Skip to main content
Acta Neuropathologica Communications logoLink to Acta Neuropathologica Communications
. 2018 Mar 2;6:20. doi: 10.1186/s40478-018-0520-6

Three-dimensional analysis of synapses in the transentorhinal cortex of Alzheimer’s disease patients

M Domínguez-Álvaro 1, M Montero-Crespo 1,2, L Blazquez-Llorca 1,5, R Insausti 4, J DeFelipe 1,2,3, L Alonso-Nanclares 1,2,3,
PMCID: PMC5834884  PMID: 29499755

Abstract

Synaptic dysfunction or loss in early stages of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is thought to be a major structural correlate of cognitive dysfunction. Early loss of episodic memory, which occurs at the early stage of AD, is closely associated with the progressive degeneration of medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures of which the transentorhinal cortex (TEC) is the first affected area. However, no ultrastructural studies have been performed in this region in human brain samples from AD patients.

In the present study, we have performed a detailed three-dimensional (3D) ultrastructural analysis using focused ion beam/scanning electron microscopy (FIB/SEM) to investigate possible synaptic alterations in the TEC of patients with AD. Surprisingly, the analysis of the density, morphological features and spatial distribution of synapses in the neuropil showed no significant differences between AD and control samples. However, light microscopy studies showed that cortical thickness of the TEC was severely reduced in AD samples, but there were no changes in the volume occupied by neuronal and glial cell bodies, blood vessels, and neuropil. Thus, the present results indicate that there is a dramatic loss of absolute number of synapses, while the morphology of synaptic junctions and synaptic spatial distribution are maintained. How these changes affect cognitive impairment in AD remains to be elucidated.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (10.1186/s40478-018-0520-6) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Keywords: Dementia, Electron Microscopy, FIB/SEM, Medial temporal lobe, Neuropil, Synapses

Introduction

Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) is the main cause of dementia, accounting for 60−80% of cases in the adult population [3]. The disease is characterized by a progressive and persistent decline of cognitive functions, such as memory and orientation. In the final stages, patients suffer a severe lack of autonomy and social life [56]. AD is characterized by two hallmark lesions: extracellular amyloid plaques, primarily consisting of amyloid-β (Aβ) peptide, and intracellular neurofibrillary tangles, which consist of filamentous aggregates of hyperphosphorylated tau protein. In addition to these main hallmarks of AD, other neuropathological changes —such as neuronal and synaptic loss— have also been described [58]. Among them, synaptic loss seems to be the major structural correlate of cognitive decline observed in AD patients, and it is considered to be the earliest mechanism that precedes neuronal loss [5, 18, 23, 43, 70, 82].

Even though it seems that AD begins in subcortical regions [12, 13, 15], the early loss of episodic memory is closely associated with the progressive degeneration of the medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures [71]. Among the MTL structures, the transentorhinal cortex (TEC) is one of the first affected areas [11]. This obliquely oriented cortex is located in the MTL between the perirhinal cortex (PRC) and the entorhinal cortex (EC) [10, 14]. The TEC is considered as a transitional zone between the periallocortex represented by EC and the proisocortex [10, 14, 36]. Although the TEC is not considered as a distinct area in Brodmann’s nomenclature, it is nonetheless part of area 35 (perirhinal cortex), which —together with Brodmann’s area 36 (ectorhinal cortex)— is nowadays frequently named perirhinal cortex (PRC) [42].

Obvious ethical reasons prevent tract-tracing connectivity studies on human brain other than in post-mortem studies; however, studies performed in monkey have shown that the PRC constitutes a convergence zone, receiving two main inputs. First, it connects extensively with MTL structures, including (in order of decreasing strength): EC, parahippocampal cortex, amygdala and hippocampus. Secondly, it has connections with cortical association areas (visual, insula, temporal, cingulate and orbitofrontal) [79, 81]. One of the main MTL reciprocal connections is established with the EC: inputs terminate in layer II and layers V−VI of the PRC (area 35 and the medial portion of area 36) [80, 81], while the main output of PRC is directed to layer I and III of the EC [35]. These strong interconnections make PRC unique, occupying a key position where sensory information converges with other information from memory-related structures, integrating memory information [78, 81]. Furthermore, it has been reported that neurons in layer II of the TEC are affected by neurofibrillary degeneration in primary age-related tauopathy [11, 72]. Thus, investigating the changes that affect synaptic connectivity in the TEC, as an early affected area in AD, is essential to better understand the mechanisms underlying this disease. As far as we know, no detailed ultrastructural studies have been performed in the human TEC.

Therefore, in order to investigate possible microanatomical changes related to this pathological condition, we performed an ultrastructural study of layer II neuropil of the TEC in human brain samples from AD patients and subjects with no apparent neurological alterations. We used focused ion beam/scanning electron microscopy (FIB/SEM) and three-dimensional (3D) reconstructions [45] to determine the density, types and features of the synapses, as well as their spatial distribution. Moreover, since it has been reported that there is a reduction in the thickness of frontal cortex, temporal cortex (areas 21 and 22 of Brodmann) and in the molecular layer of dentate gyrus (DG) in AD patients [59, 60, 63, 69], light microscopy studies were performed to evaluate the decrease in TEC thickness. Additionally, the same TEC samples were examined with light microscopy to investigate the volume occupied by neurons, glia, blood vessels and neuropil in both control and AD patients.

Materials and methods

Tissue preparation

Human brain tissue was obtained from three sources: Pathological Anatomy Service of Bellvitge University Hospital (Barcelona, Spain), Centro Alzheimer Fundación Reina Sofía, CIEN Foundation (Madrid, Spain) and Human Neuroanatomy Laboratory, School of Medicine, University of Castilla-La Mancha (Albacete, Spain).

Samples were obtained from five subjects with no apparent neurological alterations and from five patients with AD. According to the neuropathological criteria provided by the above-mentioned centers, cases were classified as control (subjects with no apparent neurological alterations) or AD (Table 1). Brain tissue samples were obtained following the guidelines and approval of the Institutional Ethical Committee. In all cases, the time between death and tissue processing was lower than 4 h.

Table 1.

Clinical and neuropathological information

Patient Gender Age (years) Cause of death Postmortem delay (h) Braak Stage CERAD Stage Neuropsychological diagnosis
AB1 Male 45 Lung cancer < 1 NA NA NA
AB2 Female 53 Pulmonary shock 4 NA NA NA
IF10 Male 66 Bronchopneumonia plus cardiac failure 2 NA NA NA
M16 Male 40 Traffic accident 3 NA NA NA
M17 Male 36 Bronchopneumonia 2.5 NA NA NA
IF1 Female 80 2 IV B No evidence of cognitive impairment and dementia
IF2 Female 94 Pulmonary tuberculosis 1.5 V C Dementia
IF6 Male 85 Pneumonia 2 III A Mild cognitive impairment
VK11 Female 87 Respiratory inflammation 1.5 III−IV A Dementia
VK22 Female 86 2 V C Dementia

NA Not applicable, NFTs neurofibrillary tangles, - Not available

Braak Stages [11]: III (NFTs in entorhinal cortex and closely related areas); III−IV (NFTs abundant in amygdala and hippocampus. Extending slightly into association cortex); V−VI (NFTs widely distributed throughout the neocortex and ultimately involving primary motor and sensory areas). CERAD Stages [48]: A (Low density of neuritic plaques); B (Intermediate density of neuritic plaques); C (High density of neuritic plaques)

Upon removal, brain tissue was fixed in cold 4% paraformaldehyde (Sigma-Aldrich, St Louis, MO, USA) in 0.1 M sodium phosphate buffer (PB; Panreac, No.131965, Spain) pH 7.4 for 24−48h. After fixation, the tissue was washed in PB and sectioned coronally in a vibratome (Vibratome Sectioning System, VT1200S Vibratome, Leica Biosystems, Germany).

