SUMMARY
Considerable theoretical and experimental effort has been dedicated to understanding how neural circuits detect visual motion. In primates, much is known about the cortical circuits that contribute to motion processing, but the role of the retina in this fundamental neural computation is poorly understood. Here, we used a combination of extracellular and whole-cell recording to test for motion sensitivity in the two main classes of output neurons in the primate retina— midget (parvocellular-projecting) and parasol (magnocellular-projecting) ganglion cells. We report that parasol, but not midget, ganglion cells are motion sensitive. This motion sensitivity is present in synaptic excitation and disinhibition from presynaptic bipolar cells and amacrine cells, respectively. Moreover, electrical coupling between neighboring bipolar cells and the nonlinear nature of synaptic release contribute to the observed motion sensitivity. Our findings indicate that motion computations arise far earlier in the primate visual stream than previously thought.
INTRODUCTION
Motion estimation is crucial to an animal’s survival and success in the natural environment. Accordingly, the neural circuits that mediate motion computations have received a great deal of attention in the scientific literature (reviewed in Borst and Euler, 2011; Borst and Helmstaedter, 2015; Clark and Demb, 2016). Motion processing begins when specialized neural circuits extract local motion signals from visual inputs (Adelson and Bergen, 1985; Barlow and Levick, 1965; Hassenstein and Reichardt, 1956). In vertebrates, these circuits serve at least two distinct functions. First, specialized subcortical neural circuits compute object motion along specific cardinal axes that align with the axes of the semicircular canals of the inner ear. These visual signals, originating in direction-selective retinal ganglion cells, are combined with vestibular signals in the brainstem, thus bringing the visual and vestibular systems into register (Oyster et al., 1980; Sabbah et al., 2017; Simpson and Alley, 1974; Simpson et al., 1979; Taylor et al., 2000; Vaney et al., 2012). A separate system is responsible for real-time control of visually guided movements (Goodale and Milner, 1992). In humans and non-human primates, this latter system arises in part from retinal projections to the ‘magnocellular’ layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) of the thalamus, which, in turn, provide input to the motion-sensitive neurons in the cortex that form the dorsal visual pathway (Kaplan and Benardete, 2001; Maunsell et al., 1990; Merigan et al., 1991b; Merigan and Maunsell, 1990; Schiller et al., 1990a, b). Despite this apparent link to motion vision, the contribution of parasol cells to motion processing has garnered remarkably little attention.
According to the orthodox view, the principal role of the retina is to provide a veridical representation of the visual environment. Similar to modern image compression algorithms, this representation is thought to arise by encoding visual inputs into distinct spatiotemporal channels that are realized at the level of different ganglion cell types (Campbell and Robson, 1968; Enroth-Cugell and Robson, 1966). In contrast to this view, work in the dominant retinal experimental systems indicates that the parallel ganglion cell pathways act as highly specialized feature detectors, performing functions that are far richer and more complex than simple spatiotemporal processing (reviewed in Gollisch and Meister, 2010; Masland and Martin, 2007). Indeed, ganglion cells have been found in several species that encode object versus background motion (Baccus et al., 2008; Ölveczky et al., 2003, 2007), direction of motion (Barlow et al., 1964; Barlow and Levick, 1965; Sabbah et al., 2017; Taylor and Vaney, 2002), orientation (Nath and Schwartz, 2016; Venkataramani and Taylor, 2010, 2016), and other very specific visual features (Baden et al., 2016; Mani and Schwartz, 2017; Sivyer et al., 2010). While the spatiotemporal-channels hypothesis has been all but abandoned in current theories of vision in nearly all vertebrate species (Gollisch and Meister, 2010; Masland and Martin, 2007), this hypothesis persists as the dominant model for visual processing in primates (Conway and Livingstone, 2003; Lennie and Movshon, 2005; Rust et al., 2005).
Based on their spatial receptive field sizes, temporal kinetics, and contrast sensitivity, parasol cells are thought to contribute to the general visual representation by bandpass filtering incoming visual signals in both space and time and by signaling changes in luminance relative to the background (Kaplan and Shapley, 1986; Lee et al., 1995; Purpura et al., 1988). It is commonly believed that specialized cortical circuits, then, use this general-purpose representation from parasol cells and other retinal ganglion cells to extract information about spatial form and visual motion (Adelson and Bergen, 1985; Lee et al., 1995; Movshon and Newsome, 1996). This view predicts that attenuating the signals arising from parasol (magnocellular) pathway would equally affect spatial/form and motion vision. However, inactivating or lesioning this pathway has little effect on spatial vision, but instead severely impairs an animal’s ability to detect motion (Maunsell et al., 1990; Merigan et al., 1991a; Schiller et al., 1990a, b), indicating that parasol cells principally contribute to motion vision.
Two earlier studies used multi-electrode array recording to demonstrate that the parasol cell population provides an incredibly precise readout of the speed and trajectory of moving objects (Chichilnisky and Kalmar, 2003; Frechette et al., 2005). This precision, however, was ascribed to the concerted activity of the population and the possibility that it originated in the properties of individual parasol cells or their presynaptic circuitries was not considered. Here, we performed a series of experiments to test for motion sensitivity in midget (parvocellular-projecting) and parasol (magnocellular-projecting) ganglion cells. While midget cells lacked motion sensitivity, parasol cell responses to motion were enhanced relative to other types of stimuli. This motion sensitivity in parasol cells was present across a wide range of contrasts and motion speeds, indicating that it was a general property of these cells. Further, we found that motion sensitivity was present at the level of glutamate release from presynaptic bipolar cells and disinhibition from amacrine cells and was mediated, in part, by electrical coupling between bipolar cell synaptic terminals. Taken together, our data indicate that motion processing begins far earlier in the primate visual stream than has been previously appreciated—at the second synapse of vision.
RESULTS
The canonical hypothesis of early visual processing in primates argues that the mosaic of parasol (magnocellular-projecting) ganglion cells contribute to a general-purpose representation of vision that is ultimately used by downstream cortical circuits to extract information about the structure/form of objects (spatial vision) as well as object motion (motion vision). Thus, our principal goal was to directly test whether these cells could distinguish between spatial form and visual motion. Inability to distinguish between the two would be taken as evidence in favor of the orthodox view. Likewise, greater sensitivity for motion would be evidence that individual parasol cells are, themselves, motion sensitive. We begin by demonstrating that parasol cells respond better to motion than to spatial form, a property which we term motion sensitivity.
Motion sensitivity of parasol ganglion cells
Do parasol ganglion cells exhibit motion sensitivity? Distinguishing a cell’s contribution to form and motion vision has proven an experimentally challenging task. For example, a white bar moving through a cell’s receptive field changes the intensity of light on the receptive field relative to the background (i.e., contrast). To separate parasol cell responses to spatial pattern versus motion, we employed an apparent-motion stimulus paradigm used previously to demonstrate motion sensitivity in the mouse retina (Kuo et al., 2016). The paradigm consisted of two interleaved stimuli: (1) an apparent-motion stimulus comprised of a bar shifting position incrementally through the receptive-field center and (2) a stimulus in which the bar positions were randomized. Except for the spatiotemporal sequence, both stimuli were identical, stimulating the same regions of the receptive-field center within the same time interval (Figure 1B, top).
We recorded the spike responses of ON and OFF parasol cells to repeated presentations of these stimuli. The average spike responses to these stimuli in example ON and OFF parasol cells are shown in Figure 1B. In both cell types, spike rates were higher for apparent motion than for the random-bar stimulus. We quantified these responses by calculating each cell’s average spike rate during the stimulus presentation (50-msec window) relative to the maintained spike rate immediately preceding the stimulus (500-msec window). The apparent-motion stimulus elicited higher spike rates than the random-bar stimulus in ON and OFF parasol cells, and these differences were statistically significant (n = 27 ON cells, p = 3.3 × 10−5; n = 25 OFF cells, p = 6.5 × 10−5).
To compare the relative response to the apparent-motion and random-bar stimuli across cells, we computed a motion sensitivity index for each neuron tested (Kuo et al., 2016):
where Rmotion and Rrandom are the responses to the apparent motion and random bar stimuli, respectively. Values greater than zero indicate a preference for the apparent-motion stimulus. The average motion sensitivity values for both cell types were greater than one, indicating that the apparent motion stimulus typically elicited more than twice as many spikes as the random bars (i.e., >100% increase; Figure 1C). The results of this experiment indicate that individual parasol ganglion cells can distinguish between motion and spatial form.
Major anatomical and physiological differences have been described between the central (macular) and peripheral regions of the primate retina and, in some cases, these differences are functionally relevant (Hecht and Verrijp, 1933; Rovamo and Raninen, 1988; Sinha et al., 2017; Tyler, 1985; Watanabe and Rodieck, 1989; Waugh and Hess, 1994). To determine whether motion sensitivity varied with retinal eccentricity, we separated the motion sensitivity of parasol cells located in or near the macula (<3 mm foveal eccentricity; n = 15 cells) from those located more peripherally (n = 37 cells). Surprisingly, we found that central cells showed, higher motion sensitivity than their more peripheral counterparts (central, 3.4 ± 0.7; peripheral, 1.4 ± 0.17; mean ± SEM; p = 2.6 × 10−4).
To assess the selectivity of our motion assay, we repeated this experiment in midget (parvocellular-projecting) ganglion cells. These cells contribute to chromatic and spatial vision and are not expected to show particular sensitivity to visual motion (reviewed in Neitz and Neitz, 2017; Wässle, 2004). Indeed, the apparent-motion and random-bar stimuli elicited similar spike responses from midget cells, indicating a lack of motion sensitivity (Figure 1D, E; n = 9 cells; p = 0.36).
Many neurons exhibit enhanced sensitivity to stimuli containing strong correlations (Rieke et al., 1995). Thus, an alternative interpretation of the results in Figure 1 is that parasol cells show enhanced sensitivity to spatiotemporal correlations, not necessarily motion. To test this possibility, we devised a stimulus containing strong correlations but lacking the spatiotemporal tilt or slant characteristic of visual motion (De Valois and Cottaris, 1998; De Valois et al., 2000; DeAngelis et al., 1993; McLean and Palmer, 1989; Movshon et al., 1978). The stimulus was a bar that alternated between two neighboring positions on every frame presentation, and the spatial location of the neighboring bars varied randomly on any given trial (Figure 2, top; see STAR Methods). Despite containing strong spatiotemporal correlations, the alternating stimulus elicited fewer spikes than the apparent-motion stimulus, producing significant motion sensitivity values for both ON (n = 10) and OFF (n = 14) cells (Figure 2; p < 1.0 × 10−4). These data indicate that the presence of spatiotemporal correlations alone is not sufficient to engage the mechanisms that mediate motion sensitivity in parasol cells. Further, these data show that motion computations arise far earlier in the visual stream of primates than previously recognized.
Motion sensitivity present across contrasts and speeds
The experiments outlined above tested motion sensitivity at a single contrast and a single speed. To probe the generality of our results, we varied these two stimulus parameters. We determined whether motion sensitivity was present across a range of contrasts by repeating the apparent-motion/random-bar experiment at four contrasts between 25–100%—positive contrasts for ON parasol cells and negative contrasts for OFF parasol cells. The motion stimulus elicited more spiking than the random bar stimulus in both cell types at all contrasts (Figure 3A, B; p < 0.01). For ON parasol cells, motion sensitivity was similar at all contrasts tested, and OFF parasol cells showed higher motion sensitivity at lower contrasts (Figure 3B). We tested midget ganglion cells in the same way and found that, unlike parasol cells, the apparent-motion and random-bar stimuli elicited similar responses in midgets (Figure 3A, right). Accordingly, these cells lacked motion sensitivity at all contrasts tested (Figure 3B, right; p > 0.25).
If parasol cells contribute to visual motion coding, they should exhibit motion sensitivity across a range of stimulus speeds. We tested this by repeating the experiment at multiple speeds between 0.15–2.4 mm sec−1 (~0.75–12 degrees sec−1; Figure 4A, B; see STAR Methods). Stimulus speed was varied by altering the duration of bar presentations at a particular spatial location (i.e., by changing the update rate of the stimulus; see STAR Methods). Parasol cells showed poor selectivity for apparent motion at speeds below 3 degrees sec−1 (p > 0.15) and robust motion sensitivity at higher speeds (p < 5.0 × 10−4). Midget cells, however, lacked a preference for motion at all of the speeds tested (Figure 4B, orange; p > 0.05).
Given the apparent dependence of motion sensitivity on speed (Figure 4B), we sought a more comprehensive understanding of the speed tuning properties of parasol cells. We were particularly interested in whether speed tuning differed systematically between central and peripheral cells. Due to technical limitations, we were unable to test for motion sensitivity at speeds greater than 12 degrees sec−1 (see STAR Methods). To obtain a more complete tuning curve that included faster speeds, we switched from the apparent-motion/random-bar stimulus paradigm to one in which bars were moved through the receptive-field center at different speeds (1–64 degrees sec−1, 0.2–12.8 mm sec−1).
The resulting speed tuning curves were fit well with a bandpass tuning function used to describe the speed tuning of motion-sensitive neurons in two cortical areas (Figure 4C, solid lines; Priebe et al., 2006). According to this function, the spike rate at a given speed, R(s), is a function of the cell’s preferred speed:
where A is the maximal spike rate (in spikes sec−1), sp is the preferred speed (in degrees sec−1), β is the bandwidth, and ζ is the skew.
The resulting speed tuning curves showed two prominent features. First, responses varied systematically as a function of speed—spiking was highest at speeds between 8–32 degrees sec−1 and decreased at higher and lower speeds (Figure 4C). Second, we observed differences in speed tuning between central and peripheral cells of the same type. On average, central cells showed higher peak speeds (ON central: 40.5 ± 6.5, n = 7; ON peripheral: 19.1 ± 5.9, n = 9; OFF central: 43.3 ± 3.8, n = 7; OFF peripheral: 25.9 ± 3.6; n = 9; mean ± SEM) and these differences were statistically significant (pON = 1.6 × 10−2; pOFF = 2.6 × 10−3). Speed tuning bandwidth was also narrower for central OFF cells (in octaves; central: 1.2 ± 0.3; peripheral: 2.7 ± 0.2; p = 2.6 × 10−3), but the tuning bandwidth was similar between central and peripheral ON cells (central: 3.0 ± 0.5; peripheral: 2.9 ± 0.3; p = 0.42).
These experiments demonstrate a preference for motion in parasol cells across a range of stimulus contrasts and speeds, indicating that motion sensitivity is a robust property of these cells. Further, midget cells lacked substantial motion sensitivity at all contrasts and speeds tested. What gives rise to motion sensitivity in parasol cells? In the following section, we seek answers to this question by considering the synaptic inputs to parasol cells.
Motion sensitivity present in excitation and crossover inhibition
The experiments above found motion sensitivity in the spike output of parasol ganglion cells. Our next goal was to understand the circuit mechanisms mediating this motion sensitivity. Detecting visual motion requires mechanisms that compare at least two points in space at different points of time, and previous studies have identified excitatory and inhibitory circuits that perform this comparison (reviewed in Borst and Euler, 2011; Demb, 2007). To distinguish between these excitatory and inhibitory mechanisms, we recorded the direct synaptic input to parasol cells in voltage clamp. Excitatory currents were isolated by clamping a cell’s membrane voltage at the reversal potential for inhibition (−70 mV) and, likewise, inhibitory currents were recorded at the excitatory reversal potential (0 mV). An increase in excitatory input to a cell is manifest by a more negative current response relative to the leak current, whereas an increase in inhibition produces a more positive current response. Thus, to avoid potential difficulty in interpreting these results, we present summary data in terms of conductance (see STAR Methods). Figure 5 compares the relative sensitivity of the excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs to ON and OFF parasol cells to the apparent-motion and random-bar sequences. Just as was observed in the spike responses, apparent motion elicited significantly larger excitatory responses than the random-bar stimulus in both cell types (Figure 5A; n = 9 ON cells, p = 6.3 × 10−3; n = 16 OFF cells, p = 5.7 × 10−5). Thus, motion sensitivity was present at the synaptic output of bipolar cells.
Does synaptic inhibition contribute to motion sensitivity in parasol cells? To answer this question, we recorded inhibitory input to parasol cells in voltage clamp. The apparent-motion stimulus elicited a net removal of inhibition (disinhibition) relative to the mean background (Figure 5B) while the random-bar stimulus elicited less disinhibition (Figure 5B; pON = 3.1 × 10−2; pOFF = 4.2 × 10−3). This disinhibition arises from circuits mediating crossover inhibition between the ON and OFF visual pathways—an ON amacrine cell provides direct inhibitory input to OFF parasol cell dendrites and an OFF amacrine cell inhibits ON parasol cells (Cafaro and Rieke, 2013; Manookin et al., 2008; Murphy and Rieke, 2006; Zaghloul et al., 2003). Thus, motion through the receptive field center elicits an increase in excitatory input and a simultaneous decrease in inhibitory input, causing parasol cells to spike (see Figure 8). Indeed, both of these circuit components were stronger for the apparent-motion than for the random-bar stimulus.
These recordings assayed the direct excitatory and inhibitory input to parasol cells, but inner retinal inhibition, including crossover inhibition, can modulate glutamate release from bipolar cells (Asari and Meister, 2012; Eggers and Lukasiewicz, 2011; Eggers et al., 2007; Liang and Freed, 2010) and voltage-clamp recordings from parasol cells would not detect these interactions as they occur upstream of the parasol cell dendrites. To determine whether presynaptic inhibition contributes to motion sensitivity, we recorded excitatory currents in parasol cells before and after blocking inner retinal inhibition with a cocktail of glycine- and GABA-receptor antagonists (strychnine, 0.5 μM; GABAzine, 5 μM; TPMPA, 50 μM). Blocking presynaptic inhibition reduced motion sensitivity by ~60% (mean: control, 2.0 ± 0.6; drug, 0.8 ± 0.4; p = 2.3 × 10−2; n = 7 cells; Figure 5E, left). This result may indicate that presynaptic inhibition plays an important role in shaping motion sensitivity in bipolar cells, but we encourage great caution in interpreting these data as wholesale inhibitory blockade can produce many off-target effects. Indeed, during inhibitory block we observed an increase in the frequency and amplitude of spontaneous excitatory currents, consistent with a disruption in the resting state of bipolar cells (Figure 5F). Further, a previous study and our computational model suggest that releasing inner retinal inhibition could affect motion sensitivity simply by driving the bipolar cell output synapse into a more linear state (Kuo et al., 2016; see Figure 7).
To test for such confounding effects of inhibitory blockade on the bipolar cell output nonlinearity, we recorded contrast-response functions in the excitatory currents of the same cells before and after drug application (Figure 5G, H). Applying inhibitory antagonists changed the contrast-response relationship in both cell types. These effects were most striking in ON cells where inhibitory blockade unmasked an OFF excitatory input, which was greater in magnitude at high contrast than the inherent ON excitation (Figure 5H, left). Due to the clear unintended effects of full inhibitory block, we sought a more ideal pharmacological manipulation to assay the potential contribution of presynaptic inhibition to motion sensitivity.
The OFF visual pathway provides an opportunity to measure the contribution of presynaptic crossover inhibition without substantially shifting the resting state of OFF bipolar cells. This is achieved by applying an mGluR6 agonist (L-AP4, 5 μM) and antagonist (LY341495, 7.5 μM) in a ratio that strongly attenuates signal transmission between cones and ON bipolar cells without dramatically changing the resting state of the ON pathway (Ala-Laurila et al., 2011). Thus, we tested for contributions from presynaptic crossover inhibition to motion sensitivity in OFF parasol cells by recording excitatory currents before and after blocking crossover inhibition. Indeed, blocking crossover inhibition reduced motion sensitivity in each cell tested (mean: control, 2.5 ± 0.8; drug, 1.4 ± 0.5; p = 3.1 × 10−2; n = 5 cells; Figure 5E, right). Moreover, we did not observe the spontaneous bursting during crossover block that was present with the inhibitory blockade, indicating that the resting state of OFF bipolar cells was not strongly affected by the pharmacological manipulation. Thus, it appears that crossover inhibition contributes to motion sensitivity both presynaptically at the level of the bipolar cells and postsynaptically at the level of OFF parasol cell dendrites (Figure 5B). This mechanism may also contribute to motion sensitivity in ON parasol cells, but, unfortunately, an unequivocal pharmacological manipulation is not currently available to determine whether this is the case.
Collectively, these data indicate that motion sensitivity in parasol cells arises from an increase in excitatory input and simultaneous decrease in inhibitory input (disinhibition). Disinhibition may also act presynaptically at the level of bipolar cell terminals. Nonetheless, the magnitude of excitation was much larger than that of disinhibition. This, coupled with the larger driving force on excitation near the resting membrane potential, suggests that excitation from presynaptic bipolar cells provides the principal driver for motion sensitivity in parasol ganglion cells. We explored the neural mechanisms mediating motion sensitivity in bipolar cells with a series of experiments and computational model, described below.
Supralinear spatial summation supports a junctional mechanism
What mechanisms mediate the motion sensitivity observed in the excitatory bipolar cell input to parasol ganglion cells? Electrical coupling between bipolar cells via gap junctions mediates a similar type of motion sensitivity in the mouse retina (Kuo et al., 2016). Further, evidence for electrical coupling between diffuse bipolar cells has been found in marmoset (Luo et al., 1999), macaque (Dacey et al., 2000), and human retina (Kántor et al., 2017), including the type-3 bipolar cells that provide direct synaptic input to OFF parasol ganglion cells (Jacoby et al., 2000; Kántor et al., 2017; Luo et al., 1999). Thus, this mechanism is poised to contribute to motion sensitivity in parasol cells.
Gap junction blockers have been used in the past to test for contributions from junctional mechanisms, but these drugs have many off-target effects that greatly complicate data interpretation (Kuo et al., 2016). Instead, we tested whether presynaptic bipolar cells were electrically coupled using a paired-bar stimulus while recording from parasol cells (Kuo et al., 2016). The stimulus was comprised of a bar (width, 32–40 μm) presented at one of two spatial locations. On separate trials, bars were presented at one of the individual positions (‘bar 1’ or ‘bar 2’) or at both positions simultaneously (‘both bars’). The edges of the two bars were spaced sufficiently to avoid stimulating any single bipolar cell with both bars (separation, 32–40 μm; Figure 6A). The reasoning is that if bipolar cells were not electrically coupled, stimulation of one bipolar cell would not affect surrounding bipolar cells that were not, themselves, directly stimulated with the bar. Thus, both bars presented simultaneously should equal the linear sum of each of the bars presented individually (Figure 6A, top). Alternatively, if the bipolar cells were electrically coupled, depolarization would spread laterally and, in concert with the nonlinear nature of synaptic release, would result in glutamate release greater than the sum of individual bar presentations—supralinear spatial summation (Figure 6A, bottom).
When tested with this stimulus, simultaneous presentation of both bars elicited higher spike rates and larger excitatory currents than the linear sum of individual bar presentations (Figure 6B, C). We quantified the degree of supralinear summation using the ‘nonlinearity index’ (Kuo et al., 2016) defined as:
where Rx is the response of the cell in spike rate or excitatory current. Nonlinearity values were greater than zero for spiking and excitation in both cell types, indicating supralinear spatial summation (p < 0.05). These data are consistent with electrical coupling between the diffuse bipolar cells providing excitatory input to both ON and OFF parasol ganglion cells.
The conductances passing through gap junctions are generally considered passive. As a result, they should decay over short distances and times (Rall, 1969). To further test for a contribution from junctional coupling, we varied the parameters of the paired-bar stimulus in order to test these predictions. We examined spatial decay by varying the spacing between bars (Figure 6E, F). Supralinear summation was greatest when bars were spaced ≤32 μm apart and was absent at a spacing of 64 μm (~0.3 degrees), approximately twice the width of a diffuse bipolar cell dendritic tree at comparable retinal eccentricities (Boycott and Wässle, 1991; Dacey et al., 2000; Tsukamoto and Omi, 2015, 2016; Turner and Rieke, 2016). These data indicate that the neural interactions mediating supralinear summation occur on a narrow spatial scale and are likely limited to interactions between neighboring bipolar cells. These narrow spatial interactions would not be recruited well by stimuli lacking spatiotemporal correlations.
We next varied the time between bar presentations to determine whether supralinear summation decayed over time (Figure 6G, H). Nonlinearity indices were positive for time lags of 0 and 17 msec after which they declined below or near zero. These lags correspond to speeds of ≥6 degrees sec−1, which is consistent with the speed tuning of motion sensitivity for these cells (Figure 4B).
Computational model predicts motion sensitivity
The pattern of supralinear spatial summation observed in the experiments, above, is consistent with significant electrical coupling between neighboring diffuse bipolar cells (Dacey et al., 2000; Kántor et al., 2017; Luo et al., 1999). Our working hypothesis is that this electrical coupling results in lateral interactions between neighboring bipolar cells, which contributes to motion sensitivity in bipolar cells (Kuo et al., 2016). Visual stimulation causes a bipolar cell to depolarize and some of this depolarization spreads laterally via gap junctions to neighboring bipolar cells. This spread potentiates these neighboring cells such that subsequent stimulation within a brief time interval produces a larger depolarization than if the cell were stimulated alone. This, combined with the highly nonlinear nature of synaptic release, biases the output of the bipolar cell mosaic to visual motion.
To further explore whether these mechanisms could explain the observed motion sensitivity, we created computational models of the bipolar cell mosaics presynaptic to midget, ON and OFF central parasol, and ON and OFF peripheral parasol cells. Spatiotemporal integration of visual inputs from cones was modeled as linear in which spatial integration was a two-dimensional Gaussian function and temporal integration was a biphasic impulse-response function (Figure 7A). Following linear spatiotemporal integration, a portion of the response spread laterally through gap junctions to neighboring bipolar cell terminals. This spread was based on the relative voltage of each of the bipolar cells and their relative distances. The resulting responses for each cell were then passed through a synaptic output nonlinearity (Figure 7B; Figure S1) before being pooled and normalized. Normalization was performed by modeling the ganglion cell receptive field as a two-dimensional Gaussian (see STAR Methods). The spatial properties of the model were based on measurements of midget and diffuse bipolar cell dendritic arbors in mid-peripheral macaque retina (Boycott and Wässle, 1991; Dacey et al., 2000; Polyak, 1941; Tsukamoto and Omi, 2015, 2016; Turner and Rieke, 2016). The temporal filters and synaptic output nonlinearities were measured directly by recording excitatory currents to a flickering uniform spot within the receptive field center; spot contrast was drawn pseudo-randomly from a Gaussian distribution on each frame presentation. These data were then used to calculate the temporal filters and output nonlinearities for the different bipolar cell types (Figure 7B; see STAR Methods).
Consistent with our direct recordings, the model predicted negligible motion sensitivity in the excitatory input to midget ganglion cells (Figure 7C). The model also showed a similar pattern of motion sensitivity to that observed from our direct recordings from parasol cells (compare Figure 7D-E to Figures 3, 4). Motion sensitivity was predicted at higher apparent motion speeds, but absent at lower speeds (Figure 7D). In addition, motion sensitivity was highest at low contrast and relatively flat at higher contrasts (Figure 7E). Further, it predicted a more high-pass speed tuning for central relative to peripheral parasol cells, similar to our direct recordings (Figure 4C, 7D-E). Overall, the model predictions were qualitatively similar to our physiological recordings, lending further support for a mechanism in which motion sensitivity arises at the level of glutamate release from diffuse bipolar cells and is mediated by electrical coupling between neighboring bipolar cells.
We next extended our model to investigate how motion sensitivity depended on the bipolar cell output nonlinearity. This was done by fixing the overall shape of the nonlinearity and varying the maintained glutamate release (i.e., release at zero contrast). This manipulation of the model emphasized different regions of the output nonlinearity, and we examined four different maintained release levels (Figure 7F). The highest maintained level was near the center of the nonlinearity and constituted a fairly linear region of the function for low stimulus contrasts. We then simulated the speed and contrast tuning curves for each nonlinearity; these simulations are shown in Figure 7G, H. The bandpass nature of speed tuning varied slightly with maintained release with the highest release level being the least bandpass (Figure 7G). The contrast simulations showed motion sensitivity for each of the nonlinearities at high contrast. At low contrast, however, motion sensitivity varied systematically with the degree of maintained release—motion sensitivity was highest at the lower release rates (Figure 7H). These simulations provide further support for the importance of electrical coupling between bipolar cells in motion sensitivity. Further, nonlinear release at the bipolar cell output synapse appears to play a particularly important role in motion sensitivity at low contrast.
DISCUSSION
We performed a series of experiments to directly address the long-standing question of whether motion computations arise in the primate retina. It is widely believed that the principal function of the early visual system in humans and non-human primates is to relay a veridical representation of the visual environment to cortical centers. The cortex, then, bears the bulk of the computational load, extracting spatial form, texture, and motion information from this rudimentary representation. In this view, the spike train of a single parasol cell represents achromatic spatial information, such as the contrast of an object, and is unconcerned about whether that object is moving. However, when measured directly, we found that visual motion elicited significantly larger responses than those produced by either uncorrelated or non-motion correlated spatiotemporal patterns (Figures 1–4). This striking motion sensitivity was also evident in the excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs to parasol cells (Figure 5).
Midget ganglion cells did not show significant motion sensitivity and this was true across a wide range of contrasts and speeds (Figures 1, 3, 4). What mechanisms account for this lack of motion sensitivity in midget ganglion cells? One possibility is that midget bipolar cells lack significant electrical coupling; it is clear, however, that OFF midget bipolar cells exhibit coupling both in their dendrites and in their axons (Kántor et al., 2017). Further, our model of the midget bipolar cells included electrical coupling and it, nonetheless, lacked significant motion sensitivity (Figure 7). Thus, we propose that the small receptive-field center size of midget ganglion cells plays a critical role in their lack of motion sensitivity. At the eccentricities that we typically record and in our computational model, midget ganglion cells collect from ~1–3 midget bipolar cells whereas a parasol ganglion cell collects from at least 30 bipolar cells in its receptive field center at comparable eccentricities. Thus, unlike the parasol cells, much of the enhanced responses from electrical coupling to motion falls in the midget cell receptive-field surround (see Figure 8A). Further, our direct recordings indicated that the midget bipolar cell output was more linear than that of diffuse bipolar cells, which likely also lessens their motion sensitivity (see Figure S1). Finally, fundamental asymmetries have been demonstrated in the inhibitory motifs of the midget and parasol pathways (Cafaro and Rieke, 2010, 2013), which may also contribute to the observed differences in motion sensitivity.
Bipolar cell coupling in primate retina
Previous studies have found evidence for extensive gap junctional coupling between bipolar cells in mouse and rabbit retina. Much of this coupling, particularly in ON-type bipolar cells, occurs through intervening amacrine cells, particularly the AII amacrine cell (reviewed in Bloomfield and Völgyi, 2009; Euler et al., 2014; Völgyi et al., 2013). Some OFF bipolar cells are also coupled and this coupling can occur at the level of their dendrites or axons (Feigenspan et al., 2004; Hilgen et al., 2011).
Far less is known about coupling patterns of primate bipolar cells, but the few key studies that have addressed this question found coupling in both ON and OFF bipolar cells (Dacey et al., 2000; Kántor et al., 2017; Kovács-Öller et al., 2017; Luo et al., 1999). Further, gap junctions were present in the axons of type-3 (OFF) bipolar cells that provide direct glutamatergic input to OFF parasol ganglion cells (Jacoby et al., 2000; Kántor et al., 2017; Luo et al., 1999). OFF parasol cells also receive synaptic input from type-2 bipolar cells, and ON parasol cells receive input from type-4 and type-5 bipolar cells (Jacoby et al., 2000; Marshak et al., 2002; Tsukamoto and Omi, 2015, 2016). However, further studies are needed to determine the coupling patterns in these bipolar cell types and to understand how these patterns contribute to information processing in the magnocellular visual pathway.
Motion sensitivity and retinal eccentricity
We found that motion sensitivity varied with retinal eccentricity—on average it was higher in the central retina than in the peripheral retina (Figure 1). What mechanisms account for this difference? Many cell types, including bipolar cells, show smaller dendritic tree diameters and closer spacing in the central versus the peripheral retina (Kolb et al., 1992; Rodieck, 1989; Watanabe and Rodieck, 1989). Given the exponential decay of passive conductances with distance, signals passing between electrically coupled bipolar cells would propagate most effectively when those cells were in close proximity (Rall, 1969). This would enhance the lateral spread of current through gap junctions, producing a concomitant enhancement of motion sensitivity. Thus, the enhanced motion sensitivity in central parasol cells may arise from closer spacing of their presynaptic bipolar cells. Counter to this hypothesis, however, our computational model was unable to replicate the higher motion sensitivity values for central versus peripheral cells (Figure 7D, E). Thus, other mechanisms, such as differences in inner retinal inhibition, may contribute to the enhanced motion sensitivity observed in central versus peripheral parasol cells and further studies will be required to elucidate these putative mechanisms.
Neural interactions within the receptive field center
Consistent with previous studies, our measurements indicate that different regions of the receptive field center of parasol cells interact, as evidenced by the observed supralinear summation (Figure 6; Crook et al., 2008; Freeman et al., 2015; Petrusca et al., 2007; Turner and Rieke, 2016). Our data further indicate that some of these interactions are mediated by electrical (gap) junctions between neighboring bipolar cells (Figure 6E-H). Indeed, including electrical coupling in our computational model of the bipolar cell mosaic reproduced the observed motion sensitivity (Figure 7).
Our working hypothesis for the synaptic mechanisms mediating motion sensitivity is shown in Figure 8B. Stimulation of a bipolar cell with light causes some of the current to spread laterally via gap junctions to neighboring bipolar cells, depolarizing their synaptic terminals. This depolarization enhances subsequent light-driven depolarization of the cell which, combined with the nonlinear nature of synaptic release, produces more glutamate release than if the cell were stimulated in isolation. For example, a 10% increase in voltage at the synaptic terminal can produce >20% increase in glutamate release. Thus, the combination of electrical coupling and the synaptic output nonlinearity biases parasol cells to visual motion (Kuo et al., 2016).
The disinhibition observed during bar motion arises from amacrine cells providing crossover inhibition to parasol cells (Manookin et al., 2008; Murphy and Rieke, 2006; van Wyk et al., 2009). We briefly summarize this mechanism for OFF parasol cells, but an analogous circuit mediates disinhibition in ON parasol cells (Cafaro and Rieke, 2010, 2013). The dark bars hyperpolarize ON cone bipolar cells which, in turn, decrease glutamate release onto ON amacrine cells. These amacrine cells, then, decrease inhibitory synaptic release onto OFF parasol cell dendrites, causing depolarization of the parasol cell (Figure 8B). Our measurements indicate that disinhibition plays a larger relative role in ON parasol cells than OFF parasol cells, but further experiments will be needed to determine whether this is the case across stimulus conditions.
Motion computations arise early in the primate visual stream
The fundamental importance of the magnocellular pathway to motion processing in primates was reported more than 25 years ago (Merigan et al., 1991a; Schiller et al., 1990a, b). These studies demonstrated that monkeys with lesions to the magnocellular LGN showed impaired motion perception. Moreover, inactivating the magnocellular, but not the parvocellular, pathway strongly attenuated neural activity in a cortical area that is critical for motion processing—the medial temporal (MT) area of cortex (Maunsell et al., 1990). Further evidence for the importance of parasol cells in motion vision comes from studies of neuronal connectivity. Cells in the magnocellular LGN that receive input from parasol cells provide input to cortical regions that contribute substantially to visual motion processing in the dorsal visual pathway of primates (Blasdel and Lund, 1983; Hawken et al., 1988; Maunsell et al., 1990; Maunsell and van Essen, 1983; Movshon and Newsome, 1996; Yabuta et al., 2001). Indeed, multi-electrode array recordings in macaque retina have demonstrated that the concerted output of the parasol cell mosaic provides a precise estimate of visual motion (Chichilnisky and Kalmar, 2003; Frechette et al., 2005). Our study adds to this previous work by demonstrating that individual parasol ganglion cells exhibit motion sensitivity and identifying the locus of motion sensitivity as the output of the diffuse bipolar cell mosaic. Moreover, our results indicate that visual cortex receives a far more informative representation of visual motion from the early visual system than has been previously recognized.
While our data indicate that parasol cells are motion sensitive, the response properties of these cells are clearly distinct from those of classical direction-selective retinal ganglion cells that respond selectively to motion in specific cardinal orientations of space (Barlow and Levick, 1965; Sabbah et al., 2017). Stimulating the receptive field centers of parasol cells produced no such selective motion preference and we expect that parasol cells contribute differently to motion vision than classical direction-selective ganglion cells. How, then, do parasol cells contribute to motion vision? To answer this question, future studies will need to determine whether these cells are sensitive to specific types of visual motion and how the receptive-field surround shapes motion sensitivity (Greschner et al., 2016; Manookin, 2016).
STAR METHODS
CONTACT FOR REAGENT AND RESOURCE SHARING
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Mike Manookin (manookin@uw.edu).
EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS
Experiments were performed in an in vitro, pigment-epithelium attached preparation of the macaque monkey retina. Eyes were dissected from terminally anesthetized macaque monkeys of either sex (Macacca fascicularis, mulatta, and nemestrina) obtained through the Tissue Distribution Program of the National Primate Research Center at the University of Washington. All procedures were approved by the University of Washington Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
METHOD DETAILS
Tissue Preparation and electrophysiology
A detailed description of the preparation is given in our previous work (Manookin et al., 2015; Puller et al., 2015). Retina was superfused with warmed (32–35°C) Ames’ medium (Sigma) at ~6–8 mL min−1. Additional D-glucose (14 mM) was added to the Ames’ medium. Recordings were performed from macular, midperipheral, or peripheral retina (2–8 mm, 10–30° foveal eccentricity). Physiological data were acquired at 10 kHz using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices), Bessel filtered at 3 kHz, digitized using an ITC-18 analog-digital board (HEKA Instruments), and acquired using the Symphony acquisition software package developed in Fred Rieke’s laboratory (http://symphony-das.github.io).
Recordings were performed using borosilicate glass pipettes containing Ames medium for extracellular spike recording or, for whole-cell recording, a cesium-based internal solution containing (in mM): 105 CsCH3SO3, 10 TEA-Cl, 20 HEPES, 10 EGTA, 2 QX-314, 5 Mg-ATP, and 0.5 Tris-GTP, pH ~7.3 with CsOH, ~280 mOsm. Series resistance (~3–9 MΩ) was compensated online by 50–75%. The membrane potential was corrected offline for the approximately –11 mV liquid junction potential between the intracellular solution and the extracellular medium. Excitatory and inhibitory currents were isolated by holding the cell at the reversal potentials for inhibition/chloride (~−70 mV) and excitation (0 mV), respectively. The pipette solution typically contained 0.5% EZ-Link biocytin (ThermoFisher) for later recovery of cellular morphology.
Visual stimuli and data analysis
Visual stimuli were generated using the Stage software package developed in the Rieke lab (http://stage-vss.github.io) and displayed on a digital light projector (Lightcrafter 4500; Texas Instruments) modified with custom LEDs with peak wavelengths of 405, 505 (or 475), and 640 nm. Stimuli were focused on the photoreceptor outer segments through the microscope objective. Mean light levels were in the low to medium photopic regimes (~3 × 103 – 3.4 × 104 photoisomerizations [R*] cone−1 sec−1). To simulate smooth motion at high speeds, the moving bar stimuli (Figure 4C) were presented at refresh rates of 360–720 Hz; otherwise, stimuli were presented at 60–120 Hz. Stimuli presented at rates >120 Hz required pre-rendering the stimulus, making this technique impractical for the apparent motion/random bar stimulus that required many repeats of the random stimuli. Contrast values are given in Weber contrast. All responses were analyzed in MATLAB (R2016a+, Mathworks).
Apparent-motion, random bar, and alternating bar stimuli were presented at a refresh rate of 120 Hz. Each bar was 16 × 128 μm and bars occupied non-overlapping regions of space. Speed was varied by changing the duration of individual bar presentations. For the alternating bar experiments (Figure 2), neighboring bars were presented in random regions of the receptive-field center on each trial such that, on average, the alternating bars stimulated the same regions as the apparent-motion and random bar stimuli. Thus, these three stimulus types varied only in their spatiotemporal correlation structure, allowing for a direct comparison.
Much of the previous work studying speed tuning in primates presented data in terms of visual angle (degrees sec−1). In order to present our findings in the context of this previous work, we converted the speed of our visual stimuli from mm sec−1 to degrees sec−1. The macaque retina exhibits a nonlinear relationship between visual angle (in degrees) and retinal eccentricity (in mm)—one degree is 0.223 mm in the fovea and decreases to ~0.17 mm in the far periphery (Perry and Cowey, 1985). Thus, we used a value of 0.22 mm degree−1 for cells recorded in the more central retina (≤3 mm foveal eccentricity) and 0.2 mm degree−1 for peripheral cells (>3 mm eccentricity), making motion stimuli 10% slower for central relative to peripheral cells.
For extracellular recordings, currents were wavelet filtered to remove slow drift and amplify spikes relative to the noise (Wiltschko et al., 2008) and spikes were detected using either a custom k-means clustering algorithm or by choosing a manual threshold. Whole-cell records were leak subtracted and responses were measured relative to the median membrane currents immediately preceding stimulus onset (0.25–0.5 sec window). Summary data are presented in terms of conductance (g), which is the ratio of the current response (I) to the driving force:
where Vm is the holding potential (in mV) and E is the reversal potential (in mV). Reversal potentials of 0 mV and −70 mV were used for excitatory and inhibitory inputs, respectively.
To directly measure the input-output properties of diffuse bipolar cells for our computational model, we presented a flickering spot within the receptive field center of parasol and midget ganglion cells. On each frame, the spot contrast was drawn pseudo-randomly from a Gaussian distribution with a mean of zero and a standard deviation of 0.1–0.3 (i.e., 10–30% root-mean-squared [RMS] contrast). The linear filter (F) was then computed as the cross-correlation of the stimulus (S) and response (R).
where τ is the temporal lag. In the Fourier domain, cross-correlation is accomplished by taking the dot product of the Fourier transform of the response and the complex conjugate of the Fourier transform of the stimulus:
where is the complex conjugate of the Fourier transform of S(t), is the Fourier transform of R(t), and * denotes the complex conjugate. The denominator is the autocorrelation of the stimulus—normalizing by this value corrects for temporal correlations in the stimulus introduced by potential non-randomness in noise generation and by a finite presentation rate (60 Hz) as in (Baccus and Meister, 2002). The resulting temporal filters were modeled as a damped oscillator with an S-shaped onset as in (Schnapf et al., 1990) with the Gaussian decay replaced by an exponential decay (Angueyra and Rieke, 2013):
where A is a scaling factor, τrise is the rising-phase time constant, τdecay is the damping time constant, τperiod is the oscillator period, and φ is the phase (in degrees). Time and time constants are expressed in seconds. The best-fit parameters for the three cell types were used in the computational model, described below (midget: A, −105.3; τrise, 5.07 × 10−2 ; τdecay, 3.53 × 10−2; τperiod, −7.58 × 10−1; φ, 222.4; ON parasol: A, 198.4; τrise, 4.86 × 10−2; τdecay, 2.22 × 10−2; τperiod, 2.43 × 10−1; φ, 9.3; OFF parasol: A, –51.08; τrise, 1.77 × 10−2 ; τdecay, 3.71 × 10−2; τperiod, −5.89 × 10−1; φ, 123.3).
The output nonlinearity was then calculated by convolving the linear filter with the stimulus to generate a linear prediction (P):
The contrasts in the noise stimulus were relatively low and we sought information about the shape of the nonlinearity at high contrast for our computational model. To obtain the complete output nonlinearity, we measured the excitatory contrast-response functions in the same cells by presenting spots that were the same size as the Gaussian noise stimulus (duration, 0.5 sec). The x-axis of the resulting contrast-response function was then multiplied by a scalar to bring it into alignment with the nonlinearity generated from Gaussian noise. The resulting nonlinearity (N) was described well by a normal cumulative distribution function (Chichilnisky, 2001; Turner and Rieke, 2016):
where α is the maximal response of the cell, ε is the offset along the y-axis, γ is the maintained drive to the cell, and β is the sensitivity of the nonlinearity to the filter output (x-axis). The best-fit parameters for the three cell types were used in the computational model, described below (midget: α, 4.3; β, 3.08 × 10−2; γ, –0.5; ε, −1.2; ON parasol: α, 56.7; β, 2.5 × 10−2; γ, −1.12; ε, −7.5; OFF parasol: α, 70.7; β, 3.3 × 10−2; γ, −1.9; ε, −1.6).
Immunostaining and confocal microscopy
Specimens were immersion-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB), pH 7.4, for 30–50 min at room temperature. Following fixation and washing in PB, samples were cryoprotected in a PB solution containing 30% sucrose and stored at −20°C until processing. Specimens were incubated free floating overnight at 4°C in Alexa-488-coupled streptavidin (1:1000, Invitrogen) and DAPI (1:2000, Invitrogen) in a PB solution containing 5% normal donkey serum, 1% bovine serum albumin, 1% Triton X-100. Following fixation, samples were washed in PB and mounted on glass slides using Vectashield mounting medium (Vector Laboratories) and cover-slipped. Confocal images were taken with a Leica TCS SB8 microscope using a 20X or 63X oil-immersion objective. Subsequent image analyses were done in Fiji/ImageJ (http://fiji.sc).
Modeling
We generated models of excitatory (bipolar cell) inputs to midget ganglion cells and central and peripheral parasol cells (Kuo et al., 2016; Schwartz et al., 2012). A previous study found that the parasol cell receptive-field diameter was approximately six times greater than that of the excitatory subunits at any given eccentricity (Turner and Rieke, 2016). We used receptive-field diameters (2-SD) of 96 μm and 192 μm for central and peripheral parasol cells, respectively. Diffuse bipolar cell receptive field diameters were 12.5 μm and 32 μm for central and peripheral eccentricities, respectively. The latter value agrees well with previous measurements from mid-peripheral macaque retina (Dacey et al., 2000; Tsukamoto and Omi, 2015, 2016; Turner and Rieke, 2016). A receptive-field center size of 25 μm and 12.5 μm was used for midget ganglion and bipolar cells, respectively (Polyak, 1941).
The location of each bipolar cell was randomly shifted in x and y spatial dimensions (SD: central, 3 μm; peripheral, 4 μm; midget, 2 μm). Each bipolar cell receptive field occupied two dimensions of space (x, y) and one dimension of time (t). The spatial dimensions were comprised of a circular Gaussian with a 2-SD width of the spatial receptive field and the temporal filter was the time axis. The model proceeded in four distinct stages. First, the stimulus (S) and spatiotemporal filter (F) were convolved to generate the linear response (R) for each bipolar cell in the mosaic.
To simulate random fluctuations in membrane potential, noise was injected into each bipolar cell; this noise was calculated from the y-offset value (ε) of the output nonlinearity (described above). This value approximates the maintained excitatory conductance at zero-contrast. Assuming Poisson noise, by definition the mean and variance are equal. The noise value is, therefore, the square-root of the mean (ε):
Following noise injections, electrical coupling between bipolar cells was simulated by allowing a portion of the response to pass between the cells. The change in response in a given bipolar cell at each time point due to electrical coupling was defined as:
where g was the coupling gain (fraction of response shared by a given bipolar cell), λ was the decay constant for coupled signals over space, and di,j was the pair-wise Euclidean distance between bipolar cell (i) and the jth bipolar cell in the mosaic and n was the total number of bipolar cells. The resulting response in each cell after coupling (Rc) was then calculated as the sum of the response before coupling (R) and the response change due to coupling (ΔR):
The output of each bipolar cell (bi) was then calculated by passing the coupled response through the output nonlinearity (N):
The final stage consisted of normalizing the response of each bipolar cell relative to the center of the model ganglion cell receptive center and pooling the normalized responses to produce the time-dependent excitatory input to the model ganglion cell (E):
where σ is the 2-SD width of the ganglion cell receptive field center and di is the Euclidean distance between the bipolar cell and ganglion cell receptive field centers. The speed tuning simulations were run with a 30% stimulus contrast, and the contrast simulations were run at a speed of 12 degrees sec−1. Motion sensitivity values were calculated by averaging the responses to the apparent motion and random bar stimuli across a sample window matched to the stimulus duration.
QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
All statistical analyses were performed in MATLAB (R2016a+, Mathworks). Reported p-values in this study were calculated using either the Wilcoxon signed-rank test, for paired data, or the Mann-Whitney U test, for unpaired data. Final figures were created in MATLAB, Igor Pro, and Adobe Illustrator.
DATA AND SOFTWARE AVAILABILITY
Visual stimulation (http://stage-vss.github.io) and data acquisition (http://symphony-das.github.io) software are freely available.
KEY RESOURCES TABLE
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
Antibodies | ||
Streptavidin, Alexa Fluor 488 | ThermoFisher Cat# S11223 | RRID:AB_2336881 |
Goat polyclonal anti-ChAT | EMD Millipore Cat# AB144P | RRID:AB_90650 |
Donkey anti-goat, Alexa Fluor 546 | ThermoFisher Cat# A11056 | RRID:AB_10584485 |
Chemicals | ||
L-AP4 | Tocris Cat# 0103 | N/A |
LY341495 disodium salt | Tocris Cat# 4062 | N/A |
Strychnine hydrochloride | Sigma Cat# S0532 | N/A |
GABAzine (SR-95531) | Sigma Cat# S106 | N/A |
TPMPA | Tocris Cat# 1040 | N/A |
Ames’ medium | Sigma Cat# 1420 | N/A |
Biological Tissue | ||
Macaque retina | Regional Primate Research Center | N/A |
Software and Algorithms | ||
Stage | Rieke Lab, U. of Washington | N/A |
Symphony | Rieke Lab, U. of Washington | N/A |
MATLAB | Mathworks | N/A |
Fiji/ImageJ | http://fiji.sc/Fiji | N/A |
Igor Pro | Wavemetrics | N/A |
Adobe Illustrator | Adobe | N/A |
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Shellee Cunnington, Mark Cafaro, Jim Kuchenbecker, and Toni Haun for technical assistance. Tissue was provided by the Tissue Distribution Program at the Washington National Primate Research Center [WaNPRC; supported through National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant P51 OD-010425], and we thank the WaNPRC staff, particularly Chris English and Drew May for making these experiments possible. Fred Rieke, Raunak Sinha, Max Turner, and Will Grimes assisted in tissue preparation. We thank Greg Schwartz for assistance with the computational model. We also thank Fred Rieke, Jay Neitz, Greg Horwitz, Max Turner, Alison Weber, Silke Haverkamp, and Christian Puller for feedback on a previous version of this manuscript. This work was supported in part by grants from the NIH (NEI R01-EY027323 to M.B.M; NEI P30-EY001730 to the Vision Core; T32-NS099578 to S.S.P.), Research to Prevent Blindness Unrestricted Grant (to the University of Washington Department of Ophthalmology), Latham Vision Research Innovation Award (M.B.M), and the Alcon Young Investigator Award (M.B.M.).
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Conceptualization, M.B.M; Methodology, M.B.M.; Software, M.B.M.; Formal Analysis, M.B.M.; Investigation, M.B.M, S.S.P., C.M.L.; Resources, M.B.M.; Data Curation, M.B.M.; Writing—Original Draft, M.B.M.; Writing—Review & Editing, M.B.M, S.S.P., and C.M.L.; Visualization, M.B.M.; Supervision, M.B.M.; Project Administration, M.B.M.; Funding Acquisition, M.B.M.
DECLARATION OF INTERESTS
The authors have no financial interests to declare.
References
- Adelson EH, Bergen JR. Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. J Opt Soc Am A. 1985;2:284–299. doi: 10.1364/josaa.2.000284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ala-Laurila P, Greschner M, Chichilnisky EJ, Rieke F. Cone photoreceptor contributions to noise and correlations in the retinal output. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:1309–1316. doi: 10.1038/nn.2927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Angueyra JM, Rieke F. Origin and effect of phototransduction noise in primate cone photoreceptors. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:1692–1700. doi: 10.1038/nn.3534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asari H, Meister M. Divergence of visual channels in the inner retina. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:1581–1589. doi: 10.1038/nn.3241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baccus SA, Meister M. Fast and slow contrast adaptation in retinal circuitry. Neuron. 2002;36:909–919. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01050-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baccus SA, Ölveczky BP, Manu M, Meister M. A retinal circuit that computes object motion. J Neurosci. 2008;28:6807–6817. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4206-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baden T, Berens P, Franke K, Román Rosón M, Bethge M, Euler T. The functional diversity of retinal ganglion cells in the mouse. Nature. 2016;529:345–350. doi: 10.1038/nature16468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barlow HB, Hill RM, Levick WR. Retinal ganglion cells responding selectively to direction and speed of image motion in the rabbit. J Physiol. 1964;173:377–407. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1964.sp007463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barlow HB, Levick WR. The mechanism of directionally selective units in rabbit’s retina. J Physiol. 1965;178:477–504. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1965.sp007638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blasdel GG, Lund JS. Termination of afferent axons in macaque striate cortex. J Neurosci. 1983;3:1389–1413. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-07-01389.1983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bloomfield SA, Völgyi B. The diverse functional roles and regulation of neuronal gap junctions in the retina. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:495–506. doi: 10.1038/nrn2636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borst A, Euler T. Seeing things in motion: models, circuits, and mechanisms. Neuron. 2011;71:974–994. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borst A, Helmstaedter M. Common circuit design in fly and mammalian motion vision. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:1067–1076. doi: 10.1038/nn.4050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boycott BB, Wässle H. Morphological Classification of Bipolar Cells of the Primate Retina. Eur J Neurosci. 1991;3:1069–1088. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1991.tb00043.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cafaro J, Rieke F. Noise correlations improve response fidelity and stimulus encoding. Nature. 2010;468:964–967. doi: 10.1038/nature09570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cafaro J, Rieke F. Regulation of spatial selectivity by crossover inhibition. J Neurosci. 2013;33:6310–6320. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4964-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell FW, Robson JG. Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings. J Physiol. 1968;197:551–566. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chichilnisky EJ. A simple white noise analysis of neuronal light responses. Network. 2001;12:199–213. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chichilnisky EJ, Kalmar RS. Temporal resolution of ensemble visual motion signals in primate retina. J Neurosci. 2003;23:6681–6689. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-17-06681.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark DA, Demb JB. Parallel Computations in Insect and Mammalian Visual Motion Processing. Curr Biol. 2016;26:R1062–R1072. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conway BR, Livingstone MS. Space-time maps and two-bar interactions of different classes of direction-selective cells in macaque V-1. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89:2726–2742. doi: 10.1152/jn.00550.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crook JD, Peterson BB, Packer OS, Robinson FR, Troy JB, Dacey DM. Y-cell receptive field and collicular projection of parasol ganglion cells in macaque monkey retina. J Neurosci. 2008;28:11277–11291. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2982-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacey D, Packer OS, Diller L, Brainard D, Peterson B, Lee B. Center surround receptive field structure of cone bipolar cells in primate retina. Vision Res. 2000;40:1801–1811. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00039-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Valois RL, Cottaris NP. Inputs to directionally selective simple cells in macaque striate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95:14488–14493. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.24.14488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Valois RL, Cottaris NP, Mahon LE, Elfar SD, Wilson JA. Spatial and temporal receptive fields of geniculate and cortical cells and directional selectivity. Vision Res. 2000;40:3685–3702. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00210-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeAngelis GC, Ohzawa I, Freeman RD. Spatiotemporal organization of simple-cell receptive fields in the cat’s striate cortex. I. General characteristics and postnatal development. J Neurophysiol. 1993;69:1091–1117. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.69.4.1091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demb JB. Cellular mechanisms for direction selectivity in the retina. Neuron. 2007;55:179–186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eggers ED, Lukasiewicz PD. Multiple pathways of inhibition shape bipolar cell responses in the retina. Vis Neurosci. 2011;28:95–108. doi: 10.1017/S0952523810000209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eggers ED, McCall MA, Lukasiewicz PD. Presynaptic inhibition differentially shapes transmission in distinct circuits in the mouse retina. J Physiol. 2007;582:569–582. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.131763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enroth-Cugell C, Robson JG. The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat. J Physiol. 1966;187:517–552. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp008107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Euler T, Haverkamp S, Schubert T, Baden T. Retinal bipolar cells: elementary building blocks of vision. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014;15:507–519. doi: 10.1038/nrn3783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feigenspan A, Janssen-Bienhold U, Hormuzdi S, Monyer H, Degen J, Söhl G, Willecke K, Ammermüller J, Weiler R. Expression of connexin36 in cone pedicles and OFF-cone bipolar cells of the mouse retina. J Neurosci. 2004;24:3325–3334. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5598-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frechette ES, Sher A, Grivich MI, Petrusca D, Litke AM, Chichilnisky EJ. Fidelity of the ensemble code for visual motion in primate retina. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:119–135. doi: 10.1152/jn.01175.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman J, Field GD, Li PH, Greschner M, Gunning DE, Mathieson K, Sher A, Litke AM, Paninski L, Simoncelli EP, Chichilnisky EJ. Mapping nonlinear receptive field structure in primate retina at single cone resolution. Elife. 2015;4 doi: 10.7554/eLife.05241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gollisch T, Meister M. Eye smarter than scientists believed: neural computations in circuits of the retina. Neuron. 2010;65:150–164. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodale MA, Milner AD. Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in neurosciences. 1992;15:20–25. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greschner M, Heitman AK, Field GD, Li PH, Ahn D, Sher A, Litke AM, Chichilnisky EJ. Identification of a Retinal Circuit for Recurrent Suppression Using Indirect Electrical Imaging. Curr Biol. 2016;26:1935–1942. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hassenstein B, Reichardt W. Systemtheoretische Analyse der Zeit-, Reihenfolgenund Vorzeichenauswertung bei der Bewegungsperzeption des Rüsselkäfers Chlorosphanus. Zeitschrift für Naturforschung B. 1956;11:513–524. [Google Scholar]
- Hawken MJ, Parker AJ, Lund JS. Laminar organization and contrast sensitivity of direction-selective cells in the striate cortex of the Old World monkey. J Neurosci. 1988;8:3541–3548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-10-03541.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hecht S, Verrijp CD. Intermittent stimulation by light : III. The relation between intensity and critical fusion frequency for different retinal locations. J Gen Physiol. 1933;17:251–268. doi: 10.1085/jgp.17.2.251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilgen G, von Maltzahn J, Willecke K, Weiler R, Dedek K. Subcellular distribution of connexin45 in OFF bipolar cells of the mouse retina. J Comp Neurol. 2011;519:433–450. doi: 10.1002/cne.22526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacoby RA, Wiechmann AF, Amara SG, Leighton BH, Marshak DW. Diffuse bipolar cells provide input to OFF parasol ganglion cells in the macaque retina. J Comp Neurol. 2000;416:6–18. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(20000103)416:1<6::aid-cne2>3.0.co;2-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kántor O, Varga A, Nitschke R, Naumann A, Énzsöly A, Lukáts Á, Szabó A, Németh J, Völgyi B. Bipolar cell gap junctions serve major signaling pathways in the human retina. Brain Struct Funct. 2017;222:2603–2624. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1360-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan E, Benardete E. The dynamics of primate retinal ganglion cells. Prog Brain Res. 2001;134:17–34. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(01)34003-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan E, Shapley RM. The primate retina contains two types of ganglion cells, with high and low contrast sensitivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1986;83:2755–2757. doi: 10.1073/pnas.83.8.2755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolb H, Linberg KA, Fisher SK. Neurons of the human retina: A Golgi study. J Comp Neurol. 1992;318:147–187. doi: 10.1002/cne.903180204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovács-Öller T, Debertin G, Balogh M, Ganczer A, Orbán J, Nyitrai M, Balogh L, Kántor O, Völgyi B. Connexin36 Expression in the Mammalian Retina: A Multiple-Species Comparison. Front Cell Neurosci. 2017;11:65. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2017.00065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuo SP, Schwartz GW, Rieke F. Nonlinear Spatiotemporal Integration by Electrical and Chemical Synapses in the Retina. Neuron. 2016;90:320–332. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee BB, Wehrhahn C, Westheimer G, Kremers J. The spatial precision of macaque ganglion cell responses in relation to vernier acuity of human observers. Vision Res. 1995;35:2743–2758. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00015-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lennie P, Movshon JA. Coding of color and form in the geniculostriate visual pathway (invited review) J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2005;22:2013–2033. doi: 10.1364/josaa.22.002013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liang Z, Freed MA. The ON pathway rectifies the OFF pathway of the mammalian retina. J Neurosci. 2010;30:5533–5543. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4733-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo X, Ghosh KK, Martin PR, Grünert U. Analysis of two types of cone bipolar cells in the retina of a New World monkey, the marmoset, Callithrix jacchus. Vis Neurosci. 1999;16:707–719. doi: 10.1017/s0952523899164101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mani A, Schwartz GW. Circuit Mechanisms of a Retinal Ganglion Cell with Stimulus-Dependent Response Latency and Activation Beyond Its Dendrites. Curr Biol. 2017;27:471–482. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manookin MB. Neuroscience: Peeking Under the Sombrero. Curr Biol. 2016;26:R713–R715. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manookin MB, Beaudoin DL, Ernst ZR, Flagel LJ, Demb JB. Disinhibition combines with excitation to extend the operating range of the OFF visual pathway in daylight. J Neurosci. 2008;28:4136–4150. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4274-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manookin MB, Puller C, Rieke F, Neitz J, Neitz M. Distinctive receptive field and physiological properties of a wide-field amacrine cell in the macaque monkey retina. J Neurophysiol. 2015;114:1606–1616. doi: 10.1152/jn.00484.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshak DW, Yamada ES, Bordt AS, Perryman WC. Synaptic input to an ON parasol ganglion cell in the macaque retina: a serial section analysis. Vis Neurosci. 2002;19:299–305. doi: 10.1017/s0952523802192078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masland RH, Martin PR. The unsolved mystery of vision. Curr Biol. 2007;17:R577–582. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maunsell JH, Nealey TA, DePriest DD. Magnocellular and parvocellular contributions to responses in the middle temporal visual area (MT) of the macaque monkey. J Neurosci. 1990;10:3323–3334. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-10-03323.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maunsell JH, van Essen DC. The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey. J Neurosci. 1983;3:2563–2586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-12-02563.1983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLean J, Palmer LA. Contribution of linear spatiotemporal receptive field structure to velocity selectivity of simple cells in area 17 of cat. Vision Res. 1989;29:675–679. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(89)90029-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merigan WH, Byrne CE, Maunsell JH. Does primate motion perception depend on the magnocellular pathway? J Neurosci. 1991a;11:3422–3429. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-11-03422.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merigan WH, Katz LM, Maunsell JH. The effects of parvocellular lateral geniculate lesions on the acuity and contrast sensitivity of macaque monkeys. J Neurosci. 1991b;11:994–1001. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-04-00994.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merigan WH, Maunsell JH. Macaque vision after magnocellular lateral geniculate lesions. Vis Neurosci. 1990;5:347–352. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800000432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Movshon JA, Newsome WT. Visual response properties of striate cortical neurons projecting to area MT in macaque monkeys. J Neurosci. 1996;16:7733–7741. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-23-07733.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Movshon JA, Thompson ID, Tolhurst DJ. Spatial summation in the receptive fields of simple cells in the cat’s striate cortex. J Physiol. 1978;283:53–77. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy GJ, Rieke F. Network variability limits stimulus-evoked spike timing precision in retinal ganglion cells. Neuron. 2006;52:511–524. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nath A, Schwartz GW. Cardinal Orientation Selectivity Is Represented by Two Distinct Ganglion Cell Types in Mouse Retina. J Neurosci. 2016;36:3208–3221. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4554-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neitz J, Neitz M. Evolution of the circuitry for conscious color vision in primates. Eye (Lond) 2017;31:286–300. doi: 10.1038/eye.2016.257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ölveczky BP, Baccus SA, Meister M. Segregation of object and background motion in the retina. 2003:0028–0836. doi: 10.1038/nature01652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ölveczky BP, Baccus SA, Meister M. Retinal adaptation to object motion. Neuron. 2007;56:689–700. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oyster CW, Simpson JI, Takahashi ES, Soodak RE. Retinal ganglion cells projecting to the rabbit accessory optic system. J Comp Neurol. 1980;190:49–61. doi: 10.1002/cne.901900105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry VH, Cowey A. The ganglion cell and cone distributions in the monkey’s retina: implications for central magnification factors. Vision Res. 1985;25:1795–1810. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90004-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrusca D, Grivich MI, Sher A, Field GD, Gauthier JL, Greschner M, Shlens J, Chichilnisky EJ, Litke AM. Identification and characterization of a Y-like primate retinal ganglion cell type. J Neurosci. 2007;27:11019–11027. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2836-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polyak SL. The retina; the anatomy and the histology of the retina in man, ape, and monkey, including the consideration of visual functions, the history of physiological optics, and the histological laboratory technique. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press; 1941. [Google Scholar]
- Priebe NJ, Lisberger SG, Movshon JA. Tuning for spatiotemporal frequency and speed in directionally selective neurons of macaque striate cortex. J Neurosci. 2006;26:2941–2950. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3936-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puller C, Manookin MB, Neitz J, Rieke F, Neitz M. Broad thorny ganglion cells: a candidate for visual pursuit error signaling in the primate retina. J Neurosci. 2015;35:5397–5408. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4369-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Purpura K, Kaplan E, Shapley RM. Background light and the contrast gain of primate P and M retinal ganglion cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1988;85:4534–4537. doi: 10.1073/pnas.85.12.4534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rall W. Time constants and electrotonic length of membrane cylinders and neurons. Biophys J. 1969;9:1483–1508. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(69)86467-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rieke F, Bodnar DA, Bialek W. Naturalistic stimuli increase the rate and efficiency of information transmission by primary auditory afferents. Proc Biol Sci. 1995;262:259–265. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1995.0204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodieck RW. Starburst amacrine cells of the primate retina. J Comp Neurol. 1989;285:18–37. doi: 10.1002/cne.902850104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rovamo J, Raninen A. Critical flicker frequency as a function of stimulus area and luminance at various eccentricities in human cone vision: a revision of Granit-Harper and Ferry-Porter laws. Vision Res. 1988;28:785–790. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(88)90025-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rust NC, Schwartz O, Movshon JA, Simoncelli EP. Spatiotemporal elements of macaque v1 receptive fields. Neuron. 2005;46:945–956. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sabbah S, Gemmer JA, Bhatia-Lin A, Manoff G, Castro G, Siegel JK, Jeffery N, Berson DM. A retinal code for motion along the gravitational and body axes. Nature. 2017;546:492–497. doi: 10.1038/nature22818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiller PH, Logothetis NK, Charles ER. Functions of the colour-opponent and broad-band channels of the visual system. Nature. 1990a;343:68–70. doi: 10.1038/343068a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiller PH, Logothetis NK, Charles ER. Role of the color-opponent and broad-band channels in vision. Vis Neurosci. 1990b;5:321–346. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800000420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schnapf JL, Nunn BJ, Meister M, Baylor DA. Visual transduction in cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. J Physiol. 1990;427:681–713. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp018193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz GW, Okawa H, Dunn FA, Morgan JL, Kerschensteiner D, Wong RO, Rieke F. The spatial structure of a nonlinear receptive field. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:1572–1580. doi: 10.1038/nn.3225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simpson JI, Alley KE. Visual climbing fiber input to rabbit vestibulo-cerebellum: a source of direction-specific information. Brain Res. 1974;82:302–308. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(74)90610-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simpson JI, Soodak RE, Hess R. The accessory optic system and its relation to the vestibulocerebellum. Prog Brain Res. 1979;50:715–724. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60868-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha R, Hoon M, Baudin J, Okawa H, Wong RO, Rieke F. Cellular and Circuit Mechanisms Shaping the Perceptual Properties of the Primate Fovea. Cell. 2017;168:413–426.e412. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sivyer B, Taylor WR, Vaney DI. Uniformity detector retinal ganglion cells fire complex spikes and receive only light-evoked inhibition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:5628–5633. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909621107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor WR, He S, Levick WR, Vaney DI. Dendritic computation of direction selectivity by retinal ganglion cells. Science. 2000;289:2347–2350. doi: 10.1126/science.289.5488.2347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor WR, Vaney DI. Diverse synaptic mechanisms generate direction selectivity in the rabbit retina. J Neurosci. 2002;22:7712–7720. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-17-07712.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsukamoto Y, Omi N. OFF bipolar cells in macaque retina: type-specific connectivity in the outer and inner synaptic layers. Front Neuroanat. 2015;9:122. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2015.00122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsukamoto Y, Omi N. ON Bipolar Cells in Macaque Retina: Type-Specific Synaptic Connectivity with Special Reference to OFF Counterparts. Front Neuroanat. 2016;10:104. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2016.00104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner MH, Rieke F. Synaptic Rectification Controls Nonlinear Spatial Integration of Natural Visual Inputs. Neuron. 2016;90:1257–1271. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tyler CW. Analysis of visual modulation sensitivity. II. Peripheral retina and the role of photoreceptor dimensions. J Opt Soc Am A. 1985;2:393–398. doi: 10.1364/josaa.2.000393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Wyk M, Wässle H, Taylor WR. Receptive field properties of ON- and OFF-ganglion cells in the mouse retina. Vis Neurosci. 2009;26:297–308. doi: 10.1017/S0952523809990137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaney DI, Sivyer B, Taylor WR. Direction selectivity in the retina: symmetry and asymmetry in structure and function. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13:194–208. doi: 10.1038/nrn3165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venkataramani S, Taylor WR. Orientation selectivity in rabbit retinal ganglion cells is mediated by presynaptic inhibition. J Neurosci. 2010;30:15664–15676. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2081-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venkataramani S, Taylor WR. Synaptic Mechanisms Generating Orientation Selectivity in the ON Pathway of the Rabbit Retina. J Neurosci. 2016;36:3336–3349. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1432-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Völgyi B, Kovács-Oller T, Atlasz T, Wilhelm M, Gábriel R. Gap junctional coupling in the vertebrate retina: variations on one theme? Prog Retin Eye Res. 2013;34:1–18. doi: 10.1016/j.preteyeres.2012.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wässle H. Parallel processing in the mammalian retina. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:747–757. doi: 10.1038/nrn1497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe M, Rodieck RW. Parasol and midget ganglion cells of the primate retina. J Comp Neurol. 1989;289:434–454. doi: 10.1002/cne.902890308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waugh SJ, Hess RF. Suprathreshold temporal-frequency discrimination in the fovea and the periphery. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 1994;11:1199–1212. doi: 10.1364/josaa.11.001199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiltschko AB, Gage GJ, Berke JD. Wavelet filtering before spike detection preserves waveform shape and enhances single-unit discrimination. J Neurosci Methods. 2008;173:34–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.05.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yabuta NH, Sawatari A, Callaway EM. Two functional channels from primary visual cortex to dorsal visual cortical areas. Science. 2001;292:297–300. doi: 10.1126/science.1057916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zaghloul KA, Boahen K, Demb JB. Different circuits for ON and OFF retinal ganglion cells cause different contrast sensitivities. J Neurosci. 2003;23:2645–2654. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-07-02645.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.