Abstract
The brain structural correlates of cognitive and psychopathological symptoms within the active phase in severely psychotic schizophrenic inpatients have been rarely investigated. Twenty-eight inpatients with a DSM-5 diagnosis of Schizophrenia (SZ), admitted for acute psychotic decompensation, were assessed through a comprehensive neuropsychological and psychopathological battery. All patients underwent a high-resolution T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging investigation.
Increased psychotic severity was related to reduced grey matter volumes in the medial portion of the right superior frontal cortex, the superior orbitofrontal cortex bilaterally and to white matter volume reduction in the medial portion of the left superior frontal area. Immediate verbal memory performance was related to left insula and inferior parietal cortex volume, while long-term visuo-spatial memory was related to grey matter volume of the right middle temporal cortex, and the right (lobule VII, CRUS1) and left (lobule VI) cerebellum. Moreover, psychotic severity correlated with cognitive inflexibility and negative symptom severity was related to visuo-spatial processing and reasoning disturbances.
These findings indicate that a disruption of the cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuit, and distorted memory function contribute to the development and maintenance of psychotic exacerbation.
Keywords: Psychotic exacerbation, Schizophrenia inpatients, Brain morphometry, Frontal cortex, Temporal cortex, Cerebellum, Cognition
1. Introduction
The majority of patients with schizophrenia (SZ) experience multiple relapses during the course of the illness (Emsley et al., 2013; Robinson et al., 1999) with up to 40% suffering a relapse within a year after being hospitalized (Hogarty and Ulrich, 1998) even under treatment (Gelder et al., 2000). Relapse rates vary from 50% to 92% (Suzuki et al., 2003) and are similar worldwide. Relapse, characterized by acute psychotic exacerbation, may have serious implications, such as progressive deterioration in social and interpersonal functioning (Kane, 2007) and is associated with loss of tissue (Andreasen et al., 2013) either in total cerebral volume and specific subregions (e.g., frontal lobe). Notably, grey matter (GM) density decrease is specifically related to relapse duration and treatment intensity, but unrelated to number of relapses, thus suggesting that acute psychotic exacerbation itself may exert a “toxic” effect on the brain (Andreasen et al., 2013). However, the loss of brain tissue over time in specific brain regions, and the positive correlation with number of hospitalizations during the scan interval observed in other longitudinal studies (van Haren et al., 2007) may suggest a progressive, not static nature of brain abnormalities in SZ. Nevertheless, it has been suggested that relapse may lead to brain tissue loss via abnormalities in the glutamatergic and/or dopaminergic systems (for a recent review see Landek-Salgado et al., 2016). In a recent elaboration of the dopamine hypothesis, it has been pointed out that dopaminergic hyperfunction, likely due to an increase in presynaptic dopamine synthesis, is associated with psychotic exacerbation (Howes et al., 2012; Howes and Kapur, 2009). Indeed, elevated dopamine synthesis has been detected in patients acutely psychotic at the time of investigation (Howes and Kapur, 2009) and associated with poor performance on cognitive tasks (Howes et al., 2009).
Past studies focused exclusively on the relationship between the number and global duration of relapses and brain structural damage, or between the severity of acute psychopathological symptoms and cognitive dysfunctions. Unfortunately, no data exist on the relationship between severity of psychopathology and severity of brain structural damage in SZ during the active phase.
Moreover, the interaction between cognition and psychopathology has been studied following different methodological designs. A recently favoured approach for characterizing the psychopathological symptoms of SZ proposes quantitative dimensions to investigate sources of heterogeneity between SZ patients. Indeed, factor analyses have consistently demonstrated that the individual psychopathological symptoms can be grouped, and that they may be better accounted for by three dimensions: psychotic, negative, and disorganized (Andreasen et al., 1995a; Grube et al., 1998). Therefore, assuming that psychopathological dimensions have neurobiological constructs (Andreasen et al., 1995a; Flaum et al., 1995; Koutsouleris et al., 2008) and that cognitive functioning represents an intermediate level between psychopathology and neurobiology (Mortimer and McKenna, 1994), experimental studies focused on different phases of the illness may provide clues about the mediating role of cognition in psychotic activation and the neurobiological mechanisms that underpin such relationship.
To the best of our knowledge, although relapses in SZ represent a crucial part of the illness, they have been scarcely investigated, probably as a consequence of patients' scarce collaboration and hospitalization context, often unsuitable for clinical research. The Italian law 180 (1978) provided that psychiatric patients could be admitted only to hospital psychiatric wards (SPDCs, psychiatric services for diagnosis and treatment), and no other admission (i.e. to psychiatric asylums, which were in fact abolished) shall be allowed. SPDCs are the perfect setting for studying psychotic relapses considering that SZ patients are admitted mainly during the active phase of the illness for their psychotic exacerbation.
The main goal of the present study was to identify the relationships among SZ phenotypic traits (psychopathology/cognition) and their brain structural correlates during psychotic exacerbation. We therefore examined the associations between brain grey/white matter volume, the three symptom dimensions and neurocognitive performance in severely psychotic SZ inpatients. We hypothesized that, similarly to what is observed in the stable phase of the illness (Flaum et al., 1995; Padmanabhan et al., 2014), different symptom dimensions would relate to selective patterns of structural brain alterations and to specific impairments in cognition. Considering that our sample was composed of severely psychotic SZ, the strongest correlations were expected between the psychotic dimension and the cognitive/brain structural markers of SZ.
More specifically, we expected to find an association between severity of psychotic symptoms and regional brain volume within the cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuit (CSCC), specifically in frontal lobe regions, previously described as functionally impaired (Andreasen, 1999; Barch, 2014; Spoletini et al., 2009), and potentially underlying symptoms such as hallucinations and delusions (Andreasen et al., 1998; Sheffield and Barch, 2016). Moreover, based on recent cognitive models which highlight the importance of cognition in the development and maintenance of psychoses (Garety et al., 2013; Keefe et al., 2011; Krishnan et al., 2011), we predicted that among severely psychotic inpatients, those with more severe symptoms would exhibit a greater cognitive alteration and a more pronounced brain volume reduction in the underlying neural circuitry.
2. Material and methods
2.1. Participants
Patients with a diagnosis of SZ were recruited from the SPDC of San Filippo Neri Hospital in Rome, center Italy. All patients were admitted in the ward for a psychotic exacerbation between November 2013 and February 2015 and were in the active phase of SZ according to DSM-5 (APA, 2013) criteria. They all suffered from clinically significant psychotic symptoms, as defined by a score greater than or equal to 4 in the Hallucination and/or Delusions subscales on the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). All potentially eligible patients were approached between 24 and 48 h of admission. Diagnoses were confirmed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID)-Clinician Edition (First et al., 2016). No subject had a Major Neurocognitive Disorder according to DSM-5 criteria. Other inclusion criteria were: (a) age between 18 and 65 years; (b) at least 5 years of education; (c) no global cognitive deterioration defined by a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975) score lower than 25, consistent with normative data in the Italian population (Measso et al., 1993); and (d) suitability for magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan. Exclusion criteria included: a) substance abuse or dependence according to DSM-5 criteria; b) history of neurologic illness or brain injury with loss of consciousness; c) major medical illness (e.g. any clinically significant and unstable blood, renal, gastrointestinal, endocrine or cardiovascular system disorder); d) any potential brain abnormality or vascular lesion as apparent on conventional FLAIR-scans (Iorio et al., 2013).
From the initial 54 inpatients recruited, 16 were excluded (12 for a score < 4 in the hallucinations and/or delusions SAPS and 4 for neuroimaging artefacts or brain lesions) and 10 did not complete the neuropsychological and/or psychopathological assessment. Thus, the final sample consisted of 28 inpatients (64.3% men). Pharmacological therapy was registered for each patient at the time of enrollment. All patients were receiving one or more antipsychotics. The mean (±SD) dosage in olanzapine equivalents (Gardner et al., 2010) on the day of evaluation was 22 ± 13 mg/day. Clinical history was collected from the clinicians in charge of patients' psychiatric care, and potentially consolidated by patients' relatives. All patients gave written informed consent to participate after study procedures were explained. The Santa Lucia Foundation ethical review board approved the study protocol.
2.2. Psychopathological and neuropsychological assessment
After the preliminary diagnosis of SZ (GD), a research psychiatrist (GS) confirmed all diagnoses using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID) - Clinician Edition (First et al., 2016). Psychopathological assessment included the SAPS (Andreasen, 1984) and the Scale for the assessment of negative symptoms (SANS) (Andreasen, 1989).
Then participants completed a comprehensive neuropsychological battery performed by trained research neuropsychologists. Individual measures included: the Trail-Making Test (TMT) (Reitan, 1992) to evaluate the speed of information processing (TMT-A) and set-switching abilities (TMT-B); the Controlled Word Fluency Test (WFT) from the Mental Deterioration Battery (MDB) (Carlesimo et al., 1996) and the Semantic Fluency Test (SFT) (Lucas et al., 1998) to assess the phonological and semantic processes central to word production; the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)- short form (Greve, 2001) to evaluate executive processes and in particular the set-shifting ability; the Rey's 15 word Immediate (RIR) and Delayed Recall (RDR) tests from the MDB (Carlesimo et al., 1996) to assess subjects' declarative verbal memory; the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCFT) (Osterrieth, 1944) - delayed recall to measure visuo-spatial memory; the ROCFT – immediate copy (Osterrieth, 1944) to evaluate visuo-constructive abilities; and the Raven's Progressive Matrices' 47 (PM47) to assess logical reasoning on non-verbal stimuli.
Before the beginning of the study, interviewers were trained by didactic instruction, live interviews, and a review of diagnostic rating. They were trained until they demonstrated an inter-rater reliability of at least 0.80 (K coefficient).
2.3. Psychopathological data processing
On the basis of previous evidence by the Andreasen's group (Andreasen et al., 1995a), we computed three summary dimension measures (psychotic, negative and disorganized) for each patient. Weighted means of each summary measure were calculated such that each global score contributed equally to the final mean. The three dimensional model (Andreasen et al., 1995a) here adopted was chosen on the basis of previous studies suggesting a good reliability for correlating neuropsychological and psychopathological scores. Moreover, the dimensional approach received strong support from studies showing that each dimension has distinct cognitive and neural substrates (Andreasen et al., 1995a; Cuesta and Peralta, 1995; Koutsouleris et al., 2008; Norman et al., 1997).
2.4. Image acquisition and processing
All participants underwent the same imaging protocol, which included 3D T1-weighted, T2-weighted and FLAIR sequences, using a 1.5 T Philips Achieva (Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands) MR imager with a standard quadrature head coil. Whole-brain T1-weighted images were obtained in the sagittal plane (TE/TR = 3.4/7.5 ms, flip angle 8°, voxel size 1 × 1 × 1 mm3).
T1-weighted images were processed and examined by using the SPM8 software (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience Group, London, UK; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) (see Supplementary materials for a detailed description of image data processing).
The segmented, normalized, modulated and smoothed GM and WM images were used for analyses.
2.5. Statistical analysis
2.5.1. Demographic and clinical data
Statistical analyses were performed using the Statview Software v5.0.1 (SAS Institute). Descriptive statistics were computed as mean and frequencies. Correlation coefficients (Pearson r) and Fisher's r to z transformation were calculated to assess the relationship between antipsychotic drug dosage (in olanzapine equivalents) and performance in neuropsychological tests, and between scores of psychopathological dimensions and neuropsychological tests. Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (p < 0.004, p = 0.05/11(number of comparisons)) was applied to analyses.
2.5.2. Image analyses: Relationship with cognitive performance and symptom dimensions
To identify brain regions in which patients showed GM and WM volumetric correlates of phenotypical traits (i.e. psychopathological symptom dimensions and cognitive test scores), a number of multiple regression models was adopted using each behavioral score as regressor.
To avoid type I errors (i.e., accepting false positives) statistical significance (p < 0.05) was corrected using the Random Fields Theory Family-wise error (FWE), which controls for the possibility of any false positive across the entire volume (Ashburner and Friston, 2005). Further, results were considered statistically significant if they were part of a spatially contiguous cluster size of 30 voxels or greater.
As ancillary preliminary findings we considered also results reaching significance at p < 0.0001 uncorrected for multiple comparisons.
3. Results
Patients' mean age was 39.75 (SD ± 12.86), the mean years of education was 12.57 (SD ± 2.97). The mean age at onset of psychotic symptoms was 23.75 (SD ± 8.53) and the mean duration of illness was 16.21 (SD ± 12.57) years. Regression analyses did not show any significant relationship between antipsychotic medication dosage and cognitive/psychopathology performance. As expected, only a small correlation was found with the psychotic dimension, which was nonsignificant after correction for multiple comparisons.
3.1. Cognitive functioning and psychopathological dimensions
Correlations between scores of the three psychopathological dimensions and cognitive functions are reported in Table 1.
Table 1.
Correlations (Pearson's r) between the three symptom dimensions and neuropsychological performances in severely psychotic schizophrenic inpatients.
Characteristics | Negative dimension | Psychotic dimension | Disorganized dimension |
---|---|---|---|
RIR | −0.321 | −0.322 | −0.124 |
RDR | −0.371 | −0.212 | −0.090 |
PM47 | −0.549⁎ | 0.04 | −0.089 |
TMT-A time (sec) | 0.405# | 0.347 | 0.142 |
TMT-B time (sec) | 0.397# | 0.439# | −0.137 |
WFT | −0.388# | −0.368 | −0.210 |
SFT | −0.393# | 0.114 | −0.117 |
WCST - category | −0.233 | −0.192 | 0.254 |
WCST - perseverative errors | 0.221 | 0.533⁎ | −0.224 |
WCST - non perseverative errors | 0.171 | 0.297 | −0.273 |
ROCFT - immediate copy | −0.536⁎ | −0.058 | −0.020 |
ROCFT - delay recall | −0.359 | −0.342 | −0.151 |
Abbreviations: RIR, Rey's 15 word Immediate Recall; RDR, Rey's 15 word Delayed Recall; PM47, Raven's Progressive Matrices' 47; TMT-A, Trail-Making Test-part A; TMT-B, Trail-Making Test-part B; WFT, Controlled Word Fluency Test; SFT, Semantic Fluency Test; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test- short form, ROCFT – immediate copy, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test immediate copy; ROCFT- delayed recall, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test delayed recall.
Significant values at the uncorrected statistical level (p < 0.05).
Significant at the corrected statistical level (p < 0.004).
The relationship between the severity of negative dimension and performance on neuropsychological tests comprised several cognitive abilities. However, only two correlations survived the correction for multiple comparisons. In particular, we found that increased severity of the negative dimension was associated with decreased performance in the PM47 (r = −0.549; df = 27; p = 0.002) and ROCFT- immediate copy (r = −0.536; df = 27; p = 0.002). Concurrently, increased severity in the psychotic dimension was correlated with a greater number of perseverative errors in the WCST (r = 0.533; df = 27; p = 0.003). No significant correlations were observed between the disorganized dimension and neuropsychological test scores.
3.2. Brain volumes and the three dimensional model of psychopathology
Only negative correlations were observed between GM/WM volumes and the severity of psychotic dimension. The results are summarized in Table 2 and Fig. 1.
Table 2.
Topography of the significant relationship between grey and white matter volumetry and Psychotic Dimension in severely psychotic schizophrenic inpatients.
Anatomical region (BA) | Brain tissue | Extent (n.voxels) | P (FWE corrected) | P (uncorrected) | t | Equiv Z | Coordinates (MNI) x, y, z (mm) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Right frontal cortex (medial/superior) (8) | GM | 401 | 0.011 | <0.0001 | 6.47 | 4.95 | 8; 35; 49 |
Left frontal area (medial/superior) (8) | WM | 46 | 0.057⁎ | <0.0001 | 5.49 | 4.44 | −10; 32; 51 |
Right orbitofrontal cortex (superior) (11) | GM | 750 | 0.062⁎ | <0.0001 | 5.66 | 4.53 | 18; 21; −15 |
Left orbitofrontal cortex (superior) (11) | GM | 743 | 0.036 | <0.0001 | 5.93 | 4.67 | −12; 12; −24 |
Abbreviations: GM, grey matter; WM, white matter; BA, Brodmann's area; FWE, Family-wise error; Coordinates are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) Space.
Approaching significance (p < 0.0001 uncorrected).
Fig. 1.
Grey and white matter volumes related to psychopathology in severely psychotic schizophrenic inpatients.
The left side of each panel depicts representative axial slices showing 4 clusters in which lower GM and WM volumes were associated with higher level of Psychotic Dimension scores. The right side reports scatterplots depicting the respective correlation. Yellow circles indicate individual patients' values. Coordinates (z values) are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space. Linear fits are shown in dotted lines.
Abbreviations: L - Left Hemisphere; R - Right Hemisphere. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)
In particular, when focusing on GM we found that higher severity of psychotic dimension was significantly associated (FWE-corrected) to lower volume of the medial portion of the right superior frontal cortex- msFC- (Brodmann Area -BA- 8) and the left superior orbitofrontal cortex- SOFC- (BA 11). At a more lenient statistical threshold (p < 0.0001 uncorrected for multiple comparisons), we also found a WM area, where higher severity of psychotic dimension was associated to lower volume in the left medial part of the superior frontal area (BA 8), and a GM area where higher severity of psychotic dimension was associated to lower volume in the SOFC (BA 11) of the right hemisphere. No significant correlations were observed between GM/WM volumes and severity of negative or disorganized dimensions.
3.3. Brain volume and neuropsychological performance
Fig. 2 shows that significant positive correlations were present between verbal and visual tests measuring memory performance and volume in different GM areas.
Fig. 2.
Grey matter volumes related to cognitive performance in severely psychotic schizophrenic.
Left panels depict representative brain axial slices showing 5 clusters in which lower GM volume was associated with decreased performance on memory tests. Right panels show scatter graphs depicting correlations between mean GM volumes of the 5 clusters and performance on the Rey's 15 words - Immediate Recall (upper panel) and the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure – Delayed Recall (lower panel). Yellow circles indicate individual patients' values.
Coordinates (z values) are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space. Linear fits are shown in dotted lines.
Abbreviations: L - Left Hemisphere; R - Right Hemisphere. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)
In fact, Table 3 shows that RIR score was related to volume of areas located in the left inferior parietal cortex (BA 40) and left insular cortex (BA 48). In addition, ROCFT– delayed recall score was related to volume of areas located in the lobule VIIb and CRUS 1 in the right cerebellum (BA 18), in lobule VI in the left cerebellum (BA 18) and in the right middle temporal cortex (BA 22). In all cases, decreased performance in the neuropsychological tests was related to decreased GM volume.
Table 3.
Topography of the significant relationship between gray matter volumetry and Neuropsychological test scores in severely psychotic schizophrenic inpatients.
NPS Test | Anatomical region (BA) | Extent (n.voxels) | P (FWE corrected) | P (uncorrected) | t | Equiv Z | Coordinates (MNI) x, y, z (mm) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
RIR | Left inferior parietal cortex (40) | 437 | 0.002 | <0.0001 | 7.32 | 5.35 | −51; −45; 49 |
Left insula (48) | 131 | 0.022 | <0.0001 | 6.16 | 4.79 | −44; 6; 6 | |
ROCFT – delayed recall | Right middle temporal cortex (22) | 166 | 0.003 | <0.0001 | 7.15 | 5.27 | 63; −52; 18 |
Left cerebellum – lobule 6 (18) | 1042 | 0.019 | <0.0001 | 6.23 | 4.83 | −9; −79; −17 | |
Right cerebellum – lobule VIIb (18) | 2321 | 0.017 | <0.0001 | 6.27 | 4.85 | 45; −63; −56 | |
Right cerebellum – CRUS 1 (18) | 1280 | 0.011 | <0.0001 | 6.49 | 4.96 | 27; −63; −36 |
Abbreviations: RIR, Rey's 15 word Test-Immediate recall; ROCFT- delayed recall, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test delayed recall; NPS, neuropsychological.
BA, Brodmann's area; FWE, Family-wise error; Coordinates are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) Space.
No significant correlations were present between WM volumetric measures and neuropsychological performance.
4. Discussion
We investigated the brain structural correlates of clinical core symptoms (psychopathology/cognition) in hospitalized patients with SZ undergoing a severe psychotic exacerbation. A first finding is that bilateral brain tissue reduction in frontal lobe regions subtended the severity of psychotic dimension. Specifically, patients with severe psychotic symptoms showed decreased GM volume bilaterally in the SOFC and in the right msFC as well as reduced WM volume in the medial portion of the left superior frontal area. This is in accordance with evidence of longitudinal studies (Andreasen et al., 2013; van Haren et al., 2007) showing that frontal brain atrophy is related to the number and duration of relapses and that the psychotic dimension measured in the stable phase is associated to frontal tissue loss over time. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first evidence of reduced brain volume in fronto-orbitofrontal regions directly related to the severity of psychotic symptoms in the active phase of SZ, with no relationship with the negative and the disorganized dimensions. Conversely, previous studies in the stable phase of the illness demonstrated that atrophy of frontal regions is associated with the severity of negative symptoms (Collin et al., 2012; Koutsouleris et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2003; Ziauddeen et al., 2011). However, the finding is not completely unexpected since the neural and cognitive mechanisms underlying symptom expression vary between the active and the stable phases of the illness, while positive symptoms (delusions) are associated with reduced cognitive flexibility (and an hypothesized frontal lobe dysfunction), predominantly in the active phase (Guillem et al., 2005). Moreover, since a dopaminergic dysfunction is linked to psychotic activation (Howes et al., 2015; Howes and Kapur, 2009) and given that the frontal lobe receives strong projections from dopaminergic neurons in the midbrain (Goldman-Rakic et al., 1992), our findings confirm the possible dopaminergic pathogenic mechanism responsible for positive symptoms in the active phase. The lack of brain volumetric correlate of the negative dimension reinforces the construct of the deficit schizophrenia (Kirkpatrick and Galderisi, 2008). Indeed, clinical and neurobiological correlates of the secondary negative symptoms occurring during active psychotic exacerbations may differ from that of enduring negative symptoms in the stable phase (Tandon et al., 2000).
It is interesting to note that we did not find any association between the disorganized dimension and cognitive performance or brain morphometric measures. The vast majority of previous studies in remitted patients found a significant relationship between disorganization and brain morphometry (Collin et al., 2012; Koutsouleris et al., 2008) and cognitive performance (Guillem et al., 2002, Guillem et al., 2005; O'Leary et al., 2000), while only one study (Klingberg et al., 2006) concluded that disorganization and cognitive impairment represent unrelated dimensions. This is in line with the concept of different neurobiological correlates of clinical phenotypes over the stable and active phases of SZ.
It has been hypothesized that the abnormalities in frontal regions consistently observed in SZ (see Mubarik and colleagues for a review) (Mubarik et al., 2016) could impact cognitive processes such as sensory integration, reward mechanisms and executive functioning (Antonova et al., 2004; Barch and Ceaser, 2012; Bechara, 2004; Happaney et al., 2004), thus contributing to positive symptoms. Our findings concerning the relationship between the psychotic dimension and performance in frontal tasks are in line with this evidence, showing increased difficulties in changing a cognitive strategy and increased perseveration as the severity of psychotic symptoms get higher (Liddle and Morris, 1991; Perry and Braff, 1998). In fact, difficulties in exerting and maintaining cognitive inhibition appear to be linked to several clinical symptoms, which are the core of the psychotic activation, such as hallucinations and delusions (Gray et al., 1991). In addition, the observed relationship between increased negative symptoms and poorer visuo-spatial processing and reasoning capacities during a severe psychotic exacerbation, may reflect the difficulty to detect, distinguish and interpret the external stimuli. Indeed, SZ subjects exhibit impaired performance in a wide range of visuo-spatial functions, from the most basic level of visual perception to more complex processes and navigation abilities (Butler et al., 2005; Doniger et al., 2001). Such deficits in perceptual organization abilities and in manipulating visuo-spatial information can negatively affect various daily activities and directly contribute to the lack of interest in the environment, apathy and social withdrawal (Doniger et al., 2001), thus explaining the observed correlation between severity of the negative dimension and visuo-perceptual processing (O'Leary et al., 2000). As previous findings (Cuesta and Peralta, 1995; Guillem et al., 2002) suggested that deluded subjects are probably less willing to gather and encode new environmental information, the correlation between negative symptoms during psychotic activation and poor visuo-spatial abilities may be justified by a reduction of exploratory and processing activity of novel stimuli (Guillem et al., 2002).
A second endpoint of our study is the pattern of neural correlates of neuropsychological performance in severely psychotic SZ. Our results clearly indicate that brain correlates of cognitive impairment are different from those of psychotic exacerbation. In particular, poor performance in immediate verbal memory was associated with decreased volume of the left inferior parietal and insular cortices, known to be associated with memory processes (Hashimoto et al., 2010; Yildiz et al., 2011). Congruently with previous findings (Leiderman and Strejilevich, 2004; Yeganeh-Doost et al., 2011), we also found that reduced visuo-spatial memory performance was related to widespread GM atrophy in temporo-cerebellar regions. Apart from the well-known role of the temporal lobe in memory processes (Karnik-Henry et al., 2012), the crucial cerebellar contribution to the reinforcement and supervision of learning (Doya, 2000; Swain et al., 2011) and to visuo-spatial and executive processes (Yeganeh-Doost et al., 2011) has been increasingly established. Since either region is encompassed in the cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuit referred as dysfunctional in SZ (Andreasen, 1999; Barch, 2014), our results are in line with previous studies indicating that the dysfunctional memory circuitry in SZ is characterized by abnormalities within large-scale brain networks (Andreasen et al., 1995b; Kraguljac et al., 2013).
Overall, these findings support recent cognitive models of psychoses (Keefe et al., 2011; Keefe and Kraus, 2009; Kraus et al., 2009) in which memory processes seem to have a special implication. Specifically, according to the memory-prediction model of SZ (Keefe et al., 2011; Keefe and Kraus, 2009; Kraus et al., 2009), primary memory deficits, and the associated altered neural circuitry, disrupt mnemonic processes, thus leading to alterations in learning-dependent predictive perception that present as reality distortion (delusions, thought disorder and hallucinations). Hence, the observed correlation between memory functions (and not other cognitive processes here investigated) and GM volume is coherent with the role attributed to memory alterations in psychotic activation since deficits in brain structure and function, particularly in cortical hubs (including the parietal, temporal and cerebellar cortices), may contribute to the development and maintenance of psychotic exacerbation (Krishnan et al., 2011).
Before reaching the concluding remarks, some potential limitations of the present study should be mentioned. First, we do acknowledge that our conclusions were mainly drawn on the basis of a cross-sectional approach in which, due to lack of a longitudinal perspective, the causal relationship between brain volume, neurocognitive functioning and acute symptoms might be blurred by inter-individual variability. Second, although a small sample size is not uncommon in this vein of research (Guillem et al., 2005), a larger sample would have provided a better understanding of the relationships between the investigated variables, allowing a more assertive interpretation of the findings and generalization beyond the present sample. Nevertheless, by admitting patients with no dementia or gross brain pathology as instantiated by standard MRI, we excluded the confounding factor of potential neurological disorders, which would likely have additive effects on brain structure beyond those of SZ itself. Moreover, the chosen cut-off for the SAPS severity score (i.e. ≥4, severe psychotic symptoms) allowed us to examine only severely psychotic SZ and to purposely investigate the neural and neuropsychological correlates of psychotic exacerbation. Actually, our results are at variance with larger studies outcomes (Keefe et al., 2006) in which neurocognitive deficits were modestly correlated with negative symptom severity, while correlations with positive symptom severity were close to zero. However, heterogeneity within the large sample in such ‘all-comer’ clinical trial might have obscured relationships that exist within a more homogenous sample like the one here investigated. Indeed, the high inter-individual variability with respect to treatment histories, procedures, and responses should be considered as a general limitation in naturalistic research, which cannot be avoided even when clustering solutions (like in the present study) are adopted.
In conclusion, the findings here exposed suggest that during severe psychotic exacerbation, although cognitive and psychopathological phenotypic markers of SZ have distinct neural correlates, the involved brain areas appear to be functionally linked as part of the cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuit (CSCC). We therefore experimentally demonstrated the pathogenic model of SZ (Andreasen et al., 1996; Wiser et al., 1998) assuming that a disruption of the CSCC underlies the combination of the vast majority of cognitive and psychotic symptoms. We also added new evidence regarding the role of frontal lobe dysfunction in psychotic exacerbation, and further demonstrated the crucial contribution of distorted memory function in the development and maintenance of psychotic exacerbation.
Future longitudinally studies (also extended to different phases of the illness assessed in the same patients) will help clarifying the causal relationship between clinical and neurobiological factors in SZ, thus contributing to improve outcome prediction and relapses prevention in schizophrenia.
Conflict of interest
All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Contributors
Gianfranco Spalletta, MD, PhD, designed the study, supervised patient recruitment, wrote the final draft of the manuscript and undertook the statistical analyses.
Nerisa Banaj, PhD, wrote the final draft of the manuscript, supervised clinical assessment, undertook the statistical analyses and contributed to the interpretation of the data.
Federica Piras, PhD, wrote the final draft of the manuscript and contributed to the interpretation of the data.
Fabrizio Piras, PhD, designed the study and undertook the statistical analyses.
Valentina Ciullo, PhD, took part at the data collection process.
Mariangela Iorio, PhD, managed neuroimaging data acquisition and processing.
Claudia Battaglia, MD, took part at the data collection process.
Donatella Pantoli, MD, coordinated neuroimaging data collection.
Giuseppe Ducci, MD, contributed to design the study and coordinated patient recruitment.
All authors have contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.
Dr. Gianfranco Spalletta has full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Role of funding source
This research has been supported by IRCCS Fondazione “Santa Lucia”. IRCCS has had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of the data; in the writing of the paper; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.
Acknowledgement
We express our gratitude to the patients who agreed to participate in this study. The authors acknowledge the valuable collaboration of Gabriella Scifoni, Chiara Cannizzaro and Sara Fraietta.
Footnotes
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2018.02.001.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary material
References
- Andreasen N. Univ. Iowa; Iowa City, IA: 1984. Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms SAPS. [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen N.C. Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) Br. J. Psychiatry. 1989;155:53–58. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen N.C. A unitary model of schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1999;56:781–787. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.9.781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen N.C., Liu D., Ziebell S., Vora A., Ho B.C. Relapse duration, treatment intensity, and brain tissue loss in schizophrenia: A prospective longitudinal MRI study. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2013;170:609–615. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12050674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen N.C., Arndt S., Alliger R., Miller D., Flaum M. Symptoms of schizophrenia. Methods, meanings, and mechanisms. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1995;52:341–351. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950170015003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen N.C., O'Leary D.S., Arndt S., Cizadlo T., Hurtig R., Rezai K., Watkins G.L., Ponto L.L., Hichwa R.D. Short-term and long-term verbal memory: a positron emission tomography study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1995;92:5111–5115. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.11.5111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen N.C., O'Leary D.S., Cizadlo T., Arndt S., Rezai K., Ponto L.L., Watkins G.L., Hichwa R.D. Schizophrenia and cognitive dysmetria: a positron-emission tomography study of dysfunctional prefrontal-thalamic-cerebellar circuitry. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1996;93:9985–9990. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.18.9985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen N.C., Paradiso S., O'Leary D.S. “Cognitive dysmetria” as an integrative theory of schizophrenia: a dysfunction in cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuitry? Schizophr. Bull. 1998;24:203–218. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antonova E., Sharma T., Morris R., Kumari V. The relationship between brain structure and neurocognition in schizophrenia: A selective review. Schizophr. Res. 2004;70:117–145. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- APA DSM 5. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2013 [Google Scholar]
- Ashburner J., Friston K.J. Unified segmentation. NeuroImage. 2005;26:839–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barch D.M. Cerebellar-thalamic connectivity in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2014;40:1200–1203. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barch D.M., Ceaser A. Cognition in schizophrenia: core psychological and neural mechanisms. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012;16:27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A. The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain Cogn. 2004 doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2003.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler P.D., Zemon V., Schechter I., Saperstein A.M., Hoptman M.J., Lim K.O., Revheim N., Silipo G., Javitt D.C. Early-stage visual processing and cortical amplification deficits in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2005;62:495. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlesimo G.A., Caltagirone C., Gainotti G. The Mental Deterioration Battery: normative data, diagnostic reliability and qualitative analyses of cognitive impairment. The Group for the Standardization of the Mental Deterioration Battery. Eur. Neurol. 1996;36:378–384. doi: 10.1159/000117297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collin G., Derks E.M., van Haren N.E.M., Schnack H.G., Pol H.E.H., Kahn R.S., Cahn W. Symptom dimensions are associated with progressive brain volume changes in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2012;138:171–176. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.03.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuesta M.J., Peralta V. Cognitive disorders in the positive, negative, and disorganization syndromes of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 1995;58:227–235. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(95)02712-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doniger G.M., Silipo G., Rabinowicz E.F., Snodgrass J.G., Javitt D.C. Impaired sensory processing as a basis for object-recognition deficits in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2001;158:1818–1826. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doya K. Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor control. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2000 doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00153-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emsley R., Chiliza B., Asmal L., Harvey B.H. The nature of relapse in schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatry. 2013;13 doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-13-50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First M., Williams J., Karg R., Spitzer R. American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2016. SCID-5-CV: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Disorders, Clinician Version. [Google Scholar]
- Flaum M., O'Leary D.S., Swayze V.W., Miller D.D., Arndt S., Andreasen N.C. Symptom dimensions and brain morphology in schizophrenia and related psychotic disorders. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1995;29:261–276. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(94)00046-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein M.F., Folstein S.E., McHugh P.R. Mini-Mental State: A practical method for grading the state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner D.M., Murphy A.L., O'Donnell H., Centorrino F., Baldessarini R.J. International consensus study of antipsychotic dosing. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2010;167:686–693. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09060802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garety P., Joyce E., Jolley S., Emsley R., Waller H., Kuipers E., Bebbington P., Fowler D., Dunn G., Freeman D. Neuropsychological functioning and jumping to conclusions in delusions. Schizophr. Res. 2013;150:570–574. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.08.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gelder M.G., Lopez-Ibor J.J., Andreasen N.C. Oxford Univ. Press; Oxford: 2000. New Oxford Textbook of Psychiatry; pp. 567–621. [Google Scholar]
- Goldman-Rakic P.S., Lidow M.S., Smiley J.F., Williams M.S. The anatomy of dopamine in monkey and human prefrontal cortex. J. Neural Transm. Suppl. 1992;36:163–177. doi: 10.1007/978-3-7091-9211-5_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray J., Feldon J., Rawlins J., Hemsley D. The neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Behav. Brain Sci. 1991;14:1–84. [Google Scholar]
- Greve K.W. The WCST-64: a standardized short-form of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2001;15:228–234. doi: 10.1076/clin.15.2.228.1901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grube B.S., Bilder R.M., Goldman R.S. Meta-analysis of symptom factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 1998;31:113–120. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(98)00011-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillem F., Bicu M., Semkovska M., Debruille J.B. The dimensional symptom structure of schizophrenia and its association with temperament and character. Schizophr. Res. 2002;56:137–147. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00257-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillem F., Ganeva E., Pampoulova T., Stip E., Lalonde P., Sasseville M. Changes in the neuropsychological correlates of clinical dimensions between the acute and stable phase of schizophrenia. Brain Cogn. 2005;57:93–101. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Happaney K., Zelazo P.D., Stuss D.T. Development of orbitofrontal function: current themes and future directions. Brain Cogn. 2004 doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Haren N.E., Hulshoff Pol H.E., Schnack H.G., Cahn W., Mandl R.C., Collins D.L., Evans A.C., Kahn R.S. Focal gray matter changes in schizophrenia across the course of the illness: a 5-year follow-up study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:2057–2066. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hashimoto R., Lee K., Preus A., McCarley R.W., Wible C.G. An fMRI study of functional abnormalities in the verbal working memory system and the relationship to clinical symptoms in chronic schizophrenia. Cereb. Cortex. 2010;20:46–60. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogarty G., Ulrich R. The limitations of antipsychotic medication on schizophrenia relapse and adjustment and the contributions of psychosocial treatment. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1998;32:243–250. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(97)00013-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howes O.D., Kapur S. The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: version III-the final common pathway. Schizophr. Bull. 2009 doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howes O.D., Montgomery A.J., Asselin M.-C., Murray R.M., Valli I., Tabraham P., Bramon-Bosch E., Valmaggia L., Johns L., Broome M., McGuire P.K., Grasby P.M. Elevated striatal dopamine function linked to prodromal signs of schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2009;66:13. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howes O.D., Kambeitz J., Kim E., Stahl D., Slifstein M., Abi-Dargham A., Kapur S. The nature of dopamine dysfunction in schizophrenia and what this means for treatment. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2012;69:776–786. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howes O., Mccutcheon R., Stone J. 2015. Glutamate and Dopamine in Schizophrenia: An Update for the 21st Century. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iorio M., Spalletta G., Chiapponi C., Luccichenti G., Cacciari C., Orfei M.D., Caltagirone C., Piras F. White matter hyperintensities segmentation: A new semi-automated method. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2013;5 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane J. Treatment strategies to prevent relapse and encourage remission. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2007;68(Suppl. 14):27–30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karnik-Henry M.S., Wang L., Barch D.M., Harms M.P., Campanella C., Csernansky J.G. Medial temporal lobe structure and cognition in individuals with schizophrenia and in their non-psychotic siblings. Schizophr. Res. 2012;138:128–135. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keefe R.S.E., Kraus M.S. Measuring memory-prediction errors and their consequences in youth at risk for schizophrenia. Ann. Acad. Med. Singapore. 2009;38:414–419. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keefe R.S.E., Bilder R.M., Harvey P.D., Davis S.M., Palmer B.W., Gold J.M., Meltzer H.Y., Green M.F., Miller D.D., Canive J.M., Adler L.W., Manschreck T.C., Swartz M., Rosenheck R., Perkins D.O., Walker T.M., Stroup T.S., McEvoy J.P., Lieberman J.A. Baseline neurocognitive deficits in the CATIE schizophrenia trial. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:2033–2046. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keefe R.S.E., Kraus M.S., Krishnan R.R. Failures in learning-dependent predictive perception as the key cognitive vulnerability to psychosis in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:367–368. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkpatrick B., Galderisi S. Deficit schizophrenia: an update. World Psychiatry. 2008;7:143–147. doi: 10.1002/j.2051-5545.2008.tb00181.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klingberg S., Wittorf A., Wiedemann G. Disorganization and cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: independent symptom dimensions? Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2006;256:532–540. doi: 10.1007/s00406-006-0704-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koutsouleris N., Gaser C., Jäger M., Bottlender R., Frodl T., Holzinger, Schmitt G.E., Zetzsche T., Burgermeister B., Scheuerecker J., Born C., Reiser M., Möller H., Meisenzahl E. Structural correlates of psychopathological symptom dimensions in schizophrenia: a voxel-based morphometric study. NeuroImage. 2008;39:1600–1612. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraguljac N., Srivastava A., Lahti A. Memory deficits in schizophrenia: a selective review of functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) studies. Behav. Sci. 2013;3:330–347. doi: 10.3390/bs3030330. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs3030330 Basel. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraus M., Keefe R., Krishnan R. Memory-prediction errors and their consequences in schizophrenia. Neuropsychol Rev. 2009;19:336–352. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9106-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krishnan R.R., Kraus M.S., Keefe R.S.E. Comprehensive model of how reality distortion and symptoms occur in schizophrenia: Could impairment in learning-dependent predictive perception account for the manifestations of schizophrenia? Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2011;65:305–317. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2011.02203.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landek-Salgado M.A., Faust T.E., Sawa A. Molecular substrates of schizophrenia: homeostatic signaling to connectivity. Mol. Psychiatry. 2016;21:10–28. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leiderman E.A., Strejilevich S.A. Visuospatial deficits in schizophrenia: Central executive and memory subsystems impairments. Schizophr. Res. 2004;68:217–223. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00215-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liddle P.F., Morris D.L. Schizophrenic syndromes and frontal lobe performance. Br. J. Psychiatry. 1991;158:340–345. doi: 10.1192/bjp.158.3.340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucas J. a, Ivnik R.J., Smith G.E., Bohac D.L., Tangalos E.G., Graff-Radford N.R., Petersen R.C. Mayo's older Americans normative studies: category fluency norms. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1998;20:194–200. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.2.194.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Measso G., Cavarzeran F., Zappalà G., Lebowitz B.D., Crook T.H., Pirozzolo F.J., Amaducci L.A., Massari D., Grigoletto F. The mini-mental state examination: Normative study of an Italian random sample. Dev. Neuropsychol. 1993;9:77–85. [Google Scholar]
- Mortimer A., McKenna P. Levels of explanation — symptoms, neuropsychological deficit and morphological abnormalities in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 1994;24:541–545. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700027690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mubarik A., Tohid H., Mubarik A., Tohid H. Frontal lobe alterations in schizophrenia: a review. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2016;38:198–206. doi: 10.1590/2237-6089-2015-0088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norman R.M.G., Malla A.K., Morrison-Stewart S.L., Helmes E., Williamson P.C., Thomas J., Cortese L. Neuropsychological correlates of syndromes in schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychiatry. 1997;170:134–139. doi: 10.1192/bjp.170.2.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Leary D.S., Flaum M., Kesler M.L., Flashman L. a, Arndt S., Andreasen N.C. Cognitive correlates of the negative, disorganized, and psychotic symptom dimensions of schizophrenia. J. Neuropsychiatr. Clin. Neurosci. 2000;12:4–15. doi: 10.1176/jnp.12.1.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osterrieth P.A. Filetest de copie d'une figure complex: Contribution a l'etude de la perception et de la memoire [The test of copying a complex figure: A contribution to the study of perception and memory] Arch. Psychol. 1944;30 Geneve. [Google Scholar]
- Padmanabhan J.L., Tandon N., Haller C.S., Mathew I.T., Eack S.M., Clementz B. a, Pearlson G.D., Sweeney J. a, Tamminga C.A., Keshavan M.S. Correlations between brain structure and symptom dimensions of psychosis in schizophrenia, schizoaffective, and psychotic bipolar I disorders. Schizophr. Bull. 2014;41:1–9. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry W., Braff D.L. A multimethod approach to assessing perseverations in schizophrenia patients. Schizophr. Res. 1998;33:69–77. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(98)00061-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reitan R.M. Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory; Tucson, AZ: 1992. Trail Making Test, Manual for Administration and Scoring. [Google Scholar]
- Robinson D., Woerner M.G., Alvir J.M., Bilder R., Goldman R., Geisler S., Koreen A., Sheitman B., Chakos M., Mayerhoff D., Lieberman J.A. Predictors of relapse following response from a first episode of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1999;56:241–247. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.3.241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheffield J.M., Barch D.M. Cognition and resting-state functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spoletini I., Cherubini A., Di Paola M., Banfi G., Rüsch N., Martinotti G., Bria P., Rubino I.A., Siracusano A., Caltagirone C., Spalletta G. Reduced fronto-temporal connectivity is associated with frontal gray matter density reduction and neuropsychological deficit in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2009;108:57–68. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki Y., Yasumura S., Fukao A., Otani K. Associated factors of rehospitalization among schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2003;57:555–561. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2003.01167.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swain R. a, Kerr A.L., Thompson R.F. The cerebellum: a neural system for the study of reinforcement learning. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2011;5:8. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tandon R., DeQuardo J.R., Taylor S.F., McGrath M., Jibson M., Eiser A., Goldman M. Phasic and enduring negative symptoms in schizophrenia: biological markers and relationship to outcome. Schizophr. Res. 2000;45:191–201. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00163-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang C.S., Yang Y.K., Chen M., Chiu N.T., Yeh T.L., Lee I.H. Negative symptoms and regional cerebral blood flow in patients with schizophrenia: a single photon emission computed tomography study. Kaohsiung J. Med. Sci. 2003;19:464–469. doi: 10.1016/S1607-551X(09)70492-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiser A.K., Andreasen N.C., O'Leary D.S., Watkins G.L., Boles Ponto L.L., Hichwa R.D. Dysfunctional cortico-cerebellar circuits cause “cognitive dysmetria” in schizophrenia. Neuroreport. 1998;9:1895–1899. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199806010-00042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeganeh-Doost P., Gruber O., Falkai P., Schmitt A. The role of the cerebellum in schizophrenia: from cognition to molecular pathways. Clinics. 2011;66(Suppl. 1):71–77. doi: 10.1590/S1807-59322011001300009. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1807-59322011001300009 Sao Paulo. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yildiz M., Borgwardt S.J., Berger G.E. Parietal lobes in schizophrenia: do they matter? Schizophr. Res. Treat. 2011;2011:1–15. doi: 10.1155/2011/581686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ziauddeen H., Dibben C., Kipps C., Hodges J.R., McKenna P.J. Negative schizophrenic symptoms and the frontal lobe syndrome: One and the same? Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2011;261:59–67. doi: 10.1007/s00406-010-0133-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supplementary material