Immunohistochemistry

Selected sections were first rinsed in PB 0.1 M, pretreated in 2% H2O2 for 30 min to remove endogenous peroxidase activity, and then incubated for 1 h at room temperature in a solution of 3% normal horse serum (for polyclonal antisera and monoclonal antibodies, respectively; Vector Laboratories Inc., Burlingame, CA) and 0.25% Triton-X (Merck, Darmstadt, Germany). Subsequently, sections were incubated for 48 h at 4 °C in the same solution with mouse anti-NeuN (1:2000; Chemicon; MAB377, Temecula, CA, USA) and anti-human PHF-Tau antibody clone AT8 (1:2000, MN1020, Thermo Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA); for the sake of clarity, we will refer to this as anti-PHF-Tau-AT8. Sections selected for anti-Aβ were first treated with 88% formic acid (Sigma-Aldrich, No. 251364, St. Louis, MO, USA) to ensure specific plaque immunostaining, and were then incubated in a solution containing mouse antibody anti-Aβ (clone 6F/3D; 1:50, Dako M0872, Glostrup, Denmark). Sections were then processed with a secondary biotinylated horse anti-mouse IgG antibody (1:200, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA), and then incubated for 1 h in an avidin-biotin peroxidase complex (Vectastain ABC Elite PK6100, Vector) and, finally, with the chromogen 3,3′-diaminobenzidine tetrahydrochloride (DAB; Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA). Finally, sections were dehydrated, cleared with xylene and cover-slipped.

Electron microscopy

Sections containing TEC were selected (Figs. 1, 2, and 3; Table 1) and postfixed for 24 h in a solution containing 2% paraformaldehyde, 2.5% glutaraldehyde (TAAB, G002, UK) and 0.003% CaCl2 (Sigma, C-2661-500G, Germany) in sodium cacodylate (Sigma, C0250-500G, Germany) buffer (0.1 M). These sections were washed in sodium cacodylate buffer (0.1 M) and treated with 1% OsO4 (Sigma, O5500, Germany), 0.1% potassium ferrocyanide (Probus, No. 23345, Spain) and 0.003% CaCl2 in sodium cacodylate buffer (0.1 M) for 1 h at room temperature. After washing in PB, sections were stained with 2% uranyl acetate (EMS, 8473, USA), and then dehydrated and flat-embedded in Araldite (TAAB, E021, UK) for 48 h at 60 °C [20]. Embedded sections were glued onto a blank Araldite block and trimmed. Semithin sections (1–2 μm thick) were obtained from the surface of the block and stained with 1% toluidine blue (Merck, No.115930, Germany) in 1% sodium borate (Panreac, No. 141644, Spain). The last semithin section (which corresponds to the section immediately adjacent to the block surface) was examined under light microscope and photographed to accurately locate the neuropil regions to be examined.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Coronal sections of human hippocampal formation. Low-power photographs of a control subject (a, c) and an AD patient (b, d), in sections stained for Nissl (a, b) and immunostained for anti-NeuN (c, d). TEC is indicated by the box. Scale bar (in d): 3 mm

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Coronal sections of human hippocampal formation. Low-power photographs of a control subject (a, c) and an AD patient (b, d), in sections immunostained for anti-PHF-Tau-AT8 (a, b) and anti-Aβ (c, d). TEC is indicated by the box. Immunostaining for anti-PHF-Tau-AT8 (b) and anti-Aβ (d) can be observed in the AD patient. These neuropathological marks are absent in the control subject (a, c). Scale bar (in d): 3 mm

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Higher magnifications of human TEC in coronal sections. Series of photomicrographs from a control subject (ad) and an AD patient (el). Sections are stained for Nissl (a, e), and immunostained with antibodies anti-NeuN (b, f), anti-PHF-Tau-AT8 (c, g, i, j) and anti-Aβ (d, h, k, l). PHF-Tau-AT8 positive neurons (g, i, j) and Aβ positive plaques (h, k, l) are observed in TEC from the AD patient. Scale bar (in panel l) corresponds to: 1 mm in panels a-d; 800 μm in panels e-h; 530 μm in panels i and k; 110 μm in panel j, and 220 μm in panel l

Tissue shrinkage estimation

Tissue shrinkage due to electron microscopy processing was estimated measuring the area before and after processing to correct the final values in both control and AD cases [45]. The area after processing was divided by the area value measured before processing, to obtain a shrinkage factor for any area measurement (p2) of 0.933.

Moreover, to estimate differences between control and AD cases, we measured the cortical thickness of TEC in three to five toluidine blue-stained semithin sections from all cases, obtained in the coronal plane of the cortex and containing the entire cortex, from the pial surface to the white matter. Measurements of the distance between the pial surface and the boundary with the white matter were performed with the aid of Fiji program (ImageJ 1.51; NIH, USA; http://imagej.nih.gov/ij/). To average data, three measurements were made per section.

In addition, FIB/SEM stacks of images were also corrected for the presence of fixation artifacts, which did not affect the proper identification and quantitation of synapses, i.e., swollen neuronal or glial processes. The volume occupied by these artifacts, calculated applying the Cavalieri principle [30], was discounted from the volume of the stacks of images to avoid under-estimation of the number of synapses per volume. Every FIB/SEM stack was examined and the volume artifact ranged between 3 and 33% of the volume stacks. Data on the number of synapses per volume were corrected accordingly.

Volume fraction estimation of cortical elements

Three to five semithin sections (1–2 μm thick) from all cases stained with toluidine blue were used to estimate the respective volume fractions occupied by (i) neuropil, (ii) cell bodies (from neurons and glia) and (iii) blood vessels. This estimation was performed applying the Cavalieri principle [30] by point counting using the integrated Stereo Investigator stereological package (Version 8.0, MicroBrightField Inc., VT, USA) attached to an Olympus light microscope (Olympus, Bellerup, Denmark) at 40× magnification. A grid, whose points covered an area of 400μm2, was overlaid over each semithin section to determine the volume fraction (Vv) occupied by the different elements: neurons, glia, blood vessels and neuropil (Additional file 1: Figure S1A). Vv was estimated with the following formulae: Vv neuropil = 100 - (Vv neurons+ Vv glia + Vv blood vessels).

Three-dimensional electron microscopy

The 3D study of the samples was carried out using a dual beam microscope (Crossbeam® Neon40 EsB, Carl Zeiss NTS GmbH, Oberkochen, Germany). This instrument combines a high-resolution field-emission SEM column with a focused gallium ion beam (FIB), which permits removal of thin layers of material from the sample surface on a nanometer scale. As soon as one layer of material is removed by the FIB (20 nm thick), the exposed surface of the sample is imaged by the SEM using the backscattered electron detector. The sequential automated use of FIB milling and SEM imaging allowed us to obtain long series of photographs of a 3D sample of selected regions [45]. Image resolution in the xy plane was 5 nm/pixel. Resolution in the z axis (section thickness) was 20 nm, and image size was 2048 × 1536 pixels. Although the resolution of FIB/SEM images can be increased, we have chosen these parameters as a compromise solution to obtain a large enough field of view where synaptic junctions could still be clearly identified, in a period of time that allowed us to have long series of sections in a relatively short, reasonable time (approximately 12 h per stack of images). The number of sections per stack ranged from 149 to 472, which corresponds to a corrected volume ranging from 260.2 to 824.4μm3 (mean: 471.3μm3). A total of 30 stacks of images of the neuropil from layer II of the TEC were obtained (three stacks per case, in all 10 cases; total volume studied: 14,140μm3).

