Abstract
Background and objectives
The world’s growing elderly population highlights the necessity for further attention to the psychological problems of the elderly, such as death anxiety. Analysis of the elderly’s perception of aging and associated factors can contribute to prediction of their future physical and mental health. The purpose of this study was to determine the relationship of perception of aging, and a group of demographic factors, with death anxiety in the elderly living in Gonabad, Iran.
Subjects and methods
This analytical descriptive study was conducted on 330 elderly residents of Gonabad, who were selected by stratified random sampling. Research tools were a standard demographic questionnaire, Barker’s Brief Aging Perceptions Questionnaire, and Collett–Lester Fear of Death Scale. Data were collected by interview of respondents at their home. Data analysis was carried out in SPSS 16 using descriptive statistics, analysis of variance, Pearson’s test, independent t-test, and linear regression.
Results
Respondents had a mean age of 73.97±7.68 years, 42.4% of respondents were men and 57.6% were women. The total Barker’s Brief Aging Perceptions Questionnaire score was 63.18±8.75, with the highest score (17.10±4.70) associated with negative consequences and control. The total Collett–Lester Fear of Death Scale score was 128±14.80, with the highest score (35.13±4.06) pertaining to the subscale fear of other people’s death. Regression results indicated that the death anxiety score was predictable according to the age and all dimensions of perception of aging, except for the consequences and negative control dimension.
Conclusion
Age and the perception of aging are good predictors of death anxiety. The authors recommend further research on the determinants of death anxiety in the elderly and the development of a comprehensive care plan to reduce this anxiety among Iranian elderly.
Keywords: perception of aging, death anxiety, elderly
Introduction
A steady rise in life expectancy and a decline in birth rate have led to gradual growth of the proportion of the elderly among the world’s total population. The traces of this demographic trend can also be seen in developing countries such as Iran, where the latest census (2011) shows a rise in the share of elderly population from 6.7% to 8.2% over a 5-year period. Population and demographic analyses anticipate a surge in the Iranian elderly population in the next 30 years and predict an elderly population ratio of 21.7% by 2021.1
One of the important domains of the elderly health is its psychological dimension, which requires special attention and prevention of disorders such as anxiety. For many reasons, older adults are vulnerable to mental illness, and about 15%–25% of them have serious psychological problems.2
Anxiety is one of the most common mental problems of old age because people face a variety of feelings of deficiencies and disabilities at this stage of life. Studies show that older adults are more exposed to anxiety due to reduced self-confidence, reduced activity and movement, loss of friends, reduced financial and physical independence, and chronic illnesses. Their most common anxiety is death anxiety.3
Death is a biological and psychological fact, but thinking of death is dreadful and most people prefer to avoid such thoughts.4 Everyone is somewhat anxious about death, as it is a unique phenomenon that they will certainly experience; but different persons may feel different degrees of death anxiety.5 Death anxiety has been defined as an unusually intensive fear of death, accompanied by feelings of apprehension or anxiety when the person thinks about the process of dying or what happens after death.6
Death anxiety is a natural and normal experience, and having a small degree of death anxiety is necessary throughout life to push us to engage in positive activities, but an abnormally high death anxiety may lead to maladaptation. Such anxiety intensifies the stressful conditions and causes a feeling of emptiness and frustration about life.5 Research has shown that death anxiety is a common phenomenon. For example, in a study by Agras et al, 16% of people had death anxiety and 3% had comorbid anxiety, and death anxiety was found to be stronger in women than in men.7
In a study by Godishala and Swath, 47% of elderly in and out of nursing homes had mild death anxiety, and 52% had moderate death anxiety.8 In a study by Depoula et al on the relationship of death anxiety with gender, age, and religiosity, it was found that women, older people, and less religious people have higher death anxiety. These groups also showed higher anxiety about different dimensions of death, such as shortness of life, complete isolation due to death, fear of inexistence, and disintegration of the body after death.9
Another important factor that affects the mental health of the elderly is the perception of aging,10 which refers to each person’s perception of the aging process within a sociocultural context.11 Understanding the experience and perception of aging is essential for understanding the health and behavioral implications of aging and a subjective sense of well-being and individual identity in middle-aged and elderly people. The perception of aging can serve as a measure to evaluate the person’s satisfaction with aging and reflects the individual’s adaptation to the changes associated with aging.12 It is commonly believed that any older person who is satisfied with his aging and has a positive attitude toward this stage of life is likely to have a better psychological condition than a person who has a negative attitude toward this issue.13 Understanding the concept and determinants of the perception of aging, as a good predictor of health condition, can contribute to improving the elderly’s satisfaction with aging and their adaptation to age-related changes.14
Many researchers believe that familiarity with an elderly’s perception of aging allows nurses and health care workers to better anticipate the care necessary for his health and well-being.5
In view of the importance of perception of aging and death anxiety and their likely association with demographic factors, this study aimed to determine the relationship of perception of aging and death anxiety with these factors among the elderly of Gonabad, Iran.
Subjects and methods
This analytical descriptive study was conducted on 330 elderly people supported by Gonabad’s community health centers from February to March 2017. Prior to the research, the necessary approval was obtained from the ethics committee of Gonabad University of Medical Sciences (IR.GMU. REC.1395.103).
Perceptions of aging and death anxiety among this group of the elderly were evaluated with the brief version of Barker’s Aging Perceptions Questionnaire (B-APQ) and Collett–Lester Fear of Death Scale (CL-FODS), respectively. The inclusion criterion was age of ≥60 years, and the exclusion criteria were a recorded history of drug abuse, consumption of psychiatric drugs, existing diagnosis of terminal illness, and any cognitive impairment that would result in failure to understand questions and answers.
Subjects were randomly selected from men and women over 60 years of age who were supported by Gonabad’s community health centers, and sampling was continued until reaching the desired sample size for every stratum. Questionnaires were filled by two trained researchers. In accordance with the ethical requirements of research, participants were briefed about the subject of questionnaires and were told that they could stop the interview at any point for any reason such as stress and anxiety. Verbal consent was obtained from the elderly. Because most of the elderly were illiterate or had a low level of literacy, the informed consent form was read to them and they expressed their agreement. Also, the elderly who were literate signed the informed consent form. The procedure was approved by the ethics committee.
For the concept of perception of aging, data were collected using the Persian translation of brief version of Aging Perceptions Questionnaire developed by Barker et al (B-APQ), which consists of 17 questions in five dimensions: timeline–chronic/cyclical, positive consequences, positive control (three items), negative consequences and control (five items), and emotional representations (three items). The minimum and maximum scores for the three-item dimensions are 3 and 15, respectively. The scores for the dimension of negative consequences and control are 5 and 25, respectively. The lowest value is 17 and the highest is 85.
This questionnaire has been translated into several languages.15–18 The Persian version of B-APQ has been developed by Sadegh Moghadam et al and has been validated with a Cronbach alpha of 0.75.19
CL-FODS was initially designed in 1969, but we used the revised version of this scale, which consists of 32 items in four subscales based on Likert grading. These four scales include one’s own death, one’s own dying process, other people’s death, and other people’s dying process. Each scale has eight items. The range of scores for each subscale is between 8 and 40 points, and between 32 and 160 points in total.
The Persian version of CL-FODS has been developed by Naderi and Esmaili, and has been validated with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.89.14
Data were statistically analyzed in SPSS software version 16. Descriptive statistics were used to determine the frequency, mean, and SDs of the variables. Pearson’s coefficient was used for interval variables, independent t-test for comparing the means in two independent groups, analysis of variance (ANOVA) test for comparing the means in several independent groups, and linear regression test for predicting criterion variables based on predictor variables. The significance level was considered as P<0.05.
Data were analyzed with descriptive statistics methods, ANOVA, Pearson’s test, independent t-test, and linear regression using SPSS 16.
Results
The subjects had a mean age of 73.97±7.68 years, and a total of 42.4% of subjects were men and 57.6% were women. In Table 1, demographic data including age, gender, education, marital status, number of children, and companionship status of the research units are described. The total B-APQ score was 63.18±8.75, with the highest score (17.10±4.70) being in the negative consequences and control dimension (Table 2). The total CL-FODS score was 128±14.80, with the highest score (35.13±4.06) pertaining to the fear of other people’s death subscale. The result of the Pearson correlation test showed no direct relationship between the total B-APQ score and the total CL-FODS score (P=0.052). However, there was a statistically significant relationship between the total CL-FODS score and the B-APQ dimensions such as timeline–chronic/cyclical, positive consequences, negative consequences and control, and emotional representations (P<0.05), as shown in Table 3.
Table 1.
Variable | Frequency | Percentage |
---|---|---|
Age, years | 73.97±7.68 | |
Gender | ||
Male | 140 | 42.4 |
Female | 190 | 57.6 |
Education/literacy | ||
Illiterate | 179 | 54.2 |
Reading and writing | 84 | 25.5 |
Elementary school certificate | 40 | 12.1 |
Middle school certificate | 13 | 3.9 |
High school diploma | 11 | 3.3 |
College/seminary degree | 4 | 0.9 |
Marital status | ||
Single | 45 | 13.6 |
Married | 195 | 59.1 |
Widow/widower | 88 | 26.7 |
Divorced | 2 | 0.6 |
Number of children | ||
0 | 6 | 1.8 |
1–5 | 200 | 60.6 |
>5 | 124 | 37.6 |
Companionship | ||
Alone | 104 | 31.5 |
Spouse | 159 | 48.2 |
Child | 36 | 10.9 |
Spouse and children | 26 | 7.9 |
Relatives | 5 | 0.9 |
Table 2.
Variable | Statistical index
|
Mean | SD | Min | Max |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dimension/subscale | |||||
Death anxiety | Fear of one’s own death | 31.85 | 5.69 | 8 | 42 |
Fear of one’s own dying process | 31.71 | 5.19 | 8 | 40 | |
Fear of other people’s death | 35.13 | 4.06 | 18 | 40 | |
Fear of other people’s dying process | 29.22 | 6.12 | 12 | 40 | |
Total score | 128 | 14.80 | 63 | 160 | |
Perception of aging | Timeline–chronic/cyclical | 11.49 | 3.98 | 3 | 63 |
Positive consequences | 11.00 | 1.59 | 3 | 15 | |
Positive control | 12.38 | 2.09 | 6 | 15 | |
Negative consequences and control | 17.10 | 4.70 | 5 | 25 | |
Emotional representations | 9.20 | 3.77 | 3 | 15 | |
Total score | 63.18 | 8.75 | 32 | 112 |
Abbreviations: B-APQ, Barker’s Brief Aging Perceptions Questionnaire; CL-FODS, Collett–Lester Fear of Death Scale.
Table 3.
B-APQ | CL-FODS
|
|
---|---|---|
R | P-value* | |
Timeline–chronic/cyclical | −0.16 | 0.02 |
Positive consequences | −0.13 | 0.01 |
Positive control | 0.09 | 0.08 |
Negative consequences and control | 0.12 | 0.01 |
Emotional representations | 0.27 | 0.00 |
Total score | 0.10 | 0.052 |
Note:
Pearson’s correlation test.
Abbreviations: B-APQ, Barker’s Brief Aging Perceptions Questionnaire; CL-FODS, Collett–Lester Fear of Death Scale.
Independent t-tests showed that perception of aging and fear of death had no statistically significant relationship with gender, place of residence, or participation in religious activities (P>0.05).
Pearson’s correlation tests revealed a significant relationship between the total CL-FODS score and age (P<0.001), but no such relationship was found for the number of children (P>0.05). There was, however, a significant relationship between the total B-APQ score and the number of children as well as age (P<0.05).
The results of ANOVA showed that death anxiety had a significant relationship with marital status (P<0.001) and the presence of a companion (P<0.001), but not with economic status, education level, income source, or illness (P>0.05).
ANOVA also showed that the perception of aging has a significant relationship with the education level (P=0.01), but not with marital status, economic status, presence of a companion, income source, or illness (P>0.05).
The results of the linear regression test showed that each unit increase in the B-APQ score corresponded to a 0.13 decrease in the CL-FODS score, and each year increase in age corresponded to a 0.25 decrease in the CL-FODS score (Table 4).
Table 4.
Death anxiety variable | Dimensions of perception of aging | B | β | SE | P-value* |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Perception of aging | Timeline–chronic/cyclical | −0.49 | −0.13 | 0.19 | 0.01 |
Positive consequences | −1.02 | −0.11 | 0.50 | 0.04 | |
Positive control | 0.77 | 0.10 | 0.37 | 0.04 | |
Negative consequences and control | 0.33 | 0.10 | 0.16 | 0.05 | |
Emotional representations | 0.88 | 0.22 | 0.21 | <0.001 | |
Age | −0.37 | −0.19 | 0.10 | <0.001 |
Note:
Linear regression test.
Abbreviation: SE, standard error.
Discussion
This study aimed to determine the relationship between the perception of aging and some of the demographic factors associated with death anxiety in older adults in Gonabad, Iran. Regression results indicated that the death anxiety score was predictable according to the age and all dimensions of perception of aging, except for the consequences and negative control dimension.
Accepting the natural aging process and the realistic attitude to the situations associated with this stage (the positive and negative results of aging and the amount of control over the accompanying situations) can reduce death anxiety.20,21
The elderly who are aware of the natural aging process and have accepted their physical and mental status at this stage of life reported less anxiety, even though they had poor physical and mental conditions. The reason is that they have accepted death as an approach to a better life and to get rid of the present painful conditions.20
This study showed that the perception of aging perfects by age and there is a more realistic view of the acceptance of aging conditions. Mental health, of course, helps this understanding. The better the cognitive status and mental health of the elderly, the lower their death and dying anxiety.22 This is because they feel having more control over their health and life and more resistance against depression and fear of death.20
As other studies have shown, with age, one’s perceptions of aging become more stable and realistic.22 Existing reports on the relationship of age with death anxiety are contradictory because some indicate a consistent direct relationship between death anxiety and age, but others imply that this relationship can be expressed as an asymmetric parabola, in the sense that it increases with age, peaks in middle-aged people, and then starts to decrease.23
Our results in regard to the relationship between death anxiety and age are consistent with the findings of Weiss and Hope24 and Wagner and Lorion,25 but the study of Nouhi et al26 did not find such a relationship.
In this study, there was no relationship between gender, death anxiety, and perception of aging. It can be argued that in small communities such as the one targeted in this research (Gonabad), population’s adherence to the tradition of respect for the elderly and people’s acceptance of roles associated with aging lead to a uniform perception of aging among men and women – an argument that is supported by numerous studies that have pointed out the role of culture in people’s perception of aging.22,24
Other studies have mentioned gender differences in death anxiety and the perception of aging. However, some studies have suggested that women have a more negative perception of aging, feel less control over their condition as an elder, and exhibit more concern about their dependencies in old age.27 In contrast, a study by de Freitas et al on the meaning of old age and aging experience among the rural elderly of Ceara (Brazil) showed that women are better in adapting to changes in their social networks and aging developments.10
Limitations of this study included limited sampling in one geographic area and the cultural differences affecting death anxiety and the perception of aging.
Conclusion
The findings of this study show that the perception of aging can predict the anxiety of death among the elderly. When considered alongside the widely accepted notion that death anxiety has significant behavioral and emotional implications, especially for the elderly, this finding highlights the need for a concerted effort to develop a comprehensive care plan aimed at reducing the death anxiety among the Iranian elderly by altering their perception of aging. Understanding the concept and determinants of the perception of aging can contribute to improving the elderly’s satisfaction with aging and their adaptation to age-related changes, but considering the involvement of numerous personal, social, and cultural factors with this perception and the fact that these factors are likely to vary with the community, this notion needs to be subjected to further research in different communities, so as to ensure the effectiveness of programs and measures to be adopted in support of the elderly.
Acknowledgments
This study was funded by the Social Development and Health Promotion Research Center at Gonabad University of Medical Sciences. The authors would like to express their gratitude to all the elderly who participated in this research and their respected families for their sincere cooperation.
Footnotes
Disclosure
The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.
References
- 1.Sadegh Moghadam L, Foroughan M, Mohammadi F, et al. Aging perception in older adults. Iran J Ageing. 2016;10(4):202–209. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bakhtiyari M, Emaminaeini M, Hatami H, Khodakarim S, Sahaf R. Depression and perceived social support in the elderly. Iran J Ageing. 2017;12(2):192–207. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Naderi F, Roushani KH. Spiritual intelligence and social intelligence and death anxiety in elderly women. J Women Cult. 2011;2(6):55–67. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Philip Rice F. Human Development: A Life-Span Approach. Tehran: Arjmand; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Sargent-Cox KA, Anstey KJ, Luszcz MA. Longitudinal change of self-perceptions of aging and mortality. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014;69(2):168–173. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.M ‘Esmaile GP. The effectiveness of therapy on anxiety mean death in the elderly. Cult Couns Psychother. 2012;3(9):53–68. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Agras S, Sylvester D, Oliveau D. The epidemiology of common fears and phobia. Compr Psychiatry. 1969;10(2):151–156. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(69)90022-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Swathi GS. Death anxiety, death depression, geriatric depression and suicidal ideation among institutionalized and noninstitutionalized elders. International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications. 2014;4(10):356–364. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Depaola SJ, Griffin M, Young JR, Neimeyer RA. Death anxiety and attitudes toward the elderly among older adults: the Role of gender and ethnicity. Death Stud. 2003;27(4):335–354. doi: 10.1080/07481180302904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.de Freitas MC, Queiroz TA, de Sousa JA. The meaning of old age and the aging experience of in the elderly. Rev Esc Enferm USP. 2010;44(2):407–412. doi: 10.1590/s0080-62342010000200024. Portuguese. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wurm S, Benyamini Y. Optimism buffers the detrimental effect of negative self-perceptions of ageing on physical and mental health. Psychol Health. 2014;29(7):832–848. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2014.891737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Shaw R, Langman M. Perceptions of being old and the ageing process. Ageing Int. 2017;42(1):115–135. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Banks J, Breeze E, Lessof C, Nazroo J. Retirement, health and relationships of the older population in England: The 2004 English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (Wave 2) Institute for Fiscal Studies; London: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Valaei N, Zalipoor S. The effectiveness of logo therapy on elderly’s death anxiety. J Aging Psych. 2015;1(1):49–55. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sexton E, King-Kallimanis BL, Morgan K, McGee H. Development of the brief ageing perceptions questionnaire (B-APQ): a confirmatory factor analysis approach to item reduction. BMC Geriatr. 2014;14(1):44. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-14-44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Chen X, Hu Y, Zhu D, Li J, Zhou L. Chinese version of the aging perceptions questionnaire (C-APQ): assessment of reliability and validity. Aging Mental Health. 2016;20(6):567–574. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2015.1028332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Slotman A, Cramm JM, Nieboer AP. Validation of the dutch aging perceptions questionnaire and development of a short version. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2015;13(1):54. doi: 10.1186/s12955-015-0248-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ingrand I, Houeto JL, Gil R, Mc Gee H, Ingrand P, Paccalin M. The validation of a French-language version of the aging perceptions questionnaire (APQ) and its extension to a population aged 55 and over. BMC Geriatr. 2012;12(1):17. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-12-17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sadegh Moghadam L, Foroughan M, Mohammadi Shahboulaghi F, Ahmadi F, Sajjadi M, Farhadi A. Validity and reliability of the persian version of the brief aging perceptions questionnaire in Iranian older adults. Clin Interv Aging. 2016;11:507–511. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S101620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lockhart LK, Bookwala J, Fagerlin A, et al. Older adults’ attitudes toward death: links to perceptions of health and concerns about end-of-life issues. Omega (Westport) 2001;43(4):331–347. doi: 10.2190/09B5-CCWE-D5GA-F0MA. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Eakes GG. The relationship between death anxiety and attitudes toward the elderly among nursing staff. Death Stud. 1985;9(2):163–172. doi: 10.1080/07481188508252512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Musaiger AO, D’Souza R. Role of age and gender in the perception of aging: a community-based survey in Kuwait. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2009;48(1):50–57. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2007.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Fortner BV, Neimeyer RA. Death anxiety in older adults: a quantitative review. Death Stud. 1999;23(5):387–411. doi: 10.1080/074811899200920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Weiss BJ, Hope DA. A preliminary investigation of worry content in sexual minorities. J Anxiety Disord. 2011;25(2):244–250. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wagner KD, Lorion RP. Correlates of death anxiety in elderly persons. J Clin Psychology. 1984;40(5):1235–1341. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198409)40:5<1235::aid-jclp2270400519>3.0.co;2-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Nouhi E, Karimi T, Iranmanesh S. Comparing fear of death of the elderly settled in elderly’s home and inhabited in city houses of Isfahan. Iran J Ageing. 2014;8(4):24–31. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Schafer MH, Shippee TP. Age identity, gender, and perceptions of decline: does feeling older lead to pessimistic dispositions about cognitive aging? J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;65B(1):91–96. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]