Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2018 Mar 16;8:4696. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-23046-6

Perinatal stress and human hippocampal volume: Findings from typically developing young adults

Klára Marečková 1,, Radek Mareček 2, Petra Bencurova 1, Jana Klánová 4, Ladislav Dušek 4,5, Milan Brázdil 1,3
PMCID: PMC5856850  PMID: 29549289

Abstract

The main objective of this study was to investigate the impact of prenatal and early postnatal stress on hippocampal volume in young adulthood. In sharp contrast to numerous results in animal models, our data from a neuroimaging follow-up (n = 131) of a community-based birth cohort from the Czech Republic (European Longitudinal Study of Pregnancy and Childhood) showed that in typically developing young adults, hippocampal volume was not associated with birth weight, stressful life events during the prenatal or early postnatal period, or dysregulated mood and wellbeing in the mother during the early postnatal period. Interestingly, mother’s anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth did show long-lasting effects on the hippocampal volume in young adult offspring irrespective of sex. Further analyses revealed that these effects were subfield-specific; present in CA1, CA2/3, CA4, GC-DG, subiculum, molecular layer, and HATA, hippocampal subfields identified by translational research as most stress- and glucocorticoid-sensitive, but not in the remaining subfields. Our findings provide evidence that the type of early stress is critical when studying its effects on the human brain.

Introduction

Early-life stress seems to have long-lasting effects on human health in later life including higher incidence of neurologic abnormalities, psychiatric disorders, age-related cognitive dysfunction, obesity and hypertension17. Animal studies suggest that early-life stress alters neurogenesis, programming of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and neurobehavior8,9. Hippocampus, the key neural region for regulation of affect and HPA axis function10, is particularly sensitive to stress11. Animal research demonstrated that prolonged stress and increased glucocorticoids were associated with atrophy and retraction of the apical dendrites of the hippocampal pyramidal cells1214 and disruption of neurogenesis in the hippocampus15,16. Both rats and tree shrews exposed to chronic stress exhibited reduced hippocampal volume17,18. In humans, psychological and psychosocial stress were related to altered structure of adult hippocampus1922. Smaller hippocampal volume was described as a risk factor for development of stress-related psychopathology23 and a key neuroanatomical feature in anxiety disorder24. Smaller hippocampal volume was reported also in a number of other psychiatric disorders, including post-traumatic stress disorder, major depressive disorder, borderline personality disorder, schizophrenia, antisocial personality disorder, and dissociative identity disorder25.

According to Teicher and colleagues, sensitivity of hippocampus to stress is particularly heightened during the early developmental period26. The evidence is mixed in the literature, though. While adverse early life environment decreased neural stem cell production in juvenile mice27, separation of monkeys from their mothers was not associated with decrease of hippocampal volume2830. Inconsistency is present also within human research. While recent research31 found smaller hippocampal volumes in youth and adults exposed to early life adversity, no similar relationship was found by a large population-based study from Australia32, a voxel-based morphometry study of neural correlates of prenatal stress in young women33, or a study relating maternal cortisol in pregnancy to hippocampal volume in childhood34. Relationship between early life events and hippocampal volume was also not found in a sample of women with remitted unipolar major depressive disorder35. Relationship between childhood abuse and smaller hippocampus was reported in depressed patients but not healthy controls36. One could hypothesize that the effects of early life stress on the hippocampal volume might be species-specific, sex-specific, and specific to the type and timing of the stress exposure.

Fetal sex modulates the responsiveness to prenatal stress34,3739, but as reviewed in MacQueen & Frodl, the effect of sex was not assessed in the majority of studies investigating the association between stress and hippocampal volumes10. For example, the three studies described above33,35,36 were done in females only. The low ability to find effects of early stress on hippocampal volume in female samples might be related to the protective effects of estrogen. Animal research showed differences in male and female rats in the effects of acute stress on performance in tasks involving hippocampal function, such as classical eye-blink conditioning or Y-maze or Morris Water Maze40. Sex differences were also found in the effects of maternal deprivation on development of synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus41. Moreover, estrogen treatment protected ovarictomized rats after chronic stress exposure from neuronal loss in the hippocampus42.

The main objective of this research was to study the effects of different types of prenatal and early postnatal stress on hippocampal volume in young adulthood and evaluate the potential modulatory role of sex. We focused on stress stemming from stressful life events during the prenatal or early postnatal period, low birth weight, which is known to be related to prenatal stress43,44, and symptoms of maternal postpartum depression including dysregulated mood and wellbeing, or anxiety and co-dependence.

As a secondary objective, we aimed to determine whether these effects might differ across the different hippocampal subfields. Most neuroimaging studies of early stress focused on potential abnormalities in the whole hippocampus, but the dorsal and ventral sectors of hippocampus have different functions45. Moreover, specialized function and different histological characteristics have been reported also in the different hippocampal subfields46,47. For example, the CA1 neuronal somata in the pyramidal layer have an ovoid shape and are populated sparsely, the CA2 neuronal somata are triangular in shape and larger than the somata of CA1, the CA3 neuronal somata are similar to those in CA2 but are more sparsely packed and the pyramidal layer is thicker than in CA2, and the somata in DG appear similar to those in CA3 but are more ovoid and sparse. Functionally, while CA1 serves for novelty detection and allocentric encoding48,49, CA3 and dentate gyrus serve for pattern separation and completition5052. Thus, examining the effects of early stress on different hippocampal subfields may clarify the role of early stress on hippocampus and related behavior. Animal research showed that stress was related specifically to damage in the dentate gyrus, which contains multipotent adult neural stem cells and is thus the key region for neurogenesis53,54, and the CA3 subfield, which is the main target for glucocorticoids55,56. For example, rats subjected to physical restraint showed reduced branching and atrophy in CA357 and tree shrews exposed to chronic psychosocial stress showed reduced dendritic length and number of branch points in CA314. In humans, childhood maltreatment was associated with volume reductions in dentate gyrus, CA3 and subiculum58. Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) was associated with smaller dentate gyrus and CA3 but not other hippocampal subfields, implying that PTSD is associated with selective volume loss59. Further research demonstrated that cortisol predicted volume of some (CA3, hippocampal head) but not other hippocampal subfields60. Whether exposure to prenatal or early postnatal stress might be associated with alteration of the same hippocampal subfields in humans is a critical but yet unanswered question that would clarify whether hippocampal subfields have unique sensitive periods when they are highly susceptible to the effects of early life stress.

