Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Body Image. 2017 Nov 21;24:1–4. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.11.004

Thinness pressures in ethnically diverse college women in the United States

D Luis Ordaz 1,*, Lauren M Schaefer 1,1, Emily Choquette 1, Jordan Schueler 1, Lisa Wallace 1, J Kevin Thompson 1
PMCID: PMC5869119  NIHMSID: NIHMS927060  PMID: 29172060

Abstract

While research consistently supports the negative impact of thinness pressures on body image, this work has primarily utilized White samples in the United States, limiting generalizability to other ethnicities. Further, limited research has examined ethnic differences in thinness pressures from distinct sociocultural influences. This study examined distinct sources of thinness pressures in 598 White, 135 Black, and 131 Hispanic college women in the United States. Mean levels of thinness pressures significantly differed across ethnicity, with Black women generally reporting the lowest levels of each pressure. Additionally, distinct sources of thinness pressures were more highly related to negative outcomes within ethnic groups. For White women, each source was salient for disordered eating. For Black women, family pressure was particularly salient for appearance evaluation. For Hispanic women, family pressure was particularly salient for disordered eating and appearance evaluation. Findings suggest possible ethnic differences in the relative salience of some pressures over others.

Keywords: Thinness pressures, Ethnicity, Appearance evaluation, Body dissatisfaction, Disordered eating

1. Introduction

Although research examining rates of disordered eating in ethnic groups in the United States has produced mixed results (Shaw, Ramirez, Trost, Randall, & Stice, 2004; Wildes, Emery, & Simons, 2001), evidence generally suggests comparable rates of disordered eating in White and Hispanic women, and lower rates of disordered eating in Black women (Gordon, Castro, Sitnikov, & Holm-Denoma, 2010). Examinations of proposed risk factors in ethnically diverse women have also produced mixed results. For example, some studies have suggested significantly lower levels of body checking, body avoidance, and thin ideal internalization in Black and Hispanic women compared to White women (Shaw et al., 2004; White & Warren, 2013), while other work suggests comparable levels of proposed risk factors among women of different ethnicities (Shaw et al., 2004). As research regarding ethnic differences in risk factors remains equivocal, more work is needed to clarify the role of proposed risk factors in the development of disordered eating and body dissatisfaction in ethnically diverse women.

Sociocultural theories, highlighting the role of cultural influences in the development of body image and eating disturbance, may provide a useful framework for understanding the unique experiences of individuals of differing ethnicities. Two well-supported sociocultural theories, the Tripartite Influence Model (Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999) and the Dual Pathway Model (Stice, Nemeroff, & Shaw, 1996), implicate appearance-related pressures in the development of negative outcomes. Briefly, the Dual Pathway Model proposes that thinness pressures and thin ideal internalization combine to produce negative effects on body image and eating patterns. The Tripartite Influence Model offers a slightly more elaborated framework. Specifically, it posits that thinness pressures from family, peers, and media lead to internalization of the thin ideal and appearance comparisons, resulting in body dissatisfaction and disordered eating. Research supports the influence of thinness pressures on negative outcomes (Stice et al., 1996). Indeed, evidence suggests that thinness pressures represent a strong and reliable risk factor for eating and body image disturbances (Culbert, Racine, & Klump, 2015). Importantly, however, this research has been predominantly conducted with White women. Moreover, existing work has tended to examine the combined impact of thinness pressures from multiple sources or only a single isolated source.

Research examining overall thinness pressures in women in the United States is mixed, with some studies suggesting higher thinness pressures in White women compared to Black (Striegel-Moore, Wilfley, Caldwell, Needham, & Brownell, 1996) and Hispanic women (McKnight Risk Factor Study, 2000), and other studies suggesting comparable thinness pressures among these groups (Shaw et al., 2004). Research examining more specific forms of thinness pressure indicates significantly lower levels of media appearance pressure in Black women compared to White and Hispanic women (Quick & Byrd-Bredbenner, 2014). Despite possible ethnic differences in thinness pressures, research largely supports the proposed negative impact of these experiences on body image and disordered eating (Rogers Wood & Petrie, 2010; Shaw et al., 2004) in women of color.

To date, no study has examined the relative contribution of distinct sources of thinness pressure (i.e., family, peer, and media) in White, Black, and Hispanic women in the United States. Given research suggesting that different sources of social influence may be more impactful within some ethnic groups (Gibbons et al., 2010), it is possible that thinness pressures from specific sources may be more strongly related to disordered eating and body image disturbance in specific ethnic groups. In particular, evidence suggests that peer and media influence may be particularly impactful for White individuals (Gibbons et al., 2010), while family influence may be particularly salient for Black and Hispanic individuals (Gaines et al., 1997).

Accordingly, the goals of the current study were to examine: (a) mean level differences in perceived thinness pressures from family, peers, and media in White, Black, and Hispanic women, and (b) the predictive power of such pressures on appearance evaluation and disordered eating within each ethnic group. Based on the limited research available, we hypothesized that Black women would report significantly less perceived pressure than White women. Additionally, we hypothesized that family pressures would be most strongly related to appearance evaluation and disordered eating in Black and Hispanic women, while peer and media influence would be most strongly related to these outcomes in White women.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Participants were 864 female undergraduate students attending a large Southeastern university in the United States who self-identified as White (n = 598), Black (n = 135), and Hispanic (n = 131). Participants ranged in age from 18 to 35, with a mean age of 20.33 years (SD = 2.73). The mean body mass index (BMI) of the sample was 23.49 kg/m2 (SD = 4.95). Characteristics of this sample and mean-level analysis of group differences on appearance evaluation and disordered eating have been reported elsewhere (Schaefer, Thibodaux, Krenik, Arnold, & Thompson, 2015).

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Demographic information

Participants were asked to indicate age, ethnicity, height, and weight. Self-reported height and weight were used to calculate BMI.

2.2.2. Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-4 (SATAQ-4)

The three pressures subscales of the SATAQ-4 (Schaefer, Burke, et al., 2015) measure perceived thinness and appearance pressures from family, peers, and media. Each subscale consists of 4 items rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (Definitely disagree) to 5 (Definitely agree). Higher scores indicate higher perceived pressure. Cronbach’s alphas within ethnic groups ranged from .88 to .90 for the Pressures: Family subscale, .88 to .91 for the Pressures: Peers subscale, and .93 to .96 for the Pressures: Media subscale.

2.2.3. Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire – Appearance Evaluation Subscale (MBSRQ-AE)

The Appearance Evaluation subscale of the MBSRQ (Brown, Cash, & Mikulka, 1990; Cash, 2002) was designed to measure positive appearance evaluation. The measure consists of 7 items rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (Definitely disagree) to 5 (Definitely agree). Higher scores indicate more positive appearance evaluation. Cronbach’s alpha within ethnic groups ranged from .88 to .92.

2.2.4. Eating Disorder Examination – Questionnaire (EDE-Q)

The EDE-Q (Fairburn & Beglin, 2008) was designed to measure disordered eating attitudes and behaviors. The measure consists of 28 items and four subscales assessing dietary restraint, eating concern, shape concern, and weight concern. Items are rated on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (No days/not at all) to 6 (Every day/markedly). The global score (i.e., mean of the four subscales) was used. Higher scores indicate greater disordered eating. Cronbach’s alpha within ethnic groups ranged from .93 to .96.

2.3. Procedure

All individuals enrolled in the University’s psychology research participant pool were invited to participate in an IRB-approved anonymous survey examining appearance attitudes. Interested participants provided digital informed consent and completed questionnaires online as part of a larger study. The full set of 13 scales were designed to be completed within approximately 30 minutes. The three scales utilized in the current study were among the first four scales presented and were completed in a set order (i.e., SATAQ-4, EDE-Q, MBSRQ-AE). Upon completion, participants were debriefed online and received extra course credit for their participation.

2.4. Statistical Analyses

Separate one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted to examine differences in family pressures, peer pressures, and media pressures. Effect size was assessed via eta-squared; an effect of .01 is considered small, .06 is medium, and .14 is large (Cohen, 1988). A significant omnibus test was followed by post hoc pairwise-comparisons, which were performed using Tukey’s HSD when the assumption of equal variances was not violated and Dunnett’s C test in cases where the homogeneity of variances assumption was violated. Six multiple regression analyses were conducted to examine thinness pressures in the prediction of appearance evaluation and disordered eating within each ethnic group. Listwise deletion was used to handle missing data, with missing data from examined variables ranging from 10% to 19%. To determine if data was missing completely at random (MCAR), Little’s MCAR test was used for pressure variables. This test was significant, p < .05, suggesting that data was not MCAR. Therefore, mean differences between participants who provided complete versus incomplete responses on all examined variables were examined using t-tests and chi-square tests. All t-tests and chi-square tests were non-significant, p > .05, indicating no relation between variables and missing data. No problems of multicollinearity or homoscedasticity were detected. Variance inflation factors (VIF) ranged from 1.12 to 1.34 for White women, from 1.11 to 1.51 from Black women, and from 1.30 to 1.81 from Hispanic women, and Levene’s test of equality of variances was not significant for all model variables, p > .05. However, the EDE-Q did not meet the normality assumption for White women using Shapiro-Wilk’s test of normality (p < .5), and as a result, the variable was log-transformed. All other indices of normality were well within limits, with the greatest level of skewness reaching 0.30 for disordered eating in Black women and the greatest level of kurtosis reaching 0.66 for appearance evaluation in Black women. Regression results for the EDE-Q used the log-transformed variable. All analyses were conducted using SPSS 23.0.

3. Results

ANOVA results indicated group differences in family pressure, peer pressure, and media pressure (see Table 1). All effect sizes were small. Pairwise comparisons indicated significantly higher perceived family pressure in Hispanic women compared with White women. White and Hispanic women both reported higher perceived media pressure compared with Black women, but did not differ from one another. White women reported higher peer pressure compared with both Black and Hispanic women, who were comparable on this variable.

Table 1.

Mean (SDs) and Results of Analyses of Variance (ANOVAs) Examining Predictor Variables by Ethnic Group.

Variable White (n = 598) Black (n = 135) Hispanic (n = 131) F p η2 Pairwise Comparisons
Family Pressures 2.29 (1.11) 2.48 (1.17) 2.78 (1.23) 9.77 <.001 .02 H > W, B
Peer Pressures 3.71 (0.77) 3.29 (0.74) 3.39 (0.79) 21.50 <.001 .05 W > B, H
Media Pressures 3.76 (1.19) 3.09 (1.30) 3.69 (1.14) 16.52 <.001 .04 W, H > B

Note: Pairwise comparisons were performed using the Dunnett’s C test when the assumption of equal variances was violated, Tukey’s HSD test when the assumption was not violated. All pairwise comparisons listed were significant at least at p < .05.

Table 2 presents results from the regression analyses. In White women, thinness pressures accounted for 28% of the variance in appearance evaluation and 54% of the variance in disordered eating. All pressure variables were significant predictors of proposed outcomes. Family and media pressures were the strongest predictors of appearance evaluation, while peer pressure was the strongest predictor of disordered eating. In Black women, appearance pressure accounted for 16% of the variance in appearance evaluation and 43% of the variance in disordered eating. Only family pressure significantly predicted appearance evaluation, while family and peer pressures were the strongest predictors of disordered eating. In His-panic women, thinness pressures accounted for 30% of the variance in appearance evaluation and 46% of the variance in disordered eating. Family and media pressures, but not peer pressure, significantly predicted appearance evaluation and disordered eating.

Table 2.

Multiple Regression Analyses with Adjusted R2 Predicting Appearance Evaluation and Disordered Eating for Each Ethnic Group.

Predictors Appearance Evaluation
Disordered Eating
Adj. R2 F (dfs) B SE β t Adj. R2 F (dfs) B SE β t
White .28 71.73*** (3, 552) .54 206.87*** (3, 532)
Family Pressures −1.74*** .22 −.31*** −8.01 0.06*** .01 .29*** 9.24
Peer Pressures −0.92** .33 −.11** −2.77 0.11*** .01 .37*** 11.28
Media Pressures − 1.48*** .22 −.28*** −6.68 0.06*** .01 .33*** 9.54
Black .16 8.57*** (3, 119) .43 28.91*** (3, 107)
Family Pressures −1.64*** .53 −.30*** − 3.01 0.06*** .01 .36*** 4.37
Peer Pressures −0.71 .76 −.08 −0.93 0.09*** .20 .32*** 4.25
Media Pressures −0.70 .50 −.14 −1.39 0.04* .01 .23* 2.61
Hispanic .30 18.09*** (3, 116) .46 33.32*** (3, 111)
Family Pressures − 1.87*** .38 −.43*** −4.95 0.09*** .01 .48*** 6.00
Peer Pressures 1.13 .61 .17 1.86 0.02 .02 .07 0.90
Media Pressures −1.25* .49 −.26* −2.54 0.06** .02 .22** 3.00

Note: Adj. R2 = adjusted R2.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

4. Discussion

Overall, results of the current study support previous research indicating variability in the amount of appearance pressure perceived by women of different ethnicities (McKnight Risk Factor Study, 2000; Striegel-Moore et al., 1996), and suggest varied salience of pressure sources within ethnic groups. Consistent with both study hypotheses and previous work (Gibbons et al., 2010; Quick & Byrd-Bredbenner, 2014), Black women reported less perceived appearance pressure from peers and media. Moreover, family pressures emerged as the single significant predictor of appearance evaluation among Black women. Although scarce, media representations of Black women are often relatively curvaceous (Hazell & Clarke, 2008), potentially reducing thinness pressures targeting Black women. While Black women did not report high levels of thinness pressures from family members, the amount of family pressure was highly related to their eating and body image concerns. These results are consistent with research suggesting that family is a central and guiding force for many Black individuals (Gaines et al., 1997), and highlight the salience of family in shaping appearance and eating attitudes for Black women.

Results with White women were generally consistent with study hypotheses. As expected, White women demonstrated higher levels of peer and media pressures compared to Black women. However, White and Black women reported similar levels of family pressures. Compared to media representations of Black women, White women in the media are often more slender (Hazell & Clarke, 2008), potentially increasing thinness pressures among this group. In addition, research suggests higher levels of negative body talk – one facet of peer thinness pressures – among White women (Fiery, Martz, Webb, & Curtin, 2016). Further work suggests that White girls may be more strongly impacted by experiences of peer weight-based teasing than girls of other racial/ethnic backgrounds (van den Berg, Neumark-Sztainer, Eisenberg, & Haines, 2008). As hypothesized, peer and media pressures emerged as significant predictors of White women’s body image and eating concerns. However, family pressures appear to play a similarly important role, suggesting that all three forms of thinness pressures inform the eating and appearance concerns of White women.

Results with Hispanic women suggest the potential importance of family and media pressures in the development or maintenance of body image and eating disturbance, demonstrated by elevated levels of family and media pressure relative to their peers. In addition, family and media pressures were both significant predictors of appearance evaluation and disordered eating, while peer pressures were not. These findings are consistent with previous studies suggesting increased experiences of family weight-based teasing among Hispanic girls (van den Berg et al., 2008), and support previous work regarding the central role of family in the development of body image and eating behaviors within Hispanic culture (Altabe and O’Garo, 2002). Media representations of Hispanic women frequently portray a thin yet curvy body type, which is often difficult to attain (Stokes, Clemens, & Rios, 2016). Additionally, Hispanic girls report using mainstream media images of thin White women to evaluate their own appearance (Schooler, 2008), which may further increase thinness pressures.

Limitations of the current study include use of a cross-sectional design and a college female sample. Future work may seek to examine causal associations between variables and to include additional underrepresented groups (e.g., males, Asian Americans). Second, as previous research has indicated that acculturation and ethnic identity may influence body image and disordered eating in diverse samples (Wildes et al., 2001), future research may bene-fit by examining these as potential moderators of the examined relationships. Additionally, other variables such as self-esteem are known to influence body image, and including such variables in future research is warranted. Lastly, differences in levels of reported appearance pressures were small in magnitude. Despite these small effects, within group comparisons revealed interesting patterns for the salience of each source on appearance evaluation and disordered eating. This might suggest that women in the United States endorse similar levels of appearance pressure from multiple sources, but each source’s influence varies within ethnic groups.

In sum, although women of all ethnic backgrounds endorsed experiences of thinness pressures, the specific source of this pressure varied in salience for women of different ethnicities. As evidence suggests that culturally-adapted interventions may lead to improved treatment outcomes (Hall, Ibaraki, Huang, Marti, & Stice, 2016), understanding these differences may aide in the continued evolution of culturally-sensitive interventions. For example, assessing and targeting family pressure may prove particularly beneficial for Black women, while an emphasis on both family and media pressures may particularly benefit Hispanic women. White women may benefit from a discussion of all three sources of pressure. Overall, this study adds to a growing body of literature indicating ethnic differences in thinness pressures among women, and further suggests that specific sources of thinness pressures may be more predictive of negative outcomes within different ethnic groups. Lastly, these findings suggest avenues for adapting existing interventions to more readily respond to the unique characteristics of women from diverse cultural backgrounds.

References

  1. Altabe M, O’Garo K. Hispanic body images. In: Cash T, Pruzinsky T, editors. Body image: A handbook of theory, research, and clinical practice. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2002. pp. 251–477. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00029157.2004.10403620. [Google Scholar]
  2. Brown TA, Cash TF, Mikulka PJ. Attitudinal body image assessment: Factor analysis of the body-self relations questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1990;55:135–144. doi: 10.1080/00223891.1990.9674053. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5501&2_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cash TF. Users’ manual for the Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire. 2002 Available from the author at. www.body-images.com.
  4. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203771587. [Google Scholar]
  5. Culbert KM, Racine SE, Klump KL. Research review: What have we learned about the causes of eating disorders – A synthesis of sociocultural, psychological, and biological research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2015;56:1141–1164. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12441. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Fairburn CG, Beglin SJ. Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q 6.0) In: Fairburn CG, editor. Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders. New York. NY: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 309–313. [Google Scholar]
  7. Fiery MF, Martz DM, Webb RM, Curtin L. A preliminary investigation of racial differences in body talk in age-diverse U.S. adults. Eating Behaviors. 2016;21:232–235. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.03.004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Gaines SO, Jr, Marelich WD, Bledsoe KL, Steers WN, Henderson MC, Granrose CS, Yum N, et al. Links between race/ethnicity and cultural values as mediated by racial/ethnic identity and moderated by gender. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;72:1460–1476. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.72.6.1460. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Gibbons FX, Pomery EA, Gerrard M, Sargent JD, Weng CY, Wills TA, Tanski SE. Media as social influence: Racial differences in the effects of peers and media on adolescent alcohol cognitions and consumption. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2010;24(4):649–659. doi: 10.1037/a0020768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gordon KH, Castro Y, Sitnikov L, Holm-Denoma JM. Cultural body shape ideals and eating disorder symptoms among White, Latina, and Black college women. Cultural Diversity And Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2010;16:135–143. doi: 10.1037/a0018671. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Hall GCN, Ibaraki AY, Huang ER, Marti CN, Stice E. A meta-analysis of cultural adaptations of psychological interventions. Behavior Therapy. 2016;47:993–1014. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.09.005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hazell V, Clarke J. Race and gender in the media: A content analysis of advertisements in two mainstream Black magazines. Journal of Black Studies. 2008;39:5–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0021934706291402. [Google Scholar]
  13. McKnight Risk Factor Study. Ethnic group differences in disordered eating attitudes and behaviors and related risk factors. Paper presented at the Ninth International Conference on Eating Disorders; 2000. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/e353642004-001. [Google Scholar]
  14. Quick VM, Byrd-Bredbenner Disordered eating, socio-cultural media influencers, body image, and psychological factors among a racially/ethnically diverse population of college women. Eating Behaviors. 2014;15:37–41. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.10.005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Rogers Wood NA, Petrie TA. Body dissatisfaction, ethnic identity, and disordered eating among African American women. Journal of Consulting Psychology. 2010;57:141–153. doi: 10.1037/a0018922. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Schaefer LM, Burke NL, Thompson JK, Dedrick RF, Heinberg LJ, Calogero RM, Swami V, et al. Development and validation of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-4 (SATAQ-4) Psychological Assessment. 2015;27:54–67. doi: 10.1037/a0037917. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.22590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Schaefer LM, Thibodaux LK, Krenik D, Arnold E, Thompson JK. Physical appearance comparisons in ethnically diverse college women. Body Image. 2015;15:153–157. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.09.002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Schooler D. Real women have curves: A longitudinal investigation of TV and the body image development of Latina adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2008;23:132–153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0743558407310712. [Google Scholar]
  19. Shaw H, Ramirez L, Trost A, Randall P, Stice E. Body image and eating disturbances across ethnic groups: More similarities than differences. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2004;18:12–18. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.18.1.12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-164x.18.1.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Stice E, Nemeroff C, Shaw HE. Test of the dual pathway model of bulimia nervosa: Evidence for dietary restraint and affect regulation mechanisms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 1996;15:340–363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1996.15.3.340. [Google Scholar]
  21. Stokes DM, Clemens CF, Rios DI. Brown beauty: Body image, Latinas, and the media. Journal of Family Strengths. 2016;16:1–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781452218540.n7. [Google Scholar]
  22. Striegel-Moore RH, Wilfley DE, Caldwell MB, Needham ML, Brownell KD. Weight-related attitudes and behaviors of women who diet to lose weight: A comparison of Black dieters and White dieters. Obesity Research. 1996;4:109–116. doi: 10.1002/j.1550-8528.1996.tb00522.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/j.1550-8528.1996.tb00522.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Thompson JK, Heinberg LJ, Altabe M, Tantleff-Dunn S. Exacting beauty: Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/t10312_000. [Google Scholar]
  24. van den Berg P, Neumark-Sztainer D, Eisenberg ME, Haines J. Racial/ethnic differences in weight-related teasing in adolescents. Obesity. 2008;16:3–10. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.445. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2008.445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. White EK, Warren CS. Body checking and avoidance in ethnically diverse female college students. Body Image. 2013;10:583–590. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.04.003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Wildes JE, Emery RE, Simons AD. The roles of ethnicity and culture in the development of eating disturbance and body dissatisfaction: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2001;21:521–551. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00071-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00071-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES