational, impulse control, and physical consequences.2,3 Although the strength of the relation between alcohol use and negative alcohol-related consequences is critical for interventions/treatments aimed at reducing alcohol use among both clinical and college student samples, this relation is not well characterized in the alcohol literature. There is currently no single study that provides a robust quantification of the alcohol use – negative alcohol-related consequences relation in college or clinical samples. Although many researchers and clinicians may assume that this relation is modest, quantifying the actual magnitude of this relation can help clinicians and researchers decide how much emphasis to place on efforts to reduce alcohol consumption, if reduction of negative alcohol-related consequences is the goal (i.e., harm reduction). Thus, it is necessary to quantify with more precision how strong the use – consequences relationship is for clinical (e.g., treatment development, clinical practice) and etiological reasons.
The Food and Drug Administration (FDA) define two end points for successful alcohol treatment that serve as a “valid surrogate for clinical benefit”4 (p. 7) without additional data. Despite no mention of alcohol consumption as a criterion for an alcohol use disorder diagnosis in current nosologies5,6, these endpoints are complete abstinence or no heavy drinking (i.e., no drinking days with greater than 4/5 drinks for females/males respectively). A similar “low-risk drinking” endpoint is utilized in Europe7 based on the assumption that alcohol use is a valid proxy for clinical benefit (i.e., reduced alcohol-related consequences). Recent critiques have arisen of utilizing cutoffs as an indicator of success following treatment.8 Two recent studies using data from two of the largest alcohol clinical trials9,10 support this critique. Specifically, Pearson and colleagues11 demonstrated that there was much variability in the level of drinking that predicts distinct negative consequences. Further, Wilson and colleagues12 found significant heterogeneity in psycho-social functioning among those deemed "treatment failures" by engaging in some “binge” drinking (based on the 4/5 cutoff for females/males). Importantly, they found that the largest subgroup of so-called treatment failures were functioning as well as individuals who had achieved abstinence or “low-risk” drinking.
One critical step to understanding the etiology of negative alcohol-related consequences is to determine the amount of variance in alcohol-related consequences left unexplained by alcohol use indicators. Although multiple factors that directly predict negative alcohol-related consequences even when controlling for level of alcohol use have been identified (e.g., impulsivity-like traits13; coping motives14), it is important to quantify the degree to which these other factors are needed to fully explain alcohol-related consequences. This conceptualization of the etiology of negative alcohol-related consequences considers alcohol use a necessary but not sufficient condition for experiencing negative alcohol-related consequences, and is open to the fact that non-drinking-related factors may play an even more important role in leading to negative alcohol-related consequences.
One unanswered question with important implications for decision-making of researchers concerns the degree of heterogeneity in the alcohol use-consequences associations across studies due to which specific alcohol use indicators and consequences measures are used. At minimum, alcohol use is operationalized as quantity of use or frequency of use,15 but researchers have also lauded the benefits of assessing level of intoxication (e.g., estimated typical BAC) and maximum or heavy use levels (e.g., number of drinks in the heaviest week, number of binge episodes, peak BAC).16 For the purpose of selecting the alcohol use indicators that are most related to negative alcohol-related consequences, these associations need to be quantified.
Despite the large number of studies that have examined the alcohol use-consequences association, we know of no study that has comprehensively sought to quantify this relationship. The goal of the present study was to use meta-analytic techniques to quantify the associations between various alcohol use indicators (e.g., frequency, quantity, binge/heavy drinking, etc.) and negative alcohol-related consequences using data from 42 studies (total n=20,145) from college and clinical populations. We expected that (a) there would be a moderate positive association between alcohol use and alcohol-related consequences in both the college and clinical samples, (b) that the relationship would vary across indices of alcohol use, and (c) that there would be sufficient unshared variance to caution against using level of alcohol use as a proxy for alcohol-related consequences.
Method
Identification of Datasets
To examine the effect size (i.e., variance explained) between alcohol use indicators and negative alcohol-related consequences among college students and clinical samples, we analyzed data from 33 college student datasets comprising of 15,618 participants and 9 treatment seeking (i.e., clinical) adult sample datasets comprising of 4,527 participants to ensure that our findings are replicable. See Table 1 for a description of the datasets. These datasets were collected by this research group or by our colleagues who were willing to provide data.
Table 1.
College Student Samples | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dataset Name | U.S. Collection Site(s) | n | % female | Measures Assessed | Reference Number for more Details |
Project Lab | Virginia | 65 | 75.4 | DDQ, B-YAACQ, RAPI | Unpublished |
Project PBS | Virginia | 118 | 76.3 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | 41 |
UCD | New York | 155 | 47.7 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | 42 |
Project Alcohol | Virginia | 172 | 52.3 | DDQ, B-YAACQ, RAPI | 43 |
Project Drinking | Virginia | 183 | 76.1 | DDQ, B-YAACQ, RAPI | 44 |
Exercise and Alcohol Use | Texas | 205 | 87.4 | DDQ, QFI, RAPI | 45 |
Thesis | Virginia | 211 | 66.4 | DDQ, RAPI | 46 |
Project Pace | Virginia | 225 | 76.9 | DDQ, B-YAACQ, RAPI | 46 |
Project SOPHE | Virginia | 235 | 62.6 | DDQ, B-YAACQ, RAPI | 47 |
Project Values | Virginia | 239 | 81.3 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | 48 |
Malleability of IN | New York | 265 | 57.0 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | 49 |
Weekly 2008 | Virginia | 280 | 68.6 | DDQ, RAPI | Unpublished |
Choice | New York | 281 | 40.2 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | 50 |
Project Kite | Virginia | 292 | 69.2 | DDQ, B-YAACQ, RAPI | 51 |
Project Culture | Virginia | 293 | 68.9 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | Unpublished |
Weekly 2009 | Virginia | 299 | 69.6 | DDQ, RAPI | Unpublished |
Brown-CDCU | Rhode Island | 326 | 61.0 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | 52 |
Project Bravo | Virginia | 343 | 49.9 | DDQ, B-YAACQ, RAPI | 53 |
Project Brown | Virginia/New Mexico | 384 | 62.6 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | 54 |
Project Norms II | Virginia | 397 | 66.0 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | Unpublished |
Project Thoughts | Virginia | 448 | 67.4 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | 55 |
Sure 1 | New York | 509 | 65.2 | DDQ, RAPI | 56 |
Project Jorge | Virginia | 539 | 72.5 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | 57 |
Project Trust | Virginia | 561 | 70.6 | DDQ, YAACQ | 58 |
Sure-UConn | Connecticut | 568 | 28.4 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | 59 |
NIAAA | New York | 598 | 53.1 | DDQ, YAACQ | 60 |
Project Norms | Virginia | 638 | 68.2 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | Unpublished |
ESP | Texas | 642 | 54.0 | DDQ, QFI, RAPI | 61 |
Sure 3 | New York | 675 | 36.6 | DDQ, RAPI | 62 |
SNAP 1 | Texas | 818 | 57.6 | DDQ, QFI, RAPI, YAAPST | 63 |
Project Beliefs | Virginia/New Mexico | 888 | 69.3 | DDQ, B-YAACQ | 64 |
SNAP 2 Norms | Texas | 1,065 | 62.1 | DDQ, QFI, RAPI | 65 |
SNAP 1 Screen | Washington | 2,103 | 58.2 | DDQ, QFI, RAPI | 63 |
Clinical Samples | |||||
Dataset Name | U.S. Collection Site(s) | n | % female | Measures Assessed | Reference for more Details |
TA R21 | New York | 71 | 34.0 | TFLB, SIP | 66 |
IVR | 4 Sites – all in NY | 116 | 43.0 | TFLB, SIP | 67 |
CBOCS | 5 Sites – all in NY | 146 | 11.6 | TFLB, SIP | 68 |
ACM | 3 Sites – NY & PA | 163 | 3.0 | TFLB, SIP | 69 |
TA R01 | New York | 175 | 34.0 | TFLB, SIP | Unpublished |
Adolescent Recovery | 7 Sites – all in PA | 184 | 34.8 | TFLB, RAPI | 70 |
RREP | 3 Sites – NY, NM, RI | 563 | 41.0 | TFLB, DrInC | 71 |
COMBINE | 11 Sites – SC, CT, MA, WI, WA, TX, RI, FL, NM, PA | 1,383 | 31.0 | TFLB, DrInC | 10 |
MATCH | 11 Sites – NY, RI, CT, WI, WA, NM, SC, TX | 1,726 | 24.0 | TFLB, DrInC | 9 |
Note. DDQ = Daily Drinking Questionnaire, QFI = Quality/Frequency Index, YAACQ = Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire, B-YAACQ = Brief-YAACQ, RAPI = Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index, YAAPST = Young Adult Alcohol Problems Screening Test, DrInC = Drinker Inventory of Consequences, TLFB = Timeline Followback (includes the Form 90 interview), SIP = Short Inventory of Problems.
Measures
Alcohol consumption
Among college student datasets, alcohol consumption was measured primarily with the Daily Drinking Questionnaire (DDQ17) or Quantity/Frequency/Peak Index (QFI18) with various other single-item measures. Across these measures, alcohol consumption was broken down into several indicators that were measured across many of the datasets including three frequency measures (i.e., past 30-day frequency of alcohol use, past 30-day frequency of getting drunk, and typical frequency [number of drinking days during a typical week]), three quantity measures (i.e., typical quantity [number of drinks consumed during a typical week], heaviest quantity [number of drinks consumed during heaviest drinking week], and peak quantity [number of drinks consumed on one’s heaviest drinking day]), and an indicator of binge drinking frequency (past 30-day frequency of drinking 4+/5+ drinks for women/men).
Among clinical samples, alcohol consumption was measured with either the Timeline Follow-Back19,20 or the Form 90 interview21, which are calendar-based measures in which participants report the number of standard drinks they consumed on each day during the assessment window (e.g., past 90 days). Alcohol consumption was broken down into three indicators: average drinks per drinking day (DDD), percent heavy drinking days (PHDD, defined using the 4+/5+ binge/heavy drinking criterion), and percent drinking days (PDD, the converse of percent days abstinent, PDA).
Negative alcohol-related consequences
For college student datasets, alcohol-related consequences were assessed using the 48-item Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire (YAACQ2), the 24-item Brief-YAACQ (B-YAACQ22), or the 23-item Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index (RAPI23). Among clinical samples, negative alcohol-related consequences were measured with either the 45-item Drinker Inventory of Consequences (DrInC3), the 15-item Short Inventory of Problems (SIP3), or the RAPI.23 Although there was some variability in assessment window (e.g., 30-day vs. 90-day) across studies, alcohol-related consequences were typically assessed on the same time window as alcohol use indicators.
Analysis Plan
Across all studies, we used cross-sectional correlation coefficients to estimate the strength of associations between alcohol use indicators and alcohol-related consequences. We examined each alcohol use indicator separately, but in situations where two alcohol-related consequences measures were used in the same dataset (not very common), we combined effects across these measures by averaging the two correlation coefficients (i.e., shifting unit of analysis24) so as not to exaggerate the cumulative information value of these associations. Note that alcohol-related consequences measures given in the same study were strongly correlated, rs > .85. Alcohol use indicators and alcohol-related consequences measures were selected based on their frequent use in either college student or clinical samples. For example, B-YAACQ and YAACQ are frequently administered to college student populations;2,22,25,26 whereas the DrInC and SIP are frequently administered to clinical populations.27–30 In clinical samples, correlations were conducted on baseline (i.e., intake) assessment data. We conducted separate analyses in the college student and clinical samples. We used random-effects aggregate data meta-analytic techniques31 to account for a distribution of true effects. This method provides weighted correlation coefficients (i.e., weighted by sample size) and confidence intervals arounds these point estimates. We used the Q statistic to determine if there was statistically significant heterogeneity in effect sizes; we used I2 as a measure of how much variation across studies was due to heterogeneity rather than chance.32
Results
All effect sizes are summarized in Table 2. Funnel plots are available from the authors. Across the alcohol use indicators in the college student samples, we found weighted correlation coefficients ranging from .393 (frequency during typical drinking week) to .474 (past 30-day frequency of getting drunk), indicating that between 15.44% and 22.47% of the variance in negative alcohol-related consequences is accounted for by specific indicators of alcohol use. Stated differently, between 77.53% to 84.56% of the variance in alcohol-related consequences was not explained by the specific alcohol use indicators that we examined. Q statistics revealed significant heterogeneity in all effect sizes and I2 statistics revealed that moderate (41.45%) to large (86.65%) amounts of the total variation across studies was due to heterogeneity (i.e., not sampling variability).
Table 2.
College Student Samples | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Use Indicator | rw (95% CI) | k | N | Q | p | I2 |
Past 30-Day Frequency of Alcohol Use | .397 (.364, .428) | 24 | 11083 | 80.90 | <.001 | 71.57 |
Past 30-Day Frequency of Getting Drunk | .474 (.448, .499) | 19 | 6627 | 30.74 | .031 | 41.45 |
Typical Frequency | .393 (.345, .439) | 17 | 10223 | 119.83 | <.001 | 86.65 |
Typical Quantity | .444 (.422, .465) | 31 | 11852 | 61.43 | .001 | 51.17 |
Heaviest Quantity | .421 (.389, .452) | 22 | 8074 | 59.52 | <.001 | 64.72 |
Peak Quantity | .412 (.384, .439) | 26 | 11880 | 73.12 | <.001 | 65.81 |
Binge Drinking Frequency | .466 (.442, .488) | 25 | 8665 | 43.50 | .009 | 44.83 |
Clinical Samples | ||||||
Use Indicator | rw (95% CI) | k | N | Q | p | I2 |
Percent Days Drinking (PDD) | .158 (.045, .266) | 9 | 4527 | 91.39 | <.001 | 91.25 |
Percent Heavy Drinking Days (PHDD) | .266 (.188, .347) | 8 | 4411 | 41.77 | <.001 | 83.24 |
Average Drinks per Drinking Day (DDD) | .368 (.342, .393) | 9 | 4527 | 5.75 | .675 | .00 |
Note. rw = weighted correlation coefficient based on random-effect model, k = number of studies used to compute effect sizes, Q = Cochrane’s Q statistic, which is distributed as a chi-square, and determines whether there is significant heterogeneity in the effect sizes across studies. p = p-value for the Q statistic, I2 = determines the percent of variability in effect sizes due to heterogeneity.
Across the alcohol use indicators in the clinical samples, the weighted correlation coefficients were .158 (percent drinking days), .266 (percent heavy drinking days), and .367 (drinks per drinking days), accounting for 2.50%, 7.08%, and 13.47% of the variance in negative alcohol-related consequences by each alcohol use indicator, respectively. Alternatively, the unexplained variability in consequences was 97.50%, 92.92%, and 86.53%, respectively. Q statistics revealed significant heterogeneity for percent drinking days and percent heavy drinking days, but not drinks per drinking day. The I2 statistics revealed that large amounts of the total variation across studies for percent drinking days (91.25%) and percent heavy drinking days (83.43%) was due to heterogeneity.
Discussion
The current study quantified the alcohol use–alcohol related consequences relationship in both college student and clinical populations using a meta-analytic quantification strategy across 33 college samples and 9 clinical samples totaling 20,145 participants. Results from the college samples suggested moderate correlations across alcohol use indicators and alcohol-related consequences measures, whereas in the clinical samples correlations ranged from small-to-moderate. Most notably, our findings indicate that the clear majority of the variability in alcohol-related consequences is not explained by any specific indicator of alcohol use. Specifically, in the college samples there was on average >77% unexplained variance in alcohol-related consequences across indicators, and in the clinical samples the average unexplained variance was astonishingly >86%. This finding clearly demonstrates both that alcohol use is not a good proxy for alcohol-related consequences and that experiencing alcohol-related consequences is related primarily to factors that extend beyond frequency or quantity of alcohol consumed. This research adds to the growing literature challenging Food and Drug Administration (FDA)4 and European Medicines Agency (EMA)7 recommendations of alcohol use as adequate indicator of treatment success.
In the college student samples, the smallest association (i.e., correlation) was between alcohol use frequency during a typical drinking week and alcohol-related consequences, and the largest associations were between frequency of getting drunk and alcohol-related consequences and quantity indicators (e.g., typical and heavy quantity) and alcohol-related consequences; all associations were in the medium-to-large effect size range. Thus, pure frequency measures may be less strongly associated with alcohol-related consequences than indices that combine frequency and quantity (e.g., frequency of getting drunk or binge drinking frequency), or pure quantity measures. Similarly, in the clinical samples, percent drinking days (PDD; a frequency measure) had the weakest association with alcohol-related consequences and drinks per drinking day (DDD; a quantity measure) had the strongest association. Therefore, in the clinical samples, the stronger factor linking use to alcohol-related consequences also appears to be the amount used per day.
The small association between PDD and alcohol-related consequences may be surprising given the prominence of PDD (or percent days abstinent [PDA]) in the literature.9,10 Clinical researchers often argue that PDA is a relevant outcome measure because clinical interventions are typically abstinence-focused.33 However, these results suggest that, for harm reduction interventions, DDD is a better proxy for alcohol-related consequences. The current findings help elucidate the importance of examining factors beyond alcohol use when trying to predict or mitigate alcohol-related consequences. For example, research suggests that negative affect,34 social norms,35 and expectancies36 also likely impact negative alcohol-related consequences independent of alcohol use.
Relatedly, we found moderate to large amounts of heterogeneity in the effect sizes across studies indicating that there are unexamined moderators of the alcohol use–consequences association. Identifying moderators that may strengthen or weaken these associations could be examined at three different levels of analysis. Using a larger sample of studies examining the alcohol use–consequences associations, researchers could examine moderators in a meta-analytic framework to attempt to account for between-study variability. Within studies, between-subject variability can be explored by examining several potential moderators of these associations (e.g., protective behavioral strategies37). Finally, using ecological momentary assessment data, explaining within-subject variability in the alcohol use–consequences association could guide individual-level interventions/treatments.
Strengths of the present study include the application of meta-analytic techniques and including both college and clinical samples in the evaluation of the alcohol use-consequences association. Given that we relied on using data available to the researchers, our selection of studies was not random (e.g., geographically limited), which causes some concern regarding the generalizability of our findings. In addition, we used only a select set of widely used self-report measures of alcohol use/consequences, and it is possible that other indicators may demonstrate weaker or stronger relationships. Although we found significant heterogeneity in most associations, our sampling strategy may have reduced heterogeneity by using data from a relatively small number of labs. We decided not to examine possible moderators of these associations given the likelihood that these findings would be biased. In addition, these analyses would often be under-powered, increasing the changes that we commit a Type II error. Unfortunately, our comparisons of college and clinical samples were limited by differences in alcohol use indicators, alcohol-related consequences measures, time window differences (typically past 30 days for college students and past 90 days for clinical samples), and age differences (college samples typically younger than clinical samples). Finally, this study focused on cross-sectional relations among alcohol use and alcohol-related consequences, which provides insight into the strength of the relationship at a single time point or at baseline (i.e., pre-intervention), but does not allow for the examination of causal inferences.
Although the present study serves as a great starting point summarizing the average associations between several indicators of alcohol use and alcohol-related consequences, future research is sorely needed. A more comprehensive meta-analysis can be conducted to examine not only the average associations between specific alcohol use indicators and specific operationalizations of alcohol-related consequences, but also important moderators of these associations. Potential moderators abound including overall sample characteristics, demographic factors, type of alcohol use indicator, specific operationalization of consequences, among other things. We consider a few examples. In terms of demographic factors, it is plausible that age moderates the use-consequences association. On the one hand, one may expect that more experience with drinking may strengthen the use-consequences association because in the face of increased familiarity with a variety of drinking contexts, experience of alcohol-related consequences may be more heavily dependent on level of drinking. On the other hand, individuals with a more extensive drinking history may experience the negative consequences of drinking at lower doses of alcohol due to impaired liver functioning. As suggested by our results, whether an alcohol use indicator assesses frequency, quantity, or the combination of the two is another potential moderator of the use-consequences association. The manner in which consequences are assessed is another factor to consider. Some consequence measures only assess the number of discrete consequences experienced (e.g., YAACQ), whereas other measures also assess the frequency of experiencing negative consequences (e.g., DrInC). Although purely speculative, the former may be more associated with measures of heavy/extreme drinking (i.e., extreme drinking occasions being more likely to be associated with experiencing a range of negative consequences), whereas the latter may be more associated with overall frequency/quantity measures (i.e., more frequent drinking being associated with more opportunities to experience negative consequences).
In summary, the present study demonstrated that the relationship between alcohol use and alcohol-related consequences is moderate for college student samples and small-to-moderate for clinical samples. There is a substantial amount of unexplained variance in alcohol-related consequences that is not attributable to alcohol use, suggesting a need to examine other direct predictors of alcohol-related consequences (e.g., protective behavioral strategies37,38, impulsivity-like traits39, affective functioning40). This manifests as heterogeneity among clinical presentations. Although some individuals can consume large amounts of alcohol and experience relatively few alcohol-related consequences, other individuals can experience many alcohol-related consequences even though they consume relatively small amounts of alcohol. Finally, when selecting alcohol use indicators as potential outcomes, researchers may want to choose indicators that focus on quantity (e.g., drinks per drinking day) or measures that combine quantity and frequency (e.g., frequency of getting drunk).
Acknowledgments
Matthew Pearson is supported by a career development grant (K01- AA023233) from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). Adrian Bravo and Kevin Montes are supported by a training grant (T32-AA018108) from the NIAAA.
We would like to thank all of the scientists who contributed data to this project in addition to the authors, including: Kate Carey, Gerard Connors, James Henson, Stephen Maisto, Clayton Neighbors, Kyle Possemato, Jennifer Read, and Katie Witkiewitz.
Footnotes
Declaration of Interest:
The ideas and results from the present manuscript have not been disseminated in any platform besides the present submission. However, the datasets used to run the analyses for the present study have been widely disseminated in a myriad of platforms see Table 1 for information on the individual datasets.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of this paper.
References
- 1.World Health Organization. Global status report on alcohol and health. Geneva, Switzerland: Author; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Read JP, Kahler CW, Strong DR, Colder CR. Development and preliminary validation of the young adult alcohol consequences questionnaire. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2016;67:169–177. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Miller WR, Tonigan JS, Longabaugh R. The drinker inventory of consequences (DrInC) [Internet] The Drinker Inventory of Consequences (DrInC): An Instrument for Assessing Adverse Consequences of Alcohol Abuse 1995. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; Available from: http://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/drinc.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Food and Drug Administration. Alcoholism: Developing drugs for treament. Silver Spring, MD: Author; 2015. (No. PDA D-0152-001). [Google Scholar]
- 5.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 6.World Health Organization. The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva, Switzerland: Author; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 7.European Medicines Agency. Guideline on the development of medicinal products for the treatment of alcohol dependence. London UK: Author; 2010. pp. 1–17. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Pearson MR, Kirouac M, Witkiewitz K. Questioning the validity of the 4+/5+ binge or heavy drinking criterion in college and clinical populations. Addiction. 2016;111(10):1720–1726. doi: 10.1111/add.13210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mattson ME, Allen JP, Miller WR, Hester RK, Connors GJ, Rychtarik RG, et al. Project MATCH: Rationale and methods for a multisite clinical trial matching patients to alcoholism treatment. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1993;17(6):1130–1145. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1993.tb05219.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Anton RF, O’Malley SS, Ciraulo DA, Cisler RA, Couper D, Donovan DM, et al. Combined pharmacotherapies and behavioral interventions for alcohol dependence: the COMBINE study: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2006;295:2003–2017. doi: 10.1001/jama.295.17.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pearson MR, Bravo AJ, Kirouac M, Witkiewitz K. The search for an elusive cutoff remains: Problems of binary classification of heavy drinking as an endpoint for alcohol clinical trials. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2017;171:91–96. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.11.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wilson AD, Bravo AJ, Pearson MR, Witkiewitz K. Finding success in failure: using latent profile analysis to examine heterogeneity in psychosocial functioning among heavy drinkers following treatment. Addiction. 2016;111(12):2145–2154. doi: 10.1111/add.13518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bravo AJ, Prince MA, Pearson MR. A multiple replication examination of distal antecedents to alcohol protective behavioral strategies. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2016;77:958–967. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2016.77.958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bravo AJ, Prince MA, Pearson MR. Does the how mediate the why? A multiple replication examination of drinking motives, alcohol protective behavioral strategies, and alcohol outcomes. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2015;76:872–883. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2015.76.872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Room R. Measuring drinking patterns: the experience of the last half century. J Subst Abuse. 2000;12:23–31. doi: 10.1016/S0899-3289(00)00038-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Dawson DA, Room R. Towards agreement on ways to measure and report drinking patterns and alcohol-related problems in adult general population surveys: The Skarpö Conference overview. J Subst Abuse. 2000;12:1–21. doi: 10.1016/S0899-3289(00)00037-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Collins RL, Parks GA, Marlatt GA. Social determinants of alcohol consumption: The effects of social interaction and model status on the self-administration of alcohol. J Consult Clin Psych. 1985;53:189–200. doi: 10.1037//0022-006X.53.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Dimeff LA, Baer JS, Kivlahan DR, Marlatt GA. Brief alcohol screening and intervention for college students (BASICS): A harm reduction approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sobell LC, Sobell MB. Timeline follow-back. In: Litten LA, Allen JP, editors. Measuring alcohol consumption. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press; 1992. pp. 41–72. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sobell MB, Sobell LC. Controlled drinking after 25 years: How important was the great debate? Addiction. 1995;90:1149–1153. doi: 10.1080/09652149541392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Miller WR, Del Boca FK. Measurement of drinking behavior using the Form 90 family of instruments. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 1994;(s12):112–118. doi: 10.15288/jsas.1994.s12.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kahler CW, Strong DR, Read JP. Toward efficient and comprehensive measurement of the alcohol problems continuum in college students: The Brief Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:1180–1189. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000171940.95813.A5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.White HR, Labouvie EW. Towards the assessment of adolescent problem drinking. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 1989;50:30–37. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1989.50.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Cooper H. Research synthesis and meta-analysis: A step-by-step approach. Vol. 2. Sage publications; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 25.DeMartini KS, Carey KB. Optimizing the use of the AUDIT for alcohol screening in college students. Psychol Assess. 2012;24(4):954. doi: 10.1037/a0028519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Read JP, Merrill JE, Kahler CW, Strong DR. Predicting functional outcomes among college drinkers: Reliability and predictive validity of the Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire. Addict Behav. 2007;32(11):2597–2610. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.06.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Blume AW, Schmaling KB, Marlatt GA. Recent drinking consequences, motivation to change, and changes in alcohol consumption over a three month period. Addict Behav. 2016;31(2):331–338. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Blanchard KA, Morgenstern J, Morgan TJ, Lobouvie EW, Bux DA. Assessing consequences of substance use: psychometric properties of the inventory of drug use consequences. Psychol Addict Behav. 2003;17(4):328–331. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.17.4.328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Johnson BA, Ait-Daoud N, Akhtar FZ, Ma JZ. Oral Topiramate Reduces the Consequences of Drinking and Improves theQuality of Life of Alcohol-Dependent Individuals: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(9):905–912. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kraemer KL, Maisto SA, Conigliaro J, McNeil M, Gordon AJ, Kelley ME. Decreased Alcohol Consumption in Outpatient Drinkers Is Associated with Improved Quality of Life and Fewer Alcohol-related Consequences. J Gen Intern Med. 2002;17(5):382–386. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2002.10613.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins J, Rothstein HR. A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res Synth Methods. 2010;1:97–111. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327:557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA) Results from the 2013 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Summary of national findings. Rockville, MD: Author; 2014. 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Park CL, Grant C. Determinants of positive and negative consequences of alcohol consumption in college students: Alcohol use, gender, and psychological characteristics. Addict Behav. 2005;30(4):755–765. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Larimer ME, Turner AP, Mallett KA, Geisner IM. Predicting drinking behavior and alcohol-related problems among fraternity and sorority members: Examining the role of descriptive and injunctive norms. Psychol Addict Behav. 2004;18(3):203–212. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.18.3.203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Patrick ME, Wray-Lake L, Finlay AK, Maggs JL. The long arm of expectancies: Adolescent alcohol expectancies predict adult alcohol use. Alcohol. 2010;45(1):17–24. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agp066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Pearson MR. Use of alcohol protective behavioral strategies among college students: A critical review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33:1025–1040. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Prince MA, Carey KB, Maisto SA. Protective behavioral strategies for reducing alcohol involvement: A review of the methodological issues. Addict Behav. 2013;38:2343–2351. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Coskunpinar A, Dir AL, Cyders MA. Multidimensionality in impulsivity and alcohol use: A meta-analysis using the UPPS model of impulsivity. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37:1441–1450. doi: 10.1111/acer.12131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Simons JS, Gaher RM, Oliver MNI, Bush JA, Palmer MA. An experience sampling study of associations between affect and alcohol use and problems among college students. J Stud Alcohol. 2005;66:459–469. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Bravo AJ, Pearson MR, Stevens LE, Henson JM. Weighing the pros and cons of using protective behavioral strategies: A qualitative examination among college students. Manuscript submitted for publication. 2017 doi: 10.1080/10826084.2018.1464026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Prince MA, Maisto SA, Rice SL, Carey KB. Development of a face-to-face injunctive norms brief motivational intervention for college drinkers and preliminary outcomes. Psychol Addict Behav. 2015;29:825–835. doi: 10.1037/adb0000118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kite BA, Pearson MR, Henson JM. The assessment of protective behavioral strategies: Comparing the absolute frequency and contingent frequency response scales. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27 doi: 10.1037/a0031366. doi: 1010.10.1037/a0031366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Pearson MR. When cognitive bias masquerades as intervention efficacy: Drinking norms as anchors and norm interventions as anchoring effects. Old Dominion University; Norfolk, VA: 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Leasure JL, Neighbors C. Impulsivity moderates the association between physical activity and alcohol consumption. Alcohol. 2014;48:361–366. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2013.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Pearson MR, Henson JM. Unplanned drinking and alcohol-related problems: a preliminary test of the model of unplanned drinking behavior. Psychology of Addict Behav. 2013;27:584–595. doi: 10.1037/a0030901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Pearson MR, Murphy EM, Doane AN. Impulsivity-like traits and risky driving behaviors among college students. Accid Anal Prev. 2013;53:142–148. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2013.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bravo AJ, Pearson MR, Wilson AD, Witkiewitz K. When Traits Match States: Examining the Associations Between Self-Report Trait and State Mindfulness Following a State Mindfulness Induction. Mindfulness. 2017:1–13. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0763-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Prince MA, Carey KB. The malleability of injunctive norms among college students. Addict Behav. 2010;35:940–947. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Carey KB, DeMartini KS, Prince MA, Luteran C, Carey MP. Effects of choice on intervention outcomes for college students sanctioned for campus alcohol policy violations. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27:596–603. doi: 10.1037/a0030333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Pearson MR, Kite BA, Henson JM. Predictive effects of good self-control and poor regulation on alcohol-related outcomes: Do protective behavioral strategies mediate? Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27:81–89. doi: 10.1037/a0028818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Carey KB, Balestrieri SG, Miller MB, Merrill JE, DiBello AM, Benz MB. Efficacy of the College Drinkers Check-Up for Student Drinkers Living Off Campus. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2017;78(4):571–579. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2017.78.571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kite BA. Protective behavioral strategy subtypes as moderators of the relationship between alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems. Old Dominion University; Norfolk, VA: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Brown DB, Bravo AJ, Roos CR, Pearson MR. Five facets of mindfulness and psychological health: Evaluating a psychological model of the mechanisms of mindfulness. Mindfulness. 2015;6:1021–1032. doi: 10.1007/s12671-014-0349-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Bravo AJ, Pearson MR, Stevens LE, Henson JM. Depressive symptoms and alcohol-related problems among college students: A moderated mediated model of mindfulness and drinking to cope. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2016;77:661–666. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2016.77.661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Carey KB, Carey MP, Maisto SA, Henson JM. Brief motivational interventions for heavy college drinkers: A randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psych. 2006;74:943–954. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.5.943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Bravo AJ, Pearson MR, Henson JM. Drinking to cope with depressive symptoms and ruminative thinking: a multiple mediation model among college students. Subst Use Misuse. 2017;52(1):52–62. doi: 10.1080/10826084.2016.1214151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Bravo AJ, Pearson MR, Stevens LE(2016) Making religiosity person-centered: A latent profile analysis of religiosity and psychological health outcomes. Pers Individ Dif. 2016;88:160–169. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.049.. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Merrill JE, Carey KB, Lust SA, Kalichman SC, Carey MP. Do students mandated to intervention for campus alcohol-related violations drink more than nonmandated students? Psychol Addict Behav. 2014;28:1265–1270. doi: 10.1037/a0037710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Read JP, Bachrach RL, Wardell JD, Coffey SF. Examining cognitive examining cognitive processes and drinking urge in PTSD. Behav Res Ther. 2017;90:159–168. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Neighbors C, Lee CM, Lewis MA, Fossos N, Walter T. Internet-based personalized feedback to reduce 21st-birthday drinking: a randomized controlled trial of an event-specific prevention intervention. J Consult Clin Psych. 2009;77:51–63. doi: 10.1037/a0014386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Carey KB, Carey MP, Henson JM, Maisto SA, DeMartini KS. Brief alcohol interventions for mandated college students: Comparison of face-to-face counseling and computer-delivered interventions. Addiction. 2011;106:528–537. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03193.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Neighbors C, Lewis MA, LaBrie J, DiBello AM, Young CM, Rinker DV, et al. A multisite randomized trial of normative feedback for heavy drinking: Social comparison versus social comparison plus correction of normative misperceptions. J Consult Clin Psych. 2016;84:238–247. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Pearson MR, Brown DB, Bravo AJ, Witkiewitz K. Staying in the moment and finding purpose: The associations of trait mindfulness, decentering, and purpose in life with depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and alcohol-related problems. Mindfulness. 2015;6:645–653. doi: 10.1007/s12671-014-0300-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Rinker DV, Neighbors C. Do different types of social identity moderate the association between perceived descriptive norms and drinking among college students? Addict Behav. 2014;39:1297–1303. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.03.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Prince MA, Connors GJ, Maisto SA, Dearing RL. Within treatment therapeutic alliance ratings profiles predict posttreatment frequency of alcohol use. Psychol Addict Behav. 2016;30:184–193. doi: 10.1037/adb0000161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Maisto SA, Ewart CK, Witkiewitz K, Connors GJ, Elder G, Krenek M, et al. Predicting drinking lapses in alcohol use disorder: The toxic combination of agonistic striving and poor anger regulation. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2016;35:235–254. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2016.35.3.235. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Acosta MC, Possemato K, Maisto SA, Marsch LA, Barrie K, Lantinga L, et al. Web-delivered CBT reduces heavy drinking in OEF-OIF veterans in primary care with symptomatic substance use and PTSD. Behav Ther. 2016;48(2):262–276. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Oslin DW, Lynch KG, Maisto SA, Lantinga LJ, McKay JR, Possemato K, et al. A randomized clinical trial of alcohol care management delivered in Department of Veterans Affairs primary care clinics versus specialty addiction treatment. J Gen Intern Med. 2014;29:162–168. doi: 10.1007/s11606-013-2625-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Maisto SA, Krenek M, Chung T, Martin CS, Clark D, Cornelius J. A comparison of the concurrent and predictive validity of three measures of readiness to change alcohol use in a clinical sample of adolescents. Psychol Assess. 2011;23:983–994. doi: 10.1037/a0024136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Lowman C, Allen J, Stout RL, Group, TRR Replication and extension of Marlatt’s taxonomy of relapse precipitants: Overview of procedures and results. Addiction. 1996;91:51–72. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.91.12s1.16.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]