Results

Prenatal stress and hippocampal volume in young adulthood

Means and standard deviations for the different measures of prenatal stress (birth weight, stressful life events in the first and second half of pregnancy) are provided in Table 1. There were no sex differences in any of these measures. There was also no relationship between the measures of prenatal stress and brain size corrected hippocampal volume (see all statistics in Table 2) or any interactions with sex. Correlations between the different measures of prenatal and early postnatal stress are presented in Supplementary Table 1.

Table 1.

Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) for the different measures of prenatal and early postnatal stress (not log-transformed) by sex.

Measure of prenatal stress Male offspring Female offspring Sex difference
M, SD M, SD
Birth weight (g) M = 3482.46, SD = 541.20 M = 3236.43, SD = 491.07 Not significant.
Stressful life events during first half of pregnancy M = 0.25, SD = 0.27 M = 0.20, SD = 0.17 Not significant.
Stressful life events during second half of pregnancy M = 0.17, SD = 0.13 M = 0.17, SD = 0.15 Not significant.
Measure of early postnatal stress M, SD M, SD
Stressful life events during first six months after birth M = 0.19, SD = 0.15 M = 0.17, SD = 0.13 Not significant.
Stressful life events during 6 to 18 months after birth M = 0.27, SD = 0.22 M = 0.25, SD = 0.21 Not significant.
Anxiety and co-dependence during first weeks after birth M = 0.25, SD = 0.27 M = 0.20, SD = 0.17 Not significant.
Dysregulated mood and wellbeing during first weeks after birth M = 0.80, SD = 0.30 M = 0.79, SD = 0.34 Not significant.
Dysregulated mood and wellbeing at six months after birth M = 0.80, SD = 0.34 M = 0.70, SD = 0.27 Not significant.
Dysregulated mood and wellbeing at 18 months after birth M = 0.79, SD = 0.31 M = 0.79, SD = 0.31 Not significant.

Table 2.

Prenatal and early postnatal stress and their relationships with hippocampal gray matter (GM) volume (corrected for brain size) in young adulthood.

Measure of prenatal stress Sample size Left hippocampus Right hippocampus
Birth weight 126 beta = −0.12, p = 0.19 beta = −0.03, p = 0.71
Stressful life events during 1st half of pregnancy 93 beta = −0.11, p = 0.29 beta = −0.09, p = 0.41
Stressful life events during 2nd half of pregnancy 122 beta = −0.07, p = 0.44 beta = −0.08, p = 0.38
Measure of early postnatal stress Left hippocampus Right hippocampus
Stressful life events during first 6 months after birth 124 beta = −0.007, p = 0.94 beta = −0.08, p = 0.37
Stressful life events during 6–18 months after birth 117 beta = 0.003, p = 0.97 beta = 0.0006, p = 0.99
Anxiety/co-dependence during first weeks after birth 122 beta = −0.25, p = 0.006, R2 = 0.06 beta = −0.24, p = 0.007, R2 = 0.06
Dysregulated mood and wellbeing during first weeks after birth 119 beta = −0.02, p = 0.81 beta = −0.06, p = 0.54
Dysregulated mood and wellbeing at 6 months after birth 124 beta = −0.10, p = 0.27 beta = −0.07, p = 0.47
Dysregulated mood and wellbeing at 18 months after birth 117 beta = −0.07, p = 0.44 beta = −0.08, p = 0.37

Early postnatal stress and hippocampal volume in young adulthood

Means and standard deviations for the different measures of early postnatal stress, namely stressful life events (measured during the first 6 months and between 6 and 18 months after birth), dysregulated mood and wellbeing (measured during the first weeks, at 6 months, and 18 months after birth), and anxiety and co-dependence (measured during the first weeks after birth) are provided in Table 1. There were no sex differences in any of these measures. There was also no relationship between stressful life events or dysregulated mood and wellbeing in the mother during the early postnatal period and hippocampal volume (see all statistics in Table 2) or any interactions with sex.

Surprisingly, the offspring of mothers with higher levels of anxiety and co-dependence during the first weeks after birth had smaller volume of both left (beta = −0.25, p = 0.006, R2 = 0.06; Fig. 1A) and right (beta-0.24, p = 0.007, R2 = 0.06; Fig. 1B) hippocampus (brain size corrected). These relationships survived the correction for multiple comparisons (p < 0.008) and remained significant even when considering the potential modulatory role of the offspring’s sex. There were no interactions between mother’s anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth and the offspring’s sex (left hippocampus: F(1,118) = 0.36, p = 0.55; right hippocampus: F(1,118) = 0.86, p = 0.36). Additional multivariate regression analyses showed that the effect of Mother’s anxiety/co-dependence on hippocampal volume was independent of the other measures of early postnatal stress (left: beta = −0.20, p = 0.04; right: beta = −0.26, p = 0.009).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Mother’s anxiety/co-dependence and offspring’s hippocampal volume. Offspring of mothers with higher anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth had smaller gray matter volume of both left (1A; beta = −0.25, p = 0.006, R2 = 0.06) and right (1B; beta = −0.24, p = 0.007, R2 = 0.06) hippocampus (corrected for brain size).

Mother’s anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth and volume of hippocampal subfields in young adulthood

Further analyses in the 122 individuals followed up the relationship between mother’s anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth and offspring’s hippocampal volume, trying to determine whether the effects were subfield specific and thus affecting the volumes of hippocampal subfields to a different extent. Means and standard deviations for the volume of different hippocampal subfields are provided in Table 3. MANOVA showed significant interaction between mother’s anxiety/co-dependence and volume of the different subfields (corrected for brain size) in both left (F(11,1320) = 3.63, p < 0.0001) and right (F(11,1320) = 3.48, p < 0.0001) hippocampus. Posthoc analyses revealed that mother’s anxiety/co-dependence was associated with volume of left and right subiculum, CA1, CA2/3, CA4, GC—DG, molecular layer and HATA (see effect sizes in Fig. 2 and all statistics in Table 4). Left and right presubiculum, parasubiculum, hippocampal fissure, hippocampal tail andfimbria showed no relationship with mother’s anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth (see all statistics in Table 4). Again, none of these relationships were modulated by sex.

Table 3.

Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) for the gray matter (GM) volume of different subfields in left and right hippocampus (not corrected for brain size).

Hippocampal subfield GM volume (M, SD)
Left hippocampus Right hippocampus
Parasubiculum M = 71.51, SD = 13.24 M = 66.02, SD = 11.11
Presubiculum M = 340.36, SD = 37.81 M = 314.20, SD = 35.54
Subiculum M = 474.40, SD = 53.67 M = 457.19, SD = 49.02
CA1 M = 700.95, SD = 85.14 M = 720.85, SD = 87.57
CA2/3 M = 233.97, SD = 35.17 M = 246.94, SD = 37.08
CA4 M = 282.79, SD = 33.40 M = 288.54, SD = 34.11
GC-DG M = 332.25, SD = 38.48 M = 336.51, SD = 39.27
HATA M = 75.72, SD = 11.69 M = 71.87, SD = 10.34
Fimbria M = 109.43, SD = 24.51 M = 107.37, SD = 21.64
Molecular layer M = 630.57, SD = 67.62 M = 636.77, SD = 68.93
Hippocampal fissure M = 151.8, SD = 23.56 M = 144.99, SD = 21.88
Hippocampal tail M = 526.69, SD = 60.71 M = 539.27, SD = 71.53
Whole hippocampus M = 3778.60, SD = 388.34 M = 3785.51, SD = 386.50

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Effect of mother’s anxiety/co-dependence on offspring’s GM volume in hippocampal subfields. Offspring of mothers with higher anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth had smaller gray matter volume of left and right subiculum, CA1, CA2/3, CA4, GC—DG, molecular layer and HATA (corrected for brain size).

Table 4.

Mother’s anxiety/co-dependence after birth and gray matter (GM) volume of hippocampal subfields (corrected for brain size).

Hippocampal subfield Sample size Left hippocampus Right hippocampus
Parasubiculum 122 beta = 0.12, p = 0.20 beta = 0.11, p = 0.22
Presubiculum 122 beta = −0.0005, p = 0.99 beta = −0.04, p = 0.65
Subiculum 122 beta = −0.20, p = 0.03, R2 = 0.04 beta = −0.20, p = 0.03, R2 = 0.04
CA1 122 beta = −0.24, p = 0.009, R2 = 0.06 beta = −0.26, p = 0.004, R2 = 0.06
CA2/3 122 beta = −0.25, p = 0.005, R2 = 0.06 beta = −0.22, p = 0.01, R2 = 0.05
CA4 122 beta = −0.29, p = 0.001, R2 = 0.08 beta = −0.25, p = 0.006, R2 = 0.06
GC-DG 122 beta = −0.31, p = 0.0004, R2 = 0.10 beta = −0.27, p = 0.003, R2 = 0.07
HATA 122 beta = −0.21, p = 0.02, R2 = 0.05 beta = −0.19, p = 0.04, R2 = 0.04
Fimbria 122 beta = −0.12, p = 0.20 beta = −0.05, p = 0.62
Molecular layer 122 beta = −0.25, p = 0.006, R2 = 0.06 beta = −0.25, p = 0.006, R2 = 0.06
Hippocampal fissure 122 beta = −0.03, p = 0.76 beta = −0.05, p = 0.57
Hippocampal tail 122 beta = −0.05, p = 0.60 beta = −0.05, p = 0.56

Hippocampal volume and depressive symptomatology in young adulthood

Additional analyses showed that in our sample of typically developing individuals, there was no relationship between depressive symptomatology, as measured with Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), in young adulthood and brain size corrected hippocampal volume (left: beta = 0.11, p = 0.22; right: beta = 0.03, p = 0.72; n = 130) or between depressive symptomatology in young adulthood and mother’s anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth (beta = −0.11, p = 0.21; n = 122).

Discussion

In this study, we have conducted a neuroimaging follow-up of a birth cohort to study the impact of prenatal and early postnatal stress on hippocampal volume in young adulthood. We showed that in typically developing humans, birth weight, stressful life events during prenatal or early postnatal period, and dysregulated mood and wellbeing in the mother during the early postnatal period were not associated with hippocampal volume in young adulthood and that sex did not modulate any of these relationships. Interestingly, mother’s anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth did show a relationship with volume of both left and right hippocampus in young adulthood and these results remained significant irrespective of sex. Further analyses revealed that the effects of mother’s anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth were subfield-specific; present in the hippocampal formation (CA1, CA2/3, CA4, GC-DG, subiculum), molecular layer and HATA but not in presubiculum, parasubiculum, fimbria, hippocampal fissure, or hippocampal tail.

Our results suggest that within typically developing individuals, mother’s anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth might be a much more sensitive predictor of hippocampal volume in young adulthood than the more objective measures of prenatal or early postnatal stress such as birth weight, number of stressful life events or the actual wellbeing of the mother. While the mechanisms are not clear, it might be that mother’s anxiety/co-dependence, characterized by great impact of others on her behavior, might result in her inconsistent behavior towards the infant, who then cannot detect the rules and remember the consequences of different actions, which might possibly represent much greater source of stress for the infant than, for example, a stressful life event in mother’s life.

The negative direction of the relationship between mother’s anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth and hippocampal volume in the offspring is consistent with the negative direction of the relationship between early stress and neurogenesis of the developing brain reported by animal research61. It is also consistent with others62 who showed that children of mothers with increased anxiety during the early postnatal period had smaller left hippocampal volume at 6 months of age. It was suggested these effects might reflect the transgenerational transmission of individual differences in vulnerability to anxiety-related disorders62. The subfield-specific results extend the findings of animal research showing evidence for the relationship between stress and alterations in CA subfields and dentate gyrus63,64 as well as research in humans reporting associations between childhood maltreatment and reduced volume of the CA3 subfield, dentate gyrus, and subiculum58. The lack of sex differences in the effects of early stress on hippocampal volume is in agreement with a review of the effects of prenatal stress on MRI outcome measures65 that showed that only one study so far reported sex differences in MRI outcome measures, namely the effect of higher cortisol at 15 weeks of gestation on larger volume of right amygdala in female but not male offspring34.

While the effect of mother’s anxiety/co-dependence was the same in left and right hippocampus (R2 = 0.06), the subfield-specific analyses showed a slightly larger effect size in the left (vs. right) CA2/3, CA4, GC-DG and HATA (see Table 4). This laterality is consistent with previous research66 showing that exposure to childhood maltreatment had the strongest associations with the volume of left CA4-DG and left CA2–3. The potential reasons why would the elevated levels of circulating corticosterone not affect both sides equally might include the differential distribution of NMDA and Glu receptor in the left vs. right hippocampus described by animal research67.

Previous studies reported small hippocampal volumes in at-risk adolescents, particularly those who experienced childhood adversity, already before the manifestation of clinical symptoms of major depressive disorder68. Both early-life adversity and smaller hippocampal volume were associated with a higher probability of depressive episodes during prospective follow-up68. In our sample of typically developing young adults, we did not observe any associations between depressive symptomatology, as measured with Beck Depression Inventory, and the brain size corrected hippocampal volume or between mother’s anxiety/co-dependence during the first weeks after birth and the brain size corrected hippocampal volume. While this absence of a relationship might be, in part, due to the relatively low variability in depressive symptomatology among the typically developing young adults, it is consistent with other structural studies that investigated the relations between depressive symptomatology and hippocampal volume and found no relationships6972. Future research, designed as a prospective follow-up of our sample might try to determine whether individuals with smaller hippocampal volumes who were exposed to maternal anxiety/co-dependence after birth might develop depressive symptomatology in later life. Deficits in the function of hippocampus and the related negative affect network were characteristic for individuals with depressive symptomatology10,73. Recent research showed that reductions in hippocampal volume were apparent only in patients with an illness duration of at least 2.5 years or more than one episode of depression74.

Since hippocampal size is highly genetically determined23,30, further research should also clarify whether there might be any genetic modulators or mediators explaining our findings. For example, SNP rs7294919, which is known to have a particularly strong link to hippocampal volume75,76, might be a good candidate. Each copy of the T allele was associated with a 107.8 mm3 decrease in hippocampal volume68. The two neighbouring genes of rs7294919 are involved in apoptosis of neurons and hippocampal neuron dendrite growth, suggesting the possible mechanisms of action68. It might be that SNP rs7294919 might modulate the effects of mother’s anxiety/co-dependence on hippocampal volume.

The current study has a number of strengths. First, it was designed as a neuroimaging follow-up of a prenatal birth cohort, which means that our relatively large sample size consisted of individuals from a very similar background (all White Caucassians, typically developing, growing up in the same area and due to the early postcommunist era in Czechoslovakia in the early 90 s, they were born into families with very similar socioeconomical status) and a very narrow age range (23 or 24 years old), thus eliminating the effects of well documented age-related hippocampal atrophy77,78. Second, the unique data from European Longitudinal Study of Pregnancy and Childhood (ELSPAC-CZ) allowed us to draw on several approaches how to measure exposure to prenatal and early postnatal stress and evaluate their effects. Third, all measures of prenatal and early postnatal stress were based on data collected in the early 90 s, thus eliminating any possible false memories and recall bias. Fourth, we reported not only the effects on hippocampal volume but also the effects on individual hippocampal subfields, which are known to have different functions47. Fifth, the use of a software to measure the volume of hippocampus and its subfields allowed us to produce results that can be easily reproduced between laboratories.

The possible limitation of our study is the fact that the questionnaires assessing prenatal and early postnatal stress differ from the commonly used questionnaires on depression, social anxiety or co-dependence such as Beck Depression Inventory79 (BDI), Social Interaction Anxiety Scale80 (SIAS) or the Spann-Fischer Codependency Scale81 (SF CDS), however, the complete translation of the questionnaires is provided in the Supplementary methods. Another limitation is the fact that we cannot control for earlier/later maternal anxiety/co-dependence and thus determine whether the effects of mother’s anxiety/co-dependence are specific to early postnatal experiences. We hypothesize that while symptoms of mother’s anxiety/co-dependence might, in part, characterize mother’s behavior also prenatally and later in life, their effects on the offspring’s hippocampus would be particularly pronounced during the sensitive period of first weeks after birth. It is also important to note that while our previous work82 with this sample demonstrated an effect of prenatal stressful life events on the brain and particularly three brain regions known to be hypometabolic in depressed patients vs. healthy controls (i.e. mid-dorsolateral frontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and precuneus), the birth weight of our typically developing participants was in the normal range (M = 3346.85, SD = 526.62; viz Table 1) and this might have limited the possibility to show an effect of birth weight on hippocampal volume.

We conclude that in sharp contrast to numerous animal models, within typically developing humans, birth weight, stressful life events during prenatal or early postnatal period, and dysregulated mood and wellbeing in the mother during the early postnatal period were not associated with hippocampal volume in young adulthood. But, mother’s anxiety and co-dependence during the first weeks after birth did show long-lasting effects on the hippocampal volume in the offspring irrespective of sex and these effects were most pronounced in the hippocampal subfields identified by translational research as most stress- and glucocorticoid-sensitive63,64. Our findings provide evidence that the type of early stress is critical when studying its effects on the human brain, possibly explaining the inconsistency in the literature.

Methods

Participants

Typically developing young adults from the European Longitudinal Study of Pregnancy and Childhood, the Czech Republic83,84 (ELSPAC–CZ), a prenatal cohort from Czech Republic whose members were born between 1991 and 1992, were invited to participate in a neuroimaging study Biomarkers and underlying mechanisms of vulnerability to depression (VULDE; FP7-IEF-2013) at Central European Institute of Technology, Masaryk University. A total of 131 individuals (61 males, 70 females) completed the neuroimaging protocol in 2015. All of the participants were 23 or 24 years old and of White Caucasian background. Ethical approval for the VULDE study was obtained from ELSPAC Ethics Committee and written informed consent was obtained from all the participants, including the agreement to merge data from VULDE with their historic data from ELSPAC-CZ. The methods of the study described bellow were in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Measures of prenatal and early postnatal stress

Between 1990 and 1994, mothers of our participants answered number of questionnaires regarding possible indicators of prenatal and early postnatal stress including stressful life events, dysregulated mood and wellbeing, and anxiety/co-dependence and provided information about the birth weight of the offspring (data available for 126 out of the 131 participants). The questionnaire about stressful life events was filled in at four time points to reflect stressful life events during the first (completed by 93 out of the 131 mothers) and second (completed by 122 out of the 131 mothers) half of pregnancy, during the first 6 months after birth (completed by 124 out of the 131 mothers), and during 6–18 months after birth (completed by 117 out of the 131 mothers). It consisted of 40 questions on stressful events such as break up or divorce with the partner, consideration of abortion, violence, serious illness or death in the family or financial difficulties answered by a 5-point Likert scale. The questionnaire on dysregulated mood and wellbeing was filled in at three time points, namely at first weeks after birth (completed by 120 out of the 131 mothers), at 6 months after birth (completed by 124 out of the 131 mothers), and at 18 months after birth (completed by 117 out of the 131 mothers) to reflect dysregulated mood and wellbeing in the past month. This questionnaire included 33 questions on anger, panic, sadness, fatigue, digestive problems, appetite, sleep and self-harming thoughts answered by a 4-point Likert scale. The questionnaire on Anxiety/co-dependence was filled in only during the first weeks after birth and completed by 122 out of the 131 mothers. It included 36 questions on anxious behavior and co-dependence such as worried about other people’s opinions, change behavior in order to please others, insecure when meeting new people, worried to say what she thinks because others might not like her, worried to be criticized, anxious when saying good bye, worried to lose a friend, all answered on a 4-point Likert scale. See the full list of questions in the three different questionnaires in Supplementary Methods.

MRI Acquisition

In 2015, all participants were scanned using a 3 T Siemens Prisma MRI scanner. T1-weighted MPRAGE images of the whole brain were acquired with 64 channel head/neck coil using the following acquisition parameters: voxel size 1 × 1 × 1 mm, repetition time (TR) 2300 ms, echo time (TE) 2.34 ms, inversion time (TI) 900 ms, flip angle 8 degrees.

Analyses

T1-weighted data were processed using Freesurfer version 6.085 and the volume of total left and total right hippocampus was calculated. Volume of the different hippocampal subfields (left and right parasubiculum, presubiculum, subiculum, cornu ammonis [CA] 1, CA2/3, CA4, granule cell layer of the dentate gyrus [GC/DG], hippocampal-amygdaloid transition area [HATA], fimbria, molecular layer, hippocampal fissure, hippocampal tail) were calculated according to Iglesias et al.47. Briefly, this automated analysis of MRI data is based on a construction of a statistical atlas of the hippocampal subfields using ultra-high resolution MRI and then building an algorithm based on Bayesian inference47. This atlas was released as part of FreeSurfer (version 6.0) and its applicability and accuracy has been demonstrated47, thus offering an alternative to the laborious manual tracing that has been the gold standard for measuring total hippocampal volume in the past and providing a tool that would allow easy replication across laboratories86. Segmentation of the hippocampal subfields was visually inspected and all subjects passed the quality control.

All subsequent statistical analyses were done in JMP version 10.0.0 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC). Volumes of left and right hippocampus as well as the subfields were corrected for the total brain volume. Mean scores of the prenatal and early postnatal stressful life events variables had to be log transformed to follow normal distribution. We used two-sided hypothesis tests and significance level of 0.05. With our sample size and power 80% we should have been able to detect medium size or larger effects. First, univariate linear regression assessed associations between early life stress and total left and total right volume of the hippocampus. Potential interactions with sex were assessed. Next, multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) assessed associations between the significant predictors of hippocampal volume and volumes of the different subfields in left and right hippocampus. Finally, additional posthoc analyses assessed the potential relationship between hippocampal volume, its predictors, and depressive symptomatology measured by Beck Depression Inventory79 (BDI) in young adulthood.

Data availability

The datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary Information (293.3KB, pdf)

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the European Union (Marie Curie Intra-European Fellowship for Career Development FP7-IEF-2013) and the Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports of the Czech Republic/MEYS (CEITEC 2020, LQ1601, LM2015051).

Author Contributions

K.M. designed the study, collected and analysed the data, and wrote the manuscript. R.M. run the Freesurfer analysis. P.B. was involved in data collection. J.K., L.D. and M.B. reviewed the manuscript.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-018-23046-6.

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Barker DJ, Winter PD, Osmond C, Margetts B, Simmonds SJ. Weight in infancy and death from ischaem heart disease. Lancet. 1989;2:577–580. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(89)90710-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lahiri DK, Maloney B, Zawia NH. The LEARn model: an epigenetic explanation for idiopathic neurobiological diseases. Mol. Psychiatry. 2009;14:992–1003. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Van O,J, Jones P, Lewis G, Wadsworth M, Murray R. Developmental precursors of affective illness in a general population birth cohort. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1997;54(7):625–631. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830190049005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Watson JB, Mednick SA, Huttunen M, Wang X. Prenatal teratogens and the development of adult mental illness. Dev. Psychopathol. 1999;11(3):457–466. doi: 10.1017/S0954579499002151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Brown AS, van Os J, Driessens C, Hoek HW, Susser ES. Further evidence of relation between prenatal famine and major affective disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2000;157(2):190–195. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.2.190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Gilman, S. E. et al. Prenatal immune programming of the sex-dependent risk for major depression. Transl Psychiatry. 6(5) (2016a). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 7.Gilman SE, Hornig M, Ghassabian A, Hahn J, Cherkerzian S, Alber tPS, et al. Socioeconomic disadvantage, gestational immune activity, and neurodevelopment in early childhood. PNAS. 2016;114(26):6728–6733. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1617698114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lai MC, Huang LT. Effects of early life stress on neuroendocrine and neurobehavior: mechanisms and implications. Pediatr. Neonatol. 2011;52:122–129. doi: 10.1016/j.pedneo.2011.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Strüber N, Strüber D, Roth G. Impact of early adversity on glucocorticoid regulation and later mental disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014;38:17–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.MacQueen G, Frodl T. The hippocampus in major depression: evidence for the convergence of the bench and bedside in psychiatric research? Molecular Psychiatry. 2011;16:252–264. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Thomas RM, Hotsenpiller G, Peterson DA. Acute psychosocial stress reduces cell survival in adult hippocampal neurogenesis without altering proliferation. J Neurosci. 2007;27:2734–2743. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3849-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Woolley CS, Gould E, McEwen BS. Exposure to excess glucocorticoids alters dendritic morphology of adult hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Brain Res. 1990;531:225–231. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90778-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Watanabe Y, Gould E, McEwen BS. Stress induces atrophy of apical dendrites of hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons. Brain Res. 1992;588:341–345. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)91597-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Magarinos AM, McEwen BS, Flugge G, Fuchs E. Chronic psychosocial stress causes apical dendritic atrophy of hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons in subordinate tree shrews. J Neurosci. 1996;16:3534–3540. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-10-03534.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Dranovsky A, Hen R. Hippocampal neurogenesis: regulation by stress and antidepressants. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:1136–1143. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Duman RS. Depression: a case of neuronal life and death? Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56:140–145. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.02.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lee T, Jarome T, Li SJ, Kim JJ, Helmstetter FJ. Chronic stress selectively reduces hippocampal volume in rats: a longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroreport. 2009;20:1554–1558. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328332bb09. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ohl F, Michaelis T, Vollmann-Honsdorf GK, Kirschbaum C, Fuchs E. Effect of chronic psychosocial stress and long-term cortisol treatment on hippocampus-mediated memory and hippocampal volume: a pilot-study in tree shrews. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2000;25:357–363. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(99)00062-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Malberg JE, Duman RS. Cell proliferation in adult hippocampus is decreased by inescapable stress: reversal by fluoxetine treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:1562–1571. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Vermetten E, Bremner JD. Circuits and systems in stress. I. Preclinical studies. Depress Anxiety. 2002;15:126–147. doi: 10.1002/da.10016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Szeszko PR, et al. Increased stress and smaller anterior hippocampal volume. Neuroreport. 2006;17:1825–1828. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000246322.58814.b8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ganzel BL, Kim P, Glover GH, Temple E. Resilience after 9/11: multimodal neuroimaging evidence for stress-related change in the healthy adult brain. Neuroimage. 2008;40:788–795. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Gilbertson MW, et al. Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:1242–1247. doi: 10.1038/nn958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Koolschijn PC, van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Crone EA. Hippocampal volume and internalizing behavior problems in adolescence. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2013;23(7):622–628. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Geuze E, Vermetten E, Bremner JD. MR-based in vivo hippocampal volumetrics: 2. Findings in neuropsychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:160–184. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Teicher MH, et al. The neurobiological consequences of early stress and childhood maltreatment. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003;27:33–44. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(03)00007-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cohen S, et al. Adverse early life environment increases hippocampal microglia abundance in conjunction with decreased neural stem cells in juvenile mice. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2016;55:56–65. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2016.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Sanchez MM, Hearn EF, Do D, Rilling JK, Herndon JG. Differential rearing affects corpus callosum size and cognitive function of rhesus monkeys. Brain Res. 1998;812:38–49. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00857-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Spinelli S, et al. Early-life stress induces long-term morphologic changes in primate brain. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:658–665. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lyons DM, Yang C, Sawyer-Glover AM, Moseley ME, Schatzberg AF. Early life stress and inherited variation in monkey hippocampal volumes. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:1145–1151. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.12.1145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Woon FL, Hedges DW. Hippocampal and amygdala volumes in children and adults with childhood maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis. Hippocampus. 2008;18:729–736. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Cohen RA, et al. Early life stress and morphometry of the adult anterior cingulate cortex and caudate nuclei. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:975–982. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Favaro A, Tenconi E, Degortes D, Manara R, Santonastoso P. Neural correlates of prenatal stress in young women. Psychol Med. 2015;45(12):2533–2543. doi: 10.1017/S003329171500046X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Buss C, et al. Maternal cortisol over the course of pregnancy and subsequent child amygdala and hippocampus volumes and affective problems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:E1312–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1201295109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lenze SN, Xiong C, Sheline YI. Childhood adversity predicts earlier onset of major depression but not reduced hippocampal volume. Psychiatry Res. 2008;162:39–49. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Vythilingam M, et al. Childhood trauma associated with smaller hippocampal volume in women with major depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:2072–2080. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Constantinof A, Moisiadis VG, Matthews SG. Programming of stress pathways: A transgenerational perspective. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol. 2016;160:175–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jsbmb.2015.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bock J, Wainstock T, Braun K, Segal M. Stress in utero: prenatal programming of brain plasticity and cognition. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78(5):315–326. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Goldstein JM, Handa RJ, Tobet SA. Disruption of fetal hormonal programming (prenatal stress) implicates shared risk for sex differences in depression and cardiovascular disease. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2014;35:140–158. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Shors TJ, Leuner B. Estrogen-mediated effects on depression and memory formation in females. J Affect Disord. 2003;74:85–96. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00428-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Derks NA, Krugers HJ, Hoogenraad CC, Joels M, Sarabdijitsingh RA. Effects of early life stress on synaptic plasticity in the developing hippocampus of male and female rats. PloS One. 2016;11(10):1–17. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0164551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Takuma K, et al. 17beta-estradiol attenuates hippocampal neuronal loss and cognitive dysfunction induced by chronic restraint stress in ovariectomized rats. Neuroscience. 2007;146:60–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Engel SM, Berkowitz GS, Wolff MS, Yehuda R. Psychological trauma associated with the World Trade Center attacks and its effect on pregnancy outcome. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2005;19:334–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2005.00676.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Sandman CA, Davis EP, Buss C, Glynn LM. Exposure to prenatal psychobiological stress exerts programming influences on the mother and her fetus. Neuroendocrinology. 2012;95:7–21. doi: 10.1159/000327017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Grigoryan, G., & Segal, M. Lasting differential effects on plasticity induced by prenatal stress in dorsal and ventral hippocampus. Neural Plast. 1–10 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 46.Adler DH, et al. Histology-derived volumetric annotation of the human hippocampal subfields in post-mortem MRI. Neuroimage. 2013;84:505–523. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Iglesias JE, et al. A computational atlas of the hippocampal formation using ex vivo, ultra-high resolution MRI: Application to adaptive segmentation of in vivo MRI. Neuroimage. 2015;115:117–137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lisman JE, Otmakhova NA. Storage, recall, and novelty detection of sequences by the hippocampus: elaborating on the SOCRATIC model to account for normal and aberrant effects of dopamine. Hippocampus. 2001;11(5):551–568. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Suthana NA, Ekstrom AD, Moshirvaziri S, Knowlton B, Bookheimer SY. Human hippocampal CA1 involvement during allocentric encoding of spatial information. J Neurosci. 2009;29(34):10512–10519. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0621-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Bakker A, Kirwan CB, Miller M, Stark CEL. Pattern separation in the human hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus. Science. 2008;319(5870):1640–1642. doi: 10.1126/science.1152882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S, Moser MB, Moser EI. Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science. 2007;315(5814):961–966. doi: 10.1126/science.1135801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Yassa MA, et al. High-resolution structural and functional MRI of hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage. 2010;51(3):1242–1252. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Duan X, Kang E, Liu CY, Ming GL, Song H. Development of neural stem cell in the adult brain. Curr Opm Neurobiol. 2008;18(1):108–115. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Abrous DN, Koehl M, Le Moal M. Adult neurogenesis: from precursors to network and physiology. Physiol Rev. 2005;85(2):523–569. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00055.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Gould E, Tanapat P, McEwen BS, Flugge G, Fuchs E. Proliferation of granule cell precursors in the dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is diminished by stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95(6):3168–3171. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.6.3168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Shiryaeva NV, Vshivtseva W, Mal’tsev NA, Sukhorukov VN, Vaido A. Neuron density in the hippocampus in rat strains with contrasting nervous system excitability after prolonged emotional-pain stress. Neurosci Behav Physiol. 2008;38(4):355–357. doi: 10.1007/s11055-008-0049-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Magarinos AM, McEwen BS. Stress-induced atrophy of apical dendrites of hippocampal CA3c neurons: involvement of glucocorticoid secretion and excitatory amino acid receptors. Neuroscience. 1995;69(1):89–98. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00259-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Teicher MH, Anderson CM, Polcari A. Childhood maltreatment is associated with reduced volume in the hippocampal subfields CA3, dentate gyrus, and subiculum. PNAS. 2012;109(9):E563–572. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1115396109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Wang Z, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging of hippocampal subfields in posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67(3):296–303. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Travis SG, et al. Effects of cortisol on hippocampal subfields volumes and memory performance in healthy control subjects and patients with major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2016;201:34–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.04.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Pryce R, Aubert Y, Maier C, Pearce PC, Fuchs E. The developmental impact of prenatal stress, prenatal dexamethasone and postnatal social stress on physiology, behaviour and neuroanatomy of primate offspring: studies in rhesus macaque and common marmoset. Psychopharmacology. 2011;214(1):33–53. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1989-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Qiu A, et al. Maternal anxiety and infants’ hippocampal development: timing matters. Transl Psychiatry. 2013;3(9):e306. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.McEwen BS. The neurobiology of stress: From serendipity to clinical relevance. Brain Res. 2000;886:172–189. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02950-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.McEwen BS. Sex, stress and the hippocampus: Allostasis, allostatic load and the aging process. Neurobiol Aging. 2002;23:921–939. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(02)00027-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Scheinost D, et al. Prenatal stress alters amygdala functional connectivity in preterm neonates. Neuroimage Clin. 2016;12:381–388. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.08.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Teicher MH, Anderson CM, Polcari A. Childhood maltreatment is associated with reduced volume in the hippocampal subfields CA3, dentate gyrus, and subiculum. PNAS. 2012;109(9):E563–E572. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1115396109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Kawakami R, et al. Asymmetrical allocation of NMDA receptor epsilon2 subunits in hippocampal circuitry. Science. 2003;300:990–994. doi: 10.1126/science.1082609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Rao U, et al. Hippocampal changes associated with early-life adversity and vulnerability to depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:357–364. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.10.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.MacQueen GM, et al. Course of illness, hippocampal function, and hippocampal volume in major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:1387–1392. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0337481100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Frodl TM, et al. Hippocampal changes in patients with a first episode of major depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1112–1118. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Frodl T. Shaippocampal volume correlates with executive dysfunctioning in major depression. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2006;31:316–323. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Caetano SC, Hatch JP, Brambilla P, Sassi RB, Nicoletti M, Mallinger AG, et al. Anatomical MRI study of hippocampus and amygdala in patients with current and remitted major depression. Psychiatry Res. 2004;132:141–147. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2004.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Mareckova K, et al. Brain activity and connectivity in response to negative affective stimuli: Impact of dysphoric mood and sex across diagnoses. Human Brain Mapping. 2016;37(11):3733–3744. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.McKinnon MC, Yucel K, Nazarov A, MacQueen GM. A meta-analysis examining clinical predictors of hippocampal volume in patients with major depressive disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2009;34:41–54. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Bis JC, et al. Common variants at 12q14 and 12q24 are associated with hippocampal volume. Nature Genetics. 2012;44:545–551. doi: 10.1038/ng.2237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Stein JL, et al. Identification of common variants associated with human hippocampal and intracranial volumes. Nature Genetics. 2012;44:552–561. doi: 10.1038/ng.2250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Du AT, et al. Age effects on atrophy rates of entorhinal cortex and hippocampus. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27(5):733–740. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Morra JH, et al. Automated mapping of hippocampal atrophy in 1 -year repeat MRI data from 490 subjects with Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, and elderly controls. Neuroimage. 2009;45:S3–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1961;4:561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Mattick RP, Clarke JC. Development and validation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1998;36(4):455–470. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10031-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Fischer J, Spann L, Crawford D. Measuring codependency. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly. 1991;8:87–99. doi: 10.1300/J020V08N01_06. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Mareckova, K. et al. Prenatal stress, mood, and gray matter volume in young adulthood. Cerebral Cortex, 10.1093/cercor/bhy030 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 83.Golding J. European Longitudinal Study of Pregnancy and Childhood (ELSPAC) Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology. 1989;3(4):460–469. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.1989.tb00533.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Piler, P. et al. Cohort profile: European Longitudinal Study of Pregnancy and Childhood (ELSPAC) in the Czech Republic. International Journal of Epidemiology. 1–7, 10.1093/ije/dyw091 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 85.Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97(20):11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Van Leemput K, et al. Automated segmentation of hippocampal subfields from ultra-high resolution in vivo MRI. Hippocampus. 2009;19:549–557. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information (293.3KB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES