Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Child Dev. 2017 Sep 5;89(6):2059–2069. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12946

Beliefs about Stress Attenuate the Relation Among Adverse Life Events, Perceived Distress, and Self-Control

Daeun Park 1,*, Alisa Yu 2,*, Sarah E Metz 3, Eli Tsukayama 4, Alia J Crum 5, Angela L Duckworth 6
PMCID: PMC5837904  NIHMSID: NIHMS898058  PMID: 28872676

Abstract

Prior research has shown that adverse events in the lives of adolescents precipitate psychological distress, which in turn impairs self-control. The current study (N = 1343) examined the protective effects of stress mindsets—beliefs about the extent to which stress might be beneficial or strictly detrimental. The results confirmed that increasing the number of adverse life events across the school year predicted rank-order increases in perceived distress, which in turn predicted rank-order decreases in self-control. Adolescents who believed in the potential benefits of stress were less prone to feeling stressed in the wake of adverse life events. These findings suggest that changing the way adolescents think about stress may help protect them from acting impulsively when confronted with adversity.


Adolescents who experience adverse life events—such as parents fighting more than usual, a serious illness in the family, or close friends moving away—are more likely to smoke, act out, drink, earn lower grades, and drop out of school (Amato, 2000; Needle, Su, & Doherty, 1990; Newcomb & Harlow, 1986; Porche, Fortuna, Lin, & Alegria, 2011). Experimental research with laboratory animals and longitudinal studies of adolescents show that these adversity-induced impairments in self-control are not merely artifacts of third-variable confounds such as socioeconomic status (Arnsten, 2009; Duckworth, Kim, & Tsukayama, 2013). Still, identical life stressors can have very different effects; some adolescents suffer more than others (Zeidner & Hammer, 1990; Bonanno & Diminch, 2013; Masten et al., 1999). How can we explain this variability?

One possibility is that adolescents differ in stress mindsets, their implicit beliefs about stress. Although research has not yet examined stress mindsets in adolescence, a study of adults has shown that some view stress as strictly harmful, whereas others view it as potentially beneficial (Crum, Salovey, & Achor, 2013). This framework may be especially useful in stress management during adolescence, a period of heightened emotional reactivity and brain plasticity (Eiland & Romeo, 2013; Steinberg, 2008). As shown in Figure 1, the current study examines whether stress mindsets can mitigate the relation between adverse life events, perceived distress, and concomitant decreases in self-control.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Conceptual moderated mediation model. The effects of adverse life events on growth in self-control are mediated by growth in perceived distress, and the effect of life events on perceived distress are moderated by stress mindset.

Self-control refers to the ability to regulate impulses that compete with valued goals (Duckworth & Steinberg, 2015). The benefits of self-control are immense. For instance, self-control predicts academic achievement (Duckworth & Carlson, 2013) and psychological well-being during adolescence (Shoda, Mischel, & Peake, 1990; Mischel, Shoda, & Peake, 1988), as well as health, social, and economic outcomes 20 years later (Moffitt et al., 2011).

It has long been theorized that the perception of threat inclines individuals toward impulsivity. In particular, Metcalfe and Mischel (1999) suggested that “quick responding driven by innately determined stimuli” may have been advantageous earlier in our evolutionary history, insofar as self-control required more reflection and planning (p. 8). Nevertheless, in contemporary times, adolescents are generally better off when they maintain top-down regulation over attention, emotion, and behavior.

Academic work and interpersonal relations are two domains of special relevance to self-control in adolescence (Caspi, Henry, McGee, Moffitt, & Silva, 1995). Thus, when middle school students are asked to report on behaviors in their daily lives that exemplify self-control failure, their responses produce a two-factor solution. Though moderately correlated (r = .30), academic self-control more robustly predicts grade point average, high school graduation rates, and college enrollment (Galla et al., 2014; Tsukayama, Duckworth, &Kim, 2013), whereas interpersonal self-control is associated with greater agreeableness and lower aggression (Tsukayama et al., 2013).

An investigation into the negative link between adverse life events and self-control may warrant greater attention during adolescence (Duckworth et al., 2013; Grant et al., 2003; Needle et al., 1990). During this stage of development, the brain undergoes considerable structural and functional change, particularly in areas related to reward systems and self-control (Steinberg, 2008). Relatedly, adolescence is a period marked by normative increases in risky, impulsive behaviors, including experimentation with drugs, unprotected sex, and reckless driving (Steinberg, 2007). These impulsive behaviors can initiate a vicious cycle by precipitating future adverse events (e.g., unwanted pregnancies, injury, or death of a friend; Moffitt et al, 2011).

Interestingly, the number of objective life stressors correlates only modestly with the degree of psychological distress experienced by adults (r = .17 to .20; Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983) and adolescents (r = .13; Duckworth et al., 2013). Stress is defined as the occurrence of adversity in goal-related efforts, whereas perceived distress refers to the emotional experience of worry and perceived inability to cope with demands (Selye, 1975). In other words, the objective stressors in our lives are one thing, but how we react to them emotionally is another (Gross, 1998; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Schachter & Singer, 1962). In adults, distress predicts physical health better than the frequency of adverse life events (Cohen et al., 1983). Likewise, in adolescents, distress mediates the relation between adverse life events and subsequent impairments in self-control (Duckworth et al., 2013).

Differences in stress mindsets may help explain why adverse life events are only loosely coupled with the subjective experience of distress. Specifically, some individuals may hold more of a stress-is-enhancing mindset, the belief that stress can have positive consequences, whereas others may have more of a stress-is-debilitating mindset, the belief that stress invariably leads to detrimental outcomes (Crum et al., 2013). Whereas individuals with a stress-is-enhancing mindset tend to embrace stressful situations as opportunities to learn and develop, those with a stress-is-debilitating mindset more rigidly interpret stress as an impediment to growth and vitality. In adults, a stress-is-enhancing mindset corresponds to lower levels of perceived distress and fewer symptoms of depression and anxiety, and predicts higher levels of energy, workplace performance, and life satisfaction (Crum et al., 2013).

Virtually nothing is known about how stress mindsets operate earlier in life, but prior research has shown that mindsets about intelligence (Blackwell, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007) and personality (Yeager, Trzeniewski, Tirri, Nokelainen, & Dweck, 2011) profoundly influence adolescent development. In the current investigation, we followed a large, socioeconomically and ethnically diverse sample of adolescents across a full school year, assessing adverse life events, perceived distress, and self-control in the fall and spring. We expected to replicate prior research showing that increases in adverse life events predict decreases in self-control, an effect mediated by concomitant increases in perceived distress (Duckworth et al., 2013). Most important, we hypothesized that adolescents with a stress-is-enhancing mindset would experience less psychological distress than peers with a stress-is-debilitating mindset, thereby mitigating the negative relation between adverse life events and self-control.

Method

Participants

Initially, data from 1,346 students were available. Of these, three students did not have any data on the variables in the model. Thus, these three students were excluded in the final sample. Final sample comprised 1,343 eighth grade (Mage = 14.77, SDage = 0.59) students in eight middle schools in Pennsylvania, Idaho, California, and Texas. Approximately 52% were African American, 29% Caucasian, 12% Hispanic, 6% Asian, and 1% of other ethnic backgrounds. About 49% were female, and 63% were from low-income families as indicated by their eligibility for free or reduced-price meals. Data was collected from October 2014 to June 2015.

Measures

Adverse life events

Following prior research (Duckworth et al., 2013; adapted from Johnson & McCutcheon, 1980), adolescents were given a checklist of 14 age-appropriate negative life events in the fall and spring (e.g., “increased arguments or fights between parents,” “close friend had problems”; Table 2), and were asked to indicate if any of these events had happened to them during the past six months. Prior research has established test-retest reliability as well as convergent and discriminant validity for this measure (Brand & Johnson, 1982). The distribution of adverse life events was positively skewed in the fall (skewness = .99, kurtosis = .92) and spring (skewness = 1.12, kurtosis = 1.51), so we log-transformed the data before performing the analyses shown here. Separately, we confirmed that the results were nearly identical to the untransformed life events variable.

Table 2.

Frequency of of adverse life events in fall and spring.

Adverse Life Events in Fall Percentage in Fall Percentage in Spring
1. Family member had problems 44% 40%
2. Close friend had problems 32% 34%
3. Family financial troubles or worries about money 30% 29%
4. Friends moved away or you moved away from friends 30% 19%
5. Increased arguments or fights between parents 29% 28%
6. Serious illness or injury of family member 26% 26%
7. Death of family member 25% 22%
8. Serious illness or injury of a close friend 14% 15%
9. Brother or sister leaving home 13% 13%
10. Parents separated or divorced 13% 8%
11. Increased absence of parents from the home 10% 10%
12. Major personal illness or injury 7% 8%
13. Death of a close friend 7% 6%
14. Mother or father lost job 6% 7%
15. None of above 22% 24%

Stress mindsets

We selected three age-appropriate items from a stress mindset scale developed for adults (Crum et al., 2003). In the fall, on a six-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 6 =strongly agree), adolescents indicated whether they believed stress leads to learning and growth, health and energy, and productivity (e.g., “Experiencing stress improves your learning and growth”; Appendix A). Higher scores represented higher levels of a stress-is-enhancing mindset. The observed alpha was .77. The distribution of stress mindsets was slightly positively skewed (skewness = .65, kurtosis = .001), so we log-transformed the data. As with adverse life events, results also were nearly identical when we used the untransformed stress mindset variable (results available upon request).

Perceived distress

In the fall and spring, adolescents completed six items adapted from the Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen et al., 1983) on a five-point Likert scale (1= never, 5 = always; see Appendix B). The scale measures the degree to which they have recently worried and experienced life as uncontrollable (e.g., “During the past month, how often have you felt stressed?” “During the past month, how often have you felt that difficulties were piling up so high that you could not overcome them?” “During the past month, how often have you felt worried?”). The observed alphas were .71 and .76 in the fall and spring, respectively.

Self-control

In the fall and spring, adolescents completed a ten-item self-control scale adapted from the Impulsivity Scale for Children (Tsukayama et al., 2013). They responded to five items measuring academic self-control behaviors (e.g., “During the past month, I paid attention, even when there were distractions”) and five items measuring interpersonal self-control behaviors (e.g., “During the past month, I behaved well even when I was upset.”) using a five-point Likert scale (1 = never, 5 = always). Prior work showed that items describing self-control in these domains correlate highly with the domain-general Brief Self-Control Scale (Tangney et al., 2004) at r = .72 to .88 (Tsukayama et al., 2013). The observed alphas were .85 in both fall and spring.

In addition, we asked English, social studies, science, and math teachers to rate each student on self-control. Following Galla et al. (2014) and Park, Tsukayama, Goodwin, Patrick, & Duckworth (2016), we showed teachers all of the self-control items students completed about themselves and asked them to rate each student on academic and interpersonal self-control, respectively, using the same five-point Likert scale (1 = never, 5 = always). The intraclass correlation coefficient was .83 for academic self-control and .81 for interpersonal self-control, indicating that teachers were highly consistent with each other in rating their students. Based on the high correlation between ratings of academic and interpersonal self-control ratings in the fall (r = .91, p <.001) and spring (r = .86, p < .001), we averaged academic and interpersonal self-control scores for each student at each time point (Appendix C).

Following Diener and Eid (2006), we created composite scores from self and teacher ratings to increase validity and reliability. The correlations between student and teacher rating were r = .46, p < .001 in the fall and r = .45, p < .001 in the spring. This compares favorably to meta-analytic correlations between child self-report and informant ratings of self-control (r = .48, Duckworth & Kern, 2011).

Demographic information

We obtained data on gender, ethnicity, and free and reduced lunch status from school records.

Results

As shown in Table 1, adolescents reported an average of between two and three adverse life events in the fall (M = 2.90, SD = 2.56) and spring (M = 2.66, SD = 2.48). As shown in Table 2, 22% and 24% of adolescents reported not having any of 14 adverse life events in the past six months in the fall and spring, respectively. The most commonly reported adverse life event was a family member having problems (44 % in the fall and 40% in the spring), followed by a close friend having problems (32% in the fall and 34% in the spring), and family financial troubles (30% in the fall and 29% in the spring).

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations among all measures

Measures M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Fall (T1)
  1. Stress-is-enhancing mindset 2.46 1.17
  2. Adverse life events 2.90 2.56 .06
  3. Perceived distress 2.83 0.71 -.06* .41***
  4. Self-control 3.56 0.70 -.09** -.07* -.13***
Spring (T2)
  5. Adverse life events 2.66 2.48 -.01 .56*** .32*** -.07*
  6. Perceived distress 2.89 0.76 -.06* .38*** .60*** -.09** .39***
  7. Self-control 3.46 0.80 -.09** -.06* -.10** .82*** -.09** -.12***
Demographics
  8. Female 49% -.10** .20*** .21*** .18*** .19*** .27*** .16***
  9. Caucasian 29% -.12*** .04 .06* .16*** .03 .01 .17***
  10. African-American 52% .11*** .02 -.09** -.29*** .03 -.04 -.30***
  11. Hispanic 12% -.01 -.01 .04 .08** -.03 .02 .09**
  12. Asian 6% .01 -.10*** .02 .20*** -.07* .03 .18***
  13. Multiracial 1% -.02 -.03 .00 .02 -.01 .01 .00
  14. Free or reduced lunch 63% .06 -.02 -.02 -.17*** -.02 -.01 -.19***

Note.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001

Like adults (Crum et al., 2013), adolescents tended to believe that stress was more detrimental than enhancing (M = 2.46, SD = 1.17). Stress mindsets only weakly correlated with perceived distress in the fall (r = -.06, p = .042) and spring (r = -.06, p = .047) and with adverse life events in the fall (r = .06, p = .067) and spring (r = - .01, p > .10). Replicating previous findings (Crum et al., 2013), the magnitude of these associations indicate that stress mindsets are distinct from other stress-related constructs.

Next, we tested our hypothesis that rank-order increases in perceived distress mediate the effect of rank-order increases in adverse life events on rank-order decreases in self-control and, moreover, that stress mindsets attenuate the association between life events and perceived distress. To do so, we fit a longitudinal moderated mediation model with path analyses using Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, 2002). The autoregressive panel design we employed analyzes change. More broadly, when a prior measure of a variable is used as a predictor of that variable, then the coefficients of the other predictors in the model are interpreted as predicting changes in the outcome (Fleeson, 2007). Kessler and Greenberg (1981) note that “linear difference equations (those involving ΔX as dependent variable and equations involving only static scores (X1 and X2)) are mathematically equivalent and can readily be transformed into one another” (p. 11). In other words, the same results are produced if actual change scores are used. We used full-information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation, which provides less biased results with random missing values (Peters & Enders, 2002). We standardized all variables to facilitate interpretation of coefficients.

Based on prior research demonstrating demographic characteristics are associated with frequency of adverse life events (Gillum, Prineas, Gomez-Marin, Chang, & Finn, 1984; Gore, Aseltine, & Colten, 1992; Wilkie, 2001), self-control (Duckworth & Seligman, 2006; Mason & Windle, 2002; Moffit et al., 2011), and GPA (Duckworth & Seligman, 2006; McCarthy, 2000), we controlled for demographic information. Furthermore, because middle school students move to different classrooms during school days and were rated by multiple teachers (English, social studies, science, and math teachers), there is no practical way to examine analyses at the classroom level. Although students were nested in schools, eight clusters are not enough to conduct random-effects multilevel structure models. Therefore, in our analyses, to adjust for clustering, we dummy-coded the school variables and controlled for them in all analyses.

Goodness of fit indices for the model summarized in Figure 2 were good, χ2(8) = 22.85, p < .01, CFI = .99, RMSEA = .037, 90% confidence interval (CI) = [.020, .056]. The model fit was good whether we used self-reported self-control (χ2(8) = 20.94, p < .01, CFI = 0.99, RMSEA = .035, 90% CI = [.017, .053]) or teacher -rated self-control (χ2(8) = 17.61, p = .024, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = .030, 90% CI = [.010, .049]).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

In this path model, the effects of negative life events on perceived distress vary as a function of stress mindset. All exogenous variables were allowed to correlate and the paths shown in the figure were freely estimated. Error variances for all endogenous variables were estimated (244 parameters were estimated). Values shown are standardized coefficients. Not shown are gender, ethnicity, lunch status, and school affiliation covariates. Solid lines indicate significant paths, and dotted lines represent non-significant paths.

As expected, when adverse life events increased across the school year, so too did perceived distress (β = .21, p < .001), which in turn predicted decreases in self-control over the same period (β = -.05, p = .012). We found that adverse life events had, via perceived distress, a significant indirect effect on self-control, 95% CI = [-.020, -.002]. Likewise, we confirmed that a stress-is-enhancing mindset attenuated the association between adverse life events and perceived distress. Specifically, the interaction term between number of adverse life events and stress mindsets reliably predicted changes in perceived distress (β = -.06, p = .013).

As shown in Figure 3, adolescents with a stress-is-debilitating mindset were more susceptible to elevated perceived distress due to adverse life events (β = .27, p < .001) than were adolescents with a stress-is-enhancing mindset (β = .15, p < .001). Additional analyses indicated that at low levels of adversity, the perceived distress level of adolescents with a stress-is-enhancing mindset and those with a stress-is-debilitating mindset were not statistically different (β = .05, p > .20). In contrast, at high levels of adversity, the perceived distress level of students with a stress-is-debilitating mindset was higher than those with a stress-is-enhancing mindset (β = -.08, p = .02).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Perceived distress level in spring as a function of adverse life events and stress mindset. Low adverse life events and stress-is- debilitating mindset each refer to values one standard deviation below the mean; high adverse life events and stress-is-enhancing mindset each refer to values one standard deviation above the mean.

In a separate model (available upon request), we examined whether stress mindsets moderate the relation between changes in perceived distress and changes in self-control, but it did not (β = -.004, p > .80). In other words, it appears that stress mindsets may influence how adolescents react emotionally to objective stressors in their lives, not how their emotional response in turn influence their ability to regulate their impulses.

Because perceived distress and self-control were measured synchronously in the fall and spring, we considered the possibility of reverse moderated mediation. In other words, we tested an alternative to our theoretical model, specifying that changes in self-control predict changes in perceived distress. The interaction effect (stress mindsets x adverse life events) on self-control was not significant (β = -.001, p < .90). The fit of this alternative path model (χ2(8) = 27.84, p < .001, CFI = .99, RMSEA = .04, 90% CI = [.026, .061]) was acceptable; however, the reverse mediation model’s Akaike information criterion (AIC; 67094.45) and Bayesian information criterion (BIC; 68363.89) were greater than the original model’s AIC (67089.46) and BIC (68358.91). Although AIC and BIC differences greater than 10 are considered very strong evidence in favor of the model with the smaller values, a difference greater than 2 is still considered positive evidence for a better model (Franken, Laceulle, Aken, & Ormel, 2017; Raftery, 1995). Thus, we concluded that this alternative model did not fit the data as well as our posited theoretical model.

Discussion

This study is the first to show that while many adolescents believe stress is entirely harmful, others see its potential benefits. In a large and diverse sample of adolescents followed for an entire school year, increases in the number of adverse life events from fall to spring predicted rank-order increases in perceived distress which, in turn, predicted rank-order decreases in self-control over the same period. As hypothesized, the negative association between adverse life events and perceived distress was weaker for those who endorsed a stress-is-enhancing mindset.

This result raises important questions: How do stress mindsets develop? Why do some adolescents come to believe that stress is necessarily detrimental, whereas others are more inclined to see how it can lead to growth? One possibility is that adolescents model their parents and teachers; their beliefs are molded by how adults in their lives respond to adversity. While the origins of stress mindsets are beyond the scope of our investigation, prior developmental studies have found that responses to success, failure, and mistakes by parents and teachers shape children’s own theories of intelligence (Gunderson et al., 2013; Haimovitz & Dweck, 2016; Park, Gunderson, Tsukayama, Levine, & Beilock, 2016; Pomerantz & Kempner, 2013).

Also, we speculate that either stress mindset can be affirmed by experience. On one hand, the belief that stress is always debilitating can be reinforced when adverse events lead to perceived distress, impulsive behavior, and bad outcomes. On the other hand, the belief that stress can sometimes lead to growth and development can also be reinforced, especially when challenges are met with effortful mastery (cf. Kubala, Christianson, Kaufman, Watkins, & Maier, 2012).

Regardless of ontogeny, our findings suggest benefits of a stress-is-enhancing mindset in adolescence. In adults, brief manipulations (e.g., video clips describing stress as either enhancing or debilitating) have been shown to produce measurable benefits for health and work performance one week later (Crum et al., 2013). Can stress mindsets likewise be changed in adolescents? If so, will such changes show enduring benefits, as has been demonstrated for changing adolescents’ mindsets about intelligence and personality (Blackwell et al., 2007; Yeager, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2013)? Additional research is needed to answer these questions.

Another fruitful direction for future research is to assess life events more comprehensively than was possible in this investigation. Following Evans (2004, 2006), we took a cumulative risk approach by counting the number of adverse life events adolescents experienced in the past six months. We found no clear relations between particular types of life events and perceived distress, but separate theoretical and empirical work suggests that the nature of adversity may matter (Delongis, Coyne, Dakof, Folkman, & Lazarus, 1982; Kanner, Coyne, Schaefer, & Lazarus, 1981). It is also true that hassles in everyday life can influence psychological and somatic symptoms at least as much as major life events (Delongis et al., 1982; Kanner et al., 1981). Furthermore, although we only asked about adverse life events during the past six months, it is possible that the effects of life events vary depending on whether they are chronic or acute. Longitudinal research with a longer time horizon is needed to more fully probe the effects of enduring versus transient adversity.

Lastly, in this first-ever study of stress mindsets in children, we did not include items asking about the detrimental effects of stress. Because stress is commonly described as damaging, we assumed that all children would agree that it has detrimental effects (see Hong, Chiu, Dweck, Lin, & Wan, 1999 for a similar methodology to avoid social desirability bias). Nevertheless, further investigation is needed to confirm our results with items that directly measure beliefs about the detrimental effects of stress.

While everything should be done to limit the occurrence of unfortunate events in the lives of young people, our findings also suggest that altering implicit beliefs about stress may buffer against associated psychological distress and, in turn, impairments in their self-regulatory capacity. Indeed, our view is that a better understanding of the ontogeny of stress mindsets will pave the way to psychologically wise interventions (Walton, 2014) that, we hope, support the well-being and development of adolescents facing adversity.

Supplementary Material

Supp info

Acknowledgments

This research was made possible by the Templeton Foundation and the National Institute on Aging (R24-AG048081-01). The content of this article is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies.

Appendix A

Stress Mindset Scale

Please rate how much each statement describes you using the scale below.

  1. Experiencing stress improves your health and increases your energy level.

  2. Experiencing stress improves your productivity. You get more done when you experience stress.

  3. Experiencing stress improves your learning and growth.

Appendix B

Perceived Distress Scale

During the past month, how often have you felt…

  1. that you were unable to control the important things in your life?

  2. confident about your ability to handle your personal problems?*

  3. that things were going your way?*

  4. difficulties were piling up so high that you could not overcome them?

  5. stressed?

  6. worried?

*reverse coded items

Appendix C

Self-Report Self-Control Measure

During the past month…

  1. I came to class prepared.

  2. I followed directions.

  3. I got to work right away instead of waiting around until the last minute.

  4. I paid attention, even when there were distractions.

  5. I stayed focused when doing independent work.

  6. I stayed calm even when others bothered or criticized me.

  7. I allowed others to speak without interruption.

  8. I was polite to classmates.

  9. I controlled my temper.

  10. I behaved well even when I was upset.

Teacher-Rating of Self-Control

Please rate [CHILD NAME] during the past month:

Academic self-control

  • came to class prepared.

  • followed directions.

  • got to work right away instead of waiting around until the last minute.

  • paid attention, even when there were distractions.

  • stayed focused when doing independent work.

Interpersonal self-control

  • stayed calm even when others bothered or criticized her/him.

  • allowed others to speak without interruption.

  • was polite to classmates.

  • controlled her/his temper.

  • behaved well even when s/he was upset.

Contributor Information

Daeun Park, Chungbuk National University.

Alisa Yu, Stanford University.

Sarah E. Metz, University of Pennsylvania

Eli Tsukayama, University of Hawaii – West Oahu.

Alia J. Crum, Stanford University

Angela L. Duckworth, University of Pennsylvania

References

  1. Amato PR. The consequences of divorce for adults and children. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2000;62:1269–1287. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.01269. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arnsten AF. Stress signaling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:410–422. doi: 10.1038/nrn2648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Blackwell L, Trzesniewski K, Dweck CS. Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development. 2007;78:246–263. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00995.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bonanno GA, Diminich ED. Positive adjustment to adversity: Trajectories of minimal-impact resilience and emergent resilience. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry: Annual Research Review. 2013;54:341–401. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brand AH, Johnson JH. Note on reliability of the Life Events Checklist. Psychological Reports. 1982;50(3c):1274. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1982.50.3c.1274. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Caspi A, Henry B, McGee RO, Moffitt TE, Silva PA. Temperamental origins of child and adolescent behavior problems: From age three to age fifteen. Child Development. 1995;66:55–68. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00855.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1983;24:385–396. doi: 10.2307/2136404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Crum A, Salovey P, Achor S. Rethinking Stress: The Role of Mindsets in Determining the Stress Response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2013;104:716–733. doi: 10.1037/a0031201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. DeLongis A, Coyne JC, Dakof G, Folkman S, Lazarus RS. Relationship of daily hassles, uplifts, and major life events to health status. Health Psychology. 1982;1:119–136. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.1.2.119. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Diener E, Eid M. Introduction: The need for multimethod measurement in psychology. In: Eid M, Diener E, editors. Handbook of multimethod measurement in psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2006. pp. 457–463. [Google Scholar]
  11. Duckworth AL, Carlson SM. Self-regulation and school success. In: Sokol BW, Grouzet FME, Müller U, editors. Self-regulation and autonomy: Social and developmental dimensions of human conduct. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2013. pp. 208–230. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Duckworth AL, Kern ML. A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Journal of Research in Personality. 2011;45:259–268. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Duckworth AL, Kim B, Tsukayama E. Life stress impairs self-control in early adolescence. Frontiers in Developmental Psychology. 2013;3:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Duckworth AL, Seligman ME. Self-discipline gives girls the edge: Gender in self-discipline, grades, and achievement test scores. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2006;98:198–208. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.198. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Duckworth AL, Steinberg L. Unpacking self-control. Child Development Perspectives. 2015;9:32–37. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Eiland L, Romeo RD. Stress and the developing adolescent brain. Neuroscience. 2013;249:162–171. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195306255.001.0001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Evans GW. The environment of childhood poverty. American Psychologist. 2004;59:77–92. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Evans GW. Child development and the physical environment. Annual Review of Psychology. 2006;57:423–451. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fleeson W. Studying personality processes: Explaining change in between-persons longitudinal and within-person multilevel models. In: Robins RW, Fraley RC, Krueger RF, editors. Handbook of research methods in personality psychology. New York: Guilford; 2007. pp. 523–542. [Google Scholar]
  20. Franken A, Laceulle OM, Van Aken MA, Ormel J. Using Response Surface Analysis to Interpret the Impact of Parent–Offspring Personality Similarity on Adolescent Externalizing Problems. European Journal of Personality. 2017;31:104–117. doi: 10.1002/per.2088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Galla BM, Plummer BD, White R, Meketon D, D’Mello SK, Duckworth AL. The Academic Diligence Task (ADT): Assessing individual differences in effort on tedious but important schoolwork. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2014;39(4):314–325. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gillum RF, Prineas RJ, Gomez-Marin O, Chang PN, Finn S. Recent life events in school children: Race, socioeconomic status, and cardiovascular risk factors: The Minneapolis Children’s Blood Pressure Study. Journal of Chronic Diseases. 1984;37:839–851. doi: 10.1016/0021-9681(84)90017-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gore S, Aseltine RH, Jr, Colten ME. Social structure, life stress, and depressive symptoms in a high school-age population. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1992;33:97–113. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2137249. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Grant KE, Compas BE, Stuhlmacher AF, Thurm AE, McMahon SD, Halpert JA. Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Moving from markers to mechanisms of risk. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129:447–466. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology. 1998;2:271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gunderson EA, Gripshover SJ, Romero C, Dweck CS, Goldin-Meadow S, Levine SC. Parent praise to 1‐to 3‐year‐olds predicts children’s motivational frameworks 5 years later. Child Development. 2013;84:1526–1541. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Haimovitz K, Dweck CS. What predicts children’s fixed and growth intelligence mind-sets? Not their parents’ views of intelligence but their parents’ views of failure. Psychological Science. 2016;27:859–869. doi: 10.1177/0956797616639727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hong YY, Chiu CY, Dweck CS, Lin DMS, Wan W. Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1999;77:588–599. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.588. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Johnson J, McCutcheon S. Assessing life events in older children and adolescents: Preliminary findings with the life events checklist. In: Sarason I, Spielberger C, editors. Stress and anxiety. Vol. 7. Washington, DC: Hemisphere; 1980. pp. 111–125. [Google Scholar]
  30. Kanner AD, Coyne JC, Schaefer C, Lazarus RS. Comparison of two modes of stress measurement: Daily hassles and uplifts versus major life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1981;4:1–39. doi: 10.1007/BF00844845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kessler RC, Greenberg DF. Linear panel analysis: Models of quantitative change. New York: Academic Press; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  32. Kubala KH, Christianson JP, Kaufman RD, Watkins LR, Maier SF. Short-and long-term consequences of stressor controllability in adolescent rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 2012;234:278–284. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.06.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lazarus RS, Folkman S. Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  34. Mason WA, Windle M. Gender, self-control, and informal social control in adolescence: A test of three models of the continuity of delinquent behavior. Youth & Society. 2002;33:479–514. [Google Scholar]
  35. Masten AS, Hubbard JJ, Gest SD, Tellegen A, Garmezy N, Ramirez ML. Competence in the context of adversity: Pathways to resilience and maladaptation from childhood to late adolescence. Developmental Psychopathology. 1999;11:143–169. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499001996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. McCarthy K. The effects of student activity participation, gender, ethnicity, and socio-economic level on high school student grade point averages and attendance. United States Department of Education. Office of Educational Research and Improvement. 2002;62:409–424. [Google Scholar]
  37. Metcalfe J, Mischel W. A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review. 1999;106:3–19. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, Caspi A, et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Peake PK. The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54:687–696. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. How to use a Monte Carlo study to decide on sample size and determine power. Structural Equation Modeling. 2002;9:599–620. doi: 10.1207/s15328007sem0904_8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Needle RH, Su S, Doherty WJ. Divorce, remarriage and adolescent substance use: A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1990;52:157–169. doi: 10.2307/352847. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Newcomb MD, Harlow LL. Life events and substance use among adolescents: Mediating effects of perceived loss of control and meaninglessness in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51:564–577. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Park D, Gunderson EA, Tsukayama E, Levine SC, Beilock SL. Young children’s motivational frameworks and math achievement: Relation to teacher-reported instructional practices, but not teacher theory of intelligence. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2016;108:300–313. doi: 10.1037/edu0000064. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Park D, Tsukayama E, Goodwin GP, Patrick S, Duckworth AL. A tripartite taxonomy of character: Evidence for intrapersonal, interpersonal, and intellectual competencies in children. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.08.001. Advance online publication . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  45. Peters CLO, Enders C. A primer for the estimation of structural equation models in the presence of missing data: Maximum likelihood algorithms. Journal of Targeting, Measurement and Analysis for Marketing. 2002;11:81–95. doi: 10.1057/palgrave.jt.5740069. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Pomerantz EM, Kempner SG. Mothers’ daily person and process praise: Implications for children’s theory of intelligence and motivation. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:2040–2046. doi: 10.1037/a0031840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Porche MV, Fortuna LR, Lin J, Alegria M. Childhood trauma events and psychiatric disorders as correlates of school dropout in a national sample of young adults. Child Development. 2011;82:982–998. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Raftery AE. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology. 1995;25:111–163. doi: 10.2307/271063. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Schachter S, Singer J. Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review. 1962;69:379–399. doi: 10.1037/h0046234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Selye H. Stress and distress. Comprehensive Therapy. 1975;1:9–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Shoda Y, Mischel W, Peake PK. Predicting adolescent cognitive and social competence from preschool delay of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology. 1990;26:978–986. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.26.6.978. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Steinberg L. Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives from brain and behavioral science. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16:55–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00475.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Steinberg L. A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review. 2008;28:78–106. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality. 2004;72:271–322. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Tsukayama E, Duckworth AL, Kim B. Domain-specific impulsivity in school-age children. Developmental Science. 2013;16:879–893. doi: 10.111/desc.12067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Walton GM. The new science of wise psychological interventions. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2014;23:73–82. [Google Scholar]
  57. Wilkie S. The traumatic effects of wife-beating: an ecological perspective. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A. Humanities & Social Sciences. 2001;62:2251. [Google Scholar]
  58. Yeager DS, Trzesniewski K, Dweck CS. An implicit theories of personality intervention reduces adolescent aggression in response to victimization and exclusion. Child Development. 2013;84:970–988. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Yeager DS, Trzesniewski K, Tirri K, Nokelainen P, Dweck CS. Adolescents’ implicit theories predict desire for vengeance after remembered and hypothetical peer conflicts: Correlational and experimental evidence. Developmental Psychology. 2011;47:1090–1107. doi: 10.1037/a0023769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Zeidner M, Hammer A. Life events and coping resources as predictors of stress symptoms in adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences. 1990;11:693–703. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(90)90254-O. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp info

RESOURCES