Synaptic three-dimensional analysis

Stacks of images obtained by the FIB/SEM were analyzed using EspINA software (EspINA Interactive Neuron Analyzer, 2.1.9; http://cajalbbp.cesvima.upm.es/espina/), which allows the segmentation of synapses in the reconstructed 3D volume (for a detailed description of the segmentation algorithm, see [49]; Fig. 4). Since the synaptic junctions were fully reconstructed as described elsewhere [45], each synapse could be classified as asymmetric (AS) or symmetric (SS) based on its prominent or thin post-synaptic density (PSD: Additional file 1: Figure S2), respectively [29, 53].

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Screenshot of the EspINA software user interface. In the main window (top), the sections are viewed through the xy plane (as obtained by the FIB/SEM microscopy). The other two orthogonal planes, yz and xz, are also shown in adjacent windows. 3D reconstruction of a synapse is shown in the three orthogonal planes. The “Raw Information Report” window shows features extracted from the reconstructed 3D synapse. The 3D windows (bottom) show the three orthogonal planes and the 3D reconstruction of segmented synapses (bottom left). Synapses appear green (asymmetric synapses) or red (symmetric synapses) according to the colors assigned by the user, and their SAS appear yellow (bottom right)

EspINA provided the number of synapses in a given volume, which allows the estimation of the number of synapses per volume. EspINA also allowed the application of an unbiased 3D counting frame to perform direct counting (for details, see [45]).

In addition, geometrical features —such as size and morphology— and spatial distribution features (centroids) of each reconstructed synapse were also calculated by EspINA. This software also extracts the Synaptic Apposition Surface (SAS) and provides its morphological properties. Since the pre- and post-synaptic densities are located face to face, their surface areas are comparable (for details, see [50]). Since the SAS adapts to the curvature of the synaptic junction we have also measured its curvature as one minus the ratio between the projected area of the SAS and the area of the SAS. This measurement would be 0 in a flat SAS, and would increase to a maximum of 1 as the SAS curvature increases. Since the SAS comprises both the active zone and the PSD, it is a functionally relevant measure of the size of a synapse (Fig. 4) [50].

Spatial distribution analysis of synapses

To analyze the spatial distribution of synapses, Spatial Point Pattern analysis was performed as described elsewhere [4, 46]. Briefly, we compared the actual position of centroids of synapses with the Complete Spatial Randomness (CSR) model — a random spatial distribution model which defines a situation where a point is equally likely to occur at any location within a given volume. For each of the 30 different samples, we calculated three functions commonly used for spatial point pattern analysis: G, F and K functions (for a detailed description, see [9]). An additional step to explore the spatial distribution of a spatial pattern is to obtain the distance to the nearest neighbor. To do this, the distance of each synapse to its nearest synapse was measured, and comparison between control and AD patients was also performed. This study was carried out using the Spatstat package and R Project program [6].

Statistical analysis

To determine possible differences between groups, statistical comparisons of synaptic density, proportion of synapses, TEC thickness, synaptic size (SAS), neuronal and glial cell bodies, blood vessels and neuropil volume fraction, as well as the distance to the nearest neighbor were carried out using the unpaired Mann-Whitney (MW) nonparametric U-test (the normality and homoscedasticity criteria were not met). Frequency distribution analysis of the SAS was performed using Kolmogorov-Smirnov (KS) nonparametric test. Statistical studies were performed with the aid of the GraphPad Prism statistical package (Prism 5.00 for Windows, GraphPad Software Inc., USA) and SPSS program (IBM SPSS Statistics v22, IBM Corp., USA).

Results

Histopathological findings

The TEC region was delimited on the basis of previous studies [10, 14]. In Nissl-stained sections and sections immunostained for anti-NeuN, the TEC was distinguished because layers III and V merge and sweep obliquely to invade layer II of the EC [10, 14, 24, 85]. TEC is considered as part of the PRC area 35. PRC is proisocortex [36] and lacks a layer IV (agranular type of cortex). The most typical feature of TEC is the presence of an oblique band of neurons between layers V and III [24, 37] of PRC. Standard histopathological assessment of all cases was performed on Nissl-stained and NeuN-immunostained sections containing the TEC. AD cases showed an apparent reduction in the total volume of MTL structures, including TEC (Figs. 1, 2). In addition, immunostaining for anti-PHF-Tau and anti-Aβ revealed the presence of a variable amount of immunoreactive PHF-Tau neurons and Aβ-plaques in the AD cases, whereas in control cases no Aβ-plaques were found and only occasional PHF-Tau neurons were present (Figs. 1, 2, and 3; Table 1).

To evaluate the degree of reduction, measurements of the whole thickness of TEC were performed in toluidine blue-stained semithin sections. These results revealed a significantly lower thickness of the TEC (35% lower) in AD patients compared to controls (MW, p = 0.02; Table 2; Additional file 1: Table S1); its average thickness was 1.74 mm in AD patients versus 2.66 mm in control subjects.

Table 2.

Volume fraction occupied by cortical elements in layer II of the TEC. All volume data are corrected for shrinkage

Group Vneu (%; mean ± SD) Vg (%; mean ± SD) Vbv (%; mean ± SD) Vn (%; mean ± SD) TEC thickness (mm; mean ± SD)
Control 7.17 ± 0.98 0.50 ± 0.14 3.28 ± 0.41 89.05 ± 1.22 2.66 ± 0.37
Alzheimer 5.86 ± 0.84 0.35 ± 0.12 3.71 ± 0.48 90.08 ± 1.22 1.74 ± 0.39

SD standard deviation, TEC transentorhinal cortex, Vneu volume fraction occupied by neurons, Vg volume fraction occupied by glia, Vbv volume fraction occupied by blood vessels, Vn volume fraction occupied by neuropil

The data for individual cases are shown in Additional file 1: Table S1

Volume fraction of cortical elements

To estimate the possible degree of neuronal loss in the TEC in AD patients, the Vv was estimated for neurons, glia, blood vessels and neuropil. In control subjects, the volumes occupied by neuronal somata, glia somata, blood vessels and neuropil were 7.17%, 0.50%, 3.28% and 89.05%, respectively. In AD patients, these volumes were 5.86%, 0.35%, 3.71% and 90.08%, respectively. There was no significant difference between control and AD cases (MW, p > 0.05; Table 2; Additional file 1: Figure S1; Additional file 1: Table S1).

Synaptic density

To compare synaptic features between control subjects and AD patients, synapses were examined in the neuropil from layer II of TEC (i.e., avoiding the cell bodies of neurons as well as glial and blood vessels; Additional file 1: Figure S2) [22]. In order to assure that FIB/SEM sampling was obtained from the same cortical layer, all the samples (from both control and AD subjects) were taken below the first row of neurons beneath layer I at a distance of approximately 267.03−506.10 μm from the surface. We refer to this as layer II regardless of whether or not layer III was distinguished.

A total of 6102 synapses were identified; of these, 4646 synapses were analyzed —after discarding incomplete ones and those excluded by the counting frame— of which 2656 synapses were from control subjects (total tissue volume analyzed 5295 μm3), and 1990 were from AD patients (total tissue volume analyzed 5266 μm3) (Table 3, Additional file 1: Table S2). The number of synapses per volume was calculated by dividing the total number of synapses by the volume of the counting frame. Although values were lower in AD patients, we did not find significant differences in synaptic density (MW, p = 0.22) between control subjects (range: 0.41−0.75 synapses/μm3) and AD patients (range: 0.16−0.49 synapses/μm3; Fig. 5; Table 3, Additional file 1: Table S2).

Table 3.

Accumulated data obtained from the ultrastructural analysis of neuropil from layer II of the TEC. All volume data are corrected for shrinkage factor

Group No. AS synapses No. SS synapses No. all synapses % AS Synapses (mean ± SD) % SS Synapses (mean ± SD) CF volume (μm3) No. AS synapses/μm3 (mean ± SD) No. SS synapses/μm3 (mean ± SD) No. synapses/μm3 (mean ± SD) Distance to nearest neighbor (nm; mean ± SD)
Control 2545 111 2656 95.64 ± 1.24 4.36 ± 1.24 5295 0.49 ± 0.14 0.02 ± 0.01 0.51 ± 0.14 791.54 ± 74.47
Alzheimer 1887 103 1990 94.47 ± 0.72 5.53 ± 0.72 5266 0.35 ± 0.14 0.02 ± 0.01 0.37 ± 0.15 881.50 ± 129.37

All includes AS+SS synapses, AS asymmetric synapses, No. number, SD standard deviation, SS symmetric synapses

The data for individual cases are shown in Additional file 1: Table S2

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Graph showing the overall mean synaptic density in control and AD patients. Control cases are represented by circles and AD cases are represented by squares. Each color corresponds to each case analyzed in the study, as denoted in the upper right corner. No significant differences were found between groups (p-value > 0.05)

For practically all synaptic junctions found in control and AD patients, 3D segmentation allowed unambiguous classification into AS (prominent PSD) and SS (thin PSD) [29, 53, 54]. The proportion of each type of synapse was 95.64% (AS) and 4.36% (SS) in control subjects, and 94.47% and 5.53%, respectively, in AD patients (Table 3, Additional file 1: Table S2). Hence, no significant differences in proportions between control subjects and AD patients (MW, p = 0.15) were observed. Thus, the proportion of AS (excitatory) and SS (inhibitory) remained unchanged in layer II from TEC originating from AD patients.

Synaptic morphology: Synaptic apposition surface (SAS)

Synaptic morphological analysis was performed by extracting the SAS from each synapse [50]. SAS features such as the area, the perimeter and the curvature showed no significant differences between groups, and this was the case for both AS and SS (MW, p > 0.05; Table 4, Additional file 1: Table S3). Frequency distribution analysis did not reveal significant differences either (KS, p > 0.05; Additional file 1: Figure S3). Values of SAS fitted to a log-normal distribution in both cases (Additional file 1: Figure S3). In summary, AD does not seem to affect the shape and size or the synaptic junctions.

Table 4.

Area (nm2), perimeter (nm) and curvature (ratio) of the SAS. All data are corrected for shrinkage factor

Group Type of synapse Area of SAS (nm2; mean ± sem) Perimeter of SAS (nm; mean ± sem) Curvature of SAS (mean ± sem)
Control AS 117,800 ± 1957 1653 ± 18.94 0.049 ± 0.001
SS 76,190 ± 4082 1423 ± 49.14 0.050 ± 0.004
Alzheimer AS 123,200 ± 2327 1697 ± 22.96 0.049 ± 0.001
SS 67,750 ± 3611 1284 ± 41.00 0.049 ± 0.003

AS asymmetric synapses, sem standard error of the mean, SAS synaptic apposition surface, SS symmetric synapses

The data for individual cases are shown in Additional file 1: Table S3

Synaptic distribution: 3D spatial analysis

To analyze the spatial distribution of synapses, we compared the actual position of the centroids of synaptic junctions with the CSR model. A random distribution follows the basic reference model of a CSR point process or homogeneous spatial Poisson point process. We calculated the G, F and K functions for the 30 samples from controls (n = 15) and AD patients (n = 15), comparing each one with 100 simulations of the CSR model. Results from these comparisons indicated a clear fit of the samples to a CSR model, since G, F and K functions closely resembled the theoretical curve that represents these functions of a homogeneous Poisson process, both in control subjects and in AD patients (Additional file 1: Figure S4). Note that in the G function there is a dead space (indicated with an arrow) due to the fact that synapses cannot be too close to each other since they cannot overlap in space.

In addition to the location of synapses in each sample, we measured the distance of each synapse to its nearest synapse. The mean distance to its nearest neighbor measured between centroids of synaptic junctions was 791.54 nm in control subjects and 881.50 nm in AD patients. No significant differences between groups (MW, p = 0.42; Table 3, Additional file 1: Table S2) were found.

Therefore, our analysis indicates that, the spatial organization of synapses in the neuropil of layer II of the TEC corresponds to a random distribution, regardless of the type of sample (control subjects or AD patients).

Discussion

There were two main findings in the present study. First, at the light microscope level, we found that cortical thickness of the TEC displayed a severe reduction in AD patients, whereas no differences were observed in the volume occupied by neuronal and glial cell bodies, blood vessels and neuropil. Second, at the ultrastructural level, the analysis of the density, morphological features and spatial distribution of synapses in 3D electron microscope samples of layer II TEC neuropil from control and AD human brain samples showed no significant differences between groups.

Our data were derived from only five control cases and five AD patients. Therefore, the data obtained in the present study cannot be extrapolated to the whole population of patients with AD. However, although we examined relatively few cases, FIB/SEM samples do allow an unprecedented number of large stacks of serial ultrastructural images to be obtained from the neuropil, which provides 3D reconstructions of synapses to accurately determine their density, types, morphological features and spatial distribution in the human brain. Thus, the present results should be considered as robust data that should be verified in more cases and brain areas.

Volume occupied by cortical elements: Loss of cells

No significant differences were found regarding the volume occupied by different cortical elements between AD and control cases. In particular, the volume fraction occupied by neurons was 7.17% in control samples and 5.86% in AD. However, considering the severe reduction of TEC thickness, the total number of neurons in AD patients would be expected to be lower than in control cases. That is, since the cortical thickness in AD patients is 35% thinner than in controls, and, in addition, we did not observe significant changes in the neuronal size in AD samples, the total number of neurons must be dramatically reduced. These results are in line with previous studies in the EC of AD patients reporting a loss of neurons [28, 86]. Similarly, a reduction of cortical thickness in AD has been previously reported in frontal cortex, in areas 21 and 22 of Brodmann, as well as in the molecular layer of DG [59, 60, 63, 69]. The reduction of TEC thickness observed in the present study was apparently unrelated to the abundance of NFTs and Aβ plaques, since patient IF1 showed a TEC thickness close to control values, but abundant NFTs and Aβ plaques in TEC (Figs. 2, 3), similar to other AD patients that showed a clear reduction in the TEC thickness. Further studies with additional AD patients would be necessary to assert the correlation between the TEC thickness and the presence of NFTs and Aβ plaques.

Synaptic changes related to AD

Analysis of neuropil in the stacks of images of layer II of the TEC revealed that AD patients did not show a significantly lower synaptic density. However, since we found that TEC thickness in AD patients was 35% thinner than in control subjects, it follows that a decrease in the total number of synapses in AD occurs in this region. Previous studies performed in both AD patients and in animal models using light or electron microscopy to identify synapses —light microscopy immunocytochemical labeling of synaptic markers (mostly used in studies of human brains) or identification of synaptic junctions at the electron microscope level (see below)— have reported a loss of synapses per volume in the molecular layer of DG, CA1, temporal and cingulate gyrus, as well as in others regions of neocortex [2, 61, 62, 6467]. However, other studies found an increase in synaptic density in neocortex and hippocampus from APP/PS1 mice [38], or no alterations in EC from AD patients [68]. Discrepancies might be attributable to specific characteristics of the animal models, or features of the analyzed regions, such as those that may be due to sampling differences. For example, near Aβ plaques there is a reduction in the number of synapses [25] and the density of plaques depends on several factors such as the animal model used and the age of the animals. Moreover, it is common to estimate synaptic density indirectly by counting —at the light microscopic level— immunoreactive puncta using synaptic markers [34, 44]. Quantification of synaptic density in single ultrathin sections using transmission electron microscopy to infer 3D characteristics of synaptic junctions observed in two dimensions [64] could be inaccurate for synaptic density estimations depending on the stereological method and other technical constraints (for further discussion, see [22, 45]). FIB/SEM technology has been proved to be an excellent tool to study the ultrastructure and alterations of synaptic organization of the human brain [8]. Using this technique, we were able to fully reconstruct synaptic junctions in a 3D volume of tissue, thus making possible the identification and classification of all synaptic junctions as AS or SS — thereby solving the technical limitations of other methodologies and obtaining more accurate data about the density of synapses [45]. Thus, the present results indicate that there is not a reduction in the number of synapses per volume of neuropil but —given that there is a decrease in thickness of TEC— it is obvious that there is a decrease in the absolute number of synapses in AD patients.

Nevertheless, we did not find differences in the proportion of AS and SS, suggesting that, in the neuropil, there is not an imbalance between excitatory and inhibitory circuits in layer II of the TEC. Since the reduction in the absolute number of synapses affected AS and SS equally, and in the cortex the majority of synapses are AS, the major decrease might be due to the loss of AS. It is well known that AS are mostly formed with dendritic spines of pyramidal cells [21]. This suggests that it is likely that dendritic spine disconnection or dendritic spine loss in the TEC of AD patients occurs. Our results are in agreement with the previously reported alterations of dendritic spines in AD patients [40, 47, 55]. However, since our data are derived from the study of the neuropil, we cannot rule out alterations in the axo-somatic or axo-axonic synapses (i.e., changes in the number, size and shape).

Synaptic changes observed in AD have been proposed to occur during early phases of the disease in subcortical regions and TEC, as these regions represent the areas that are first altered, particularly affecting the monoaminergic system [11, 12, 72]. However, since most AD patients examined in the present study correspond to advanced stages of the diseases, we do not know when the synaptic loss occurred. Aβ peptides and tau proteins play normal roles at the synapse, but under pathological conditions, they may produce toxic effects at both pre- and post-synaptic elements, leading to synaptic loss and causing dysfunction in neurotransmitter release [25, 32, 57, 73, 89]. Association of Aβ oligomers with synaptic structures in AD has been related to alterations in both synapses and dendritic spines [31, 39, 87]. Further analysis of the identity of post-synaptic elements (i.e., dendritic spines versus dendritic shafts) in layer II of the TEC is necessary in order to elucidate whether there are changes in the synaptic targets due to AD.

Synaptic changes and interindividual variability

Despite the results regarding decrease in the number of synapses, it is important to note that there was remarkable interindividual variability. Technical effects were ruled out given that the postmortem delays were all similar and the procedures used were the same. We found that, in particular, two AD patients (IF6, VK11) showed synaptic density values similar to control subjects. According to the neuropathological criteria, these two patients presented NFTs only in the hippocampus and a low density of plaques (Braak/CERAD stage: III/A). By contrast, the AD patient who had the lowest synaptic density (VK22) presented NFTs widely distributed throughout the neocortex and a high density of plaques (Braak/CERAD stage: V/C). Thus, variability in synaptic density may be related to differences in the disease progression, that is, the more pathological signs, the less synaptic density. However, we also found differences in the synaptic density in AD cases with similar stages (Braak/CERAD stage: V/C), such as IF2 and VK22, whose synaptic densities were 0.40 synapses/μm3 and 0.16, respectively. In this regard, cognitive reserve has been proposed to account for the disjunction between the degree of brain pathology and its clinical manifestations [74]. This concept relies on the idea that individual differences in task processing may allow some individuals to cope better than others with brain changes, in general, including AD-related changes [75, 76]. Interestingly, case IF1 (Braak/CERAD stage: IV/B) apparently did not display cognitive impairment and, although its synaptic density was relatively low (0.33 synapses/μm3), the TEC had a greater thickness (2.3 mm) than cases with cognitive impairment. As pointed out by Ferrer [26], it should be kept in mind that AD—at least limited to the EC and TEC (stages I–II)— affects about 80% of individuals over 65 years, but dementia only occurs in a small percentage of individuals at this age (the prevalence of dementia in AD increases to 25% in 80-year-old individuals). Thus, it is possible that this particular case (IF1) may represent a pre-dementia stage of AD (prodromal AD) [26].

Nevertheless, there is an ongoing debate about the relationship of hyperphosphorylated-tau protein and the cognitive deficits in AD [16], due to the fact that the basic mechanism or mechanisms of cognitive deterioration are still not well understood.

In a previous study assessing the possible alterations to dendritic spines in pyramidal cells from AD patients [47], a remarkable loss of dendritic spines from pyramidal cells depending on the state of neurofibrillary pathology was found: in the so-called putative ‘pre-tangle’ stage, the dendritic trees of pyramidal neurons were unchanged. In the presence of well-developed NFTs, however, dendritic spine loss was obvious. In cases with an intermediate state of neurofibrillary pathology, the loss of dendritic spines was more variable. Since pyramidal neurons represent the principal building blocks of the cerebral cortex and dendritic spines are the main post-synaptic elements of cortical excitatory synapses and are fundamental structures in memory, learning and cognition [19], these alterations constitute what we think is an important event in the pathogenesis of AD. Therefore, the presence of hyperphosphorylated-tau protein in neurons does not necessarily mean that they suffer severe and irreversible effects as thought previously, but rather the characteristic cognitive impairment in AD is likely to depend on the relative number of neurons that have well-developed NFTs.

Finally, interindividual variability also emerges in the control group (synaptic density ranged from 0.41 to 0.75 synapses/μm3; see Additional file 1: Table S2). Although there are no studies in this brain region, previous studies on synaptic density in the temporal human cortex have shown variability depending on the gender (from 0.72 to 1.06 synapses/μm3) [1]. Furthermore, it has been found that some cortical areas of the macaque monkey display synaptic differences linked to aging [52]. Thus, although the number of cases analyzed in the present study is relatively low, the interindividual variability found in the present study might also be explained by gender or age differences.

Analysis of the synaptic morphology

Synapses of layer II from TEC in AD patients showed similar morphological features to control cases. In particular, our results did not find any differences in the SAS area, perimeter and curvature. These results are also in line with some previous studies showing no changes in synaptic apposition length in AD patients [62, 68]. However, other studies performed in human tissue samples or animal models have shown an increase in synaptic apposition length, and these changes have been suggested to occur as a compensatory mechanism in response to the decrease in the number of synapses [2, 59, 60, 63, 69]. In this regard, it is important to point out that in the present study, we have measured the SAS instead of the synaptic apposition length because of its advantages over other methods. First, it is extracted automatically from the previously segmented synaptic junction with no user intervention, avoiding any manual tracing and possible associated user bias [2, 50]. Second, the SAS, despite being a surface, is also a 3D object that adapts to, and reproduces the shape and curvature of the PSD. Third, quantitative information on the surface area, perimeter and curvature can also be extracted from the SAS, so size and shape can easily be correlated. Thus, the present results obtained from a large number of synaptic junctions segmented in 3D (n = 4646 synapses) provide support for there being no change in the SAS parameters evaluated here.

It is well established that synapses are dynamic structures than can undergo modifications due to variations in the activity patterns, and they are continuously remodeled and replaced [54]. Thus, our ultrastructural analysis indicates that this synaptic dynamism may not be altered in AD. Furthermore, the shape and size of the synaptic junctions are strongly correlated with release probability, synaptic strength, efficacy and plasticity [7, 27, 33, 51, 77, 83, 84]. However, we cannot rule out changes in the composition of neurotransmitter receptors, ion-channels, structural and signaling proteins, etc. For example, comparative human post-synaptic density (PSD) proteome analysis between control and AD patients has shown changes in several proteins involved in various cellular functions [17, 88]. Moreover, some loss of dendritic spines may occur in layer II of the TEC in AD or some dendritic spines may turn into non-synaptic dendritic spines [41, 47]. Thus, although no changes in the number of synapses were found in the present study, it is possible that a reorganization of synaptic targets (i.e., changes in the proportion of axo-spinous versus axo-dendritic shafts synapses) may occur in AD.

Synaptic distribution: 3D spatial analysis

The spatial distribution of synapses can be distributed according to 3 patterns: random pattern (where each synapse is equally likely to occur at any position); regular pattern (where each synapse is located as far as possible from its neighbors); and clustered pattern (where synapses tend to concentrate in groups). Moreover, when modeling the spatial distribution of synapses, we should take into account the fact that synaptic junctions cannot overlap because they are independent objects, and thus the minimum intersynaptic distances must be limited by the size of the synaptic junctions themselves [46].

Spatial distribution analysis showed that in both control and AD groups, synapses in the neuropil follow a random spatial distribution. Previous studies on the spatial distribution of synapses in plaque-free regions of neuropil from the frontal cortex in AD patients, in the molecular layer of DG from APP/PS1 mice, and in control somatosensory cortex of rat have also found a random pattern distribution [2, 8, 46]. Thus, it seems that random distribution of synapses is a widespread rule of the cerebral cortex that does not appear to be affected in AD.

In addition to the analysis of the spatial distribution of synapses, we examined the distance of each synapse to its nearest synapse and we did not find significant differences between control subjects and AD patients. The lowest mean distance to the nearest synapse (678.91 nm) corresponds to the subject with the highest synaptic density, while the highest mean distance to the nearest synapse (1102.29 nm) belongs to the AD patient showing the lowest synaptic density. Thus, despite synaptic variability found between subjects, it seems that lower synaptic densities are related to higher distances. Since we found a significant reduction in TEC cortical thickness and not significant differences in the synaptic density, we would expect a reduction in the distance to the nearest synapse in AD patients. However, this distance was slightly higher (but not significant) supporting the notion of a reduction in the overall number of synapses in layer II of TEC in these patients.

Conclusions

In summary, the present results provide support for neuronal and synaptic loss also occurring in the TEC, in line with previous studies. However, this decrease is not due to a reduction in the synaptic density, but it is inferred from the reduction in TEC cortical thickness. Since it is likely that there is shrinkage of the dendritic arborization of neurons concomitant with the reduced cortical thickness, there must be a compensatory mechanism (for example, generation of new dendritic branches) in the surviving or healthy neurons in order to explain the lack of changes in synaptic density. Finally, whether or not synaptic loss precedes neuronal loss by synaptic disconnection is not known, but our previous studies on the morphological alterations in dendritic spines of pyramidal cells in AD patients [47] seem to provide strong support for synaptic loss preceding neuronal loss.

Additional file

Additional file 1: (1.2MB, pdf)

Figure S1. Estimation of the volume occupied by cells, blood vessels and neuropil using the method of Cavalieri. Figure S2. Serial images obtained by FIB/SEM from the neuropil of layer II of TEC from an Alzheimer’s disease patient. Figure S3. Frequency plots of synaptic apposition surface (SAS) distribution. Figure S4. Analysis of the 3D synaptic spatial distribution. Table S1. Light microscopy data on volume fraction occupied by cortical elements in layer II of the TEC. Table S2. Data from the ultrastructural analysis of neuropil from layer II of the TEC. Table S3. Data regarding area (nm2), perimeter (nm) and curvature (ratio) of the SAS by individual cases. (PDF 1216 kb)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Carmen Álvarez, Miriam Marín and Lorena Valdés for their helpful assistance, and Nick Guthrie for his excellent text editing.

Funding

This study was funded by grants from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (SAF 2015–66603-P), Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red sobre Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas (CIBERNED, Spain, CB06/05/0066), the Alzheimer’s Association (ZEN-15-321663), and the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under grant agreement No. 720270 (Human Brain Project). MM-C was awarded a research fellowship from the Spanish Ministry of Education, Culture and Sport (grant FPU 14/02245).

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analyzed during the current study available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

3D

Three-dimensional

AD

Alzheimer's disease

APP/PS1

Amyloid precursor protein/presenilin-1 mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease

AS

Asymmetric synapses

Amyloid-β

CA1

Cornu ammonis 1

CSR

Complete Spatial Randomness

DG

Dentate gyrus

EC

Entorhinal cortex

FIB/SEM

Focused ion beam/scanning electron microscopy

KS

Kolmogorov-Smirnov

MTL

Medial temporal lobe

MW

Mann-Whitney

NFTs

Neurofibrillary tangles

PB

Phosphate buffer

PRC

Perirhinal cortex

PSD

Post-synaptic density

SAS

Synaptic apposition surface

SD

Standard deviation

sem

Standard error of the mean

SS

Symmetric synapses

TEC

Transentorhinal cortex

Vv

Volume fraction

Authors’ contributions

JDeF and LA-N oversaw and design the project. MD-A and LA-N designed and performed experiments. MD-A performed data analysis. LB-L and MM-C performed experiments and helped interpret experiments. RI provided some clinical specimens and helped interpret experiments. MD-A and LA-N drafted the initial manuscript. All authors read, reviewed, helped to edit, and approved the final manuscript.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Brain tissue samples were obtained following the guidelines and approval of the Institutional Ethical Committee from all involved institutions: Bellvitge University Hospital (Barcelona, Spain); Centro Alzheimer Fundación Reina Sofía, CIEN Foundation (Madrid, Spain); and School of Medicine, University of Castilla-La Mancha, (Albacete, Spain).

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (10.1186/s40478-018-0520-6) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

  • 1.Alonso-Nanclares L, Gonzalez-Soriano J, Rodríguez JR, DeFelipe J. Gender differences in human cortical synaptic density. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:14615–14619. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803652105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Alonso-Nanclares L, Merino-Serrais P, González S, DeFelipe J. Synaptic changes in the dentate gyrus of APP/PS1 transgenic mice revealed by electron microscopy. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2013;72:386–395. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e31828d41ec. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Alzheimer's Association Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13:325–373. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.02.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Anton-Sanchez L, Bielza C, Merchán-Pérez A, Rodríguez JR, DeFelipe J, Larrañaga P. Three-dimensional distribution of cortical synapses: a replicated point pattern-based analysis. Front Neuroanat. 2014;8:85. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2014.00085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Arendt T. Synaptic degeneration in AD. [review] Acta Neuropathol. 2009;118:167–179. doi: 10.1007/s00401-009-0536-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Baddeley A (2010) Analysing spatial point patterns in R. Clayton south. CSIRO, Canberra
  • 7.Biederer T, Kaeser PS, Blanpied TA. Transcellular Nanoalignment of synaptic function. Neuron. 2017;96:680–696. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Blazquez-Llorca L, Merchán-Pérez Á, Rodríguez JR, Gascón J, DeFelipe J. FIB/SEM technology and AD: three-dimensional analysis of human cortical synapses. J Alzheimers Dis. 2013;34:995–1013. doi: 10.3233/JAD-122038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Blazquez-Llorca L, Woodruff A, Inan M, Anderson SA, Yuste R, DeFelipe J, et al. Spatial distribution of neurons innervated by chandelier cells. Brain Struct Funct. 2015;220:2817–2834. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0828-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Braak H, Braak E. On areas of transition between entorhinal allocortex and temporal isocortex in the human brain. Normal morphology and lamina-specific pathology in AD. Acta Neuropathol. 1985;68:325–332. doi: 10.1007/BF00690836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 1991;82:239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Braak H, Del Tredici K. Where, when, and in what form does sporadic Alzheimerʼs disease begin? Curr Opin Neurol. 2012;25:708–714. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32835a3432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Braak H, Del Tredici K. The preclinical phase of the pathological process underlying sporadic AD. [review] Brain. 2015;138:2814–2833. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Braak H, Braak E, Yilmazer D, Bohl J, Braak H, Braak E. Topical review: functional anatomy of human hippocampal formation and related structures. [review] J Child Neurol. 1996;11:265–275. doi: 10.1177/088307389601100402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Braak H, Thal DR, Ghebremedhin E, Del Tredici K. Stages of the pathologic process in Alzheimer disease: age categories from 1 to 100 years. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2011;70(11):960–969. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e318232a379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Castellani RJ, Nunomura A, Lee HG, Perry G, Smith MA. Phosphorylated tau: toxic, protective, or none of the above. J Alzheimers Dis. 2008;14:377–383. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2008-14404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Chang RYK, Nouwens AS, Dodd PR, Etheridge N. The synaptic proteome in AD. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2013;9:499–511. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Coleman P, Federoff H, Kurlan R. A focus on the synapse for neuroprotection in Alzheimer disease and other dementias. [review] Neurology. 2004;63:1155–1162. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000140626.48118.0A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.DeFelipe J. The dendritic spine story: an intriguing process of discovery. [review] Front Neuroanat. 2015;9:14. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2015.00014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.DeFelipe J, Fairén A. A simple and reliable method for correlative light and electron microscopic studies. J Histochem Cytochem. 1993;41:769–772. doi: 10.1177/41.5.8468459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.DeFelipe J, Fariñas I. The pyramidal neuron of the cerebral cortex: morphological and chemical characteristics of the synaptic input. [review] Prog Neurobiol. 1992;39:563–607. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(92)90015-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.DeFelipe J, Marco P, Busturia I, Merchán-Pérez A. Estimation of the number of synapses in the cerebral cortex: methodological considerations. [review] Cereb Cortex. 1999;9:722–732. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.7.722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Dickson DW, Crystal HA, Bevona C, Honer W, Vincent I, Davies P. Correlations of synaptic and pathological markers with cognition of the elderly. Neurobiol Aging. 1995;16:285–304. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(95)00013-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ding SL, Van Hoesen GW. Borders, extent, and topography of human Perirhinal cortex as revealed using multiple modern neuroanatomical and pathological markers. Hum Brain Map. 2010;31:1359–1379. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Dorostkar MM, Zou C, Blazquez-Llorca L, Herms J. Analyzing dendritic spine pathology in AD: problems and opportunities. [review] Acta Neuropathol. 2015;130:1–19. doi: 10.1007/s00401-015-1449-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ferrer I. Defining Alzheimer as a common age-related neurodegenerative process not inevitably leading to dementia. [review] Prog Neurobiol. 2012;97:38–51. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ganeshina O, Berry RW, Petralia RS, Nicholson DA, Geinisman Y. Differences in the expression of AMPA and NMDA receptors between axospinous perforated and nonperforated synapses are related to the configuration and size of postsynaptic densities. J Comp Neurol. 2004;468:86–95. doi: 10.1002/cne.10950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gomez-Isla T, Price JL, McKeel DW, Morris JC, Growdon JH, Hyman BT. Profound loss of layer II entorhinal cortex neurons occurs in very mild AD. J Neurosci. 1996;16:4491–4500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-14-04491.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gray EG. Axo-somatic and axo-dendritic synapses of the cerebral cortex: an electron microscope study. J Anat. 1959;4:420–433. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Gundersen HJG, Bendtsen TF, Korbo L, Marcussen N, Miziller A, Nielsen K, et al. Some new, simple and efficient stereological methods and their use in pathological research and diagnosis. [review] APMIS. 1988;96:379–394. doi: 10.1111/j.1699-0463.1988.tb05320.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Gylys KH, Fein JA, Yang F, Wiley DJ, Miller CA, Cole GM. Synaptic changes in AD: increased amyloid-beta and gliosis in surviving terminals is accompanied by decreased PSD-95 fluorescence. Am J Pathol. 2004;165:1809–1817. doi: 10.1016/S0002-9440(10)63436-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Henstridge CM, Pickett E, Spires-Jones TL. Synaptic pathology: a shared mechanism in neurological disease. [review] Ageing Res Rev. 2016;28:72–84. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Holderith N, Lorincz A, Katona G, Rózsa B, Kulik A, Watanabe M, Nusser Z. Release probability of hippocampal glutamatergic terminals scales with the size of the active zone. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:988–997. doi: 10.1038/nn.3137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Honer WG, Dickson DW, Gleeson J, Davies P. Regional synaptic pathology in AD. Neurobiol Aging. 1992;13:375–382. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(92)90111-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Insausti R, Amaral DG. Entorhinal cortex of the monkey: IV. Topographical and laminar organization of cortical afferents. J Comp Neurol. 2008;509:608–641. doi: 10.1002/cne.21753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Insausti R, Muñoz-López M, Insausti AM, Artacho-Pérula E. The human Periallocortex: layer pattern in Presubiculum, Parasubiculum and entorhinal cortex. [review] Front Neuroanat. 2017;11:84. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2017.00084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Insausti R, Salinas A, Sanz E, Insausti A, Sobreviela T, Gonzalo LM (1994) The human perirhinal cortex. Architecture in controls and in Alzheimer's disease. Proceedings of the 24th annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience November, Miami, USA
  • 38.King DL, Arendash GW. Maintained synaptophysin immunoreactivity in Tg2576 transgenic mice during aging: correlations with cognitive impairment. Brain Res. 2002;926:58–68. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)03294-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Lacor PN, Buniel MC, Furlow PW, Clemente AS, Velasco PT, Wood M, et al. Abeta oligomer-induced aberrations in synapse composition, shape, and density provide a molecular basis for loss of connectivity in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci. 2007;27:796–807. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3501-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Llorens-Martín M, Fuster-Matanzo A, Teixeira CM, Jurado-Arjona J, Ulloa F, DeFelipe J, et al. GSK-3β overexpression causes reversible alterations on postsynaptic densities and dendritic morphology of hippocampal granule neurons in vivo. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18:451–460. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Llorens-Martín M, Blazquez-Llorca L, Benavides-Piccione R, Rábano A, Hernandez F, Avila J, et al. Selective alterations of neurons and circuits related to early memory loss in AD. [review] Front Neuroanat. 2014;8:38. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2014.00038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Mai J, Majtanik M, Paxinos G (2016) Atlas of the human brain, 4th edn. Academic Press/Elsevier, San Diego
  • 43.Masliah E, Mallory M, Alford M, DeTeresa R, Hansen LA, McKeel DW, et al. Altered expression of synaptic proteins occurs early during progression of AD. Neurology. 2001;56:127–129. doi: 10.1212/WNL.56.1.127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Masliah E, Terry RD, Alford M, Deteresa R. Quantitative immunohistochemistry of Synaptophysin in human neocortex: an alternative method to estimate density of presynaptic terminals in paraffin sections. J Histochem Cytochem. 1990;38:837–844. doi: 10.1177/38.6.2110586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Merchán-Pérez A, Rodríguez JR, Alonso-Nanclares L, Schertel A, DeFelipe J. Counting synapses using FIB/SEM microscopy: a true revolution for ultrastructural volume reconstruction. Front Neuroanat. 2009;3:18. doi: 10.3389/neuro.05.018.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Merchán-Pérez A, Rodríguez JR, González S, Robles V, DeFelipe J, Larrañaga P, et al. Three-dimensional spatial distribution of synapses in the neocortex: a dual-beam electron microscopy study. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:1579–1588. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Merino-Serrais P, Benavides-Piccione R, Blazquez-Llorca L, Kastanauskaite A, Rábano A, Avila J, et al. The influence of phospho-tau on dendritic spines of cortical pyramidal neurons in patients with AD. Brain. 2013;136:1913–1928. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Mirra SS, Heyman A, McKeel D, Sumi SM, Crain BJ, Brownlee LM, et al. The consortium to establish a registry for AD(CERAD). Part II. Standarization of the neuropathologic assessment of AD. Neurology. 1991;41:479–486. doi: 10.1212/WNL.41.4.479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Morales J, Alonso-Nanclares L, Rodríguez JR, DeFelipe J, Rodríguez Á, Merchán-Pérez Á. Espina: a tool for the automated segmentation and counting of synapses in large stacks of electron microscopy images. Front Neuroanat. 2011;18:18. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2011.00018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Morales J, Rodríguez A, Rodríguez JR, DeFelipe J, Merchan-Pérez A. Characterization and extraction of the synaptic apposition surface for synaptic geometric analysis. Front Neuroanat. 2013;7:20. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2013.00020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Nusser Z, Lujan R, Laube G, Roberts JD, Molnar E, Somogyi P. Cell type and pathway dependence of synaptic AMPA receptor number and variability in the hippocampus. Neuron. 1998;21:545–559. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80565-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Peters A, Kemper T. A review of the structural alteratio ns in the cerebral hemispheres of the aging rhesus monkey. [review] Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:2357–2372. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.11.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Peters A, Palay SL. The morphology of synapses. [review] J Neurocytol. 1996;25:687–700. doi: 10.1007/BF02284835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Peters A, Palay SL, Webster HD. The fine structure of the nervous system: the neurons and supporting cells. New York: Oxford University Press; 1991. p. 528. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Pozueta J, Lefort R, Shelanski ML. Synaptic changes in AD and its models. [review] Neurosci. 2013;251:51–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.05.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Prieto C, Eimil M, López de Silanes C, Llanero M. Fundación Española de Enfermedades Neurológicas. 2011. Impacto social de la enfermedad de alzheimer y otras demencias. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Rajmohan R, Reddy PH. Amyloid-Beta and Phosphorylated tau accumulations cause abnormalities at synapses of AD neurons. [review] J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;57:975–999. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Raskin J, Cummings J, Hardy J, Schuh K, Dean RA. Neurobiology of AD: integrated molecular, physiological, anatomical, biomarker, and cognitive dimensions. [review] Curr Alzheimer Res. 2015;12:712–722. doi: 10.2174/1567205012666150701103107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Scheff SW, Price DA. Synapse loss in the temporal lobe in AD. Ann Neurol. 1993;33:190–199. doi: 10.1002/ana.410330209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Scheff SW, Price DA. Synaptic density in the inner molecular layer of the hippocampal dental gyrus in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 1998;57:1146–1153. doi: 10.1097/00005072-199812000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Scheff SW, Price DA. Synaptic pathology in AD: a review of ultrastructural studies. [review] Neurobiol Aging. 2003;24:1029–1046. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Scheff SW, Price DA. AD-related alterations in synaptic density: neocortex and hippocampus. [review] J Alzheimers Dis. 2006;9:101–115. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2006-9S312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Scheff SW, Dekosky ST, Price DA. Quantitative assessment of cortical synaptic density in Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 1990;11:29–37. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(90)90059-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Scheff SW, Price DA, Ansari MA, Roberts KN, Schmitt FA, Ikonomovic MD, et al. Synaptic change in the posterior cingulate gyrus in the progression of AD. Alzheimers Dis. 2015;43:1073–1090. doi: 10.3233/JAD-141518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Scheff SW, Price DA, Schmitt FA, Dekosky ST, Mufson EJ. Synaptic alterations in CA1 in mild Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2007;68:1501–1508. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000260698.46517.8f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Scheff SW, Price DA, Schmitt FA, Mufson EJ. Hippocampal synaptic loss in early AD and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27:1372–1384. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Scheff SW, Price DA, Schmitt FA, Scheff MA, Mufson EJ. Synaptic loss in the inferior temporal gyrus in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2011;24:547–557. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-101782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Scheff SW, Sparks DL, Price DA. Quantitative assessment of synaptic density in the entorhinal cortex in AD. Ann Neurol. 1993;34:356–361. doi: 10.1002/ana.410340309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Scheff SW, Sparks DL, Price DA. Quantitave assessment of synaptic density in the outer molecular layer of the hippocampal dentate gyrus in Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia. 1996;7:226–232. doi: 10.1159/000106884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Selkoe DJ. AD is a synaptic failure. Science. 2002;298:789–791. doi: 10.1126/science.1074069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Serrano-Pozo A, Frosch MP, Masliah E, Hyman BT. Neuropathological alterations in Alzheimer disease. [review] Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med. 2011;1:a006189. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a006189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Šimić G, Leko MB, Wray S, Harrington CR, Delalle I, Jovanov-Miloševic N, et al. Monoaminergic neuropathology in Alzheimer’s disease. [review] Prog Neurobiol. 2017;151:101–138. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2016.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Spires-Jones TL, Hyman B. The intersection of amyloid Beta and tau at synapses in AD. [review] Neuron. 2014;82:756–771. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. [review] JINS. 2002;8:448–460. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Stern Y. Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. [review] Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2006;20:112–117. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000213815.20177.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Stern Y. Cognitive Reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Südhof TC. The presynaptic active zone. Neuron. 2012;75:11–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Suzuki WA. The anatomy, physiology and functions of the perirhinal cortex. [review] Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1996;6:179–l 88. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80071-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Suzuki WA, Amaral DG. Topographic organization of the reciprocal connections between the monkey entorhinal cortex and the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices. J Neurosci. 1994;14:1856–1877. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-03-01856.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Suzuki WA, Amaral DG. Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: cortical afferents. J Comp Neurol. 1994;350:497–533. doi: 10.1002/cne.903500402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Suzuki WA, Naya Y. The Perirhinal cortex. [review] Annu Rev Neurosci. 2014;37:39–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Sze CI, Troncoso JC, Kawas C, Mouton P, Price DL, Martin LJ. Loss of the presynaptic vesicle protein synaptophysin in hippocampus correlates with cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 1997;56:933–944. doi: 10.1097/00005072-199708000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Takumi Y, Ramirez-Leon V, Laake P, Rinvik E, Ottersen OP. Different modes of expression of AMPA and NMDA receptors in hippocampal synapses. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:618–624. doi: 10.1038/10172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Tarusawa E, Matsui K, Budisantoso T, Molnar E, Watanabe M, Matsui M, Fukazawa Y, Shigemoto R. Input-specific intrasynaptic arrangements of ionotropic glutamate receptors and their impact on postsynaptic responses. J Neurosci. 2009;29:12896–12908. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6160-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Taylor KI, Probst A. Anatomic localization of the transentorhinal region of the perirhinal cortex. Neurobiol Aging. 2008;29:1591–1596. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Van Hoesen GW, Hyman BT, Damasio AR. Entorhinal cortex pathology in AD. Hippocampus. 1991;1:1–8. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450010102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Wilcox KC, Lacor PN, Pitt J, Klein WL. Aβ oligomer-induced synapse degeneration in AD. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2011;31:939–948. doi: 10.1007/s10571-011-9691-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Zhou J, Jones DR, Duong DM, Levey AI, Lah JJ, Peng J. Proteomic analysis of postsynaptic density in Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Chim Acta. 2013;420:62–68. doi: 10.1016/j.cca.2013.03.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Zhou L, McInnes J, Wierda K, Holt M, Herrmann AG, Jackson RJ, et al. Tau association with synaptic vesicles causes presynaptic dysfunction. Nat Commun. 2017;8:15295. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Additional file 1: (1.2MB, pdf)

Figure S1. Estimation of the volume occupied by cells, blood vessels and neuropil using the method of Cavalieri. Figure S2. Serial images obtained by FIB/SEM from the neuropil of layer II of TEC from an Alzheimer’s disease patient. Figure S3. Frequency plots of synaptic apposition surface (SAS) distribution. Figure S4. Analysis of the 3D synaptic spatial distribution. Table S1. Light microscopy data on volume fraction occupied by cortical elements in layer II of the TEC. Table S2. Data from the ultrastructural analysis of neuropil from layer II of the TEC. Table S3. Data regarding area (nm2), perimeter (nm) and curvature (ratio) of the SAS by individual cases. (PDF 1216 kb)

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used and analyzed during the current study available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from Acta Neuropathologica Communications are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES