Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Mar 8.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Biol. 2018 Feb 19;28(4):R160–R162. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.01.020

Cognitive Science: Persistent Apes Are Intelligent Apes

Benjamin R Eisenreich 1, Benjamin Y Hayden 1,*
PMCID: PMC5843479  NIHMSID: NIHMS946641  PMID: 29462583

Abstract

In humans, self-control is correlated with general intelligence; a new study finds that this correlation extends to chimpanzees as well. The new results highlight the cognitive bases of self-control and suggest a common evolutionary history for human and primate self-control.


Self-control is among the most difficult of cognitive processes to understand, and also to study [1]. Most of us have a strong intuition about what self-control is, but it is nonetheless difficult to define rigorously enough to study in the laboratory. The field is characterized not only by the standard empirical debates, but also by elementary definitional debates about what is and is not self-control, and whether it is a single thing or multiple distinct things [2,3]. These issues, difficult enough to approach in human studies, loom even larger in animal studies, where we cannot directly talk to our subjects [4,5]. A new study by Beran and Hopkins, reported in this issue of Current Biology [6], makes a great stride by linking self-control to general intelligence in chimpanzees.

Despite the difficulty in defining and measuring self-control, the problem is not merely philosophical. Indeed, understanding self-control is vitally important. Diminished self-control is a defining feature of many diseases, including addiction and depression, and treatments designed to improve self-control ameliorate these problems [7]. Self-control is also a central player in a wider variety of social problems, including obesity and educational disparities. As in psychiatric diseases, treatments designed to improve self-control have shown some preliminary successes (for example [8]).

These possible links between self-control and other aspects of cognition suggest that a fertile path for studying self-control is to focus on the broader links between measures of self-control and more general measures of cognitive functioning. General intelligence is linked to many important features of the mind, and as such provides a valuable entry point into much of cognition. In humans, general intelligence has been linked with the ability to delay immediate gratification in favor of larger future gain, a trait that is closely related to self-control [9]. Likewise, children’s performance in the classic marshmallow task, which involves persisting in a decision to refrain from eating a single marshmallow, has been linked with scores on general intelligence tests [10]. These links suggest that a common set of mental functions may underlie a wide variety of cognitive abilities.

The field of animal self-control is beset by a dearth of validated measures. For example, the inter-temporal choice task is perhaps the most widely used tool for studying self-control in nonhuman animals; however, foraging-inspired critics have argued that the test measures task understanding and attentional bias, psychological factors that are important, but that are not strictly self-control [11,12]. Nonetheless, understanding self-control in animals is extremely important: it is much easier for scientists to measure and manipulate brains in nonhuman animals than in humans. Moreover, studying a variety of animal species gives us a broader picture: by comparing across species, we can understand the general properties of self-control and its evolution [13].

In their new study, Beran and Hopkins [6] show that self-control in apes is associated with general intelligence. They took advantage of two heretofore unrelated, but felicitous, methodological advances. First, Hopkins and colleagues have developed a robust measure of ape intelligence; these measures are focused on the domains of physical social cognition, not elements that have any clear connection to inhibitory processing. Second, Beran and colleagues [14,15] have developed original techniques for measuring self-control in animals; this work bypasses problems with previous measures of self-control.

Unlike typical inter-temporal tasks, animals performing the Hybrid Delay Task have the option of ending the delivery of the large reward early by taking the accrued food items before the entire reward set has been delivered. This means that aspects of maintenance during the delay period can be dissociated from choice artifacts dealing with preferences for larger rewards. Indeed the drive to impulsively point to larger rewards is a confound for many types of inter-temporal choice tasks [16]. By allowing for early termination of reward accrual a critical element of self-control, persistence within a selected behavior pattern, can be directly measured.

Importantly, the strongest intelligence correlate with self-control that Beran and Hopkins [6] observed is a component that they call efficiency, which measures persistence, not self-controlled choice. This result is consistent with the idea that the ability to persist across time in the face of temptation is the key to self-control; in comparison, the ability to choose the controlled option tends to be weakly correlated, if at all, with self-control in both humans and animals. This idea is reminiscent of the observation that behavior in the Marshmallow task (a persistence task) is strongly predictive of later measures of success, but behavior in the inter-temporal choice task (which does not require persistence) is only modestly correlated. These results then provide some validation for the idea that the intertemporal choice task is a poor measure of self-control in animals, and that using tasks that require persistence in animals will be critical for an understanding of self-control [17,18].

That self-control performance and general intelligence share a relationship in both humans and primates raises interesting possibilities for gaining further insights into the evolution of intelligent behavior. It may be that selective pressures for inhibitory processes may have served as a driver for primate cognitive evolution. Such an explanation would account for the relationship between the ability of primates to withhold responding in order to gain larger rewards and intelligence. Alternatively it may be that cognitive monitoring may underlie successful performance in both the hybrid delay task and tests of general intelligence. Evidence that monitoring, a metacognitive process, is the key link, comes from the strong observed relationship between efficiency (which requires monitoring) with general intelligence but not preferences for larger later options (which does not).

These results are still somewhat speculative — as they should be given the innovation demonstrated here. One interesting debate in the recent literature is whether self-control is somehow qualitatively different from other forms of economic choice [19,20]. If there is no important difference, then self-control may relate to general intelligence because it is just one way of asking about the coherent functioning of the brain systems involved in integrating information about the environment to guide adaptive behavior.

References

  • 1.Inzlicht M, Schmeichel BJ, Macrae CN. Why self-control seems (but may not be) limited. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18:127–133. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Duckworth AL, Kern ML. A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. J Res Pers. 2011;45:259–268. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Saunders B, Milyavskaya M, Etz A, Randles D, Inzlicht M. Reported self-control does not meaningfully assess the ability to override impulses. PsyArXiv. 2017 https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/BXFSU.
  • 4.Rachlin H. Self-control: Beyond commitment. Behav Brain Sci. 1995;18:109–121. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hayden BY. Time discount and time preference in animals: a critical review. Psychonom Bull Rev. 2016;23:39–53. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0879-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Beran MJ, Hopkins WD. Self-control in chimpanzees relates to general intelligence. Curr Biol. 2018;28:574–579. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Robbins TW, Dalley JW. Impulsivity. Springer International Publishing; 2017. Dissecting impulsivity: Brain mechanisms and neuropsychiatric implications; pp. 201–226. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Walters GD. Behavioral self-control training for problem drinkers: A meta-analysis of randomized control studies. Behav Therap. 2001;31:135–149. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Shamosh NA, Gray JR. Delay discounting and intelligence: A meta-analysis. Intelligence. 2008;36:289–305. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mischel W, Shoda Y, Peake PK. The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54:687–696. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.4.687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Blanchard TC, Hayden BY. Monkeys are more patient in a foraging task than in a standard intertemporal choice task. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0117057. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Stephens DW, Anderson D. The adaptive value of preference for immediacy: when shortsighted rules have farsighted consequences. Behav Ecol. 2001;12:330–339. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.MacLean EL, Hare B, Nunn CL, Addessi E, Amici F, Anderson RC, Boogert NJ. The evolution of self-control. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:E2140–E2148. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1323533111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Paglieri F, Focaroli V, Bramlett J, Tierno V, McIntyre JM, Addessi E, Beran MJ. The hybrid delay task: Can capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) sustain a delay after an initial choice to do so? Behav Process. 2013;94:45–54. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Beran MJ, Evans TA, Paglieri F, McIntyre JM, Addessi E, Hopkins WD. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) can wait, when they choose to: A study with the hybrid delay task. Anim Cogn. 2014;17:197–205. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0652-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Paglieri F, Addessi E, Sbaffi A, Tasselli MI, Delfino A. Is it patience or motivation? On motivational confounds in intertemporal choice tasks. J Exp Anal Behav. 2015;103:196–217. doi: 10.1002/jeab.118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Blanchard TC, Hayden BY. Ramping ensemble activity in dorsal anterior cingulate cortex during persistent commitment to a decision. J Neurophysiol. 2015;114:2439–2449. doi: 10.1152/jn.00711.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hillman KL, Bilkey DK. Neurons in the rat anterior cingulate cortex dynamically encode cost–benefit in a spatial decision-making task. J Neurosci. 2010;30:7705–7713. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1273-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Berkman E, Hutcherson C, Livingston JL, Kahn LE, Inzlicht M. Self-control as value-based choice. SSRN. 2016 doi: 10.1177/0963721417704394. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2665823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 20.Shenhav A. The perils of losing control: Why self-control is not just another value-based decision. Psychol Inq. 2017;28:148–152. doi: 10.1080/1047840x.2017.1337407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES