Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2017 Jun 23;30(2):497–510. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000992

Self-regulation as a Predictor of Patterns of Change in Externalizing Behaviors from Infancy to Adolescence

Nicole B Perry 1, Susan D Calkins 2, Jessica M Dollar 2, Susan P Keane 3, Lilly Shanahan 4
PMCID: PMC5858969  NIHMSID: NIHMS949008  PMID: 28641597

Abstract

We examined associations between specific self-regulatory mechanisms and externalizing behavior patterns from ages 2 to 15 (N=443). The relation between multiple self-regulatory indicators across multiple domains (i.e., physiological, attentional, emotional, and behavioral) at age 2 and at age 5 and group membership in four distinct externalizing trajectories was examined. By examining each of these self-regulatory processes in combination with one another, and therefore accounting for their shared variance, we aimed to better understand which specific self-regulatory skills were associated most strongly with externalizing behavioral patterns. Findings suggest that behavioral inhibitory control and emotion regulation are particularly important in distinguishing between children who show normative declines in externalizing behaviors across early childhood and those who demonstrate high levels through adolescence.

Keywords: externalizing behaviors, externalizing trajectories, self-regulation, emotion regulation, inhibitory control


It is widely accepted that childhood externalizing behavior problems, defined as aggressive, destructive, and oppositional behaviors, are associated with a host of difficulties in adolescence and adulthood (Broidy et al., 2003; Campbell, 2002; Odgers et al., 2008). Although externalizing behaviors typically peak around age 2 and show a normative decline across early childhood (Hartup, 1974; Kopp, 1982), considerable evidence indicates that some children continue to show high levels of externalizing behaviors beyond childhood (e.g., Campbell, Spieker, Vandergrift, Belsky, & Burchinal, 2010). These children who show continued high levels of externalizing behaviors are at the greatest risk for later indicators of maladjustment, such as social difficulties, school failure, and delinquent behavior (e.g., Fergusson, Lynskey, & Horwood, 1996; Loeber, Farrington, Stouthamer-Loeber, Moffitt, & Caspi, 1998). Thus, the identification of subgroups of children who consistently engage in higher levels of externalizing behaviors as compared to those engaging in more transient, age-related difficulties is key.

In addition to identifying children who engage in specific patterns of externalizing behaviors, it is vital to identify intraindividual correlates of these longitudinal patterns. The development of children’s self-regulatory abilities, in particular, is likely associated with differences in developmental trajectories of externalizing behavior problems (Calkins & Perry, 2016). This is expected because the development of the self-regulatory system is critical for the successful negotiation of childhood challenges and adaptive functioning across various environmental contexts that are characterized by increasingly complex social, emotional, and cognitive demands. Unfortunately, existing empirical work assessing children’s self-regulatory skills and externalizing behaviors has largely focused on only one self-regulatory process at a given time; as such, this work is unable to address which specific self-regulatory abilities (i.e., physiological, attentional, emotional, and behavioral) are most strongly associated with varying patterns of children’s externalizing behaviors.

The current study had two aims. The first aim was to add to the relatively small body of literature examining children’s externalizing trajectories by confirming the existence of developmental patterns of externalizing behaviors across six time points from infancy into adolescence. Although prior work has examined developmental trajectories of externalizing symptoms such as physical aggression, few studies include a broad measure of externalizing behavior problems that include other symptoms such as delinquency and rule-breaking. Moreover, very few studies follow children over several developmental periods. Thus, although the work on physical aggression has added to our current knowledge of externalizing trajectories, examining patterns of externalizing behaviors from age 2 to age 15, significantly contributes to the current body of literature. The second aim was to examine the influence of domain-specific self-regulatory skills simultaneously to identify which individual components of the self-regulatory system during early childhood are most salient in the prediction of externalizing trajectories.

Trajectories of Externalizing Behaviors across Childhood and Adolescence

Moffitt (1993) addressed the etiology of externalizing behaviors and outlined two life course patterns of antisocial behavior: one that originates in childhood and persists across time (life-course persistent), and another that begins in adolescence and ends by young adulthood (adolescence limited). According to Moffitt’s theory, the life-course persistent group is small and may be rooted in intraindividual risks early in life (i.e., hyperactivity, subtle cognitive deficits). In contrast, externalizing behavior that emerges in late childhood and adolescence is believed to be characterized by developmentally normative, short-lived antisocial behavior that develops alongside puberty.

Recent longitudinal studies employing person-centered methodological approaches have supported Moffitt’s original theory by empirically identifying groups of children who follow a life-course persistent and an adolescent onset pattern of externalizing behaviors from childhood through adolescence. In addition, a “childhood limited” group of individuals who demonstrate high levels of externalizing behaviors in childhood but not in adolescence have been identified, as well as a group of individuals characterized by displaying low levels of externalizing behaviors across time (e.g., Broidy et al., 2003). It is important to note that there is also a body of work that suggests more nuance to these patterns of externalizing behaviors, depending on the age of the sample, measure of externalizing behaviors/aggression, and methodology employed (i.e., Bongers, Koot, van der Ende, & Verhulst, 2004; Brame, Nagin, & Tremblay, 2001; Nagin & Tremblay, 2005).

In one study, Odgers and colleagues (2008) identified four antisocial behavior trajectories from age 7 to age 26: life-course persistent, adolescent-onset, childhood-limited, and a low trajectory. When compared to the adolescent-onset group of individuals, children in the life-course persistent group were identified by social, familial, and neurodevelopmental risk factors in childhood. Individuals in the adolescent-onset group were also identified as having childhood risk factors, although to a lesser extent than the life-course persistent individuals. In another study, Roisman and colleagues (2010) found 5 varying externalizing groups of individuals from kindergarten through age 15: low, moderate, childhood limited, adolescent onset, and early onset/persistent. Consistent with other research and Moffitt’s (1993) theory, the early-onset/persistent group experienced greater contextual difficulties and higher intraindividual disadvantages during childhood than children in the other groups.

Importantly, these studies demonstrated that all children who showed elevated externalizing behaviors had greater childhood intraindividual disadvantages relative to children with consistently low externalizing behaviors (Odgers et al., 2008; Roisman, Monahan, Campbell, Steinberg, & Cauffman, 2010). Thus, it is crucial to identify early developmental antecedents that are associated with a greater likelihood of engaging in maladaptive behavioral patterns over time. Although the ability to self-regulate during early childhood is widely accepted to be influential for the development and display of externalizing behaviors, it is far less understood which specific self-regulatory skills are most strongly associated with distinct externalizing behavior patterns. Thus, the current study sought to replicate and extend prior work by substantiating the existence of externalizing trajectories from infancy through adolescence, while also identifying specific self-regulatory processes that may predict these trajectories.

Self-regulation and Externalizing Behaviors

Self-regulation refers to the ability to modulate arousal and behavior in the context of environmental demands. Self-regulatory functioning has been conceptualized as a system in which adaptive self-control can be observed across physiological, attentional, emotional, behavioral, and cognitive domains. These individual self-regulatory processes continuously build upon one another such that earlier developing self-regulatory mechanisms are thought to provide the basis for more advanced self-regulatory processes that account for the more sophisticated behavior we observe as children mature (Calkins & Fox, 2002; Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996). This viewpoint suggests that deficits in the early acquisition of self-regulatory skills may constrain later functioning in other domains, subsequently impacting children’s displays of problem behaviors (Calkins & Fox, 2002). And indeed, previous work has theorized that the normative decline in externalizing behaviors from infancy through early childhood may be a function of these emerging self-regulatory abilities (e.g., Calkins & Dedmon, 2000; Cicchetti, Ganiban, & Barnett, 1991).

Infants rely mostly on automatic physiological mechanisms to modulate arousal. During the preschool years, however, children become increasingly effortful in their use of regulatory strategies, gain greater control of their impulses, and become aware of factors that can influence their attention, arousal, and behavior such as motivation and distractions (Miller & Zalenski, 1990). Thus, by the end of early childhood there has been rapid maturation of self-regulatory processes across domains that allow children to initiate and organize their behavior in an adaptive and flexible manner (Cicchetti, Ackerman, & Izard, 1995); these important developmental gains may allow children to transition to school with less frustration, learn more efficiently, and develop better interpersonal relationships with parents, teachers, and peers, thus decreasing the likelihood of displaying externalizing behaviors. In contrast, children who are aroused easily and do not develop the self-regulatory skills needed to modulate their emotions, redirect and focus their attention, and inhibit impulsive responses, will be less efficient in controlling their behavior, and may therefore be more likely to be defiant or act aggressively. Therefore, the extent to which children have acquired self-regulatory skills during the early childhood period may play a particularly important role in understanding adjustment and maladjustment across the school years.

Concurrent and longitudinal empirical work has supported this notion by linking physiological, attentional, emotional, and behavioral self-regulatory indicators with the display of externalizing behavior problems. When assessing children’s physiological capabilities, a primary measure of interest has been baseline respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), or the variability in heart rate that occurs at the frequency of breathing. Researchers have described RSA as an index of neural control of the heart that underlies regulatory abilities necessary for behavioral control (Doussard-Roosevelt, Porges, Scanlon, Alemi & Scanlon, 1997). While some empirical work demonstrates no association between baseline RSA and children’s behavior problems (e.g., Calkins, Graziano, & Keane, 2007; Fortunato, Gatzke-Kopp, & Ram, 2013), The majority of empirical studies have linked lower levels of baseline RSA with increased externalizing behaviors, supporting its association with appropriate engagement with the environment and behavioral regulation (e.g., Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, & Mead, 2007; Crowell, Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, Sylvers, & Mead, 2006).

Attention regulation refers to children’s ability to allocate attention to different stimuli, focus attention when faced with distraction, and keep a task in memory (Posner & Rothbart, 2000). Individual differences in these skills not only influence children’s ability to regulate their behavioral responses across contexts, but they also contribute to the development of more sophisticated executive functions, and allow for the modulation of reactivity to environmental stimuli (Garon, Bryson, & Smith, 2008; Reck & Hund, 2011). Thus, attentional regulation not only allows for greater behavioral control and subsequently fewer externalizing behaviors (e.g., Belsky, Pasco, & Bell, 2007), it also serves as a fundamental building block for the development of emotional and more advanced cognitive self-regulation.

The inability to regulate emotion is considered a core symptom for children who display externalizing behavior problems (Gilliom & Shaw, 2004). The prevailing perspective on the relation between the two is that children who are quicker to experience intense anger without the ability to effectively reduce that arousal are more likely to engage in destructive behavior or act aggressively (Vitaro, Brendgen, & Tremblay, 2002). Indeed, extensive work has indicated that children high in anger but lacking the ability to self-regulate are more likely to display peer conflict and behavioral maladjustment, including externalizing behaviors (Deater-Deckard et al., 2010; Eisenberg et al., 2001).

In addition, children’s behavioral inhibitory control is thought to be a critical self-regulatory ability associated with behavior problems. Behavioral inhibitory control involves withholding responses that, although may be prompted, may not be appropriate for the current situation (Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2005). Because successfully controlling behavior across contexts requires children to effectively inhibit inappropriate responses, it is not surprising that a number of studies have linked deficits in inhibitory control with the display of externalizing behaviors across childhood (e.g., Buss, Kiel, Morales, and Robinson, 2014; Hardaway, Wilson, Shaw & Dishion, 2012).

In addition to investigating the longitudinal associations between individual aspects of self-regulatory functioning and subsequent externalizing outcomes at a single time point, empirical studies using group-based trajectory modeling have also found associations between externalizing group membership and self-regulatory processes (e.g., Aguilar, Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson, 2000). Two studies using the same sample as the current study have assessed early self-regulatory predictors of children’s externalizing trajectories from age 2 to age 5. In the first study, poor emotion regulation and inattention at age 2 predicted membership in the chronic-clinical profile for girls, whereas socioeconomic status and inattention at age 2 predicted membership in the chronic-clinical profile for boys (Hill, Degnan, Calkins, & Keane, 2006). In the second study, a high disruptive behavior profile was associated with higher reactivity when combined with higher maternal control or when lower regulation was combined with lower maternal control (Degnan, Calkins, Keane, & Hill-Soderland, 2008).

This brief review of the literature supports the notion that self-regulatory processes across multiple domains are not only associated with one another in complex ways, but are also associated with children’s engagement in externalizing behaviors. Importantly, and central to the aims of the current study, this work has largely focused on only one dimension of self-regulation at a time and has not considered the influence of multiple processes in the context of one another for developmental patterns of externalizing behaviors. As highlighted above, self-regulation has been conceptualized as a system because adaptive control across domains build upon one another and are greatly intertwined (Calkins & Fox, 2002); self-regulation in any given domain cannot be effective without bolstering from other self-regulatory processes within the self-regulatory system. For example, regulation of emotion requires the regulation of physiological systems, the focusing of attention, and the inhibition of behavior. Because individual self-regulatory processes do not function in isolation, their associations with the development of externalizing behaviors are not independent. Empirical work considering these interdependences may elucidate which specific self-regulatory processes are most strongly associated with various externalizing behavioral patterns across time and is therefore a critical next step in advancing our understanding of the development of behavioral maladjustment.

The Current Study

The first aim of the study, preliminary in nature, was to substantiate the existence of distinct trajectories of externalizing behaviors from age 2 to age 15. Consistent with prior work, we hypothesized that 4 distinct patterns would emerge: a group of children who continuously display low levels of externalizing behaviors (stable/low), a group of children who decrease in externalizing behaviors across early childhood and continue to show low levels in late childhood and adolescence (childhood decreasing), a group of children who show increases in externalizing behaviors at the onset of adolescence (adolescent onset), and a group of children who continuously display high externalizing behaviors (stable/high).

The second aim was to assess whether specific self-regulatory skills across domains at age 2 and age 5 were associated with membership in the externalizing group-based trajectories. We chose to assess self-regulatory functioning at both ages given the considerable maturation in self-regulatory skills that occurs from infancy to early childhood (Kopp, 1982). On average, externalizing behaviors are the highest for children at age 2, and thus assessing age 2 self-regulation may provide valuable insight regarding the way in which very early self-regulatory skills may be associated with developmental patterns of externalizing behaviors from toddlerhood into adolescence. However, self-regulatory skills are relatively rudimentary in infancy and toddlerhood, likely related to the greater incidence of externalizing symptoms at age 2, and there are rapid gains in self-regulatory functioning occurring across all levels of analysis across early childhood (e.g., Kochanska, Coy, & Murray, 2001). Further, given that it is not until age 5 or 6 that children are capable of true self-regulation (e.g., Bronson, 2000), in our model we include indictors of self-regulation both before (age 2) and after this rapid period of self-regulatory growth (age 5).

Importantly, we sought to extend previous empirical work by identifying which self-regulatory skills are most strongly associated with group membership of externalizing patterns, a goal that has significant implications for intervention efforts targeting children’s behavior problems. We hypothesized that even after accounting for their associations with one another, physiological, attentional, emotional, and behavioral self-regulatory processes across ages would be associated group membership, although the effect of each self-regulatory predictor would likely depend on the behavioral pattern being predicted.

Methods

Participants

This study utilized data from three cohorts of children who are part of an ongoing longitudinal study of social and emotional development. The goal for recruitment was to obtain a sample of children who were at risk for developing future externalizing behavior problems, and who were representative of the surrounding community in terms of race and socioeconomic status (SES). All cohorts were recruited through child day care centers, the County Health Department, and the local Women, Infants, and Children (WIC) program. Potential participants for cohorts 1 and 2 were recruited at 2-years of age (cohort 1: 1994–1996 and cohort 2: 2000–2001) and screened using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL 2–3; Achenbach, 1992), completed by the mother, in order to over-sample for externalizing behavior problems. Children were identified as being at risk for future externalizing behaviors if they received an externalizing T-score of 60 or above. Efforts were made to obtain approximately equal numbers of boys and girls. This recruitment effort resulted in a total of 307 children. Cohort 3 was initially recruited when infants were 6 months of age (in 1998) for their level of frustration, based on laboratory observation and parent report, and were followed through the toddler period (see Calkins, Dedmon, Gill, Lomax, & Johnson, 2002, for more information). Children from Cohort 3 whose mothers completed the CBCL at two-years of age (N = 140) were then included in the larger study. Of the entire sample (N = 447), 37% of children were identified as being at risk for future externalizing problems at age 2. There were no significant demographic differences between cohorts with regard to gender, race, or two-year SES.

Of the 447 originally selected participants, six were dropped because they did not participate in any data collection at 2 years-old. An additional 12 families participated at recruitment, did not participate at two-year, but did participate at later years. At 4 years of age, 399 families participated. Families lost to attrition included those who could not be located, moved out of the area, declined participation, or did not respond to phone and letter requests to participate. There were no significant differences between families who did and did not participate at each age in terms of gender, race, two-year SES, or 2-year externalizing T scores unless otherwise noted. At age 5, 365 families participated, including four that did not participate in the four-year assessment. At 7 years of age, 350 families participated, including 19 that did not participate in the 5-year assessment. Families with lower 2-year SES, t (432) = −2.61, p < .01, were less likely to participate in the 7-year assessment. At age 10, 357 families participated, including 31 families that did not participate in the 7-year assessment. At age 15, 327 families participated, including 27 families that did not participate in the 10-year assessment. Boys were less likely to participate in the 15-year assessment χ2 (1, N = 447) = 9.31, p = .002.

The sample for the current study included 443 children (52% girls, 48% boys) who had available externalizing behavior data for at least one time point (6% of children [N=29] had data available at only one time point); 67% of the sample was European American, 27% African American, 4% biracial, and 2% identified as “other.” In addition, four participants were dropped from the current study due to developmental delays. Families were economically diverse based on Hollingshead (1975) scores at the 2-year assessment, with a range from 14 to 66 (M = 39.57, SD = 10.92), thus representing families from each level of social strata typically captured by this scale. Hollingshead scores that range from 40 to 54 reflect minor professional and technical occupations considered to be representative of middle class.

Procedures

Children and their mothers participated in an ongoing longitudinal study beginning at age 2. The current analyses include data collected when children were 2, 4, 5, 7, 10 and 15 years of age. Measures of externalizing behaviors at each time point and measures of self-regulatory processes at ages 2 and 5 were utilized. At each laboratory visit, mothers completed questionnaires that included the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983) and other developmentally appropriate questionnaires assessing children’s social, emotional, and behavioral functioning. Children were videotaped participating in tasks designed to elicit emotional and behavioral responding (partially derived from the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery; Lab-TAB; Goldsmith & Rothbart, 1993). Videotapes were used for behavioral coding. Only the measures relevant for the current study are reported here.

Measures

Indicators of Externalizing Trajectories

The Child Behavior Checklist’s (CBCL; Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983) Externalizing subscale, which includes items measuring aggressive, destructive, and oppositional behaviors, was used as an index of parent report of externalizing behavior problems at each age. When the children were 2 years old, mothers completed the CBCL for 2–3-year-olds (Achenbach, 1992). When the children were 4, 5, and 7 years of age mothers completed the CBCL for 4–18-year-olds (Achenbach, 1991). When children were 10 and 15 years of age mothers completed the CBCL for 6–18-year-olds (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Achenbach and colleagues (e.g., Achenbach, 1992; Achenbach, Edelbrock, & Howell, 1987) have found these scales to be a reliable index of externalizing behavior problems across these developmental periods. The mother indicated how true the statement was of her child on a scale of 0 (not true) to 2 (often true) for each version. Mean scores of externalizing behaviors at each age were used in analyses.

Self-Regulatory Predictors of Externalizing Trajectories

2 and 5-year Baseline Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia (RSA)

Baseline/resting RSA was obtained at age 2 and age 5 while children watched a 5-minute segment of a neutral “Spot” video about a puppy that explores its neighborhood. Although this task was not a true baseline, as children’s attention was engaged, it has been used in multiple studies (e.g., Hastings & De, 2008), and was sufficient to gain a measure of RSA while children were sitting quietly and showing neutral affect. Given the age of these children, such a stimulus was necessary in order to limit movement artifact in the heart rate data. In work utilizing this sample, the data obtained during the baseline video task differed significantly from tasks requiring active engagement (Calkins, Graziano, Berdan, Keane, & Degnan, 2008).

To obtain heart rate data, an experimenter placed three electrodes in an inverted triangle pattern on the child’s chest. The electrodes were connected to a preamplifier, the output of which was transmitted to a vagal tone monitor (VTM-I, Delta Biometrics, Inc, Bethesda, MD) for R-wave detection. The vagal tone monitor displayed ongoing heart rate and computed and displayed an estimate of RSA every 30 seconds. A data file containing the inter-beat intervals (IBIs) for the entire period of heart rate collection was saved on a laptop computer for later artifact editing (e.g., resulting from child movement) and analysis.

The MXEDIT software (Delta Biometrics, Inc., Bethesda, MD) was used to analyze and edit IBI files. Porges (1985) method of analyzing the IBI data was used to calculate RSA. This method applies an algorithm to the sequential heart period (HP) data. The algorithm uses a moving 21-point polynomial to detrend periodicities in heart period that are slower than RSA. Then, a bandpass filter extracts the variance in HP within the frequency band of spontaneous respiration in young children, 0.24 – 1.04 Hz. The natural log of this variance is taken and reported in units of ln(msec)2. To edit the files, the data were scanned for outlier points, relative to adjacent data, and the outliers were replaced by dividing or summing them so they would be more consistent with the surrounding data. Only data files in which less than 10% of the data required editing were included in the current study (36 files were removed at age 2; 69 files were removed at age 5).

2-year Attention

At age 2, two coders watched videotapes of children attending to the “Spot” video and recorded the total amount of time the child spent looking at the video. The proportion of time spent looking at the video in relation to the total time of the task was utilized in the current study. Coders were trained by working together on 15% of the videotapes and independently scoring another 15% for reliability. The intra-class correlation coefficient was .98.

5-year Attention

The attentional focusing subscale of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire-Short Form (CBQ-SF; Putnam & Rothbart, 2006) was used at age 5 to assess attention. This parent reported measure was chosen over the laboratory attention task because it captured children’s attentional control skills across multiple contexts and showed significantly more variability at age 5 than the video attention task. The attentional focusing subscale includes 6 items measuring the tendency to maintain focus on a particular task. The mean of the items was calculated to obtain the subscale score. Internal reliability was acceptable (α =.72).

2-year Emotion Regulation

Children’s observed emotion regulation abilities were indexed by a measure of global regulation during the high chair task, which was defined as the use of behavioral skills (e.g., distraction, sucking) in an effort to decrease distress. During the high chair task, children’s arms were held down by their sides to restrict movement. The scale ranged from 0 (no control of distress across the task) to 4 (regulation of distress during most of the task). Two coders trained by working together on 15% of the videotaped sessions and independently scoring another 15% for reliability purposes. Cohen’s Kappa was .96.

5-year Emotion Regulation

Children’s observed emotion regulation abilities were indexed by a measure of global regulation during the “I’m Not Sharing” task (Lab-TAB version 2.0; Goldsmith & Rothbart, 1993), a task designed to elicit child frustration. During this task, the experimenters divided candy between themselves and the child. The experimenters gave themselves more candy than the child and also took the child’s candy and ate it. Observed regulation was defined as the use of behavioral skills in an effort to decrease distress. The scale ranged from 0 (child demonstrates no control of distress to stimuli) to 4 (the child seems to completely regulate distress or distracts away from distress most of the time). Two coders trained by working together on 15% of the videotaped sessions and independently scoring another 15% for reliability purposes. Cohen’s Kappa was .78.

It should be noted that the observational tasks used in the current study to assess emotion regulation were designed to elicit frustration or anger from children, thereby confounding emotional valance with emotion regulation. For example, it would be inaccurate to assess the level at which a child displays negative emotions or the extent to which they employ regulatory strategies if the child did not find the task to be emotionally upsetting (for a discussion see Cole, Martin, & Dennis, 2004). Although we are not able to disentangle children’s feelings of anger from their regulation of anger during our laboratory tasks, we include the maternal reported Anger Proneness subscale of the Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire at age 2 (TBAQ; Goldsmith, 1996) as a covariate in our model to index a trait-like measure of children’s anger.

The emotion regulation subscale of the Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC; Shields & Cicchetti, 1997) was used to assess parent report of children’s broader emotion regulation abilities at age 5. The emotion regulation subscale is comprised of 8 items assessing children’s ability to articulate and display their emotional arousal such as “displays appropriate negative emotions,” “is cheerful,” and “can voice when feeling sad, angry, or afraid.” Thus, this subscale provides a measure of children’s ability to regulate both their positive and negative emotions across a variety of contexts. Mean scores were created and used in the current analyses. Internal reliability was acceptable (α =.63).

5-year Behavioral Inhibitory Control

Behavioral inhibitory control was assessed using the inhibitory control subscale of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire-Short Form (CBQ-SF; Putnam & Rothbart, 2006). The inhibitory control subscale is comprised of 6 items measuring the capacity to plan and to suppress inappropriate approach responses under instructions or in novel or uncertain situations. The mean of the items was calculated to obtain inhibitory control subscale score. Internal reliability was acceptable (α =.70).

Analytic Strategy

Semi-parametric group-based methods (SPGM; Nagin, 2005) were used to identify the number and shape of distinct trajectories of externalizing behaviors across 6 time points from age 2 to age 15. SPGM is a data-driven technique that utilizes a clustering algorithm to identify groups that follow similar patterns of behavior across time and are distinctly different from one another. Using this group-based modeling technique we were able to estimate the probability that each individual belongs to a particular group (representing different trajectories) based on the data and the derived maximum-likelihood parameter estimates associated with group membership (i.e., posterior probabilities of group membership). Individuals were then assigned to groups based on the posterior probabilities. Because raw scores are thought to be ideal for growth modeling (Seltzer, Frank, & Byrk, 1994), models were estimated with raw externalizing scores using Mplus version 7 (Muthen & Muthen, 2012) via latent class growth analysis (LCGA). Given the prior theoretical and empirical work suggesting up to 5 distinct trajectories (e.g., Campbell et al., 2010; Odgers et al., 2008; Roisman et al., 2010) of externalizing behaviors across early childhood and adolescence, up to a 5-class solution was examined. Evaluation of the best fitting model was based on consideration of the following criteria: (1) the Bayesian information criterion (BIC); (2) the Lo-Mendell-Rubin adjusted likelihood ratio test (LMR-LRT); (3) and conceptual clarity. To test the predictive significance of each of the self-regulatory covariates for membership in the externalizing trajectory groups, the classes in which individuals were assigned were saved and used as the dependent variable in a single multinomial logistic regression model in SPSS version 18 (SPSS, Chicago, IL).

We chose the high/stable trajectory as the reference group given its association with the most serious maladjustment (Fergusson et al., 1996). Researchers have theorized that the development of self-regulatory skills during early childhood strongly contributes to the normative decreases we see in externalizing behaviors during that same developmental time period. By using the high/stable group as the reference group we are better able to address whether the important decreases in externalizing behaviors across childhood are predicted by children’s greater self-regulatory skills, and elucidate which self-regulatory skills may be particularly important in differentiating children who continue to display high externalizing behaviors and those who are able to minimize these problems over time.

Missing data for the predictor variables used in the multinomial logistic regression were imputed using multiple imputation (Schafer & Graham, 2002). Demographic and family characteristics, along with longitudinal child characteristics were used as predictors in the imputation dataset to estimate missing data accurately. Schafer’s (Schafer & Graham, 2002) recommended procedure, an iterative EM algorithm, was used. Ten data sets were created in which all observed data was represented and missing data estimated. The pooled results across datasets were used in the final regression analyses. Full information maximum likelihood (FIML) was used to handle missing data in the LCGA analyses conducted in Mplus. FIML estimation utilizes all available information to account for missing data and does not exclude participants with partial data, resulting in unbiased parameter estimates and appropriate standard errors (Schafer & Graham, 2002).

Results

Table 1 contains descriptive statistics and correlations among all externalizing variables and self-regulatory variables. As indicated in Table 1, externalizing behaviors across ages were highly correlated, reflecting expected stability in problem behavior over time. As expected, self-regulatory skills showed low to moderate correlations across early childhood, reflecting stability in self-regulatory skills within each domain while also providing support for the great deal of change hypothesized to take place across the preschool years. Correlations among individual self-regulatory abilities within each age also emerged, providing support for the broader self-regulatory construct across domains. Finally, self-regulatory skills at each level were negatively correlated with children’s displays of externalizing behaviors, supporting the notion that greater self-regulation within multiple domains is associated with children’s increased ability to behave in socially acceptable ways.

Table 1.

Correlations and Descriptive Statistics

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
 1. 2yr Externalizing --
 2. 4yr Externalizing .62* --
 3. 5yr Externalizing .55* .77* --
 4. 7yr Externalizing .41* .70* .71* --
 5. 10yr Externalizing .46* .60* .63* .69* --
 6. 15yr Externalizing .42* .43* .46* .50* .69* --
 7. Age 2 Anger .50* .40* .38* .30* .37* .25* --
 8. Age 2 Baseline RSA .02 .04 .08 .08 .10 .03 .10* --
 9. Age 2 Attention −.15* −.07 −.12* .00 −.17* −.14* −.20* .12* --
 10. Age 2 Emotion Reg (Obs) −.15* −.14* −.10 −.06 −.10 −.06 −.18* .13* .22* --
 11. Age 5 Baseline RSA .00 −.01 .04 −.02 .02 .00 .14* .45* −.12* −.02* --
 12. Age 5 Emotion Reg (Obs) −.11* −.20* −.15* −.13* −.13* −.13* −.12* −.08 .01 .23* −.17* --
 13. Age 5 Emotion Reg (Rep) −.25* −.23* −.30* −.21* −.24* −.24* −.17* .01 .10* .13* −.03 .03 --
 14. Age 5 Attention −.34* −.40* −.38* −.25* −.27* −.24* −.26* −.04 .17* .07 −.02 .10* .24* --
 15. Age 5 Inhibitory Control −.38* −.45* −.48* −.36* −.41* −.27* −.34* −.19* .12* .12* −.16* .20* .35* .57* --

Mean .589 .297 .294 .225 .181 .186 .231 5.46 .80 3.10 6.08 2.86 3.32 4.80 4.73
Standard Deviation .328 .189 .214 .182 .186 .231 .88 1.29 .18 1.22 1.15 .91 .32 .91 .91
Minimum 0 0 0 0 0 0 1.65 1.10 .16 0.00 3.28 1.00 2.38 1.83 1.00
Maximum 1.77 .97 1.08 .92 1.03 1.44 6.43 8.79 1.00 4.00 9.42 4.00 4.00 6.83 6.83
Skew .50 .85 1.05 1.10 1.60 2.24 .18 −.13 −1.23 −1.30 .14 −.27 −.16 −.45 −.63
(SE) (.12) (.13) (.13) (.14) (.14) (.14) (.13) (.13) (.12) (.12) (.14) (.14) (.13) (.13) (.13)
N 443 371 339 324 316 300 347 345 418 399 296 319 343 340 340

Note:

*

p < .05,

Obs = Observed; Rep = Parent Reported; Values reported prior to standardizing

Identifying the Externalizing Group-Based Trajectory Model

Fit indices for a 1-trajectory model (Bayesian Information Criteria [BIC] = −186.69; Lo-Mendell-Rubin likelihood ratio test [LMR-LRT], not applicable), 2-trajectory model (BIC = −843.10; LMR-LRT, p < .05), 3-trajectory model (BIC =−1086.85; LMR-LRT, p < .05), 4-trajectory model (BIC = −1209.88; LMR-LRT, p < .05); and 5-trajectory model (BIC = −1208.91; LMR-LRT, p = .51) were compared. Taken together, evaluation of model fit statistics suggested that a 4-class solution was empirically and theoretically justified (see Figure 1). In the 4-class trajectory model, the majority of children demonstrated a low/stable level of externalizing behaviors across childhood and into adolescence (53% of the sample; n = 235; 56% female), with the second largest group following a decreasing pattern across early childhood and remaining low (37% of the sample; n = 163; 44% female). The third largest group followed an elevated or high/stable pattern of externalizing behaviors across childhood and adolescence (7% of the sample; n = 30; 60% female). Finally, the smallest group displayed an increase in their externalizing behavior starting at age 7 and into adolescence (3% of the sample; n = 15; 60% female). Prior work examining patterns of externalizing trajectories has typically found increases beginning at the onset of adolescence. However, because increases appear to take place from age 7 onward for this small subgroup of children we chose to label this pattern childhood increasing. Although the childhood increasing group only comprised 3% of the sample, we chose to retain this group and keep the 4-class solution because it was a better statistical fit to the data (i.e., lower BIC and significant LRT) and because it was in accordance with previous literature identifying a pattern of increased externalizing symptoms in late childhood and adolescence when examining trajectories of externalizing behaviors across development (Odgers et al., 2008; Roisman et al., 2010). Examination of the posterior probabilities indicated that individuals were well matched (Nagin, 2005) to their group (.93 for the high/stable trajectory, .92 for the adolescent onset trajectory, .86 for the decreasing trajectory, and .92 for the low/stable trajectory).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

4-class Group Trajectory Model of Externalizing Behaviors from age 2 to age 15

Self-regulatory Predictors Associated with Externalizing Trajectory Group Membership

To examine whether 5-year self-regulatory abilities across domains might differentially be associated with membership in the externalizing trajectory groups, we simultaneously estimated the effect of each self-regulatory variable in a single multinomial logistic regression model (see Table 3). Because self-regulatory indicators were set on different scales, standardized z-scores for each self-regulation variable were used in the model for greater interpretability. As previously indicated, the self-regulatory predictors were moderately correlated with one another, indicating that they were in fact part of a larger self-regulatory construct reflecting children’s overall ability to regulate themselves in a variety of contexts across multiple developmental domains. Importantly, by including self-regulatory predictors at each level in the same model, and thereby accounting for their shared variance, we had increased confidence that any significant associations that emerged between our self-regulatory variables and externalizing trajectories were unique to that domain-specific self-regulatory skill.

Because there were differences in the proportion of boys and girls in the externalizing trajectory groups, the predictive significance of each self-regulatory covariate was examined in combination with child sex to determine any potential interactive associations. No interactions between sex and self-regulation emerged. Thus, interaction terms were removed from the model for parsimony and child sex was included as a covariate. Given the previously reported relations between socioeconomic status (SES) and behavior problems (e.g., Hinshaw, 1992), SES via the Hollingshead (1975) was also included as a covariate in the model. Results indicated being male (male = 1, female = 0) slightly increased children’s odds of being classified in the low/stable and the childhood decreasing group as compared to the high/stable group although the effect size was small (see Table 2). There were no sex differences for the childhood increasing trajectory.

Table 2.

Multinomial Logistic Regression of Externalizing Behavior Trajectory Groups with Self-Regulatory Abilities at age 5

Trajectory Group Contrasts

Low/Stable vs. High/Stable Decreasing vs. High/Stable Childhood Increasing vs. High/Stable
(n = 235; 53%) (n = 30; 7%) (n = 163; 37%) (n = 30; 7%) (n = 15; 3%) (n = 30; 7%)

B (SE) eb p 95% CI B (SE) eb p 95% CI B (SE) eb p 95% CI
Covariates
 Sex 1.18(.58) 3.24 .04 (1.04, .10.16) 1.36 (.56) 3.90 .02 (1.30, 11.69) .68 (.79) 1.97 .39 (.42, 9.22)
 SES .05 (.03) 1.05 .11 (.99, 1.11) .04 (.03) 1.05 .09 (.99, 1.10) −.01 (.04) .99 .80 (.93, 1.06)
 Anger age 2 −1.65 (.38) .19 .00 (.09, .41) −1.05 (.39) .35 .01 (.16, .77) −1.12 (.49) .33 .03 (.12, .86)
2-year Self-Regulation
 Baseline RSA .15 (.31) 1.16 .63 (.63, 2.11) .13 (.27) 1.14 .63 (.66, 1.95) .10 (.46) 1.11 .83 (.44, 2.78)
 Attention −.22 (.32) .80 .49 (.43, 1.50) −.20 (.31) .81 .51 (.45, 1.50) −.54 (.38) .58 .16 (.27, 1.24)
 Emotion Regulation (Obs) −.30 (.32) .74 .35 (.40, 1.39) −.26 (.31) .77 .41 (.42, 1.42) −.04 (.42) .96 .92 (.42, 2.18)
5-year Self-Regulation
 Baseline RSA .69 (.35) 1.99 .05 (.99, 4.00) .26 (.34) 1.30 .45 (.66, 2.55) .34 (.50) 1.40 .50 (.53, 3.72)
 Attention .73 (.30) 2.08 .02 (1.15, 3.73) .38 (.30) 1.46 .20 (.82, 2.62) .51 (.44) 1.67 .24 (.71, 3.96)
 Emotion Regulation (Obs) .91 (.33) 2.47 .01 (1.28, 4.80) .70 (.32) 2.02 .04 (1.06, 3.84) .48 (.41) 1.61 .24 (.72, 3.61)
 Emotion Regulation (Rep) 1.04 (.39) 2.82 .01 (1.29, 6.15) .91 (.38) 2.49 .02 (1.16, 5.31) .63 (.49) 1.87 .21 (.71, 4.94)
 Inhibitory Control 1.21 (.38) 3.36 .00 (1.59, 7.09) .73 (.35) 2.08 .04 (1.03, 4.18) .64 (.48) 1.91 .18 (.74, 4.86)

Note: Obs = Observed; Rep = Parent Reported; All variables are standardized for interpretability

Interestingly, none of the self-regulatory indicators at age 2 were uniquely associated with membership in any of the trajectory groups. However, greater maternal reported trait-like anger proneness was significantly associated with a lower likelihood of being in the low/stable (.18 (95% CI = .09, .41), decreasing (.35 (95% CI = .16, .77), and childhood increasing (.33 (95% CI = .12, .86) groups when compared to the high/stable group (see Table 3).

In contrast, greater 5-year self-regulatory skills across domains were uniquely associated with membership in the low/stable trajectory group when compared to the high/stable group. Specifically, for every one SD change in baseline RSA at age 5, the odds of being in the low/stable group, as compared to the high/stable group increased by 1.99 (95% CI = .99, 4.00), suggesting that as physiological self-regulatory abilities increase children were about 2 times more likely to follow a low/stable externalizing trajectory. For every one SD change in 5-year observed emotion regulation during the frustration task, the odds of being in the low/stable group, as compared to the high/stable group increased by 2.47 (95% CI = 1.28, 4.80), suggesting that as observed emotion regulation increases, children were about 2.5 times more likely to be classified in the low/stable externalizing trajectory group. Parental report of emotion regulatory skills was also a significant predictor; for every one SD change in parental report of children’s emotion regulation, the odds of being in the low/stable group as compared to the high/stable group increased by 2.82 (95% CI = 1.29, 6.15), suggesting that as the ability to regulate both positive and negative emotions, as well as express emotions appropriately increases children were about 3 times more likely to be classified in the low/stable externalizing trajectory group. Parent-reported attentional and behavioral inhibitory control were also uniquely associated. For every one SD change in children’s attentional and behavioral inhibitory control, the odds of being in the low/stable group as compared to the high/stable group increased by 2.08 (95% CI = 1.15, 3.73) and 3.36 (95% CI = 1.59, 7.09) for attentional and behavioral inhibitory control respectively. These findings suggest that for one SD change in attentional skills children are about 2 times more likely to be in the low/stable group compared to the high/stable group, and 3 times more likely to be in the low/stable group compared to the high stable group for one SD change in inhibitory control abilities.

When examining predictors of the decreasing group as compared to the high/stable group, only emotion regulation and behavioral inhibitory control were uniquely associated with belonging to the childhood decreasing externalizing trajectory. For every one unit increase in observed and reported emotion regulation the odds of being in the childhood decreasing group, as compared to the high/stable group, increased by 2.02 (95% CI = 1.06, 3.84) and 2.49 (95% CI = 1.16, 5.31) for observed regulation of frustration and broader emotion regulation abilities respectively. These findings indicate that children were about 2 times more likely to be in the childhood decreasing group compared to the high/stable group as their ability to behaviorally regulate frustration goes up, and about 2.5 times more likely to be in the childhood decreasing group as compared to the high/stable group as more general parent-reported emotion regulation abilities increased. Lastly, as behavioral inhibitory control increased one unit, the odds of being in the childhood decreasing group increased by 2.08 (95% CI = 1.03, 4.18), suggesting that as children’s inhibitory control abilities increased they were about 2x more likely to be in the childhood decreasing group. There were no unique self-regulatory predictors of the adolescent onset group when compared to the high/stable trajectory group.

Discussion

We aimed to replicate and extend the existing work on the development of externalizing behavior problems by substantiating the existence of distinct patterns of externalizing trajectories across 6 time points from infancy to adolescence. The large majority of studies examining externalizing behavioral patterns across childhood do not have infant data available and therefore are not able cover all developmental periods as is done in the current study. Thus, we have the unique opportunity to extend the current literature on externalizing trajectories in a meaningful way. As hypothesized, and consistent with other work on externalizing trajectories, we identified four distinct trajectories of externalizing behaviors: high/stable, low/stable, childhood decreasing, and a small subset of children who increased during middle childhood and adolescence. The majority of children were classified as low/stable, and showed a low and stable pattern of externalizing behavior problems from early childhood into adolescence. The second largest group, labeled childhood decreasing, showed the normative decline in externalizing behavior problems across early childhood and remained low into adolescence. The third largest group demonstrated an elevated pattern of externalizing behavior problems across childhood and adolescence and was thus labeled high/stable. This group is very similar to the life-course persistent group that is found consistently in other studies (e.g., Odgers et al, 2008; Roisman et al., 2010). Finally, there was a group of individuals, childhood increasing, showing a significant increase in externalizing behavior problems starting at age 7 through adolescence.

Interestingly, we saw no association between childhood SES and externalizing group membership. Child sex, however, was a significant predictor such that females were slightly less likely to be in the low/stable and decreasing group than in the high/stable group. The effect sizes are small and likely emerged because of the somewhat larger percentage of females in the high/stable trajectory (63% female). Although the majority of work in this area demonstrates that males are more likely to display high and stable levels of externalizing behaviors across development, prior work has shown that it is not uncommon for females to also display this pattern (e.g., Broidy et al., 2003). Specifically, when trajectory models of antisocial behavior are conducted separately for males and females across childhood, similar behavioral patterns are found (Odger et al., 2008). Thus, although we did not expect more females to follow a high/stable pattern, our findings are consistent with work indicating that self-regulatory predictors of externalizing trajectories function similarly for both sexes (Roisman et al., 2010).

Although it is important to validate the developmental trajectories of externalizing behaviors over time, the next step in this area of work is to better understand the correlates of these distinct trajectories. Thus, the second aim of the current study was to identify which specific self-regulatory abilities were most strongly associated with distinct patterns of externalizing behaviors. Given the theoretical link between self-regulatory development and normative decreases in externalizing behaviors (Calkins & Perry, 2016), we hypothesized that variation in self-regulatory abilities across multiple domains (i.e., physiological, emotional, attentional, and behavioral) would serve to discriminate among children on varying developmental externalizing trajectories. To date, there is little empirical evidence that sheds light on which specific self-regulatory processes are associated with the various patterns of externalizing behaviors. This study is the first of our knowledge to examine self-regulatory processes across domains at multiple time points during early childhood to better understand how domain-specific self-regulatory processes might be uniquely associated with specific developmental patterns of externalizing behaviors.

Because such substantial gains in independent self-regulatory abilities are made from toddlerhood to early childhood, we considered multiple self-regulatory indicators at age 2 and at age 5 as correlates of externalizing behavior trajectories. Only the 5-year self-regulatory indicators were associated with externalizing trajectory group membership; no associations between 2-year self-regulatory abilities and externalizing patterns emerged. We hypothesized that 2-year self-regulatory processes were not associated with patterns of externalizing behaviors over time because most children’s self-regulatory abilities are poorly developed during infancy and toddlerhood (Kopp, 1982). For example, from age 2 to age 5 attention abilities rapidly develop (Rothbart, 1989) and significantly contribute to the maturation of behavioral control and emotion regulation during that time period (Calkins, 2009). Thus, self-regulatory skills by the end of early childhood, a time during which self-regulatory development slows and most children transition to the academic environment, may be more strongly associated with externalizing trajectories over time than self-regulatory abilities in toddlerhood when these abilities are under-developed. That is, greater self-regulatory abilities by kindergarten may reflect appropriate and important self-regulatory gains that occur across early childhood. These increased skills may help to facilitate children’s ability to learn and interact positively with teachers, parents, and peers and therefore may be associated with differing patterns of behavioral adjustment. In contrast, lower self-regulatory abilities by school entry may reflect deficits in the acquisition of self-regulation during early childhood. Children with poor self-regulatory skills may find school more frustrating and have fewer social supports, likely contributing to greater externalizing behavior problems across childhood and adolescence.

When examining specific probabilities of group membership, we found that children with greater 5-year self-regulatory skills across physiological, attentional, emotional, and behavioral domains were more likely to be in the low/stable group when compared to the high/stable group. These findings are expected given the sizeable literature outlining the negative association between each of these self-regulatory abilities and the presence of externalizing behavior problems (e.g., Gerstein et al., 2011; Martel et al., 2007). We also found that self-regulatory abilities across all domains were not uniquely associated with membership in the childhood decreasing group when compared to the high/stable group; instead only emotion regulation and behavioral inhibitory control were associated with a decreasing externalizing trajectory. Given that aggressive, destructive behaviors characterize externalizing problems, it is logical that the ability to regulate emotion and behavior would be significantly associated with the decline of these problematic behaviors over time.

Similarly, children who can control behavioral impulses are less likely to act out or be defiant. There are significant changes in the prefrontal cortex that occur across early childhood and are believed to be associated with children’s increased inhibitory control skills (Carlson & Wang, 2007), which have been found to be associated with a decrease in externalizing behaviors (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 2009). Further, inhibitory control is shown to be a promising skill to foster in interventions aiming to decrease behavior problems. For example, inhibitory control abilities mediated the effect between The Promoting Alternative Thinking Strategies curriculum and decreases in externalizing behavior problems (Riggs et al., 2006).

Finally, no unique self-regulatory predictors of the childhood increasing group emerged when compared to the high/stable trajectory group. It is possible that significant differences in self-regulatory abilities between children who increase in their behavior problems during childhood and adolescence and a life-course persistent pattern in externalizing behaviors may not be as evident given that individuals showing both patterns are known to have early difficulties that distinguish them from individuals who demonstrate decreasing and low externalizing behaviors over time (e.g., Odgers et al., 2008). Further, although significant findings between adolescent onset and high/stable groups have been identified (e.g., Roisman et al., 2010), these differences are minimal in number (i.e., difficult temperament, having a single parent). Regardless, it is possible that we did not have enough power to detect significant associations between the levels of self-regulatory functioning and group membership due to the small number of individuals within our childhood increasing group. Thus, it is important to consider this when interpreting the lack of findings presented here. Future work testing these associations with a larger subset of childhood increasing individuals is needed.

Limitations and Future Directions

Despite the many methodological and theoretical strengths of this study, it is not without limitations. First, data was not collected between the 10-year and 15-year time point. Thus, externalizing behaviors throughout late childhood and early adolescence did not contribute to the patterns of externalizing behaviors identified in the current study. However, given that previous work utilizing early adolescent time points when assessing externalizing trajectories has identified similar developmental patterns (e.g., Broidy et al., 2003; Roisman et al., 2010), externalizing behaviors at the 15-year time point may be a sufficient indicator of children’s likely trajectory of externalizing behaviors during this developmental gap.

A second limitation is the small number of individuals identified as members of the childhood increasing group. In our sample, a smaller number of individuals were classified in this group as compared to the existing work on externalizing behavior trajectories. The small percentage of individuals classified in this group likely limited our ability to identify unique self-regulatory predictors of adolescent-onset group membership. Yet, the 4-class solution that included the adolescent onset trajectory was statistically a better fit to the data than the 3-class solution and this group has been repeatedly identified in existing work. Thus, we believe the inclusion of this group in the current study is the best decision both empirically and theoretically.

Third, given our focus on the influence of multiple intraindividual self-regulatory predictors, we did not consider the multiple environmental factors that are likely to contribute to differences in externalizing behavioral patterns over time, such as familial factors or social supports. Investigating the effects of specific parenting behaviors may be a particularly important avenue for future work examining the links between self-regulatory functioning and patterns of externalizing behaviors. Differences in parenting have not only been found to be associated with changes in externalizing behaviors over time (e.g., Galambos, Barker, & Almeida, 2003), but they have also been linked to the development of self-regulatory processes (e.g., Grolnick & Farkas, 2002). Therefore, it is possible that parenting behavior is indirectly related to externalizing trajectories through its influence on children’s self-regulatory development.

Finally, because self-regulatory abilities at age 5 were used as predictors of externalizing trajectories from age 2 to 15, the results must be interpreted without inferring causal associations. Specifically, given the lack of temporal precedence, the results of the current study do not suggest that self-regulatory skills at age 5 cause differences in the trajectories of externalizing behaviors over time. Instead, children’s self-regulatory skills at age 5, which in combination with the age 2 measures reflects the growth in children’s self-regulation from age 2 to age 5, may help us differentiate which externalizing trajectories children are more likely to follow. This work has both intervention and prevention implications. Specifically, the results from this study suggest that the developmental changes that occur by age 5 in emotion regulation and inhibitory control are associated with the likelihood that children may follow varying patterns of externalizing behaviors; as such, the assessment of these skills during the transition to school may allow preventive intervention programs to target children most at risk while also providing specific skills that may be most effective for behavioral adjustment.

The current study highlights the importance of employing a multi-domain perspective of self-regulation and provides greater insight into which specific self-regulatory processes are the most salient predictors of externalizing trajectories from early childhood to adolescence. Assessing domain-specific self-regulatory skills is necessary to parse out the unique role of self-regulatory mechanisms for the development of various externalizing behavioral patterns. Identifying which self-regulatory processes differentiate individuals who decrease in their externalizing behaviors from those who continue to demonstrate high levels of externalizing behaviors into adolescence has important practical implications for the implementation of preventive intervention programs during early childhood, and underscores the need to focus intervention efforts on specific self-regulatory abilities.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Behavioral Science Track Award for Rapid Transition (MH 55625), an NIMH FIRST Award (MH 55584) and an NIMH K-Award (MH 74077) to Susan D. Calkins, and an NIMH Grant (MH 58144) awarded to Susan D. Calkins and Susan P. Keane. The authors also thank the families who generously gave their time to participate in the study.

References

  1. Achenbach TM. Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach TM. Manual for the child behavior checklist/2-3 and 1992 Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  3. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS. Manual for the child behavior checklist and revised behavior profile. Burlington: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  4. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C, Howell CT. Empirically based assessment of the behavioral/emotional problems of 2- and 3-year-old children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1987;15(4):629–650. doi: 10.1007/BF00917246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2001. Empirically based assessment of the behavioral/emotional problems of 2- and 3- year-old children. [Google Scholar]
  6. Aguilar B, Sroufe LA, Egeland B, Carlson E. Distinguishing the early-onset/persistent and adolescence-onset antisocial behavior types: From birth to 16 years. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12(2):109–132. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400002017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beauchaine TP, Gatzke-Kopp L, Mead HK. Polyvagal Theory and developmental psychopathology: Emotion dysregulation and conduct problems from preschool to adolescence. Biological Psychology. 2007;74(2):174–184. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Belsky J, Pasco Fearon RM, Bell B. Parenting, attention and externalizing problems: Testing mediation longitudinally, repeatedly and reciprocally. Journal of Child Psychology And Psychiatry. 2007;48(12):1233–1242. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01807.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bongers IL, Koot HM, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Developmental trajectories of externalizing behaviors in childhood and adolescence. Child Development. 2004;75(5):1523–1537. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00755.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brame B, Nagin DS, Tremblay RE. Developmental trajectories of physical aggression from school entry to late adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology And Psychiatry. 2001;42(4):503–512. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Broidy LM, Tremblay RE, Brame B, Fergusson D, Horwood JL, Laird R, … Vitaro F. Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six-site, cross-national study. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39(2):222–245. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bronson MB. Self-regulation in early childhood: Nature and nurture. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  13. Buss KA, Kiel EJ, Morales S, Robinson E. Toddler inhibitory control, bold response to novelty, and positive affect predict externalizing symptoms in kindergarten. Social Development. 2014;23(2):232–249. doi: 10.1111/sode.12058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Calkins SD. Regulatory competence and early disruptive behavior problems: The role of physiological regulation. In: Olson S, Sameroff A, editors. Regulatory processes in the development of behavior problems: Biological, behavioral, and social-ecological interactions. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2009. pp. 86–115. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Calkins SD, Dedmon SA. Physiological and behavioral regulation in two-year-old children with aggressive/destructive behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2000;28:103–118. doi: 10.1023/A:1005112912906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Calkins SD, Dedmon SE, Gill KL, Lomax LE, Johnson LM. Frustration in infancy: Implications for emotion regulation, physiological processes, and temperament. Infancy. 2002;3(2):175–197. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0302_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Calkins SD, Fox NA. Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: A multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression. Development and Psychopathology. 2002;14:477–498. doi: 10.1017/s095457940200305x. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S095457940200305X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Calkins SD, Graziano PA, Berdan LE, Keane SP, Degnan KA. Predicting cardiac vagal regulation in early childhood from maternal-child relationship quality during toddlerhood. Developmental Psychobiology. 2008;50(8):751–766. doi: 10.1002/dev.20344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Calkins SD, Graziano PA, Keane SP. Cardiac vagal regulation differentiates among children at risk for behavior problems. Biological Psychology. 2007;74(2):144–153. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Calkins SD, Perry NB. The development of emotion regulation: Implications for child adjustment. In: Cicchetti D, editor. Developmental Psychopathology: Maladaptation and psychopathology. 3. Vol. 3. Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 2016. pp. 187–242. [Google Scholar]
  21. Campbell SB. Behavior problems in preschool children: Clinical and developmental issues. 2. New York, NY US: Guilford Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  22. Campbell SB, Spieker S, Vandergrift N, Belsky J, Burchinal M. Predictors and sequelae of trajectories of physical aggression in school-age boys and girls. Development & Psychopathology. 2010;22(1):133–150. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409990319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Carlson SM, Wang TS. Inhibitory control and emotion regulation in preschool children. Cognitive Development. 2007;22(4):489–510. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cicchetti D, Ackerman BP, Izard CE. Emotions and emotion regulation in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1995;7(1):1–10. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400006301. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cicchetti D, Ganiban J, Barnett D. Contributions from the study of high-risk populations to understanding the development of emotion regulation. In: Garber J, Dodge KA, Garber J, Dodge KA, editors. The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press; 1991. pp. 15–48. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:597–600. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400007318. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cole PM, Martin SE, Dennis TA. Emotion Regulation as a Scientific Construct: Methodological Challenges and Directions for Child Development Research. Child Development. 2004;75(2):317–333. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00673.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Crowell SE, Beauchaine TP, Gatzke-Kopp LM, Sylvers P, Mead H. Autonomic correlates of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and oppositional defiant disorder in preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115:174–178. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Deater-Deckard K, Beekman C, Wang Z, Kim J, Petrill S, Thompson L, DeThorne L. Approach/positive anticipation, frustration/anger, and overt aggression in childhood. Journal of Personality. 2010;78(3):991–1010. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00640.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Degnan KA, Calkins SD, Keane SP, Hill-Soderlund AL. Profiles of disruptive behavior across early childhood: Contributions of frustration reactivity, physiological regulation, and maternal behavior. Child Development. 2008;79(5):1357–1376. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01193.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Doussard-Roosevelt JA, Porges SW, Scanlon JW, Alemi B, Scanlon KB. Vagal regulation of heart rate in the prediction of developmental outcome for very low birth weight preterm infants. Child Development. 1997;68(2):173–186. doi: 10.2307/1131844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Spinrad TL, Fabes RA, Shepart SA, Reiser M, … Guthrie IK. The relations of regulation and emotionality to children’s externalizing and internalizing problem behavior. Child Development. 2001;72:1112–1134. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Spinrad TL, Cumberland A, Liew J, Reiser M, … Losoya SH. Longitudinal relations of children’s effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(4):988–1008. doi: 10.1037/a0016213. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0016213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Fergusson DM, Lynskey MT, Horwood LJ. Childhood sexual abuse and psychiatric disorder in young adulthood: I. Prevalence of sexual abuse and factors associated with sexual abuse. Journal of The American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35(10):1355–1364. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199610000-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Fortunato CK, Gatzke-Kopp LM, Ram N. Associations between respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity and internalizing and externalizing symptoms are emotion specific. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2013;13(2):238–251. doi: 10.3758/s13415-012-0136-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Galambos NL, Barker ET, Almeida DM. Parents do matter: Trajectories of change in externalizing and internalizing problems in early adolescence. Child Development. 2003;74:578–594. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM. Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134(1):31–60. doi: 10.137/0033-2909.134.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gerstein ED, Pedersen y Arbona A, Crnic KA, Ryu E, Baker BL, Blacher J. Developmental risk and young children’s regulatory strategies: Predicting behavior problems at age five. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39(3):351–364. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9471-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gilliom M, Shaw DS. Codevelopment of externalizing and internalizing problems in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16(2):313–333. doi: 10.1017/S0954579404044530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Goldsmith HH. Studying temperament via construction of the Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire. Child Development. 1996;67(1):218–235. doi: 10.2307/1131697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Goldsmith HH, Rothbart MK. The Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery: Locomotor Version 2.02 (LAB-TAB) Madison: University of Wisconsin Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  42. Grolnick WS, Farkas M. Parenting and the development of self-regulation. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of parenting; Vol. 5: Practical issues in parenting. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 2002. pp. 89–110. [Google Scholar]
  43. Hardaway CR, Wilson MN, Shaw DS, Dishion TJ. Family functioning and externalizing behaviour among low-income children: Self-regulation as a mediator. Infant and Child Development. 2012;21(1):67–84. doi: 10.1002/icd.765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hartup WW. Aggression in childhood: Developmental perspectives. American Psychologist. 1974;29:336–341. doi: 10.1037/h0037622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hastings PD, De I. Parasympathetic regulation and parental socialization of emotion: Biopsychosocial processes of adjustment in preschoolers. Social Development. 2008;17(2):211–238. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00422.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hill AL, Degnan KA, Calkins SD, Keane SP. Profiles of externalizing behavior problems for boys and girls across preschool: The roles of emotion regulation and inattention. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42(5):913–928. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hinshaw SP. Externalizing behavior problems and academic underachievement in childhood and adolescence: Causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;111(1):127–155. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hollingshead AB. Unpublished manuscript. New Haven, CT: Yale University; 1975. Four-factor index of social status. [Google Scholar]
  49. Kochanska G, Coy KC, Murray KT. The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development. 2001;72(4):1091–1111. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kopp CB. Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology. 1982;18(2):199–214. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.18.2.199. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Loeber R, Farrington DP, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Moffitt TE, Caspi A. The development of male offending: Key findings from the first decade of the Pittsburgh Youth Study. Studies on Crime & Crime Prevention. 1998;7(2):141–171. doi: 10.1177/0306624X99432003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Martel MM, Nigg JT, Wong MM, Fitzgerald HE, Jester JM, Puttler LI, … Zucker RA. Childhood and adolescent resiliency, regulation, and executive functioning in relation to adolescent problems and competence in a high-risk sample. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19(2):541–563. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Miller PH, Zalenski R. Preschoolers’ knowledge about attention. Developmental Psychology. 1982;18(6):871–875. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.18.6.871. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Moffitt TE. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review. 1993;100:674–701. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 7. Los Angeles, CA: 2012. [Google Scholar]
  56. Nagin DS. Group-based Modeling of Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  57. Nagin DS, Tremblay RE. What Has Been Learned from Group-Based Trajectory Modeling? Examples from Physical Aggression and Other Problem Behaviors. Annals of The American Academy Of Political And Social Science. 2005;602:82–117. doi: 10.1177/0002716205280565. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Odgers CL, Moffitt TE, Broadbent JM, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, … Caspi A. Female and male antisocial trajectories: From childhood origins to adult outcomes. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20(2):673–716. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Porges SW. Patent no. 4,510,944. Method and apparatus for evaluating rhythmic oscillations in aperiodic physiological response systems. 1985 Apr 16;
  60. Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:427–441. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400003096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Putnam SP, Rothbart MK. Development of short and very short forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2006;87:103–113. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_09. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Reck SG, Hund AM. Sustained attention and age predict inhibitory control during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011;108(3):504–512. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Riggs NR, Greenberg MT, Kusché CA, Pentz MA. The mediational role of neurocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social-emotional prevention program in elementary school students: Effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prevention Science. 2006;7(1):91–102. doi: 10.1007/s11121-005-0022-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Roisman GI, Monahan KC, Campbell SB, Steinberg L, Cauffman E. Is adolescence-onset antisocial behavior developmentally normative? Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22(2):295–311. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Rothbart MK. Temperament and development. In: Kohnstamm GA, Bates JE, Rothbart MK, editors. Temperament in childhood. Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons; 1989. pp. 187–247. [Google Scholar]
  66. Rueda MR, Posner MI, Rothbart MK. The development of executive attention: Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;28(2):573–594. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods. 2002;7(2):147–177. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Seltzer MH, Frank KA, Bryk AS. The metric matters: The sensitivity of conclusions about growth in student achievement to choice of metric. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis. 1994;16(1):41–49. doi: 10.2307/1164382. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  69. Shields A, Cicchetti D. Emotion regulation among school-age children: The development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:906–916. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Vitaro F, Brendgen M, Tremblay RE. Reactively and proactively aggressive children: Antecedent and subsequent characteristics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43:495–506. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES