Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Aging Health. 2017 Sep 17;29(7):1119–1143. doi: 10.1177/0898264317703559

Social Isolation and Medicare Spending: Among Older Adults, Objective Social Isolation Increases Expenditures while Loneliness Does Not

Jonathan G Shaw (1), Monica Farid (2), Claire Noel-Miller (3), Neesha Joseph (1), Ari Houser (3), Steven M Asch (1),(4), Jay Bhattacharya (1), Lynda Flowers (3)
PMCID: PMC5847278  NIHMSID: NIHMS946996  PMID: 29545676

Abstract

Objectives

Evaluate objective isolation and loneliness’ impact on Medicare spending and outcomes.

Methods

We linked Health and Retirement Study data to Medicare claims to analyze objective isolation (scaled composite of social contacts and network) and loneliness (positive response to 3-item loneliness scale) as predictors of subsequent Medicare spending. In multivariable regression adjusting for health and demographics, we determined marginal differences in Medicare expenditures. Secondary outcomes included spending by setting, and mortality.

Results

Objective isolation predicts greater spending, $1,644(p<0.001) per beneficiary annually, whereas loneliness predicts reduced spending, −$768(p<0.001). Increased spending concentrated in inpatient and nursing-home (SNF) care; despite more healthcare, objectively isolated beneficiaries had 31%(p<0.001) greater risk of death. Loneliness did not predict SNF use nor mortality, but predicted slightly less inpatient and outpatient care.

Conclusions

Objectively isolated seniors have higher Medicare spending, driven by increased hospitalization and institutionalization, and face greater mortality. Policies supporting social connectedness could reap significant savings.

Keywords: Social Isolation, Loneliness, Medicare, Health Services, Mortality

INTRODUCTION

Loneliness (subjective social isolation), encompasses qualitative and experiential factors that reflect a person’s sense of belonging and quality of relationships (Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2003; Cornwell & Waite, 2009b). In contrast, objective isolation is quantifiable social disconnectedness, and encompasses such factors as the size and structure of social networks, the frequency and duration of social interactions, and the extent of social supports received (Cornwell & Waite, 2009b). Both loneliness and objective isolation are demonstrated risk factors for mortality and a variety of poor health outcomes (Wilkinson & Weltgesundheitsorganisation, 2003; Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2003; Julianne Holt-Lunstad, Smith, & Layton, 2010). A growing population of older adults, increasingly living alone, is especially vulnerable to the consequences of social isolation—surveys suggest 17% of seniors are isolated (Ortiz, 2011) and approximately 20%–35% report feelings of loneliness (Theeke, 2009; Wilson & Moulton, 2010).

Despite the prevalence of loneliness and objective isolation and their well-established links to poorer health (Julianne Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010), their relationship to healthcare cost and utilization has yet to be comprehensively studied (Valtorta & Hanratty, 2012). Recent work has highlighted the importance of empirically distinguishing between these two forms of social isolation, as the two only loosely correlate (Coyle & Dugan, 2012; Hughes, Waite, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2004) and have different impacts on health outcomes (Luo, Hawkley, Waite, & Cacioppo, 2012; Steptoe, Shankar, Demakakos, & Wardle, 2013; Tomaka, Thompson, & Palacios, 2006). It is probable they have distinct impacts on healthcare spending.

In the current era of cost-containment, clarifying the role loneliness and objective isolation play in healthcare costs is of renewed importance, and may be relevant to efforts to curb Medicare spending. No studies have yet explored the association between objective isolation, loneliness and medical expenditures by older adults in the United States. In this study, we aim to expand understanding of this subject by employing a longitudinal sample of Medicare beneficiaries linked to robust social survey and healthcare spending data.

BACKGROUND

A wide body of research has demonstrated social isolation in various forms—including loneliness and objective isolation—to be a risk factor for mortality, worse health outcomes, and poorer self-reported health. Research focusing specifically on loneliness suggests its health consequences are most pronounced among vulnerable populations, including older adults (Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2003; Tomaka et al., 2006), while more recent meta-analysis of mortality confirms both loneliness and objective measures of isolation similarly predict upwards of 30% increase in mortality risk (J. Holt-Lunstad, Smith, Baker, Harris, & Stephenson, 2015). In two nationally representative samples (Cornwell & Waite, 2009b; Coyle & Dugan, 2012), both objective isolation and loneliness were shown to be associated with poorer self-reported health, with the added nuance that objective isolation more strongly correlated with poor physical health while loneliness more strongly correlated with poor mental health. The relationship between social isolation and healthcare use, however, is uncertain and has yet to be comprehensively examined (Valtorta & Hanratty, 2012), especially with explicit attention paid to the distinct parallel constructs of loneliness and objective isolation.

There are multiple mechanisms by which social isolation (objective isolation or loneliness) may affect health care use. It may affect health care use through its influence on various health behaviors (Broman, 1993; Umberson, Crosnoe, & Reczek, 2010). Socially isolated individuals may seek services as a substitute for social relationships (Coulton & Frost, 1982). Social relationships may: (1) moderate or “buffer” the impact of stress, thereby enhancing an individual’s health and well-being and precluding the need for health services (Gourash, 1978; Cassel, 1976; Cobb, 1976; Cohen & Wills, 1985); (2) obviate the need for professional health services by providing social support (Gourash 1978); (3) facilitate the use of health care by serving as “screening and referral agent(s)” for these services (Bass & Noelker, 1987; Gourash, 1978) and (4) transmit norms, attitudes, and values regarding seeking health care (Auslander & Litwin, 1990; Gourash, 1978).

Prior Research Findings on Social Isolation and Health Services Use

In various studies, disparate aspects of social isolation have been shown to influence physician services use, independent of health status, however the significance and direction of that impact conflict across studies. Contributing to the difficulties in finding consensus among existing studies are variations in: study setting (e.g. different nations with differing healthcare systems), measures of social isolation, measures of healthcare use (e.g., emergency department visits vs. primary care vs. inpatient admissions) and patient sample group (e.g., seniors, older mental health patients, psychogeriatric patients). Despite these inconsistencies, prior studies are broadly consistent in relying on the Andersen Behavioral Model, which theorizes that a person’s use of healthcare is influenced by three sets of factors: (1) need for care (2) predisposing factors (e.g. health beliefs and social structure), and (3) enabling factors (e.g. health insurance, community resources) (R. M. Andersen, 1995; Ronald Max Andersen, 2008). As summarized below, prior research into specific predisposing and enabling factors provides an intriguing but incomplete picture of social isolation’s connection to healthcare use.

The Role of Kin and Non-Kin Relationships

Several studies suggest that the impact of social isolation on health care service use depends on the types of relationships involved, be they kin or non-kin. Coe, Wolinsky, Miller, & Prendergast (1985) found that older adults without kin support use hospital emergency rooms 7 to 30 times more often than those with kin in the area, while differences between them for physician visits and other hospital use were not statistically significant. Wolinksy and Johnson (1991) found that both kin and non-kin support are associated with a modest increase in physician visits, while having non-kin support is also associated with fewer hospitalizations and a lower likelihood of nursing home admission; kin relationships were not correlated with any other form of health care use in the study. Some have interpreted this to suggest that there is a stronger substitution effect (whereby informal social supports substitute for formal health care services) present in non-kin relationships (Wolinsky & Johnson 1991).

The Role of Marital Status and Living Alone

Some have examined the relationship between social isolation and healthcare use through the lens of marital status and living arrangements. In examining the impact of marital status, Cafferata (1987) and Bowling, Farquhar, & Browne (1991) found no significant direct effect between marital status and the use of health services. In contrast, a recent study of healthcare use patterns among a nationally representative population of older Americans showed married individuals significantly more likely than unmarried to use outpatient services, but no significant difference in their use of hospital care (Manski et al., 2013). Wolinsky & Arnold (1988) and Evashwick et al (1984) reported contradictory findings with respect to the impact living alone has on the general use of health services. In examining its impact on physician service and hospital use, Wan & Odell (1981) and Cafferata (1987) both found that individuals living alone use more physician services, however their findings conflict with regards to the impact on hospitalization. The use of ‘living alone’ as an indicator of objective isolation has subsequently been called into question by more recent methodologic work by Cornwell & Waite (2009a) which confirmed that living alone (or household size) is not necessarily indicative of broader objective isolation.

The Role of Social Support

Several older studies from diverse nations (US, China, Canada) suggest social support, including instrumental (e.g., financial and transportation), informational (e.g., advice, guidance and referrals), and emotional (e.g., empathy and trust) support each have unique associations with health services use (Foreman, Yu, Barley, & Chen, 1998; Penning, 1995; Coulton & Frost, 1982). The patterns shown across these studies are complex; the various forms of support may either facilitate or inhibit the use of health care, depending on a number of factors, including the type and level of support provided and the health services in question. Regardless of either a positive or negative relationship between social support and health service use, studies across numerous predictors suggest that social support plays at most a minor role in determining healthcare use, when compared to medical need (Coulton & Frost, 1982; Wolinsky & Coe, 1984), but this work was limited to frequency of physician visits, and did not include broader measures of intensity of care, such as cost.

Prior Research Findings on Loneliness and Health Services Use

Only recently have a handful of studies looked explicitly at loneliness as a predictor of variation in healthcare use among older adults. Among the largest is work by Molloy et al (2010) in Ireland, which attempted to clarify contradictory findings from smaller studies out of the United Kingdom by separating out emergency versus planned hospitalizations. They found greater loneliness correlated with increased emergency hospitalizations only. In the United States, recent analysis of the Health and Retirement Survey found increased loneliness (and objective isolation) associated with a lower likelihood of receipt of dental care (Burr & Lee, 2013); no such work has yet explored the association of loneliness with overall medical care use in the United States.

The Need for Additional Research

By and large, studies suggest that social isolation does influence the use of health services, including hospitalization and physician visits. However, its exact role is complex and appears to vary based on a number of factors, including the dimension of social isolation or social support evaluated, and the type of outcome in question. The research literature is further complicated by conceptual ambiguity regarding social isolation, variability in measurement approaches, and the use of non-representative population samples (Burr & Lee 2013). Several decades have passed since a majority of the existing studies were published, and many early studies are limited by variations in how social isolation is defined and measured, a lack of nationally representative samples to draw from, variations in the age profiles of study samples, cross-sectional or limited follow-up, and self-reported use of health services. Thus, the results of studies examining the impact of social isolation on healthcare use frequently conflict, and both the net effect of social isolation and the specific impacts of loneliness versus objective isolation on service use remain unclear.

Few studies have looked at a representative sample of older adults in the U.S. in particular, and none have examined health service use in terms of net medical costs. This is especially salient as evidence is mixed when examining different types of service utilization—robust cost data could both differentiate the patterns of care of socially isolated patients, and capture their overall costs—providing a tangible measure of the burden of social isolation.

Medicare as Context to Explore Social Isolation and Health Services Use in Older Adults

Medicare, the United States’ federal health insurance for persons over 65 (and persons under 65 with certain disabilities and end stage renal disease), covers over 50 million individuals, representing approximately 15% of the United States population. Medicare has two main components: 1) Part A Hospital Insurance (“Part A”) which helps cover inpatient acute care, hospice care and when indicated post-hospitalization “nursing home” care in skilled nursing facilities (SNF) and 2) Part B Medical Insurance (“Part B”) which helps cover outpatient services and medical providers’ fees including primary care, specialist care, occupational/physical therapies and some home health services. (Part A is provided free of charge to all eligible beneficiaries, while Part B requires a monthly premium paid by the beneficiary, often as a deduction from their social security payments). A third option for Medicare beneficiaries known as Medicare Advantage, created in 1997 as an alternative to traditional Parts A & B, enables beneficiaries to receive their coverage from private healthcare systems through a system of managed care (e.g. HMOs and PPOs), and covers a growing minority (31%) of Medicare beneficiaries. (The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation, 2016)

Since Medicare’s inception 50 years ago, the proportion of very elderly beneficiaries has grown, and with this the population has become increasingly complex—two thirds of beneficiaries have multiple chronic conditions(Hamel, Blumenthal, Davis, & Guterman, 2015). This aging population is relevant as the U.S. faces the daunting reality of massive healthcare spending (approximately 3 trillion dollars annually), which in 2013 surpassed 17% of GDP, and at 5–6% average annual increase, continues to outpace rises in GDP. In the coming decade the growth of spending on Medicare beneficiaries is expected to rise to 7.9%, as the aging baby-boomer population increases its use of health services. (Klees, Wolfe, & Curtis, 2015)

Currently, Medicare is in the early phases of shifting provider payment methodology from fee-for-service towards value-based payment that attempts to incentivize higher quality care and better outcomes, while maintaining (or lowering) costs. With these payment changes underway, there is increased awareness that social risk factors (including social isolation) are important contributors to poor health outcomes. Currently, policymakers are evaluating whether it is necessary to adjust current payment policy to consider social risk factors. (Committee on Accounting for Socioeconomic Status in Medicare Payment Programs, 2016).

Objective and Hypotheses

Based on the current literature, we sought to expand understanding of the relationship between social isolation and healthcare spending among older adults by using: data from a nationally representative sample of older adults; a clear distinction between loneliness and objective isolation; and precise measurement of healthcare use via Medicare expenditures.

In doing so, we expected to confirm that both forms of social isolation correlate with chronic illness and thus—prior to adjustment for health—would predict greater healthcare use. Second, we expected that—once adjusted for health and other socio-demographic factors—the two forms of social isolation would have distinct independent associations with healthcare use, though we remained agnostic as to whether net effect for each would show them to be barriers or accelerators of total healthcare use. Third we believed examination of the types of care used (inpatient, outpatient, SNF) would reveal distinct signals for loneliness and objective isolation. Specifically we anticipated those who were objectively isolated would have higher need and use of SNF care given lack of social supports that might facilitate return to home after inpatient admissions; and we anticipated outpatient care, as potentially more optional type of care, might show more variation for both the most objectively isolated and lonely, but remained agnostic regarding the direction of effect, recognizing they could potential to both act as hindrances to (e.g. via transportation and mood/motivation respectively) and precipitants of use (e.g. substituting for informal health assistance and social contacts respectively).

METHODS

Data Source and Study Population

We analyzed data from 2006, 2008, and 2010 waves of the University of Michigan Health and Retirement Study (HRS; N=25,904), a nationally representative panel survey of adults age 50+, linked to Medicare beneficiary summary files for 2006–2012. In 2006, HRS introduced a Psychosocial & Lifestyle Questionnaire (response rate 90%) with questions on social relationships, activities and perceptions (Smith et al., 2013). The questionnaire is administered to alternating halves of the HRS panel; as a new group of respondents age into Medicare each wave, we combined unique respondents from three waves and linked them to subsequent Medicare spending data through 2012, to create a longitudinal cohort of 9,438 respondents, aged 65 or older at time of survey.

We limited our analytic sample to beneficiaries continuously enrolled in Original Medicare Parts A or B (n=5,938), because spending data is unavailable for those who instead enroll in the newer, managed care Medicare Advantage. Our exclusion criteria (Appendix Figure 1) removed those who died within one year after initial interview (n=171), based on the rationale that psychosocial survey responses and spending captured only in the final months of life would not be representative. Our final sample was 5,270 beneficiaries, linked to all available Medicare data following their baseline interview. Follow up time for each respondent varied, with start determined by timing of initial psychosocial interview, and end reached with completion of the 2012 HRS survey wave or, if sooner, death. Median follow-up was 4.5 years (range 1 to 7).

Measures

Subjective Isolation: Loneliness

Loneliness was captured by a 3-item scale contained in the HRS Psychosocial & Lifestyle Questionnaire. The scale is validated, and shown to capture information distinct from measures of social network (Hughes et al., 2004). It asks how often respondents (1) feel left out, (2) feel lack of companionship, or (3) feel isolated from others. For each question, there are three possible responses: “never or hardly ever”, “some of the time” or “often.” Our analysis defines as “lonely” those who answered “some of the time” or “often” to any of the three items. This dichotomous approach to defining loneliness is identical to methods applied to HRS data by Perissinoto, Stijacic Cenzer, & Covinsky (2012) in their work demonstrating loneliness predicts increased mortality and functional decline. (It assumes that each of the three loneliness scale items can be taken to reflect “different ways a person might express loneliness rather than additive components of loneliness” (Perissinotto et al., 2012). In preparatory work we considered a 3-level approach, but found no suggestion of stronger effect for those responding “often” (vs “some of the time”), and it instead found it only increased standard error for the few (9%) in the highest category of loneliness. We also considered treating loneliness as a continuous measure, but with only 6 levels of response and marked leftward skew, decided this was less appropriate; notably, prior work by Perissinoto et al (2012) examined 4 different ways to treat the same loneliness scale in a similar HRS population (examining its association with mortality and functional decline) and demonstrated the dichotomous approach was as predictive and informative as the alternatives.

Objective Isolation

To identify objective isolation, we created a scaled measure of social connectedness, based on normalized values of several survey questions capturing social network and interaction, and applied two thresholds to define 3 categories of objective isolation status: we defined as objectively “isolated” those with social connectedness score <1 standard deviation below the mean, defined as “not isolated” those with scores within 1 standard deviation of the mean, and defined as objectively “well-connected” those with scores >1 standard deviation above the mean. In preparatory work we explored using the scale as a continuous or binary predictor, but confirmed the relationship was non-linear and it was more appropriate to look at the extremes; once this was decided 1 standard deviation was chosen a priori for cut-offs.

We derived our measure by applying methodology developed by Cornwell & Waite (2009a) (see Appendix Methods for full details), and arrived at an internally consistent scale (Cronbach alpha coefficient=0.72) based on responses to HRS questions capturing: (1) social network size; (2) network range (number of types of relationships in respondents social network); (3) The number of close friends; (4) How often respondents are in contact (write, talk, or meet) with children, other family, and friends; and how often respondents meet in person with (5) children, (6) family, and (7) friends.

Outcomes

For each respondent we derived mean monthly spending, dividing total Medicare reimbursement by number of months of follow-up claims data for that individual. Medicare spending was obtained from HRS-specific Medicare Beneficiary Interview Summary Files which, instead of summarizing yearly data, summarize total spending on each beneficiary from one wave of biennial survey to the next (“MedRIC Documentation for HRS Data Requestors,” n.d.). Given varying entry and follow-up times, we normalized costs as monthly averages per beneficiary, with inflation adjustment to 2012 using the consumer price index. Our primary outcome was average Medicare spending per beneficiary per month, with regressions estimating marginal differences in monthly spending; we also present estimates of annual spending (monthly average x 12).

Secondary outcomes include Medicare reimbursement examined by type of care. Specifically we looked at inpatient, outpatient, and skilled nursing facility (SNF) care, to clarify if loneliness and isolation predicted distinct patterns. Since many beneficiaries did not receive any SNF or inpatient care, we also examined utilization as binary outcomes (for SNF) and frequency counts (for inpatient and outpatient care). Finally, to broadly capture health outcomes we examined likelihood and time to death during follow-up–i.e., specifically exploring if spending differences by social isolation status accompanied better or worse outcomes.

Covariates

We adjusted for covariates that capture a wide array of factors known to influence access and use of healthcare (Andersen & Newman, 1973), and thus healthcare spending. Time-varying covariates were from the same HRS wave as respondent’s social isolation measures.

Demographic characteristics collected in HRS include age, sex, self-reported race and education level; all of these were treated as categorical variables, with age represented by five groups (65–69, 70–74, 75–79, 80–84, 85+). Marital status was characterized as currently married/partnered or unmarried in the main analysis (with more granular treatment of past marital status explored in secondary analysis). Socioeconomic status was captured using HRS reported household-level income and net worth (converted to 5 income levels and 6 net worth categories for the purpose of analysis), and respondent’s working status. Urban or rural status was based on Beale Rural-Urban Continuum Codes.

Functional and health status data includes self-reported measures of comorbid disease, functional ability (activities of daily living, or ADLs), substance use history, depressive symptoms, and body mass index. Comorbidity is measured by asking respondents which, if any, of seven chronic conditions a doctor has ever told them they have: high blood pressure, diabetes, lung disease, heart problems, cancer, stroke, or arthritis/rheumatism. We coded ADL impairment by summing the activities in which respondents reported limitations, among: walking, bathing, dressing, transferring, and eating. Tobacco use was based on self-report of ever having smoked; alcohol use was based on self-reported current use. Depression was measured using the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression (CES-D) 8-Item Scale (Steffick, 2000). Body mass index was calculated based on self-reported height and weight, at time of response to the Psychosocial and Lifestyle Questionnaire, and converted to a 5-level categorical variable using standard cut-offs. (See appendix Table 1 for the full set of covariates and the definitions of levels within categorical variables.)

Statistical Analysis

We first calculated descriptive statistics to compare Medicare spending and covariate distribution across each of our two measures of social isolation. We examined correlation coefficients between loneliness and objective isolation, and between loneliness and depression to answer concerns they might represent overlapping conditions.

To determine how loneliness and isolation predicted Medicare costs, we performed multivariable regression using generalized linear modeling (GLM) to estimate the marginal difference in per-person per-month healthcare spending. In our GLM models we assumed a Gamma distribution of the errors with a log link function, as an appropriate estimator for non-normally distributed healthcare spending data. All models adjust for HRS’ complex sampling design, using population weights to obtain nationally representative estimates, and adjusting estimates of standard error using clustering at the geographic region.

We ran three regression models, each one controlling for additional sets of covariates, to clarify the influence of covariate-adjustment on the effect size of loneliness and objective isolation on spending. Our first model controls for basic demographic information (age, gender, marital status) only. Our second model controls for the full set of sociodemographic variables (adding race, education, BMI, geographic, and economic covariates). And finally, our third model adjusts for all collected covariates, adding adjustment for functional and health status.

To demonstrate how loneliness and objective isolation are distinct, we explicitly include both together in our models. Given, however, the potential that the two measures were collinear we also modeled each alone; we found their individual effects to be consistent regardless of whether we adjusted for the other. We further ruled out multicollinearity by calculating variance inflation factors (VIFs) for our full model—all were within acceptable range at <4, and near 1 for our primary predictors.

In sensitivity analyses (see Appendix Sensitivity Analysis), we explored the robustness of our findings to alternative definitions of function, depression, and health status. Lastly, we examined potential sub-population effects, applying our main model to three sub-cohorts defined by marital status (married, widowed, or unmarried); we were specifically interested because older studies treat marital status as a proxy for social connectedness (Bowling et al., 1991; Cafferata, 1987; Evashwick et al., 1984; Wolinsky & Arnold, 1988) and recent work suggests marriage increases some healthcare use (Manski et al., 2013).

For our secondary outcomes, inpatient, outpatient, and SNF spending, we estimated separate GLM models, using the same covariate sets. We then used Poisson regression to estimate how counts of inpatient and outpatient visits differed by loneliness and objective isolation status, and used logistic regression to analyze the likelihood of any SNF use during follow-up. Finally, we estimated the effect of loneliness and objective isolation on mortality with a Cox proportional hazards model.

RESULTS

Our cohort of beneficiaries had an average age of 74 years, with mean and median monthly Medicare spending of $1,024 and $489 respectively. Of the 5,270 participants, 55.3% reported being lonely, while 13.7% were objectively isolated by our social connectedness scale. Baseline characteristics (Table 1, Appendix Table 1) and Medicare spending (Table 2) varied across both measures of social isolation. For example, both lonely and isolated participants were significantly more likely to have lower economic means, be more depressed, and report chronic illness and impaired activities of daily living. The lonely were disproportionately very elderly (≥80 years) and unmarried, whereas objective isolation varied less across age and marital status. The lonely were more likely to be female, the isolated more likely to be male.

Table 1.

Sample Description: Characteristics by Social Isolation Status, Among Medicare Beneficiaries in the Health and Retirement Study 2006 to 2010

Characteristic Overall Lonely Status
Objective Isolation Status
Lonely Not Lonely Isolated Not Isolated Well-connected
Basic Demographics
 Age (years)
  65 – 69 31.1% 29.2%*** 33.5%*** 31.4%* 32.0%* 27.2%*
  70 – 74 25.2% 24.9% 25.6% 22.7% 25.3% 27.2%
  75 – 79 19.5% 19.1% 19.9% 20.2% 19.2% 20.2%
  80 – 84 13.6% 14.6%* 12.4%* 13.5% 13.3% 14.9%
  85 + 10.6% 12.2%*** 8.57%*** 12.3% 10.3% 10.5%
 Male 42.8% 40%*** 46.2%*** 56.1%*** 41.1%*** 38.7%***
 Married 62.1% 52.3% 74.2% 63.1% 62.0% 61.7%
Health and Functional Status
 Comorbid Conditions
  High Blood Pressure 64.5% 66.2%** 62.4%** 63.1% 65.6% 61.0%
  Diabetes 20.9% 22.3%** 19.2%** 21.5% 21.1% 19.4%
  Lung Disease 12.1% 13.4%*** 10.5%*** 14.8%** 11.9%** 10.8%**
  Heart Attack/Heart 32.5% 34.9%*** 29.6%*** 35.2% 31.7% 33.5%
 Disease
  Cancer 19.3% 19.7% 18.7% 20.9% 18.9% 19.7%
  Stroke 10.1% 11.6%*** 8.31%*** 13.4%** 9.92%** 8.32%**
  Arthritis 69.5% 71.9%*** 66.6%*** 69.6% 69.0% 71.6%
 Depression Score (CES-D, range 0–8) 1.27 1.73*** 0.69*** 1.71*** 1.24*** 1.05***
 Number ADLs Impaired (range 0–5) 0.29 0.38*** 0.18*** 0.39*** 0.28*** 0.24***
 Ever Smoker 57.2% 57.9% 56.4% 63.7%*** 57.7%*** 49.9%***
 Current Alcohol Use 48.1% 45.2%*** 51.7%*** 49.4% 48.7% 44.6%

 N (person observations) 5,270 2912 2358 724 3681 865

NOTE: Reported are un-weighted percentages (%) or means. HRS = Health and Retirement Study. Married = married or partnered. CES-D = Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale; ADL = Activities of daily living.

p-values:

*

p <0.05,

**

p <0.01,

***

p<0.001.

Chi-square (categorical) or ANOVA (continuous variables)

Table 2.

Unadjusted Monthly Medicare Spending and Mortality, by Social Isolation Status

Overall Lonely Status
Objective Isolation Status
Lonely Not Lonely p-valuea Isolated Not Isolated Well-connected p-valuea
Average Medicare spending (per beneficiary per month)
Total $1,024 $1,092 $939 <.001 $1,215 $995 $986 0.001
 Inpatient $396 $418 $369 0.03 $496 $383 $368 0.02
 Outpatient $140 $147 $131 0.07 $130 $137 $159 0.11
 SNF $83 $102 $61 <.001 $127 $75 $84 <.001
Death during follow-up 18.9% 21.5% 15.6% <.001 25.4% 18.0% 16.9% <.001

N (person observations) 5,270 2912 2358 724 3681 865

NOTE: Reported spending is in 2012 US Dollars. Total spending includes Inpatient, Outpatient, Carrier, Skilled Nursing Facility, and Durable Medical Equipment Claims.

HRS = Health and Retirement Study. SNF = Skilled nursing facility.

a

ANOVA

Both the lonely and the isolated had higher Medicare spending: lonely beneficiaries spent $153 more per month than non-lonely counterparts; the isolated spent $220 more than their non-isolated counterparts. Despite similar patterns overall, the correlation between loneliness and objective isolation was limited, with 68% of those identified as objectively isolated also being lonely (Pearson correlation coefficient r= 0.20). Loneliness was also distinct from, and only weakly correlated with, depression (r=0.25).

Adjusting only for basic demographics, both loneliness and isolation were predictive of increased Medicare spending: +$68/month and +$202/month, respectively (p<0.001 for both). However, further, adjusting for socio-economic and health status the association with spending changed markedly for loneliness and reversed (Appendix Table 2). In our fully adjusted model, loneliness predicted a $64/month reduction (an estimated −$764 annually) in Medicare spending, while isolation predicted a $137/month increase (+$1643 annually) in Medicare spending (p<0.001 for both; Table 3).

Table 3.

Estimates of Effect of Loneliness and Objective Isolation, by Type of Healthcare Use

Predictor Total Inpatient Outpatient SNF

Marginal Spending per month Marginal Spending per month Incidence (aIRR) Marginal Spending per month Incidence (aIRR) Marginal Spending per month Incidence (aOR)

Lonely (reference = Not Lonely) −$63.7*** −$54.4** 0.96** $3.7 0.96 $36.10 1.20*
Socially Isolated (reference = Not Isolated) $136.9*** $84.9*** 1.03 −$6.2 0.92* $73.7*** 1.28**

NOTE: Spending adjusted to 2012 US dollars. Total includes Inpatient, Outpatient, Carrier, Skilled Nursing Facility, and Durable Medical Equipment claims. SNF = Skilled nursing facility; aIRR = adjusted incident rate ratio, using Poisson regression; aOR = adjusted odds ratio, using logistic regression adjusted for months follow-up. All models include adjustment for sociodemographic, health and functional status factors--see Appendix Table 2 for covariate details.

*

p<0.1,

**

p<0.05,

***

p<0.01.

In sub-analysis by type of care (Table 3), we found similar patterns for inpatient spending, with isolation predicting higher (+$85/month) and loneliness predicting lower inpatient spending (−$54/month) after full adjustment. There was, however, no statistically significant difference in outpatient spending predicted by loneliness, or by isolation. Poisson regression of frequency of inpatient and outpatient use showed loneliness predicted less frequent use of both types of services (incidence rate ratio of 0.96 for each), however this was only statistically significant for inpatient care. Isolation, but not loneliness, predicted increased spending on SNF care (+$74/month, p<0.001); both predicted increased likelihood of receiving any SNF care, but only reached statistical significance for isolation (adjusted odds ratio 1.28, p=0.04) not loneliness (1.20, p=0.05).

Unadjusted analysis suggested both loneliness and isolation were associated with approximately 40% higher annual mortality during follow-up (Table 2; Appendix Figures 2 & 3). After fully adjusting for socio-demographic and health factors with Cox hazard regression (Table 4) isolation, but not loneliness, significantly predicted higher mortality with an increased hazard of 31% (p <0.001).

Table 4.

Loneliness and Objective Isolation as Predictors of Death in Medicare Beneficiaries, HRS 2006–2010, Cox Hazard Regression

Limited Demographic Adjustment Full Sociodemographic Adjustment Full Sociodemographic and Health and Functional StatusAdjustment
aHR aHR aHR
Lonely (reference = not lonely) 1.24 (1.10 – 1.40)*** 1.18 (1.05 – 1.32)*** 1.05 (0.94 – 1.18)
Objective Isolation
Isolated 1.42 (1.23 – 1.64)*** 1.36 (1.21 – 1.53)*** 1.31 (1.19 – 1.44)***
 Not Isolated REF REF REF
 Well-connected 0.86 (0.67 – 1.11) 0.85 (0.68 –1.07) 0.87 (0.72 – 1.06)

NOTE: aHR = adjusted hazard ratios. N = 5,441, including 171 individuals excluded from main analytic cohort due to <1 year of Medicare claims data.

***

p<0.001.

Lastly, in our sensitivity analyses exploring the robustness of our results, alternative measures and modeling assumptions had little effect on the overall findings (see Appendix Sensitivity Analysis). Notably, depression consistently demonstrated an opposite effect on spending compared to loneliness. Sub-population analysis by marital status (married, widowed, unmarried), reduced our power to detect significance, but suggested objective isolation was most prominently a predictor of increased spending (+$273/month, or $3,276 annually, p<0.05) in the 27% of our cohort that was widowed; loneliness was most markedly associated with reduced spending (−$489/month, or $5,868 annually, p<0.01) in the 11% of our cohort that was unmarried (see Appendix Table 3).

DISCUSSION

This study is the first to measure the association between loneliness, objective isolation and health expenditures in the Medicare population. We find that objective isolation is associated with increased Medicare spending, while loneliness’ association appears opposite. In general, Medicare spends an estimated $1643 more annually on objectively isolated beneficiaries than on similar individuals with greater social connections. The increased spending is more pronounced for widowed seniors: Medicare spends $3,276 more per year on beneficiaries who are dually widowed and isolated, than on their widowed peers who are not isolated. We did not find that increased spending on isolated beneficiaries produced better outcomes (i.e., reduced hospitalization or mortality). Instead, adjusting for differences in initial health status, we found that objectively isolated individuals used more skilled nursing services and had over 30% higher annual risk of death.

Conversely, we find that Medicare spends $768 less per year on lonely beneficiaries than it does for similar beneficiaries (i.e. after adjusting for health status) who do not report being lonely. This latter finding suggests that loneliness might be a barrier to accessing healthcare, while the former suggests that providing objective social support to older adults (especially the widowed) has potential to reduce healthcare spending.

While several prior works have explored psychosocial contributors to accessing health services (Babitsch, Gohl, & von Lengerke, 2012) most have not explicitly measured healthcare spending, nor focused on the ever-growing Medicare population. Our study adds a contemporary perspective particularly relevant in the era of cost-containment and, together with studies such as Unützer et al (2009) showing depression predicts higher Medicare costs (incidentally reconfirmed in our models), forms a nascent body of work exploring psychosocial influences on Medicare spending.

As we hypothesized, we confirm that loneliness and objective isolation are more prevalent in those with poor health status and poorer socioeconomic indicators, and (once adjusting for those factors) the two forms of social isolation are distinct with regards to healthcare use patterns. Among the various hypothesized pathways by which social isolation might influence quantity of healthcare used—including potentially increased use (e.g. due to lack of social support’s “buffering” effect against stress and illness) and decreased use (e.g. via less presentation to care due to lack of facilitating social agents) (Gourash 1978)—we demonstrate that objective isolation’s net impact is an increase in total healthcare use. As we expected, we found that SNF use is higher among objectively isolated. The degree to which increased SNF care spending appears to contribute to the total increased spending for objectively isolated is impressive and, we propose, also intuitive as it likely reflects those with less robust social supports are more likely to be directed to “nursing home” care post-hospitalization, rather than home with assistance from their social supports. We did not confirm our hypothesis that outpatient spending would be more influenced by social isolation, despite its potentially more elective nature. This may in part reflect a lack of statistical power, as outpatient care is cheaper and a small fraction of total healthcare costs in our cohort. The trend towards less frequent outpatient care we observed (IRR 0.92, p=0.1) for beneficiaries who were objectively isolated, suggests such isolation may present a barrier to accessing outpatient care, which remains compatible with a possible substitution effect of higher-cost inpatient and SNF care for lower cost outpatient care.

Loneliness is extremely common among older adults (Theeke, 2009; Wilson & Moulton, 2010). A majority (55%) of Medicare beneficiaries in this nationally representative sample met our criteria for being lonely. Though objective isolation is not as common (14% within our cohort), it nevertheless affects a substantial proportion of the American older adult population—17% of seniors live alone, putting them at high risk of isolation (Ortiz, 2011). Given the scope of the problem, as well as the differential effect of objective isolation and loneliness on Medicare spending our findings suggest two distinct responses.

The first, in response to objective isolation, is clear and involves efforts to promote concrete assistance for those who are isolated. At the level of the healthcare delivery, innovative care models that incorporate social assessment and linkage to social support services hold promise for significant cost savings. Already several such programs (largely arising from new incentives and initiatives of the Affordable Care Act, such as the Healthcare Innovation Challenge Awards) have shown some early promise to reduce costs (Shier, Ginsburg, Howell, Volland, & Golden, 2013). Our study findings lend additional optimism that cost savings (in addition to improved wellbeing) can be found via care models that assess and address objective social support gaps.

Clinicians and policy makers alike have a role in responding to increasing evidence of social isolation’s influence. Clinicians can help identify social isolated patients by asking about social network and support structures. The Center for Medicare and Medicaid Services (CMS) can play an important role by reimbursing interventions that address social isolation. Encouragingly, CMS leadership has demonstrated exploratory steps in this direction as, in their own words, “CMS is testing various approaches for medical practices, including methods for linking practices and patients to social support services” through their Innovation Center (Kassler, Tomoyasu, & Conway, 2015).

The second call to action, for loneliness, is more nuanced, but begins with increased recognition of its prevalence, its co-occurrence with poor health, and its potential role as a barrier to seeking care. In light of our findings this effort need be justified, rightly, more on the basis of reducing suffering than on potential cost savings. Our finding of loneliness’ inverse association with spending (after adjusting for health status) may be less important than our re-demonstration of its strong correlation and co-occurrence with poorer socioeconomic, mental and physical wellbeing—i.e., reported loneliness might provide a simple and useful marker of high risk older adults in need of intervention. This idea is supported by Pitkala et al’s (Pitkala, Routasalo, Kautiainen, & Tilvis, 2009) randomized control trial of psychosocial group rehabilitation targeted explicitly at lonely older adults which intriguingly showed, despite no effect on reported feelings of loneliness (Routasalo, Tilvis, Kautiainen, & Pitkala, 2009), significantly improved self-reported health and reduced mortality and healthcare spending.

We demonstrate loneliness is a more prevalent form of psychological distress than depression, suggesting clinicians can play an important role in identifying and addressing loneliness (similarly as they do for depression), e.g., with a simple questionnaire such as the 3-question loneliness scale (Hughes et al., 2004). Once identified, they can offer referral to appropriate social and behavioral health resources. While the most effective management of loneliness per se remains uncertain, a recent meta-analysis of interventions to reduce loneliness showed consistent support for approaches that addressed maladaptive social cognition, such as cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) (Masi, Chen, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2011). Whether the reductions in reported loneliness seen with CBT alters subsequent health and healthcare use warrants future investigation.

As with any study, ours has limitations. We focus on community dwelling, fee-for-service (Medicare parts A and B) beneficiaries. Social isolation’s relationship to spending may be different within Medicare Advantage, which historically attracts a healthier population (McWilliams, Hsu, & Newhouse, 2012). However, examining the >3000 excluded HRS respondents aged 65+ with Medicare Advantage, we found no difference in rates of loneliness and objective isolation, and only minimal skew towards a younger healthier population compared to our analytic cohort (mean age 72 vs 73, and mean count of chronic conditions 2.2 vs. 2.3). Regardless, future study of social isolation in managed care and nursing home populations is warranted. Another concern is that our definition of loneliness is overly sensitive, with half of beneficiaries meeting our criteria; however, our cohort’s prevalence of loneliness only somewhat exceeds that reported (43%) in a slightly younger HRS sample, of adults age 60 and above, in work by Perissinotto et al (2012) which demonstrated the same definition of loneliness reliably predicted increased risk of functional decline (2-fold) and mortality (45% increase). Further, as described in our methods, when we explored the option of using an additional threshold to discriminate “very lonely” respondents, we found this small fraction of respondents were not distinct in their healthcare spending compared to the plurality of “mildly lonely.”

As with all observational studies, unobserved differences, including residual differences in baseline medical complexity, could bias the observed findings. However, the fact that loneliness and objective isolation switch from having parallel associations (without adjustment) to having opposite associations once we adjust for best available health measures makes it unlikely that more complete adjustment for health would reverse this overall finding. Conversely, given the reversal of loneliness’ effect once adjusting for health status, it is possible our health covariates include yet unrecognized pathways (rather than confounders) by which loneliness influences healthcare patterns—e.g. if loneliness were to directly cause increased substance use, hypertension, or other such conditions, adjusting for these health factor might attenuate loneliness’ true impact on healthcare spending by excluding these mechanisms from the effect estimate. Lastly, analysis of two closely related constructs raises concern for collinearity and interactions that could bias results, however, in addition to our reassuring findings of limited correlation and lack of multicollinearity, we tested modeling each construct alone and found the effects of objective isolation and loneliness were consistent regardless of whether we adjusted for the other.

These findings highlight the importance of accounting for both forms of social isolation and approaching them distinctly in future research and when devising policy solutions. Novel healthcare initiatives that focus on maximizing objective social supports and connectedness merit further funding and evaluation, as reducing objective isolation is not only inherently valuable to affected individuals, but also holds promise for reducing healthcare spending.

Supplementary Material

Appendix

Acknowledgments

Sincere thanks to Lena Schoemaker of Stanford University for analytic support. Data use was by permission of the Health & Retirement Study, University of Michigan, and the Centers for Medicare and Medicaid.

FUNDING

This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging at the National Institutes of Health (AG017253 to JGS, JB) and the AARP Public Policy Institute.

References

  1. Andersen RM. Revisiting the behavioral model and access to medical care: does it matter? Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1995;36(1):1–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andersen RM. National health surveys and the behavioral model of health services use. Medical Care. 2008;46(7):647–653. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0b013e31817a835d. https://doi.org/10.1097/MLR.0b013e31817a835d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andersen R, Newman JF. Societal and individual determinants of medical care utilization in the United States. The Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly. Health and Society. 1973;51(1):95–124. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Auslander GK, Litwin H. Social Support Networks and Formal Help Seeking: Differences Between Applicants to Social Services and a Nonapplicant Sample. Journal of Gerontology. 1990;45(3):S112–S119. doi: 10.1093/geronj/45.3.s112. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/45.3.S112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Babitsch B, Gohl D, von Lengerke T. Re-revisiting Andersen’s Behavioral Model of Health Services Use: a systematic review of studies from 1998–2011. Psycho-Social Medicine. 2012;9:Doc11. doi: 10.3205/psm000089. https://doi.org/10.3205/psm000089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bass DM, Noelker LS. The influence of family caregivers on elder’s use of in-home services: an expanded conceptual framework. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1987;28(2):184–196. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bowling A, Farquhar M, Browne P. Use of services in old age: data from three surveys of elderly people. Social Science & Medicine (1982) 1991;33(6):689–700. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(91)90023-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Broman CL. Social relationships and health-related behavior. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1993;16(4):335–350. doi: 10.1007/BF00844776. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00844776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Burr JA, Lee HJ. Social relationships and dental care service utilization among older adults. Journal of Aging and Health. 2013;25(2):191–220. doi: 10.1177/0898264312464497. https://doi.org/10.1177/0898264312464497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC. Social isolation and health, with an emphasis on underlying mechanisms. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 2003;46(3 Suppl):S39–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cafferata GL. Marital status, living arrangements, and the use of health services by elderly persons. Journal of Gerontology. 1987;42(6):613–618. doi: 10.1093/geronj/42.6.613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cassel J. The contribution of the social environment to host resistance: the Fourth Wade Hampton Frost Lecture. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1976;104(2):107–123. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cobb S. Presidential Address-1976. Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1976;38(5):300–314. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197609000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Coe RM, Wolinsky FD, Miller DK, Prendergast JM. Elderly persons without family support networks and use of health services. A follow-up report on social network relationships. Research on Aging. 1985;7(4):617–622. doi: 10.1177/0164027585007004007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin. 1985;98(2):310–357. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Committee on Accounting for Socioeconomic Status in Medicare Payment Programs, Board on Population Health and Public Health Practice, Board on Health Care Services, Health and Medicine Division, & National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Accounting for Social Risk Factors in Medicare Payment: Data. Washington, D.C: National Academies Press; 2016. Retrieved from https://www.nap.edu/catalog/23605. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cornwell EY, Waite LJ. Measuring social isolation among older adults using multiple indicators from the NSHAP study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2009a;64(Suppl 1):i38–46. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp037. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbp037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cornwell EY, Waite LJ. Social disconnectedness, perceived isolation, and health among older adults. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2009b;50(1):31–48. doi: 10.1177/002214650905000103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Coulton C, Frost AK. Use of Social and Health Services by the Elderly. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1982;23(4):330. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136491. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Coyle CE, Dugan E. Social isolation, loneliness and health among older adults. Journal of Aging and Health. 2012;24(8):1346–1363. doi: 10.1177/0898264312460275. https://doi.org/10.1177/0898264312460275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Evashwick C, Rowe G, Diehr P, Branch L. Factors explaining the use of health care services by the elderly. Health Services Research. 1984;19(3):357–382. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Foreman SE, Yu LC, Barley D, Chen LW. Use of health services by Chinese elderly in Beijing. Medical Care. 1998;36(8):1265–1282. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199808000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gourash N. Help-seeking: a review of the literature. American Journal of Community Psychology. 1978;6(5):413–423. doi: 10.1007/BF00941418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hamel MB, Blumenthal D, Davis K, Guterman S. Medicare at 50 — Origins and Evolution. New England Journal of Medicine. 2015;372(5):479–486. doi: 10.1056/NEJMhpr1411701. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMhpr1411701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and Social Isolation as Risk Factors for Mortality: A Meta-Analytic Review. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2015;10(2):227–237. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614568352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB. Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. PLoS Medicine. 2010;7(7):e1000316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A Short Scale for Measuring Loneliness in Large Surveys: Results From Two Population-Based Studies. Research on Aging. 2004;26(6):655–672. doi: 10.1177/0164027504268574. https://doi.org/10.1177/0164027504268574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kassler WJ, Tomoyasu N, Conway PH. Beyond a traditional payer--CMS’s role in improving population health. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2015;372(2):109–111. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1406838. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMp1406838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Klees B, Wolfe C, Curtis C. BRIEF SUMMARIES Of MEDICARE & MEDICAID - Title XVIII and Title XIX of The Social Security Act. Office of the Actuary Centers for Medicare & Medicaid Services Department of Health and Human Services; 2015. Retrieved from https://www.cms.gov/Research-Statistics-Data-and-Systems/Statistics-Trends-and-Reports/MedicareProgramRatesStats/Downloads/MedicareMedicaidSummaries2015.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  30. Luo Y, Hawkley LC, Waite LJ, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness, health, and mortality in old age: a national longitudinal study. Social Science & Medicine (1982) 2012;74(6):907–914. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.11.028. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Manski RJ, Moeller JF, Chen H, Schimmel J, St Clair PA, Pepper JV. Patterns of older Americans’ health care utilization over time. American Journal of Public Health. 2013;103(7):1314–1324. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.301124. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2012.301124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Masi CM, Chen HY, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A meta-analysis of interventions to reduce loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Review: An Official Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc. 2011;15(3):219–266. doi: 10.1177/1088868310377394. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868310377394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. McWilliams JM, Hsu J, Newhouse JP. New risk-adjustment system was associated with reduced favorable selection in medicare advantage. Health Affairs (Project Hope) 2012;31(12):2630–2640. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2011.1344. https://doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.2011.1344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. MedRIC Documentation for HRS Data Requestors. Acumen, LLC; n.d. Retrieved from http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/rda/cmsdocs/MedRICdocumentation.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  35. Molloy GJ, McGee HM, O’Neill D, Conroy RM. Loneliness and emergency and planned hospitalizations in a community sample of older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2010;58(8):1538–1541. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.02960.x. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.02960.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Ortiz H. Crossing New Frontiers: Benefits Access among Isolated Seniors. National Council on Aging. National Center for Benefits Outreach and Enrollment (NCBOE); 2011. Retrieved from https://www.ncoa.org/resources/crossing-new-frontiers/ [Google Scholar]
  37. Penning MJ. Health, social support, and the utilization of health services among older adults. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 1995;50(5):S330–339. doi: 10.1093/geronb/50b.5.s330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Perissinotto CM, Stijacic Cenzer I, Covinsky KE. Loneliness in older persons: a predictor of functional decline and death. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2012;172(14):1078–1083. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2012.1993. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinternmed.2012.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Pitkala KH, Routasalo P, Kautiainen H, Tilvis RS. Effects of psychosocial group rehabilitation on health, use of health care services, and mortality of older persons suffering from loneliness: a randomized, controlled trial. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2009;64(7):792–800. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glp011. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glp011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Routasalo PE, Tilvis RS, Kautiainen H, Pitkala KH. Effects of psychosocial group rehabilitation on social functioning, loneliness and well-being of lonely, older people: randomized controlled trial. Journal of Advanced Nursing. 2009;65(2):297–305. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2008.04837.x. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2008.04837.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Shier G, Ginsburg M, Howell J, Volland P, Golden R. Strong social support services, such as transportation and help for caregivers, can lead to lower health care use and costs. Health Affairs (Project Hope) 2013;32(3):544–551. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2012.0170. https://doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.2012.0170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Smith J, Fisher G, Ryan L, Clarke P, House J, Weir DR. Health and Retirement Study. University of Michigan; Ann Arbor: 2013. Feb, Psychosocial and Lifestyle Questionnaire 2006 – 2010: Documentation Report. Retrieved from http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/HRS2006-2010SAQdoc.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  43. Steffick DE. Health and Retirement Study. University of Michigan; Ann Arbor: 2000. Documentation of Affective Functioning Measures in the Health and Retirement Study. Retrieved from http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/dr-005.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  44. Steptoe A, Shankar A, Demakakos P, Wardle J. Social isolation, loneliness, and all-cause mortality in older men and women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2013;110(15):5797–5801. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1219686110. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1219686110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation. An overview of Medicare - April 2016 - Issue Brief. 2016 Apr 1; Retrieved December 1, 2016, from http://kff.org/medicare/issue-brief/an-overview-of-medicare/
  46. Theeke LA. Predictors of Loneliness in U.S. Adults Over Age Sixty-Five. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing. 2009;23(5):387–396. doi: 10.1016/j.apnu.2008.11.002. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apnu.2008.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Tomaka J, Thompson S, Palacios R. The relation of social isolation, loneliness, and social support to disease outcomes among the elderly. Journal of Aging and Health. 2006;18(3):359–384. doi: 10.1177/0898264305280993. https://doi.org/10.1177/0898264305280993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Umberson D, Crosnoe R, Reczek C. Social Relationships and Health Behavior Across the Life Course. Annual Review of Sociology. 2010;36(1):139–157. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-120011. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-120011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Unützer J, Schoenbaum M, Katon WJ, Fan MY, Pincus HA, Hogan D, Taylor J. Healthcare costs associated with depression in medically Ill fee-for-service medicare participants. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2009;57(3):506–510. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.02134.x. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.02134.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Valtorta N, Hanratty B. Loneliness, isolation and the health of older adults: do we need a new research agenda? Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine. 2012;105(12):518–522. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.2012.120128. https://doi.org/10.1258/jrsm.2012.120128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wan TTH, Odell BG. Factors Affecting the Use of Social and Health Services Among the Elderly. Ageing and Society. 1981;1(01):95–115. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X81000068. [Google Scholar]
  52. Wilkinson R, editor. Weltgesundheitsorganisation, editor. The solid facts: social determinants of health. 2. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  53. Wilson C, Moulton B. Loneliness among Older Adults: A National Survey of Adults 45+ AARP; Washington DC: 2010. Retrieved from http://assets.aarp.org/rgcenter/general/loneliness_2010.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  54. Wolinsky FD, Arnold CL. A different perspective on health and health services utilization. Annual Review of Gerontology & Geriatrics. 1988;8:71–101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Wolinsky FD, Coe RM. Physician and hospital utilization among noninstitutionalized elderly adults: an analysis of the Health Interview Survey. Journal of Gerontology. 1984;39(3):334–341. doi: 10.1093/geronj/39.3.334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Wolinsky FD, Johnson RJ. The use of health services by older adults. Journal of Gerontology. 1991;46(6):S345–357. doi: 10.1093/geronj/46.6.s345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Appendix

RESOURCES