Abstract
Objective
To determine the change in neurocognitive test performance in children with primary hypertension after initiation of antihypertensive therapy.
Study design
Subjects with hypertension and normotensive control subjects had neurocognitive testing at baseline and again after 1-year, during which time the subjects with hypertension received antihypertensive therapy. Subjects completed tests of general intelligence, attention, memory, executive function, and processing speed, and parents completed rating scales of executive function.
Results
Fifty-five subjects with hypertension and 66 normotensive control subjects underwent both baseline and 1-year assessments. Overall, the BP of subjects with hypertension improved (24-hour systolic BP load: mean baseline versus 1-year, 58% versus 38%, P < .001). Primary multivariable analyses showed that the hypertension group improved in scores of subtests of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), Grooved Pegboard, and Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Tower Test (p < .05). However, the control group also improved in the same measures with similar effects sizes. Secondary analyses by effectiveness of antihypertensive therapy showed that subjects with persistent ambulatory hypertension at 1-year (n = 17) did not improve in subtests of RAVLT and had limited improvement in Grooved Pegboard.
Conclusions
Overall, children with hypertension did not improve in neurocognitive test performance after 1 year of antihypertensive therapy, beyond that also seen in normotensive controls, suggesting improvements with age or practice effects due to repeated neurocognitive testing. However, the degree to which antihypertensive therapy improves BP may affect its impact upon neurocognitive function. .
Keywords: neuropsychological testing, blood pressure, obesity, treatment
Young adults with hypertension have lower performance on neurocognitive testing compared with matched normotensive control subjects, a finding postulated to represent an early manifestation of hypertensive target organ damage to the brain.(1, 2) Furthermore, hypertension in both adolescence and young adulthood has been associated with decreased neurocognitive test performance in mid-life, raising concern for a link between early hypertension and subsequent cognitive decline later in life.(3–5) Despite these observations, results of studies of the impact of hypertension treatment in adults on subsequent neurocognitive test performance have been mostly inconsistent and inconclusive.(6) As a consequence, a recent scientific statement from the American Heart Association on the impact of hypertension on cognitive function identified as a critical question whether treatment as early in life as possible, such as treatment in adolescence, would offer advantages for subsequent cognitive function.(7)
Studies focusing on the impact of childhood primary hypertension during youth itself have found that children with hypertension often demonstrate similar target organ damage findings as do adults, particularly left ventricular hypertrophy and increased carotid intima-media thickness.(8, 9) However, there have been only limited assessments of hypertensive target organ effects on the brains of children. We established a prospective, multicenter study of neurocognition in children with primary hypertension.(10) Our specific aims were to compare the performance on neurocognitive testing of newly diagnosed subjects with untreated hypertension with that of the performance of matched normotensive controls at baseline and to evaluate the effect of 1-year of antihypertensive therapy on neurocognitive test performance. We recently reported results of the baseline comparison, showing that children with hypertension had worse performance on neurocognitive testing compared with that of the normotensive control subjects, particularly in the domains of attention, learning, and memory.(11) Here, we report the results of the effect of 1-year of antihypertensive therapy on neurocognitive test performance in the same cohort. We hypothesized that children with primary hypertension would show improvement in neurocognitive test performance after antihypertensive therapy; whereas the neurocognitive test performance of the normotensive control subjects would remain unchanged over the same time period.
Methods
The participants in this study were the subjects with hypertension and control subjects from our initial report who subsequently returned for reassessment after 12 months. During the 1-year interval between study visits, the subjects with hypertension received standard of care antihypertensive therapy as detailed below. Control subjects were not seen between the initial assessment and the 1-year visit. Hypertension and control subjects completed the same neurocognitive assessment at baseline and again at 1- year. The study methods have been previously described in detail.(10)
Participating recruitment sites included the University of Rochester, Emory University, Maimonides Medical Center, and the McGovern Medical School at UTHealth. Newly diagnosed children ages 10–18 years with untreated hypertension were enrolled through the Pediatric Hypertension Clinics at each site. For comparison, normotensive, healthy 10- to 18-year-old children were enrolled from participating general pediatrics and family medicine primary care practices. Our initial report compared 75 hypertension and 75 control subjects who were frequency matched for sex, proportion with obesity [body mass index (BMI) ≥95th percentile], and maternal education. Race and ethnicity were not formally matched, but the results were adjusted for these characteristics in the multivariate analyses. At baseline, each subject with hypertension had a history of office hypertension that was confirmed with 24-hour ambulatory blood pressure monitoring (ABPM) by the presence of mean systolic and/or diastolic awake blood pressure (BP), mean sleep BP, or both ≥95th percentile.(12) Hypertension subjects were also included if the mean ambulatory BP was <95th percentile, but the subject had both BP load >25% (ambulatory prehypertension) and left ventricular hypertrophy (LVH) on echocardiogram. Only 3 hypertension subjects were included by these alternate criteria; therefore, their results were combined with the other hypertension subjects in the current analysis. Normotensive control subjects were required to have office normotension, confirmed by mean awake and sleep SBP and DBP <95th percentile and 24-hour SBP and DBP load <25% on ABPM.(12) Both hypertension and control subjects underwent repeat ABPM at the 1-year visit to assess the adequacy of the hypertension treatment in the hypertension subjects and to confirm the persistence of normotension in the control subjects. All subjects with hypertension underwent a complete 2-dimensional echocardiogram at the baseline visit that was read centrally at the University of Rochester. Left ventricular hypertrophy was defined as a left ventricular mass index ≥95th percentile.(13) Echocardiogram and ABPM procedures have been described in our earlier report.(11) All subjects had baseline central laboratory evaluations, including fasting lipid profile, insulin level, glucose, and C- reactive protein (CRP). Homeostatic model assessment (HOMA) for insulin resistance was calculated as glucose x insulin/405.
Exclusion criteria were as follows: being on medication for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), having a pre-existing learning problem/disability (defined as having an Individual Educational Plan or Section 504 Plan at school), any disorder of cognitive impairment, history of chelation treatment for elevated lead level, history of chronic disease (known renal, cardiovascular, gastrointestinal tract, hepatic, endocrine, or rheumatologic disease), pregnancy or breastfeeding, previous sleep study diagnosis of obstructive sleep apnea, a diagnosis of secondary hypertension, and previous or current treatment with antihypertensive medication. The study was approved by the institutional review board at each site, and parental permission was obtained (as well as subject assent when age-appropriate).
Hypertension and control subjects underwent the same neurocognitive assessment at baseline and at the 1-year follow-up visit, a study design that allowed the assessment of change in test performance in the hypertensive subjects after one year of antihypertensive therapy. The neurocognitive assessment in the control subjects was repeated at 1-year in order to detect any improvement in test performance due to increasing age or due to the practice effect, the propensity for scores to improve by virtue of learned strategies or recall of task content from repeated test administration.(14) As previously described, the neurocognitive assessment included both laboratory performance-based measures and behavior rating scales.(10) The laboratory tests included measures of executive function (measures of problem solving/planning, set-shifting, response inhibition, vigilance, and working memory), verbal learning and memory, attention, fine-motor dexterity, and general intellectual functioning. Behavior ratings of executive function included the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF), completed by the parent. Table I lists the neurocognitive measures, along with the primary subtests for each test and the cognitive domain assessed. Mood symptoms were also evaluated with the child self- report measures of the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC) and the Child Depression Inventory (CDI). Lastly, parents completed the Sleep-Related Breathing Disorder Scale of the Pediatric Sleep Questionnaire (PSQ-SRBD) as an estimate of disordered sleep, a common comorbidity in obese children and a potential confounder of neurocognitive test performance.(15, 16)
Table 1.
Neurocognitive Test Battery
Neurocognitive Measures | Subtests | Cognitive Domain |
---|---|---|
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) | List A Trial 1* List A Total* List A Short Delay Recall* List A Long Delay Recall |
Attention, learning, and memory Learning and memory Learning and memory Learning and memory |
CogState Groton Maze Learning Task (GMLT) | GMLT Total Error GMLT Delayed Recall* |
Planning/Problem Solving Memory |
Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence (WASI) | Vocabulary*
Matrix Reasoning Full Scale IQ (FSIQ)* |
General intelligence |
Grooved Pegboard Test | Time to completion Dominant hand* Non-dominant hand |
Fine motor dexterity |
Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (DKEFS), Tower Test | Total Achievement | Planning/Problem Solving |
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children -4th ed (WISC-IV) | Digit Span Forward and Backward Spatial Span Forward and Backward |
Working memory, Attention |
CogState Set Shifting | Set Shifting Total Error | Set Shifting |
Conners’ Continuous Performance Test-II (CPT-II) | Omission Errors Commission Errors Variability Detectability |
Attention and vigilance Response inhibition Attention Attention |
Parent Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) | Metacognition Index (MI) Behavior Regulation Index (BRI) |
Behavioral correlates of executive function |
Measures previously reported to distinguish HTN from control subjects at baseline in the same cohort
This study was not a clinical trial, but instead an observational study of neurocognitive changes that occur during usual standard of care. We did not randomize subjects to different treatments. Instead the hypertension subjects were treated according to local standards and national consensus guidelines as decided by the treating physician,(17) with some standardization across sites as detailed below. The control subjects were normotensive children who did not receive any treatment. Antihypertensive treatment began only after completion of the baseline neurocognitive assessment. All hypertension subjects met with a nutritionist at the beginning of the study and again at 3 - 6 months to receive exercise counseling and to review the Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension (DASH) diet, salt restriction, and if needed, weight loss counseling.(17) Antihypertensive medication was initiated at the discretion of the site physician with the following standardizations: For hypertension subjects with stage 1 hypertension without LVH, antihypertensive medication was considered after three months if there was no indication of improvement with lifestyle modification. Antihypertensive medication could be started sooner in subjects who had already failed a concerted effort at lifestyle modification in the judgment of the site physician. In accordance with consensus guidelines, subjects with stage 2 hypertension and/or LVH were started on antihypertensive medication from the outset. When antihypertensive medication was indicated, the initial drug was lisinopril, as angiotensin converting enzyme (ACE) inhibitors are the most common class of antihypertensive medication prescribed by pediatric nephrologists for the treatment of primary hypertension.(18) Female subjects of child bearing potential and their parents were counseled on the potential teratogenic risks of ACE inhibitors, and urine pregnancy tests were performed prior to initiating ACE inhibitor therapy, and then every 3 months for the duration of the study. Blood chemistry measures for potassium and creatinine were performed within 4 to 6 weeks of starting lisinopril. Thiazide diuretics or calcium channel blockers were used as second- line agents if needed. Amlodipine was recommended as an alternate choice for subjects who did not tolerate lisinopril as the first-line treatment. Clonidine and beta blockers were avoided.
Once antihypertensive medication was initiated, subjects were seen every 4 to 6 weeks in the site's Pediatric Hypertension Clinic to have their therapy titrated until adequate control of hypertension was achieved, defined as manual office BP < 95th percentile.(17) Initial dosing of antihypertensive medication and subsequent dose adjustments were at the discretion of the site physician. Once adequate control was achieved, the hypertension subjects were followed every 3 months to monitor BP control and adjust antihypertensive therapy accordingly. The number of visits with adequate control before extending the visit interval to every 3 months was at the discretion of the site physician. If a medication increase was needed, the subject was seen again every 4 to 6 weeks until BP control was re-established. Subjects achieving BP control with lifestyle modification alone were seen in the Pediatric Hypertension Clinic every 3 months throughout the course of the study. Upon enrollment, the subjects were given a home digital BP monitor (OMRON BP760) to measure their home BP once daily in the week prior to their next visit. The subjects kept a log of these measurements and they were asked to bring the log to all visits to help determine the response to therapy and to determine the presence of a white-coat effect at the visit. At both the baseline and 1-year neurocognitive assessment, BP was measured three times at 5 minute intervals by an automated oscillometric device at the site Clinical Research Center, and the BP for that study visit was calculated as the average of the 2nd and 3rd reading.
In order to encourage compliance, the hypertension subjects on antihypertensive medication were contacted at least monthly by the site study coordinator to remind the subject to take his/her medication. Hypertension subjects managed with lifestyle modification alone were not contacted monthly. Coordinators also reminded all subjects of their upcoming interim visit a week prior to the scheduled appointment. When a hypertension subject missed an interim visit, they were contacted within a week by the study coordinator and rescheduled for as soon as possible. Subjects with multiple consecutive missed interim visits were repeatedly rescheduled until the site physician felt that further efforts were futile and that the subject was lost to follow-up. Control subjects were contacted every 3 months to update their contact information.
Statistical Analyses
Demographics and baseline outcomes were compared between hypertensive and control groups using t-tests or Chi-square tests when appropriate. For the primary analyses, the changes in neurocognitive test scores from baseline to 1-year were compared between the hypertension and control groups using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) models with the study group as an independent variable after adjusting for corresponding baseline of the neurocognitive test score, age, sex, PSQ-SRBD score, maternal education (< high school, high school, college, > college), household income (<$25,000, $25,000–75,000, >$75,000), African-American race, baseline body mass index (BMI) z-score, change in BMI z-score from baseline to 1-year, and baseline HOMA value. Least Square Means (LSM) for within group effect, along with the between group treatment differences, and associated p-values were calculated. All tests of treatment effects were conducted as a two-sided alpha level of 0.05. Effect sizes (ES) were calculated by dividing the study group differences in the LSM changes by the pooled standard deviation of the changes. We did not adjust for testing of multiple hypotheses in the analyses. Patient baseline characteristics were compared between those subjects who remained and those subjects who were lost to follow-up. To ensure that the assumption of data missing completely at random for the primary analyses did not bias the results, we also carried out a version of the analyses after multiple imputation procedures for missing data using a multiple imputation algorithm based on multiple regression models. Using this imputation regression model, a missing value for a subject was imputed as a draw from the predictive distribution given the subject’s baseline values and characteristics. This process was repeated five times and the results were combined into one multiple imputation inference.(19, 20)
Because a subset of the hypertension subjects still had BP in the ambulatory hypertension range at the 1-year assessment, we performed a secondary analysis by effectiveness of antihypertensive therapy with three groups: hypertension improved (HTN-I), hypertension not improved (HTN-NI), and controls with sustained normotension (Con-S). ANCOVA models were used to fit the data similarly as the primary analysis. All statistical analyses were conducted using SAS Version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, North Carolina.).
Results
Of 150 subjects, who were enrolled initially 121 (81%) provided baseline data returned for the 1-year visit, including 55 (73%) of the 75 subjects with hypertension and 66 (88%) of the 75 control subjects (p = 0.04). The subjects who returned were similar to the subjects who did not return in age, sex, race, ethnicity, maternal education, household income, WASI full scale IQ, CDI and MASC scores, and the baseline parent BRIEF MI summary score (data not shown). However, subjects who did not return had higher (worse) mean score on the baseline parent BRIEF BRI summary score compared with subjects who did return (T-score, 54.5 vs 46.6, p < 0.001). Among subjects who did not return, a higher proportion had BRI scores in the clinically significant range for executive dysfunction [BRI T-score ≥65; 6 of 29 (21%) vs 5 of 121 (4%), p = 0.007). Of the 6 subjects who did not return and who had BRI scores in the clinically significant range, 5 were from the hypertension subject group.
The 55 subjects with hypertension and 66 control subjects with both baseline and 1- year follow-up data were similar in age, sex, race, ethnicity, laboratory evaluation, and maternal education, but differed in household income. By definition, the hypertensive and control groups differed in baseline BP (Table 2; available at www.jpeds.com).
Table 2.
Baseline Demographic Characteristics
Characteristics | Normotensive controls (N = 66) | Hypertension subjects (N = 55) | P value |
---|---|---|---|
Age, y | 15.3 ± 1.9 | 15.0 ± 2.3 | 0.35 |
BMI z score | 1.88 ± 0.60 | 1.79 ± 0.80 | 0.50 |
Obese, % | 71.2 | 63.6 | 0.43 |
African American, % | 30.3 | 38.2 | 0.44 |
Hispanic, % | 16.7 | 18.1 | 0.99 |
Maternal education, % | 0.72 | ||
Less than high school | 15.1 | 9.1 | |
High school | 37.9 | 41.8 | |
College | 40.9 | 40.0 | |
More than college | 6.1 | 9.1 | |
Household income, % | 0.03 | ||
Low | 19.7 | 38.2 | |
Mid | 48.5 | 27.3 | |
High | 31.8 | 34.5 | |
Male, % | 65.2 | 74.5 | 0.32 |
CRP, mg/L | 2.57 ± 3.17 | 2.12 ± 3.23 | 0.45 |
HOMA-IR | 5.4 ± 3.7 | 4.5 ± 3.3 | 0.15 |
Triglycerides, mg/dL | 96 ± 59 | 112 ± 73 | 0.19 |
LDL cholesterol, mg/dL | 88.8 ± 26 | 92.0 ± 32.5 | 0.55 |
HDL cholesterol, mg/dL | 47.2 ± 11.5 | 46.2 ± 12.3 | 0.65 |
24-h SBP load, % | 6.2 ± 6.2 | 57.6 ± 16.9 | <0.001 |
24-h DBP load, % | 5.3 ± 4.8 | 28.0 ± 17.6 | <0.001 |
Anti-Hypertensive Treatment
Of the 55 subjects with hypertension, 11 were treated with lifestyle modification counseling alone and 44 were treated with lifestyle modification counseling and lisinopril. Three patients required the addition of a second antihypertensive medication, 2 with chlorthalidone and 1 with amlodipine. Two patients were switched from lisinopril to losartan for complaints of cough and headache, respectively. One patient developed increased creatinine on lisinopril and was switched to amlodipine.
Comparison of ABPM measures at baseline and at 1-year confirmed that the hypertension subjects had lower BP after 1-year of antihypertensive therapy. Control subject blood pressure remained similar over the year in most ABPM measures, but increased by nighttime SBP index and 24hr SBP load. Body mass index (BMI) z-score did not change among hypertensive subjects, whereas there was a trend toward decrease in BMI z-score in the control subjects (Table 3). Further analysis of ABPM measures showed that of the 55 hypertensive subjects, 38 (69%) had successful treatment of their hypertension defined as ambulatory systolic and diastolic BP index <1.0 for both awake and sleep periods (24hr SBP load, baseline vs 1-year, 58.6% vs 24.5%, p < 0.001). By contrast, 17 (31%) of the hypertensive subjects still had persistent ambulatory hypertension at the 1-year assessment, defined as systolic and/or diastolic BP index ≥1.0 for either the awake or sleep period or both (24hr SBP load, baseline vs 1-year, 55.4% vs 69.5%, p = 0.005; Table 4 [available at www.jpeds.com]). The subjects with successful treatment did not differ significantly from those with persistent ambulatory hypertension in age, baseline BMI z-score, sex, maternal education, household income, race, or ethnicity (data not shown). Of the 17 subjects with persistent ambulatory hypertension, 4 (24%) had elevated office systolic and/or diastolic BP at the 1-year assessment, but 13 (76%) had normal office BP, consistent with ambulatory masked hypertension. Of the 38 hypertension subjects with successful treatment of their hypertension, 33 (87%) received antihypertensive medication compared with 9 (53%) of 17 hypertension subjects with persistent ambulatory hypertension (p = 0.08). Of the 66 control subjects, 56 had sustained ambulatory normotension at the 1-year assessment, but 10 had developed elevated ambulatory BP.
Table 3.
ABPM and BMI measures at the baseline and 1-year assessments
Normotensive controls N = 66 | Hypertension N = 55 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Measure | Baseline | 1-year | P value | Baseline | 1-year | P value |
dSBP index | 0.87 ± 0.05 | 0.88 ± 0.06 | 0.07 | 1.02 ± 0.05 | 0.96 ± 0.06 | < 0.001 |
dDBP index | 0.80 ± 0.06 | 0.80 ± 0.06 | 0.83 | 0.91 ± 0.08 | 0.85 ± 0.07 | < 0.001 |
nSBP index | 0.87 ± 0.06 | 0.88 ± 0.06 | 0.02 | 1.02 ± 0.07 | 0.98 ± 0.10 | 0.014 |
nDBP index | 0.82 ± 0.07 | 0.83 ± 0.08 | 0.22 | 0.95 ± 0.10 | 0.91 ± 0.11 | 0.013 |
24hr SBP load | 6.2 ± 6.2 | 10.1 ± 10.1 | 0.001 | 57.6 ± 16.9 | 37.8 ± 26.3 | < 0.001 |
24hr DBP load | 5.3 ± 4.8 | 6.6 ± 6.0 | 0.098 | 28.0 ± 17.6 | 17.6 ± 13.2 | < 0.001 |
BMI z-score | 1.88 ± 0.60 | 1.80 ± 0.72 | 0.07 | 1.79 ± 0.80 | 1.80 ± 0.81 | 0.74 |
dSBP = daytime systolic BP index, dDBP = daytime diastolic BP index, nSBP = nighttime systolic BP index, nDBP index = nighttime diastolic BP index.
Table 4.
ABPM parameters in hypertension and control subgroups
Normotensive controls | Hypertension | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sustained Normotension N = 56 | Developed Increased BP N = 10 | Improved N = 38 | Persistent Ambulatory Hypertension N = 17 | |||||||||
Parameter | Baseline | 1-year | P value | Baseline | 1-year | P value | Baseline | 1-year | P value | Baseline | 1-year | P value |
dSBP index | 0.86 ± 0.05 | 0.86 ± 0.05 | 0.88 | 0.88 ± 0.03 | 0.96 ± 0.03 | <0.001 | 1.03 ± 0.05 | 0.93 ± 0.05 | <0.001 | 1.01 ± 0.05 | 1.04 ± 0.04 | 0.07 |
dDBP index | 0.80 ± 0.06 | 0.79 ± 0.06 | 0.40 | 0.79 ± 0.04 | 0.85 ± 0.06 | 0.008 | 0.91 ± 0.08 | 0.83 ± 0.06 | <0.001 | 0.92 ±0.09 | 0.91 ± 0.05 | 0.99 |
nSBP index | 0.86 ± 0.05 | 0.87 ± 0.06 | 0.14 | 0.91 ± 0.05 | 0.95 ± 0.05 | 0.025 | 1.00 ± 0.07 | 0.93 ± 0.07 | <0.001 | 1.04 ± 0.08 | 1.08 ± 0.10 | 0.044 |
nDBP index | 0.82 ± 0.07 | 0.83 ± 0.08 | 0.51 | 0.81 ± 0.06 | 0.86 ± 0.08 | 0.21 | 0.94 ± 0.09 | 0.87 ± 0.08 | <0.001 | 0.98 ± 0.11 | 1.01 ± 0.12 | 0.11 |
24hr SBP load | 5.4 ± 5.6 | 6.9 ± 6.6 | 0.12 | 11.0 ± 7.8 | 29.4 ± 5.2 | <0.001 | 58.6 ± 15.8 | 24.5 ± 17.2 | <0.001 | 55.4 ± 19.5 | 69.5 ± 14.0 | 0.005 |
24hr DBP load | 5.5 ± 4.8 | 6.1 ± 6.0 | 0.40 | 4.5 ± 5.2 | 9.3 ± 5.1 | 0.24 | 26.8 ± 15.2 | 12.7 ± 9.2 | <0.001 | 30.6 ± 22.2 | 29.3 ± 14.0 | 0.47 |
dSBP = daytime systolic BP index, dDBP = daytime diastolic BP index, nSBP = nighttime systolic BP index, nDBP index = nighttime diastolic BP index.
Neurocognitive Test Results
Primary Analyses
Within-group changes from baseline to 1-year with effect sizes are listed in Table 5 for both the hypertension and control groups. Comparisons of the between group changes are also shown to evaluate whether any improvement in the hypertension group was larger than that which may have also occurred in the control group. The hypertension group improved in scores of subtests of the RAVLT (verbal learning and memory), Grooved Pegboard (manual dexterity), and DKEFS Tower Test (executive function) with moderate effect sizes. However, the control group also improved in the same measures with similar effects sizes. There was no statistical difference in the change in scores between groups for these measures. The control group improved in scores for WISC-IV Spatial Span Forward (visual attention and memory) with a moderate effect size, whereas the hypertension group did not, and the between group comparison showed that the change in scores for the control and hypertension groups for this measure were significantly different. There were several other subtests where either the hypertension or the control group had statistically different within-group change in scores with small effect sizes, but where the difference in change between groups was not statistically different. Repeat analyses after multiple imputation procedures to account for missing data did not show significantly different results from the primary analyses (data not shown).
Table 5.
Results of Primary Analyses of Neurocognitive Test Performance
Neurocognitive Measure | Group | Baseline | LSM change | Effect Size Within group |
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
RAVLT (total correct, raw scores) | ||||
HTN | 6.3 | 0.18 | 0.11 | |
List A Trial 1 | Con | 6.7 | 0.35 | 0.22 |
HTN | 48.0 | 1.40 | 0.23 | |
List A Total (Trials 1–5) | Con | 53 | 2.07** | 0.35 |
HTN | 9.4 | 0.73** | 0.41 | |
List A Short Delay Recall | Con | 10.4 | 0.78*** | 0.44 |
HTN | 9.4 | 0.87** | 0.43 | |
List A Long Delay Recall | Con | 10.3 | 1.17*** | 0.59 |
| ||||
WASI (T scores) | ||||
Vocabulary | HTN | 47.5 | −0.42 | −0.07 |
Con | 50.5 | 0.12 | 0.02 | |
Matrix Reasoning | HTN | 48.9 | 2.5* | 0.32 |
Con | 59.7 | 1.2 | 0.15 | |
| ||||
CogState GMLT | ||||
GMLT Total Error | HTN | 59.9 | −0.18 | 0.12 |
Con | 56.1 | −4.4* | 0.28 | |
GMLT Delayed Recall Error | HTN | 9.6 | −0.87 | 0.19 |
Con | 7.0 | −1.46* | 0.33 | |
| ||||
Grooved Pegboard (time, s) | ||||
Dominant hand | HTN | 84.3 | −5.6*** | 0.59 |
Con | 78.0 | −6.7*** | 0.71 | |
Non-dominant hand | HTN | 89.4 | −4.9** | 0.42 |
Con | 87.9 | −7.1*** | 0.61 | |
| ||||
CPT–II (T scores) | ||||
Omissions errors | HTN | 49.6 | 1.7 | −0.15 |
Con | 48.5 | 1.4 | −0.12 | |
Commissions errors | HTN | 54.2 | −1.3 | 0.15 |
Con | 52.1 | −1.5 | 0.17 | |
Variability | HTN | 46.6 | 2.7 | −0.24 |
Con | 46.3 | 3.4* | −0.31 | |
Detectability | HTN | 55.0 | −1.6 | 0.19 |
Con | 51.9 | −1.8 | 0.21 | |
| ||||
WISC-IV (scaled scores) | ||||
Spatial Span Forward | HTN | 9.1 | −0.01 | −0.01 |
Con | 9.3 | 1.03** † | 0.40 | |
Spatial Span Backward | HTN | 10.2 | 0.55* | 0.31 |
Con | 10.3 | 0.21 | 0.12 | |
Digit Span Forward | HTN | 9.4 | 0.37 | 0.17 |
Con | 9.0 | 0.63* | 0.30 | |
Digit Span Backward | HTN | 9.4 | 0.01 | 0.01 |
Con | 9.4 | 0.39 | 0.16 | |
| ||||
CogState Set Shift | ||||
Total Error | HTN | 38.5 | −2.4 | 0.18 |
Con | 38.4 | −4.6* | 0.33 | |
| ||||
DKEFS Tower (scaled scores) | ||||
Total Achievement | HTN | 9.5 | 0.93** | 0.40 |
Con | 9.2 | 0.85** | 0.37 | |
| ||||
Parent BRIEF (T scores) | ||||
BRI | HTN | 48 | −0.22 | 0.04 |
Con | 45.4 | −0.01 | 0.01 | |
MI | HTN | 49.8 | −1.2 | 0.18 |
Con | 48.4 | −1.3 | 0.20 |
RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
WASI = Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence
CogState GMLT = CogState Groton Maze Learning Test
CPT-II = Conners’ Continuous Performance Test–II
WISC-IV = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV
DKEFS Tower = Delis Kaplan Executive Function System Tower
Parent BRIEF = Parent Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function
BRI = Behavior Regulation Index
MI = Metacognition Index
HTN = Hypertension, all subjects, N = 55
Con = Control subjects, all subjects, N = 66
LSM = Least Square Means
Within group treatment differences, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001
Between group comparisons, † = p < 0.05 for HTN vs. Con
Sign (+/−) of effect size oriented such that positive values indicate better performance in relation to baseline and vice versa
All comparisons adjusted for age, sex, maternal education, race, income, BMI z-score at baseline, change in BMI z-score in 1 year, Pediatric Sleep Questionnaire, and HOMA (HOMA is a measure of insulin resistance).
Secondary Analyses
Table 6 shows neurocognitive test results according to effectiveness of treatment. For these analyses, there were three groups: 1) Hypertension subjects whose BP improved by ABPM (HTN-I, N = 38), 2) Hypertension subjects whose BP did not improve by ABPM (HTN-NI, N = 17), and 3) Control subjects who sustained normotension by ABPM (Con-S, N = 56). The group of control subjects who developed elevated BP (N = 10) was felt to be too small for secondary analysis of the neurocognitive data. Similar to results in the primary analyses, on multivariable analyses, HTN-I subjects and Con-S subjects showed significant within-group improvement in subtests of the RAVLT, with moderate effect sizes. In contrast, the HTN-NI group scored worse on the RAVLT List A Trial 1 and RAVLT List A Total subtests at 1-year compared with baseline, and the between-group difference was statistically significant between the HTN-NI group and both the HTN-I and the Con-S groups. HTN-NI group did show a slight improvement in RAVLT Short Delay Recall, but the effect size was small (ES = 0.12) compared with a moderate effect size for HTN-I (ES = 0.5) and Con-S (ES = 0.41). Also similar to results in the primary analyses, the HTN-I and Con-S groups showed within-group improvement in Grooved Pegboard with large effect sizes for both dominant hand (ES, HTN-I = 0.78; Con-S = 0.70) and non-dominant hand (ES, HTN-I = 0.61; Con-S = 0.69). In comparison, the HTN-NI group had only minimal improvement with only small effect sizes (ES, dominant hand = 0.27, non-dominant hand = 0.14),.
Table 6.
Results of Neurocognitive Test Performance by Effectiveness of Antihypertensive Treatment
Neurocognitive Measure | Group | Baseline | LSM change | Effect Size Within group |
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
RAVLT (total correct, raw scores) | ||||
List A Trial 1 | HTN I | 6.5 | 0.50† | 0.32 |
HTN NI | 5.9 | −0.48 | −0.29 | |
Con-S | 6.6 | 0.57**† | 0.37 | |
List A Total (Trials 1–5) | HTN I | 48.3 | 2.7**† | 0.46 |
HTN NI | 46.4 | −1.4 | −0.23 | |
Con-S | 50.2 | 2.3**† | 0.38 | |
List A Short Delay Recall | HTN I | 9.1 | 0.89** | 0.50 |
HTN NI | 9.6 | 0.23 | 0.12 | |
Con-S | 10.6 | 0.74** | 0.41 | |
List A Long Delay Recall | HTN I | 9.3 | 0.98** | 0.49 |
HTN NI | 9.1 | 0.64 | 0.30 | |
Con-S | 10.4 | 1.1*** | 0.56 | |
| ||||
WASI (T scores) | ||||
Vocabulary | HTN I | 47.6 | −0.36 | −0.06 |
HTN NI | 47.1 | −0.68 | −0.12 | |
Con-S | 50.9 | 0.44 | 0.08 | |
Matrix Reasoning | HTN I | 48.7 | 2.2 | 0.28 |
HTN NI | 49.6 | 3.1 | 0.39 | |
Con-S | 50.3 | 0.11 | 0.01 | |
| ||||
CogState GMLT | ||||
GMLT Total Error | HTN I | 59.4 | −1.6 | 0.10 |
HTN NI | 60.1 | −3.5 | 0.21 | |
Con-S | 57.2 | −3.9 | 0.24 | |
GMLT Delayed Recall Error | HTN I | 8.8 | −1.2 | 0.27 |
HTN NI | 11.7 | −0.75 | 0.15 | |
Con-S | 7.4 | −1.4* | 0.29 | |
| ||||
Grooved Pegboard (time, s) | ||||
Dominant hand | HTN I | 83.7 | −7.3*** | 0.78 |
HTN NI | 85.3 | −2.7 | 0.27 | |
Con-S | 78.0 | −6.8*** | 0.70 | |
Non-dominant hand | HTN I | 87.7 | −6.5*** | 0.61 |
HTN NI | 92.8 | −1.6 | 0.14 | |
Con-S | 87.3 | −7.4*** | 0.69 | |
| ||||
CPT–II (T scores) | ||||
Omissions errors | HTN I | 48.2 | 2.7 | −0.23 |
HTN NI | 52.4 | −1.6 | 0.12 | |
Con-S | 48.7 | 1.8 | −0.15 | |
Commissions errors | HTN I | 52.8 | −1.7 | 0.20 |
HTN NI | 56.4 | −1.2 | 0.12 | |
Con-S | 52.7 | −1.7 | 0.19 | |
Variability | HTN I | 46.1 | 4.9**† | −0.45 |
HTN NI | 47.6 | −2.0 | 0.17 | |
Con-S | 45.9 | 3.9** | −0.36 | |
Detectability | HTN I | 53.5 | −2.1 | 0.26 |
HTN NI | 57.4 | −1.0 | 0.11 | |
Con-S | 52.7 | −1.8 | 0.21 | |
| ||||
WISC-IV (scaled scores) | ||||
Spatial Span Forward | HTN I | 9.4 | 0.05 | 0.02 |
HTN NI | 8.3 | −0.17 | −0.06 | |
Con-S | 9.2 | 1.2** | 0.45 | |
Spatial Span Backward | HTN I | 10.5 | 0.52 | 0.30 |
HTN NI | 10.1 | 0.48 | 0.28 | |
Con-S | 10.6 | 0.17 | 0.10 | |
Digit Span Forward | HTN I | 9.9 | 0.26 | 0.12 |
HTN NI | 8.6 | 0.42 | 0.18 | |
Con-S | 9.1 | 0.53 | 0.25 | |
Digit Span Backward | HTN I | 9.4 | −0.07 | −0.03 |
HTN NI | 9.6 | 0.13 | 0.05 | |
Con-S | 9.5 | 0.33 | 0.13 | |
| ||||
CogState Set Shift | ||||
Total Error | HTN I | 36.2 | −3.1 | 0.22 |
HTN NI | 43.9 | −0.42 | 0.03 | |
Con-S | 38.4 | −3.7 | 0.26 | |
| ||||
DKEFS Tower (scaled scores) | ||||
Total Achievement | HTN I | 9.3 | 0.82* | 0.35 |
HTN NI | 10.3 | 1.2 | 0.48 | |
Con-S | 9.3 | 0.85** | 0.36 | |
| ||||
Parent BRIEF (T scores) | ||||
BRI | HTN I | 48.2 | −1.0 | 0.16 |
HTN NI | 48.5 | 1.4 | −0.20 | |
Con-S | 45.8 | 0.07 | −0.01 | |
MI | HTN I | 48.4 | −1.6 | 0.23 |
HTN NI | 49.2 | −0.12 | 0.02 | |
Con-S | 48.4 | −1.0 | 0.16 |
RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
WASI = Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence CogState
GMLT = CogState Groton Maze Learning Test
CPT-II = Conners’ Continuous Performance Test–II
WISC-IV = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV
DKEFS Tower = Delis Kaplan Executive Function System Tower
Parent BRIEF = Parent Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function
BRI = Behavior Regulation Index
MI = Metacognition Index
HTN I = Hypertension improved, N = 38
HTN NI = Hypertension not improved, N = 17
Con-S = Control subjects with sustained normotension over the year, N = 56
LSM = Least Square Means
Within group treatment differences, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001
Between group comparisons, † = p < 0.05 for HTN or Con-S vs HTN NI
Sign (+/−) of effect size oriented such that positive values indicate better performance in relation to baseline and vice versa
All comparisons adjusted for age, sex, maternal education, race, income, BMI z-score at baseline, change in BMI z- score in 1 year, Pediatric Sleep Questionnaire, and HOMA (HOMA is a measure of insulin resistance).
Likewise similar to results in the primary analyses, all groups improved in DKEFS Tower with similar effect sizes, and the Con-S group improved in Spatial Span Forward whereas the hypertension groups did not. The Con-S and HTN-I groups scored worse on CPT-II Variability, but all groups performed similarly in the other CPT-II subtests.
In order to explore further the difference in results for the HTN-NI group compared with the HTN-I and Con-S groups, we compared the neurocognitive test results of hypertension subjects who received antihypertensive medication to that of hypertension subjects who did not receive antihypertensive medication (regardless of categorization by BP improvement), in case ACE inhibition had a direct beneficial effect on cognition independent of its effect on BP. However, there was no significant difference in change in neurocognitive test scores between hypertensive subjects who received antihypertensive medication and those who did not (data not shown).
Discussion
We previously reported that subjects with untreated hypertension had lower performance in measures of verbal learning, memory, vocabulary, and manual dexterity compared with that of normotensive control subjects at baseline.(11) We now report that the neurocognitive test performance of the hypertension subjects improved after 1- year of antihypertensive therapy in some neurocognitive tests; in particular this was noted on measures of verbal learning and memory (RAVLT), manual dexterity (Grooved Pegboard), and executive function (DKEFS Tower Test). However, the test performance of the control subjects also improved to a similar degree on these same measures, such that there was no significant difference in the change in scores between hypertension and control subjects after adjusting for potential confounders. The fact that the control group improved to a similar degree in these measures suggests that the improvement seen in the hypertension subjects may not have been due to the antihypertensive therapy, but instead to practice effects, the propensity of scores to improve on repeat testing due to learned strategies (21) or to normal growth in skills with age. These findings are consistent with results of clinical trials in adults, which do not clearly prove or disprove a beneficial effect of antihypertensive therapy, in the short term, on subsequent cognition.(7)
However, examination of the results by effectiveness of the antihypertensive treatment (according to ABPM results) suggests that the findings are more complicated. The secondary analyses differentiated between the hypertensive subjects with successful treatment of their hypertension (HTN-I) and the hypertensive subjects with unsuccessful treatment (persistent ambulatory hypertension, HTN-NI). Similar to results of the primary analyses, the hypertension subjects with successful treatment and the control subjects both had improvement in scores on the RAVLT (verbal learning and memory) at 1-year to a similar degree, suggesting significant practice effect or normal growth in skills with age. However, the hypertension subjects with poor control of their hypertension did not show the same improvement in scores on the RAVLT, suggesting that they did not have the same practice effect or normal growth in skills with age and continued to show the same neurocognitive profile at the end of treatment as at baseline. This finding suggests that poor hypertension control was associated with a blunted ability to recall learned strategies for improvement and/or to recall the content of test items. There was a similar pattern for the results on Grooved Pegboard (manual dexterity), where the subjects with poor hypertension control had blunted improvement in scores compared with successfully treated hypertension subjects and control subjects. The association between poor hypertension control and diminished practice effect or diminished growth in skills with age in some neurocognitive measures raises the concern that the adequacy of hypertension treatment may have an impact on cognition over time. Effective antihypertensive therapy appeared necessary for developmentally-expected growth in cognition over time to occur, suggesting that under- treated hypertension in youth may contribute to lower cognitive reserve and the beginnings of cognitive decline. With the epidemic of obesity and hypertension in youth, the path to these lower cognitive outcomes may be beginning ever earlier. A recent report from the Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study was consistent with this scenario, showing that systolic hypertension in adolescence was associated with lower cognitive test performance in midlife.(5)
Our experience underscores important challenges to the study of cognition in hypertensive youth and the treatment of primary hypertension in children in general. The lost to follow-up rate of the hypertension group was high at 27%, despite significant study coordinator efforts to retain these subjects. Many of these subjects were not only lost to follow-up from the study, but also from clinical care (personal communication), a finding consistent with reports that, in the United States, young adults have the lowest hypertension control rates and visit adherence rates among adult age groups.(22, 23) Our findings demonstrate that visit and medication adherence is also a problem in an adolescent population with hypertension and this factor might affect the impact of hypertension management. Data on the longitudinal treatment of children with primary hypertension are limited, but similar lost to follow-up rates have been reported.(24) The subjects who were lost to follow-up had higher parent ratings of executive dysfunction (BRIEF BRI scale) compared with that of subjects not lost to follow-up. The BRIEF BRI summary score evaluates the child’s ability to control impulses, alter problem solving strategies, regulate attention, and modulate emotional responses in real life settings.(14, 25) Decreased executive functioning has been implicated as a factor in low medication adherence in chronic conditions in both adults and children.(26–28) One can postulate that lower executive functioning in the hypertensive adolescent could be associated with medication and visit non-adherence. This concern is a particular challenge for studies of cognition in hypertension as the subjects most likely to withdraw may be those of most interest.
Among the hypertension subjects who completed follow-up visits, approximately 30% (17 of 55) had inadequate control of their hypertension by ABPM. However, all but 4 of the 17 had normal office BP at the 1-year study visit, suggesting the presence of masked hypertension.(29) Masked hypertension occurs when patients have normal office BP but elevated BP by ABPM, and it is associated with increased risk for hypertensive target organ damage.(30) Others have described the persistence of sustained hypertension by ambulatory monitoring in children who appeared controlled by office readings.(31, 32) The use of ABPM for determining adequacy of hypertension control has been largely absent from adult studies of the effect of hypertension treatment on subsequent cognitive decline, a potential explanation, in part, for the inconsistencies in study results. Our results suggest that investigators and clinicians should consider monitoring the adequacy of hypertension treatment by ABPM rather than relying solely on office readings.
The current study has several limitations. The antihypertensive management was not administered in a strict protocol-driven manner across sites, but instead allowed for discretion of the site physician, albeit with some general standardization. Although this approach is closer to real world treatment, it did allow for some differences in treatment approach from patient to patient that could have had an impact on the neurocognitive outcomes. Hypertension subjects on antihypertensive medication were contacted monthly to encourage compliance whereas the hypertension subjects managed with lifestyle modification alone were not, a difference in approach which could have potentially led to a difference in compliance between these two groups. In accordance with national consensus guidelines at the time of study design, we relied on office manual BP to guide treatment. We did not perform interim ABPM to assess adequacy of treatment, an approach which would miss nocturnal hypertension and masked hypertension. Similarly, we did not assess compliance with the recommendation for dietary modification and regular exercise and we did not have a formal mechanism to measure medication compliance, factors with potential influence on neurocognitive test performance. Further, our initial sample size was small and our lost to follow-up rate among the hypertension group was relatively high, a situation which may have lessened our ability to detect changes in neurocognitive test performance with treatment. Although the secondary subgroup analyses suggested that subjects with successful hypertension treatment performed differently than those with unsuccessful treatment, these results need to be interpreted with caution due to the smaller sample size and potential biases associated with subgroup analyses. In addition, there was relatively wide variability in the neurocognitive test scores with relatively small differences between groups, implying that serial neurocognitive testing alone may not be the most optimal method of detecting an effect of hypertension treatment on hypertensive target organ damage to the brain in youth. Our study did not include neuroimaging, an additional potential marker of hypertensive target organ damage, and these types of assessment procedures may have augmented our ability to detect changes over time. Lastly, we waited a full year before retesting in order to minimize practice effects. However, by waiting a year, we may have increased the chance of improvement in neurocognitive measures due to normal growth in skills with age. Both residual practice effect and normal growth in skills with age complicate the interpretation of the improvement in some of the neurocognitive tests in the hypertension group. Conversely, an interval of one year might not have allowed for sufficient evolution of differences between groups.
In summary, our data showed that 1-year of antihypertensive therapy did not improve the previously described lower neurocognitive test performance in hypertension subjects beyond the improvement also seen in control subjects over the same time period. However, the secondary analyses suggested that the effectiveness of antihypertensive therapy may have an impact on neurocognitive test performance by enabling hypertensive children to benefit from strategic learning to the same extent as nonhypertensive children. Further longitudinal research with larger numbers of subjects followed for longer periods of time are needed to determine the influence of BP control from youth on neurocognitive test performance during youth itself and on possible amelioration of cognitive decline over time. Future studies should utilize ABPM in hypertension treatment management and should include additional surrogate markers of central nervous system target organ damage, such as different types of neuroimaging, in order to address the important question of whether treating hypertension from adolescence would achieve improvement in subsequent cognitive outcomes.
Acknowledgments
Funded by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (R01HL098332[to M.L.). The authors declare no conflicts of interest. Conflicts of interest: none. Payments to write the manuscript: none
We thank the study coordinators and neuropsychologists at each participating center for all of their work in keeping this study going.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Waldstein SR. Hypertension and neuropsychological function: A lifespan perspective. Exp Aging Res. 1995 Oct-Dec;21(4):321–52. doi: 10.1080/03610739508253989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Waldstein SR, Jennings JR, Ryan CM, Muldoon MF, Shapiro AP, Polefrone JM, et al. Hypertension and neuropsychological performance in men: Interactive effects of age. Health Psychol. 1996 Mar;15(2):102–9. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.15.2.102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Elias PK, Elias MF, Robbins MA, Budge MM. Blood pressure-related cognitive decline: Does age make a difference? Hypertension. 2004 Nov;44(5):631–6. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000145858.07252.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Yaffe K, Vittinghoff E, Pletcher MJ, Hoang TD, Launer LJ, Whitmer R, et al. Early adult to midlife cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive function. Circulation. 2014 Apr 15;129(15):1560–7. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.113.004798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Rovio SP, Pahkala K, Nevalainen J, Juonala M, Salo P, Kahonen M, et al. Cardiovascular risk factors from childhood and midlife cognitive performance: The young finns study. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2017 May 09;69(18):2279–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2017.02.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.McGuinness B, Todd S, Passmore P, Bullock R. Blood pressure lowering in patients without prior cerebrovascular disease for prevention of cognitive impairment and dementia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2009 Ot;4(4) doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004034.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Iadecola C, Yaffe K, Biller J, Bratzke LC, Faraci FM, Gorelick PB, et al. Impact of hypertension on cognitive function: A scientific statement from the american heart association. Hypertension. 2016 Dec;68(6):e67–94. doi: 10.1161/HYP.0000000000000053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lande MB, Carson NL, Roy J, Meagher CC. Effects of childhood primary hypertension on carotid intima media thickness: A matched controlled study. Hypertension. 2006 Jul;48(1):40–4. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000227029.10536.e8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.McNiece KL, Gupta-Malhotra M, Samuels J, Bell C, Garcia K, Poffenbarger T, et al. Left ventricular hypertrophy in hypertensive adolescents: Analysis of risk by 2004 national high blood pressure education program working group staging criteria. Hypertension. 2007 Aug;50(2):392–5. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.107.092197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lande MB, Adams HR, Kupferman JC, Hooper SR, Szilagyi PG, Batisky DL. A multicenter study of neurocognition in children with hypertension: Methods, challenges, and solutions. J Am Soc Hypertens. 2013 Sep-Oct;7(5):353–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jash.2013.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lande MB, Batisky DL, Kupferman JC, Samuels J, Hooper SR, Falkner B, et al. Neurocognitive function in children with primary hypertension. J Pediatr. 2017 Jan;180:148, 155.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.08.076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Flynn JT, Daniels SR, Hayman LL, Maahs DM, McCrindle BW, Mitsnefes M, et al. Update: Ambulatory blood pressure monitoring in children and adolescents: A scientific statement from the american heart association. Hypertension. 2014 May;63(5):1116–35. doi: 10.1161/HYP.0000000000000007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Khoury PR, Mitsnefes M, Daniels SR, Kimball TR. Age-specific reference intervals for indexed left ventricular mass in children. Journal of the American Society of Echocardiography. 2009 Jun;22(6):709–14. doi: 10.1016/j.echo.2009.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Straus E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. 3. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Chervin RD, Hedger K, Dillon JE, Pituch KJ. Pediatric sleep questionnaire (PSQ): Validity and reliability of scales for sleep-disordered breathing, snoring, sleepiness, and behavioral problems. Sleep Med. 2000;1(1):21–32. doi: 10.1016/s1389-9457(99)00009-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Acosta AA. Obstructive sleep apnea and hypertension. In: Flynn JT, Ingelfinger JR, Portman RJ, editors. Pediatric Hypertension. 3. New York: Humana Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 17.National High Blood Pressure Education Program Working Group on High Blood Pressure in Children and Adolescents. The fourth report on the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of high blood pressure in children and adolescents. Pediatrics. 2004 Aug;114(2 Suppl 4th Report):555–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Woroniecki RP, Flynn JT. How are hypertensive children evaluated and managed? A survey of north american pediatric nephrologists. Pediatr Nephrol. 2005 Jun;20(6):791–7. doi: 10.1007/s00467-004-1804-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Little RJA, Rubin DB. Statistical analysis with missing data. 2. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Schafer JL. Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman and Hall/CRC; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological assessment. 4. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Johnson HM, Warner RC, LaMantia JN, Bowers BJ. "I have to live like I'm old." young adults' perspectives on managing hypertension: A multi-center qualitative study. BMC Fam Pract. 2016 Mar 11;17:31. doi: 10.1186/s12875-016-0428-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Johnson HM, Warner RC, Bartels CM, LaMantia JN. "They're younger..it's harder." primary providers' perspectives on hypertension management in young adults: A multicenter qualitative study. BMC Res Notes. 2017 Jan 03;10(1):9. doi: 10.1186/s13104-016-2332-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.DiPietro A, Kees-Folts D, DesHarnais S, Camacho F, Wassner SJ. Primary hypertension at a single center: Treatment, time to control, and extended follow-up. Pediatr Nephrol. 2009 Dec;24(12):2421–8. doi: 10.1007/s00467-009-1297-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Lutz, FL: Pyschological Assessments Resources; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Insel KC, Einstein GO, Morrow DG, Koerner KM, Hepworth JT. Multifaceted prospective memory intervention to improve medication adherence. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2016 Mar;64(3):561–8. doi: 10.1111/jgs.14032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Duke DC, Harris MA. Executive function, adherence, and glycemic control in adolescents with type 1 diabetes: A literature review. Curr Diab Rep. 2014 Oct;14(10):532. doi: 10.1007/s11892-014-0532-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.O'Hara LK, Holmbeck GN. Executive functions and parenting behaviors in association with medical adherence and autonomy among youth with spina bifida. J Pediatr Psychol. 2013 Jul;38(6):675–87. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jst007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lurbe E, Torro I, Alvarez V, Nawrot T, Paya R, Redon J, et al. Prevalence, persistence, and clinical significance of masked hypertension in youth. Hypertension. 2005 Apr;45(4):493–8. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000160320.39303.ab. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mitsnefes M, Flynn J, Cohn S, Samuels J, Blydt-Hansen T, Saland J, et al. Masked hypertension associates with left ventricular hypertrophy in children with CKD. J Am Soc Nephrol. 2010 Jan;21(1):137–44. doi: 10.1681/ASN.2009060609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Samuels J, Ng D, Flynn JT, Mitsnefes M, Poffenbarger T, Warady BA, et al. Ambulatory blood pressure patterns in children with chronic kidney disease. Hypertension. 2012 Jul;60(1):43–50. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.111.189266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Seeman T, Dostalek L, Gilik J. Control of hypertension in treated children and its association with target organ damage. Am J Hypertens. 2012 Mar;25(3):389–95. doi: 10.1038/ajh.2011.218. [cited 20120215] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]