Abstract
This study was conducted to investigate the effect of dietary supplement containing astaxanthin-rich extract derived from Paracoccus carotinifaciens (astaxanthin supplement) on cognitive function of subjects aged 45–64 years. Cognitive functions of 28 subjects orally administered 8 mg astaxanthin/day of astaxanthin supplement for 8 weeks (astaxanthin group) and 26 subjects given a placebo (placebo group) were compared by word memory test, verbal fluency test, and Stroop test. The astaxanthin group experienced significantly larger increase in blood astaxanthin level than the placebo group. However, there were no significant intergroup differences in the results of the tests. A subgroup analysis was performed after dividing subjects into the <55 years old and ≥55 years old age groups. The result of “words recalled after 5 minutes” in word memory test in <55 years old subjects showed significant improvement in the astaxanthin group than in the placebo group, which was not found in ≥55 years old subjects. Our results indicate that people aged 45–54 years may experience improved cognitive function after ingesting astaxanthin supplement for 8 weeks. On the basis of the parameters tested, administration of astaxanthin supplement was not associated with any problems related to safety.
Keywords: astaxanthin, Paracoccus carotinifaciens, cognitive function, adonirubin, adonixanthin
Introduction
Astaxanthin is a carotenoid similar to β-carotene and lycopene and is classified as xanthophylls. Astaxanthin is designated as 3,3'-dihydroxy-β,β-carotene-4,4'-dione in IUPAC nomenclature. It is a red-tinged pigment naturally accumulated in aquatic creatures, such as fish and shrimp, and it is also known to have a powerful antioxidant effect.(1)
Astaxanthin is used in animal feed to improve the color of the fish raised in hatcheries, and it has also been sold in human use such as dietary supplements and cosmetics. Several studies have reported on the biological role of astaxanthin, including protection of the retina,(2) amelioration of eye fatigue,(3) enhancement of mitochondria function,(4) promotion of the use of fat as an energy source,(5) increase in motor function,(6) beautification of skin,(7) control of arteriosclerosis,(8) prevention of hyposalivation(9) and inhibitory effect of blood pressure (BP) elevation.(10) Recently, astaxanthin has gained worldwide attention as a multi-functional health ingredient.
In addition to synthetic astaxanthin, natural astaxanthin extracted from various organisms are commonly used. The most commonly used natural form of astaxanthin is derived (or extracted) from Haematococcus pluvialis (H. pluvialis), a type of green algae.(11) Astaxanthin derived from H. pluvialis is used in the majority of functional studies. Astaxanthin is the predominant component of the carotenoids derived from H. pluvialis, and only the optically pure (3S,3'S)-astaxanthin, more than 90% terminal hydroxyl group of which is esterized with fatty acids, has been isolated.(12) In addition, the red yeast Phaffia rhodozyma is known to produce astaxanthin.(13) The chemical composition of the astaxanthin that is produced by this yeast is the opposite to the commonly occurring natural astaxanthin (3S,3'S-configuration) and is designated as the 3R,3'R-configuration.(14) The terminal ends have hydroxyl groups that are not esterized or modified in any way.(15) Paracoccus carotinifaciens (P. carotinifaciens) is also known to produce carotenoid compounds that contain astaxanthin.(16) Astaxanthin derived from P. carotinifaciens is a “free” form devoid of terminal modification and has a 3S,3'S-configuration.(17) In addition to astaxanthin, 20–30% of the extract derived from P. carotinifaciens consisted of the potent antioxidant adonirubin and adonixanthin.(18) Thus, natural astaxanthin shows a wide variety of characteristics depending on their source.
Healthy aging, which includes improving diet, habits of daily living and life expectancy, has been receiving increasing attention as populations around the world continue to age. In particular, studies on preserving cognitive functions, which declines by aging and dementia, have been conducted. Because omega-3 fatty acids (e.g., docosahexaenoic acid and eicosapentaenoic acid) are known to maintain cognitive function, it is now recommended that people receive 1 g of the fatty acids daily.(19) It is reported that tocotrienol markedly counteracts the decline in learning and memory function in aged rats.(20) Krikorian et al.(21) suggest that supplementation of blueberry congaing anthocyanins can confer neurocognitive benefit for older adults. In addition, a study on elderly non-demented people indicated that administering the extract of Ginkgo biloba led to a reduction in blood viscosity, improvement of cerebral perfusion in specific areas and global cognitive functioning.(22) Moreover, it has been shown that the use of an astaxanthin-rich extract from H. pluvialis leads to an improvement in cognitive function.(23) Therefore, there is considerable evidence supporting the effectiveness of dietary supplements on improving cognitive function, and it is likely that such supplements may be useful for preventing dementia.
In the present study, we compared the improvements in cognitive function by word memory test, verbal fluency test and Stroop test between middle-aged and old-aged subjects who were administered dietary supplement containing astaxanthin-rich extract derived from P. carotinifaciens and subjects who received a placebo. Unlike the extract derived from H. pluvialis, the astaxanthin-rich extract derived from P. carotinifaciens has not been subjected to extensive clinical study. The astaxanthin-rich extract derived from P. carotinifaciens has different features such as free-form astaxanthin, adonirubin and adonixanthin, compared with that derived from H. pluvialis. Therefore, the present study suggests not only additional evidence of astaxanthin supplement but also efficacy of astaxanthin supplement differently derived, which will be helpful to develop dietary supplements for cognitive function and promote carotenoid research.
Materials and Methods
Study design
This study was a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel inter-group comparison.
Subjects
Classification and details regarding subjects who participated in this study are shown in Fig. 1. The placebo group was designated as group P and the astaxanthin group was designated as group A. A total of 245 healthy subjects aged 45–64 years old were initially identified as candidates for this study. On the basis of results of pre-study tests [physical tests and the Hasegawa Dementia Scale-Revised (HDS-R)] and pre-administration tests (medical interview and cognitive function tests), 60 subjects (group P: n = 30; group A: n = 30) were determined to be appropriate for participation as subjects and were enrolled in this study.
Of the 60 total subjects, one subject (ID 4245: group A) developed contact dermatitis (arms, left foot and face) on the 9th day of administration and discontinued the study to undergo treatment. The remaining 59 subjects completed the prescribed study schedule and performed all study tests. In addition, four subjects in group P and one in group A were excluded for per-protocol analysis because one of them was unconfirmed to ingest the test supplement, and the others changed their drinking habit, sport habit or working arrangement during this study. Thus, only the remaining 54 subjects (group P: n = 26; group A: n = 28) were available for efficacy analysis. Regarding safety, 60 adverse events (30 in group P, 30 in group A) were observed in subjects who were administered the supplement preparations even once. Data of all 59 of the original 60 subjects (group P: n = 30; group A: n = 29) who completed the study schedule and all study procedures were used in our analysis of safety. The administration rate of the supplement was between 94.6 and 106.7% for all 59 subjects.
Clinical registration
This study has been registered at University hospital Medical Information Network (https://upload.umin. ac.jp/cgi-open-bin/ctr_e/ctr_view.cgi?recptno=R000025682) on 15th May 2016 as “Efficacy study of oral astaxanthin to improve brain function for the middle aged and elderly”, Identification No. UMIN000022299.
Supplements
We prepared an astaxanthin-rich extract from P. carotinifaciens using the method described by Katsumata et al.(17) We then added arabic gum, maltodextrin, dl-α-tocopherol, medium-chain triglycerides, and l-ascorbic acid to the astaxanthin-rich extract, mixed the ingredients and dissolved them in water. After emulsification, we spray-dried the mixture to obtain 1% astaxanthin powder which also contains 0.14% adonirubin and 0.05% adonixanthin. We produced a jelly containing 2 mg of astaxanthin, which contains 1% astaxanthin powder, pH adjuster, sweetener, gelling agent, flavor (mixed berry) and water, and then it was administered to subjects in group A as astaxanthin supplement (Table 1). As a substitute, subjects in group P were given jelly containing artificial red coloring (Food Red No. 2, Amaranth) that was prepared the same method except 1% astaxanthin powder as the supplement provided to group A. The astaxanthin-rich extract from P. carotinifaciens and 1% astaxanthin powder were examined in terms of safety by Food and Drug Administration in United States, and have been filed as New Dietary Ingredients.
Table 1.
Item | Astaxanthin supplement | Placebo | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Form | 10 g of jelly × 4 pieces | 10 g of jelly × 4 pieces | ||
Ingredients (mg) | 1% Astaxanthin powder | 800 | Food coloring | 2 |
pH adjuster | 896 | pH adjuster | 896 | |
Sweetener | 4,092 | Sweetener | 4,092 | |
Gelling agent | 630 | Gelling agent | 630 | |
Flavor | 200 | Flavor | 200 | |
Water | 33,382 | Water | 34,180 | |
Astaxanthin content (mg) | 8 mg | — | ||
Energy (kJ) | 92 | 92 | ||
Protein (g) | 0.04 | 0.04 | ||
Fat (g) | 0.16 | 0.15 | ||
Carbohydrate (g) | 6.04 | 6.03 | ||
Sodium (mg) | 108 | 108 |
In previous studies with astaxanthin, 6 mg of astaxanthin per day and 12 mg of astaxanthin per day were administered to subjects for 12 weeks, and their cognitive function and erythrocyte antioxidant status were improved in the both groups.(23,24) Because the administration period of this study was 8 weeks which was shorter than those of the previous studies, the astaxanthin amount per day of administration was decided to 8 mg of astaxanthin per day in this study. All subjects were requested to ingest two jellies of their respective supplement preparations after breakfast and again after dinner for 8 weeks to be administered four jellies (8 mg of astaxanthin) per day or placebo.
Institutional review board
This study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki (October 2013 revision). The Ethical Guidelines for Biomedical Research Involving Human Subjects (Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology and the Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare, December 22, 2014) and the Ethical Guidance for Biomedical Research Involving Human Subjects [Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, and the Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare, February 9, 2015 (March 31, 2015 partial revision)] were followed.
Physical measurements
We measured the subjects’ height and weight, calculated their body mass index (BMI) and conducted the following tests: systolic and diastolic BP levels, heart rate, hematologic test, biological examination of blood and urine test (protein, sugar and occult blood).
Analysis of astaxanthin, adoniurbin, and adonixanthin concentration in human serum
The astaxanthin, adonirubin and adonixanthin (Fig. 2) concentration in human serum were analyzed by the method described by Fukuda et al.(25) A 1 ml of water and a 1 ml of ethanol, containing 0.85 µg of β-apo-8'-carotenal (internal standard) and 50 µg of 2,6-dibuthylhydroxytoluene, was added to 300 µl of serum. Then, 5 ml of hexane was added to the mixture and was shaken for 20 min. Then the hexane layer was taken in another test tube after centrifugation [4°C, 3,000 rpm (1,464 × g), 10 min] and was dried over under nitrogen gas flow. The residue was dissolved in 100 µl of chloroform/ethanol and subjected to UPLC-MS/MS analysis using Cadenza CD-C18 column with mixture of 85% acetonitrile in 2 mM ammonium acetate and acetonitrile/methanol/tetrahydrofuran (60:38:2) in 2 mM ammonium acetate as solvent. Detection and quantification were performed by selective reaction monitoring (SRM) of protonated molecule (MH+) of astaxanthin, adonirubin, and adonixanthin.
Cognitive function tests
Word memory test
The word memory test assesses the capacity of the subjects’ immediate and short-term memory as well as retention and ability to repeat words. We performed a test based on the references.(26,27) After reading seven words aloud one at a time to the subjects, they were asked to repeat the words. Immediately after they had repeated the seventh word, they were asked to recall all of the words (number of immediate-recall words). The subjects were given hints consisting of the initial sounds and categories of the words when they could not spontaneously recall. Words that they were then able to recall were recorded as the number of cued-recall words. In addition, subjects were given interference tasks [(2) Verbal fluency test] and were then asked to recall the words 5 min after the immediate-recall test (number of words recalled after 5 min). Once again, the subjects were given hints consisting of the initial sounds and categories of the words when they were unable to recall. The words which they were then able to recall were recorded as the number of cued-recall words. We then assessed the number of immediate-recall words, immediate-recall words + cued-recall words, recalled words after 5 min, and recalled words after 5 min + cued-recall words. Increases in the measured values indicate a memorization power improving action.
Verbal fluency test
The verbal fluency assessed the subjects’ memory-recall ability and was based on the reference.(28) In the “vegetable-verbal fluency test,” the subjects were instructed to “say the names of the vegetables to the greatest extent possible for 1 min.” After the first list was read, the number of words that could be recalled in 60 s was recorded and did not include duplicates. The same procedure was used for lists of words beginning with the words for “a” and lists of animal names. These tests were performed before administration and at the 4th and the 8th week after administration in differing order. Numbers of words the subjects were able to correctly list in each test were assessed. Increases in the number of correct answers indicate a cognitive function improving.
Stroop test
The Stroop test assesses the brain’s ability to distinguish and process two different types of data (verbal and color information) that enter the brain simultaneously. We followed the testing procedure of the New Stroop Test II(29,30) which consists of four steps (steps 1, 2, 3 and 4), each considered as separate sub-tests. In step 1, the subjects were asked to mark a check-box of the color of the ink indicated by the word. In step 2, the subjects were asked to mark a check-box of the color of the ink indicated by the word which did not match the color of ink. In step 3, the subjects were asked to select the word that corresponded to the color of the ink and mark the check box. In step 4, the subjects were asked to select the word that corresponded to the color of the ink used to write the word which did not match the color of the ink and mark the check-box. As the steps advance, the level of difficulty increases. The number of correct answers at each step was assessed. Increases in the measured values for the number of correct answers indicate improvement of brain information processing ability.
Sample size
We estimated sample size based on Word memory test (logical memory immediate retrieval) from data published by Santos et al.(22) It was assumed that the mean difference would be 2.0, with a SD of 2.5. To detect this difference, with a power of 80% and a significance level of 5% and taking into account that 10% of the patients will be lost to follow-up, it was calculated that thirty patients would be needed to be studied in each group.
Statistical analysis
For the subject background factors, a chi-square test was used to compare sex (male/female) between group A and P, and 2-sample t test was also used to compare the others between both groups. For the serum concentrations of carotenoid and cognitive function tests, 1-sample t test was used to compare changes between pre-administration baseline values to those at 4 weeks and 8 weeks after administration in groups A and P. Two-sample t test was also used to compare the amount of change between both groups in the serum concentrations of carotenoid. For cognitive function tests, we used two-way-repeated measures of ANOVA to compare the amount of change between both groups. Numerical values were expressed as the mean and SD, and the standard of significance for the tests was set at 5% on both sides. All statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 23.
Results
Background factors of subjects
The background factors of the 54 subjects who underwent effective analysis are listed in Table 2. The mean age of subjects in group P was 54.4 ± 6.0 years old, and there were a total of 13 men and 13 women. The mean age of the subjects in group A was 56.0 ± 5.2 years old, and there were 12 men and 16 women. HDS-R scores were 28.6 ± 1.2 and 28.5 ± 2.2 points in groups P and A, respectively, both of which were within the normal range. No intergroup differences were observed in data pertaining to the subjects’ age, sex, HDS-R score, physical measurement, or physical examination.
Table 2.
Item | Group P (n = 26) |
Group A (n = 28) |
p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |||
Age (years) | 54.4 | 6.0 | 56 | 5.2 | 0.307 | |
Sex (male/female) | Males: 13/Females: 13 | Males: 12/Females: 16 | 0.785 | |||
Height (cm) | 163.85 | 9.04 | 162.38 | 9.2 | 0.557 | |
Weight (kg) | 59.67 | 13.11 | 61.31 | 12.38 | 0.638 | |
BMI (kg/m2) | 22 | 3.09 | 23.09 | 3.04 | 0.197 | |
Systolic BP (mmHg) | 123.3 | 17.9 | 124.1 | 18.5 | 0.878 | |
Diastolic BP (mmHg) | 74.5 | 13.1 | 74.1 | 11.8 | 0.900 | |
Heart rate (bpm) | 71.2 | 8.9 | 68.3 | 10.1 | 0.280 | |
HDS-R score (points) | 28.6 | 1.2 | 28.5 | 2.2 | 0.816 |
Inter-group comparisons with group P (sex: chi-square test; items other than sex: 2-sample t test).
BP, blood pressure; bpm, beats per minutes; HDS-R, physical tests and the Hasegawa Dementia Scale-Revised.
Blood carotenoid levels
Table 3 presents changes in the measured values and the amount of change compared with the baseline for blood astaxanthin, adonirubin and adonixanthin levels. In group A, the values at 8 weeks after administration for all items were associated with significantly greater increases than group P (group P vs group A; astaxanthin: 0.000 ± 0.000 vs 0.173 ± 0.058 µg/ml, adonirubin: 0.000 ± 0.000 vs 0.042 ± 0.014 µg/ml and adonixanthin: 0.000 ± 0.000 vs 0.016 ± 0.006 µg/ml). In comparison with the baseline, though the values at 8 weeks after administration did not increase in group P, all the values at 8 weeks after administration were significantly elevated in group A.
Table 3.
Item | Numerical item | Group | Baseline |
Week 8 |
p (Inter-group) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |||||
Astaxanthin (µg/ml) | Measured value | P | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | ||
A | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.173* | 0.058 | ||||
Amount of change | P | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | ||||
A | 0.173 | 0.058 | ||||||
Adonirubin (µg/ml) | Measured value | P | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | ||
A | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.042* | 0.014 | ||||
Amount of change | P | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | ||||
A | 0.042 | 0.014 | ||||||
Adonixanthin (µg/ml) | Measured value | P | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | ||
A | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.016* | 0.006 | ||||
Amount of change | P | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | ||||
A | 0.016 | 0.006 |
Group P: n = 26, group A: n = 28. Intra-group comparison at baseline *p<0.01 (1-sample t test). Inter-group comparison with group P (2-sample t test).
Cognitive function tests
Table 4 presents changes in the measured values and the amount of change compared with the baseline for all the items of Word memory test, Verbal fluency test and Stroop tests.
Table 4.
Item | Numerical item | Group | Baseline |
Week 4 |
Week 8 |
p (Inter-group) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |||||||
Word memory test | Immediate recall | Measured value | P | 4.6 | 1.1 | 4.7 | 1.0 | 4.9 | 1.0 | |||
A | 4.6 | 0.8 | 5.0* | 0.9 | 5.1 | 1.0 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 0.0 | 1.5 | 0.3 | 1.7 | 0.600 | ||||||
A | 0.4 | 1.0 | 0.5 | 1.3 | ||||||||
Immediate recall + cued recall | Measured value | P | 5.8 | 0.9 | 5.6 | 1.0 | 6.1 | 0.9 | ||||
A | 5.8 | 0.9 | 6.1 | 0.9 | 6.3 | 0.7 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | –0.2 | 1.2 | 0.3 | 1.3 | 0.317 | ||||||
A | 0.3 | 1.2 | 0.4 | 1.3 | ||||||||
Recall after 5 min | Measured value | P | 4.0 | 1.5 | 4.0 | 1.6 | 4.8* | 1.5 | ||||
A | 4.5 | 1.3 | 4.8 | 1.3 | 4.9 | 1.4 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 0.0 | 2.1 | 0.8 | 1.7 | 0.389 | ||||||
A | 0.2 | 1.5 | 0.3 | 1.8 | ||||||||
Recall after 5 min + cued recall | Measured value | P | 5 | 1.2 | 4.8 | 1.3 | 5.5 | 1.3 | ||||
A | 5.3 | 1 | 5.3 | 1.3 | 5.9* | 0.8 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | –0.2 | 1.7 | 0.5 | 1.7 | 0.876 | ||||||
A | 0.0 | 1.5 | 0.6 | 1.3 | ||||||||
Verbal fluency test | Names of vegetables | Measured value | P | 16.6 | 3.4 | 16.2 | 4.1 | 17.2 | 3.5 | |||
A | 16 | 3.1 | 16.8 | 3.3 | 17.4** | 3.8 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | –0.4 | 3.1 | 0.6 | 3.1 | 0.348 | ||||||
A | 0.7 | 2.9 | 1.4 | 2.5 | ||||||||
Words that begin with ”a” | Measured value | P | 13.6 | 3.7 | 14.1 | 4.4 | 15.0* | 4.5 | ||||
A | 13.7 | 3.8 | 13.9 | 4.2 | 16.3** | 3.6 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 0.5 | 2.8 | 1.5 | 3.5 | 0.361 | ||||||
A | 0.2 | 4 | 2.5 | 3.9 | ||||||||
Animal words | Measured value | P | 18.3 | 4.8 | 20.0* | 4.8 | 20.5** | 5.1 | ||||
A | 17.9 | 4.4 | 19.1 | 4.6 | 20.1** | 4.5 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 1.7 | 4 | 2.2 | 3.8 | |||||||
A | 1.3 | 4 | 2.3 | 2.9 | 0.865 | |||||||
Stroop test | Step 1 | Measured value | P | 56.8 | 6.1 | 58.1 | 6.5 | 61.2** | 7.6 | |||
A | 58 | 6.3 | 60.7** | 6.9 | 63.6** | 6.5 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 1.3 | 5.3 | 4.5 | 5.4 | 0.545 | ||||||
A | 2.7 | 4.9 | 5.6 | 4.2 | ||||||||
Step 2 | Measured value | P | 48.2 | 5.7 | 50.2* | 5.7 | 52.4** | 6.9 | ||||
A | 49.3 | 5.7 | 51.5* | 5.5 | 53.5** | 5.1 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 2.0 | 3.9 | 4.2 | 5.3 | 0.977 | ||||||
A | 2.2 | 4.6 | 4.2 | 4.3 | ||||||||
Step 3 | Measured value | P | 38.6 | 3.9 | 40.0* | 4.6 | 41.6** | 5 | ||||
A | 39.6 | 3.6 | 41.5** | 4.4 | 42.8** | 4.7 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 1.4 | 2.9 | 3.0 | 3.0 | 0.824 | ||||||
A | 1.9 | 3.5 | 3.2 | 3.7 | ||||||||
Step 4 | Measured value | P | 33.6 | 3.8 | 35.2 | 5.6 | 35.6 | 7.8 | ||||
A | 33.5 | 4.3 | 35.1 | 4.4 | 36.3** | 5.3 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 1.6 | 5.5 | 2.0 | 6.9 | 0.863 | ||||||
A | 1.5 | 5.1 | 2.8 | 4.9 |
Group P: n = 26, group A: n = 28. Intra-group comparison at baseline *p<0.05 and **p<0.01 (1-sample t test). Inter-group comparison with group P (two-way-repeated measures of ANOVA).
Word memory test
There were no significant intergroup differences regarding the amount of change after administration for any of the items. In comparison with the baseline, the number of recalled words after 5 min was significantly increased 8 weeks after administration in group P. Moreover, the number of immediate-recall words 4 weeks after administration and recalled words after 5 min + cued-recall words 8 weeks after administration were significantly increased in group A compared with the baseline.
Verbal fluency test
There were no observed significant intergroup differences in the amount of change after administration for any of the items. In comparison with the baseline, words that begin with “a” 8 weeks after administration and animal words at both 4 and 8 weeks after administration exhibited significant increases in group P, and all items at 8 weeks after administration were significantly elevated in group A.
Stroop test
There were no significant intergroup differences in the amount of change after administration for any of the items. Compared with the baseline, there were significant increases at 8 weeks after administration for step 1 in group P and at 4 and 8 weeks after administration for group A. In addition, both groups were associated with significant increases at 4 and 8 weeks after administration for steps 2 and 3. Group A showed significant increases at 8 weeks after administration for step 4.
Additional analysis by the age group
Salthouse(31) reported the existence of cross-sectional age-related declines for many cognitive variables prior to age 60. Therefore, as an exploratory effective analysis, we divided the subjects into two age groups: (1) <55 (45–54) years old and (2) ≥55 (55–64) years old. We then performed an additional analysis for their cognitive functions by age group. The background factors of the subjects by age group are shown in Table 5A and B. Intergroup differences between all subjects aged <55 years old and ≥55 years old in Table 5A were observed in data pertaining to the subjects’ age, weight, BMI, systolic pressure and diastolic pressure (<55 years old vs ≥55 years old; age: 50.0 ± 2.9 years vs 59.7 ± 2.8 years, weight: 56.63 ± 9.90 kg vs 63.88 ± 13.91 kg, BMI: 21.59 ± 2.58 kg/m2 vs 23.40 ± 3.28 kg/m2, systolic pressure: 113.0 ± 15.1 mmHg vs 133.0 ± 15.2 mmHg and diastolic pressure: 68.4 ± 11.3 mmHg vs 79.4 ± 10.9 mmHg). Heart rate of the subjects aged <55 years old was lower than that of the subjects aged ≥55 years old (p<0.1). In addition, the HDS-score of the subjects aged <55 years old was larger than that of the subjects aged ≥55 years old (p<0.1). No intergroup differences between group P and group A divided by age 55 were observed in data pertaining to the subjects’ age, sex, HDS-R score, physical measurement, or physical examination (Table 5B).
Table 5A.
Item | <55 years (n = 25) |
≥55 years (n = 29) |
p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |||
Age (years) | 50 | 2.9 | 59.7 | 2.8 | 0.000 | |
Sex (males/females) | Males: 9/Females: 16 | Males: 16/Females: 13 | 0.182 | |||
Height (cm) | 161.55 | 7.9 | 164.41 | 9.91 | 0.252 | |
Weight (kg) | 56.63 | 9.9 | 63.88 | 13.91 | 0.034 | |
BMI (kg/m2) | 21.59 | 2.58 | 23.4 | 3.28 | 0.03 | |
Systolic pressure (mmHg) | 113 | 15.1 | 133 | 15.2 | 0.000 | |
Diastolic pressure (mmHg) | 68.4 | 11.3 | 79.4 | 10.9 | 0.001 | |
Heart rate (bpm) | 67 | 7.5 | 72 | 10.6 | 0.058 | |
HDS-R score (points) | 29 | 0.9 | 28.2 | 2.3 | 0.092 |
Inter-group comparisons with group P (sex: chi-square test; items other than sex: 2-sample t test).
bpm, beats per minutes; HDS-R, physical tests and the Hasegawa Dementia Scale-Revised.
Table 5B.
Item | <55 years |
≥55 years |
|||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Group P (n = 14) |
Group A (n = 11) |
p | Group P (n = 12) |
Group A (n = 17) |
p | ||||||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||||
Age (years) | 49.7 | 3.1 | 50.5 | 2.7 | 0.537 | 59.9 | 3.1 | 59.6 | 2.6 | 0.759 | |
Sex (males/females) | Males: 6/Females: 8 | Males: 3/Females: 8 | 0.677 | Males: 7/Females: 5 | Males: 9/Females: 8 | 1.000 | |||||
Height (cm) | 161.98 | 9.06 | 161.01 | 6.52 | 0.768 | 166.03 | 8.9 | 163.26 | 10.69 | 0.470 | |
Weight (kg) | 57.64 | 12.12 | 55.35 | 6.39 | 0.577 | 62.04 | 14.34 | 65.17 | 13.88 | 0.560 | |
BMI (kg/m2) | 21.8 | 3.09 | 21.32 | 1.87 | 0.657 | 22.23 | 3.22 | 24.23 | 3.15 | 0.106 | |
Systolic pressure (mmHg) | 114 | 16 | 111.6 | 14.5 | 0.706 | 134.2 | 13.6 | 132.1 | 16.5 | 0.727 | |
Diastolic pressure (mmHg) | 68.4 | 11.5 | 68.4 | 11.6 | 0.999 | 81.7 | 11.4 | 77.8 | 10.6 | 0.352 | |
Heart rate (bpm) | 68.1 | 6.2 | 65.6 | 9.1 | 0.421 | 74.7 | 10.4 | 70.1 | 10.6 | 0.255 | |
HDS-R score (points) | 28.9 | 1.0 | 29.2 | 0.8 | 0.389 | 28.3 | 1.4 | 28.1 | 2.7 | 0.753 |
Inter-group comparisons with group P (sex: chi-square test; items other than sex: 2-sample t test).
bpm, beats per minutes; HDS-R, physical tests and the Hasegawa Dementia Scale-Revised.
Changes in the measured values and the amount of change for all items of the subjects aged <55 years old and aged ≥55 years old in comparison to the baseline in the cognitive function tests are presented in Table 6A and B respectively. In subjects aged <55 years old, the group A indicated statistically significant improvement in the number of word recalled after 5 min of Word memory test, compared to the placebo group (group P vs group A; at 4 weeks: –0.4 ± 0.8 vs 1.2 ± 1.5 points, at 8 weeks: 0.9 ± 1.4 vs 1.2 ± 1.3 points). In addition, the number of immediate-recall words + number of cued-recall words and the number words recalled after 5 min + number of cued-recall words in group A were larger than those of group P (p<0.1). An investigation into the amount of change for all items in Word memory test among the subjects aged ≥55 years old showed no significant intergroup differences. Also, there were no significant intergroup differences in the amount of change after administration for any of the items in Verbal fluency test and Stroop test for both age groups.
Table 6A.
Item | Numerical item | Group | Baseline |
Week 4 |
Week 8 |
p (Inter-group) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |||||||
Word memory test | Immediate recall | Measured value | P | 4.7 | 1.4 | 4.6 | 1.1 | 5.1 | 1.1 | |||
A | 4.6 | 0.7 | 5.0 | 0.9 | 5.5* | 1.1 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | –0.1 | 1.8 | 0.4 | 2.1 | 0.650 | ||||||
A | 0.4 | 1.0 | 0.9 | 1.1 | ||||||||
Immediate recall + cued recall | Measured value | P | 5.9 | 1.0 | 5.7 | 0.8 | 6.3 | 1.0 | ||||
A | 5.6 | 0.8 | 6.5* | 0.7 | 6.5* | 0.7 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | –0.1 | 1.2 | 0.4 | 1.4 | 0.085 | ||||||
A | 0.9 | 1.1 | 0.9 | 1.1 | ||||||||
Recall after 5 min | Measured value | P | 4.5 | 1.3 | 4.1 | 1.4 | 5.4* | 1.2 | ||||
A | 4.4 | 1.4 | 5.5* | 0.9 | 5.5* | 1.1 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | –0.4 | 1.8 | 0.9 | 1.4 | 0.027 | ||||||
A | 1.2 | 1.5 | 1.2 | 1.3 | ||||||||
Recall after 5 min + cued recall | Measured value | P | 5.4 | 1.2 | 5.0 | 1.0 | 6.0 | 1.2 | ||||
A | 5.1 | 0.9 | 6.0 | 1.1 | 6.2** | 0.8 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | –0.4 | 1.6 | 0.6 | 1.5 | 0.074 | ||||||
A | 0.9 | 1.4 | 1.1 | 1.0 | ||||||||
Verbal fluency test | Names of vegetables | Measured value | P | 17.4 | 3.5 | 17.1 | 3.0 | 18 | 3.4 | |||
A | 15.9 | 2.2 | 17.7* | 2.8 | 17.6** | 2.8 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | –0.2 | 2.8 | 0.6 | 3.0 | 0.11 | ||||||
A | 1.8 | 2.1 | 1.7 | 1.7 | ||||||||
Words that begin with “a” | Measured value | P | 13.7 | 3.7 | 13.2 | 3.6 | 15.6 | 5.4 | ||||
A | 14.1 | 3.2 | 14.4 | 4.8 | 17.5** | 3.2 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | –0.5 | 3.0 | 1.9 | 3.9 | 0.607 | ||||||
A | 0.3 | 4.3 | 3.4 | 2.9 | ||||||||
Animal words | Measured value | P | 19.6 | 3.6 | 20.5* | 3.3 | 21.1* | 3.7 | ||||
A | 19.5 | 4.5 | 20.4 | 4.9 | 22.5* | 3.8 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 0.9 | 1.4 | 1.6 | 2.2 | 0.437 | ||||||
A | 0.8 | 4.8 | 2.9 | 3.3 | ||||||||
Stroop test | Step 1 | Measured value | P | 55.6 | 6.0 | 58.6* | 6.0 | 62.7** | 6.5 | |||
A | 60.6 | 5.4 | 62.6 | 5.8 | 67.6** | 4.8 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 2.9 | 4.7 | 7.1 | 3.5 | 0.836 | ||||||
A | 2.0 | 5.1 | 7.0 | 4.2 | ||||||||
Step 2 | Measured value | P | 48.6 | 5.6 | 50.7* | 6.7 | 53.9** | 5.8 | ||||
A | 51 | 4.6 | 52 | 4.3 | 54.6** | 4.9 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 2.1 | 3.3 | 5.4 | 3.3 | 0.510 | ||||||
A | 1.0 | 3.9 | 3.6 | 2.9 | ||||||||
Step 3 | Measured value | P | 38.9 | 4.4 | 41.0** | 5.2 | 42.0** | 5.8 | ||||
A | 40.7 | 3.0 | 41.5 | 4.7 | 44.3* | 5.0 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 2.1 | 2.6 | 3.1 | 3.0 | 0.366 | ||||||
A | 0.8 | 3.7 | 3.5 | 4.0 | ||||||||
Step 4 | Measured value | P | 33.7 | 3.6 | 35.5 | 7.1 | 35.3 | 10.1 | ||||
A | 33.7 | 4.1 | 36.4 | 4.7 | 37.7** | 5.6 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 1.8 | 7.3 | 1.6 | 8.8 | 0.702 | ||||||
A | 2.6 | 5.1 | 4.0 | 3.4 |
Group P: n = 14, group A: n = 11. Intra-group comparison at baseline *p<0.05 and **p<0.01 (1-sample t test). Inter-group comparison with group P (two-way-repeated measures of ANOVA)
Table 6B.
Item | Numerical item | Group | Baseline |
Week 4 |
Week 8 |
p (Inter-group) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |||||||
Word memory test | Immediate recall | Measured value | P | 4.5 | 0.7 | 4.8 | 0.9 | 4.6 | 0.9 | |||
A | 4.6 | 0.9 | 5.0 | 0.9 | 4.8 | 0.7 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 0.3 | 1.0 | 0.1 | 1.1 | 0.906 | ||||||
A | 0.4 | 1.0 | 0.2 | 1.3 | ||||||||
Immediate recall + cued recall | Measured value | P | 5.7 | 0.8 | 5.5 | 1.2 | 5.9 | 0.8 | ||||
A | 5.9 | 1.0 | 5.9 | 1.0 | 6.1 | 0.7 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | –0.2 | 1.3 | 0.3 | 1.1 | 0.883 | ||||||
A | –0.1 | 1.2 | 0.1 | 1.4 | ||||||||
Recall after 5 min | Measured value | P | 3.4 | 1.5 | 3.9 | 1.8 | 4.0 | 1.4 | ||||
A | 4.6 | 1.2 | 4.2 | 1.2 | 4.4 | 1.3 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 0.5 | 2.4 | 0.6 | 2.1 | 0.343 | ||||||
A | –0.4 | 1.2 | –0.2 | 1.9 | ||||||||
Recall after 5 min + cued recall | Measured value | P | 4.6 | 1.2 | 4.7 | 1.6 | 4.9 | 1.3 | ||||
A | 5.4 | 1.0 | 4.8 | 1.2 | 5.8 | 0.8 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 0.1 | 1.8 | 0.3 | 2.1 | 0.428 | ||||||
A | –0.6 | 1.2 | 0.4 | 1.4 | ||||||||
Verbal fluency test | Names of vegetables | Measured value | P | 15.7 | 3.2 | 15.1 | 5.1 | 16.2 | 3.5 | |||
A | 16.1 | 3.6 | 16.1 | 3.5 | 17.3 | 4.4 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | –0.6 | 3.6 | 0.5 | 3.4 | 0.837 | ||||||
A | 0.0 | 3.1 | 1.2 | 3.0 | ||||||||
Words that begin with “a” | Measured value | P | 13.4 | 3.8 | 15.1* | 5.1 | 14.3 | 3.3 | ||||
A | 13.5 | 4.1 | 13.6 | 3.8 | 15.5 | 3.7 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 1.7 | 2.2 | 0.9 | 3.2 | 0.198 | ||||||
A | 0.1 | 4.0 | 2.0 | 4.4 | ||||||||
Animal words | Measured value | P | 16.9 | 5.8 | 19.4 | 6.3 | 19.8 | 6.4 | ||||
A | 16.8 | 4.0 | 18.3 | 4.3 | 18.6* | 4.3 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 2.5 | 5.8 | 2.8 | 5.0 | 0.756 | ||||||
A | 1.5 | 3.4 | 1.8 | 2.6 | ||||||||
Stroop test | Step 1 | Measured value | P | 58.1 | 6.2 | 57.6 | 7.3 | 59.5 | 8.7 | |||
A | 56.3 | 6.3 | 59.5* | 7.5 | 61.0** | 6.2 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | –0.5 | 5.6 | 1.4 | 5.7 | 0.110 | ||||||
A | 3.2 | 4.9 | 4.7 | 4.1 | ||||||||
Step 2 | Measured value | P | 47.8 | 6.0 | 49.6 | 4.5 | 50.6 | 7.8 | ||||
A | 48.1 | 6.2 | 51.1* | 6.3 | 52.7** | 5.3 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 1.8 | 4.7 | 2.8 | 7.0 | 0.619 | ||||||
A | 3.0 | 4.9 | 4.6 | 5.0 | ||||||||
Step 3 | Measured value | P | 38.3 | 3.5 | 38.8 | 3.7 | 41.2** | 4.2 | ||||
A | 38.8 | 3.7 | 41.5** | 4.3 | 41.8** | 4.3 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 0.5 | 3.1 | 2.8 | 3.2 | 0.160 | ||||||
A | 2.6 | 3.2 | 3.0 | 3.6 | ||||||||
Step 4 | Measured value | P | 33.5 | 4.1 | 34.8 | 3.4 | 36 | 4.4 | ||||
A | 33.4 | 4.5 | 34.2 | 4.1 | 35.4 | 5.0 | ||||||
Amount of change | P | 1.3 | 2.7 | 2.5 | 4.1 | 0.946 | ||||||
A | 0.8 | 5.2 | 1.9 | 5.6 |
Group P: n = 12, group A: n = 17. Intra-group comparison at baseline *p<0.05 and **p<0.01 (1-sample t test). Inter-group comparison with group P (two-way-repeated measures of ANOVA).
Safety assessment
We believe that the cause of the rash (i.e., redness and slight itchiness) that was observed on subject ID 4245 (group A) who discontinued the study was related to the subject trimming garden trees on the day before the onset of the rash, resulting in a case of contact dermatitis. The principal investigator (a physician) determined that the rash was “unrelated” to the supplement.
Subject ID 4161 (group P) developed mild acne vulgaris on nine occasions between the 17th and 48th day after administration. Although the cause could not be identified, the intermittent appearance of acne disappeared without the use of medication and it did not reappear after the 48th day after administration. On the basis of these observations, the principal investigator determined that the condition was “likely unrelated” to the supplement.
Investigation of other adverse events indicated that all were “unrelated” to the supplement. Therefore, we concluded that the supplement caused no adverse events (side effects). Results of our investigation regarding changes in numerical values for physical measurements (including blood and urine tests) in all groups indicated that although there were minor changes, none were determined to be clinically significant.
Discussion
Astaxanthin is a powerful antioxidant that can penetrate the blood-brain barrier, and it is thus often used in research related to cognitive function. Nakagawa et al.(24) reported that the astaxanthin administration led to a reduction in phospholipid peroxidation in red blood cells. Because patients with dementia are known to have increased phospholipid peroxidation in their red blood cells, ingesting astaxanthin is considered to be putative preventative effect against dementia. In addition, a study utilizing a model of Parkinson’s disease using 6-hydroxydopamine indicated that adding astaxanthin suppressed neuronal apoptosis.(32) Furthermore, astaxanthin administration has been shown to decrease amyloid-β levels in red blood cells, which is considered to decrease the ability of red blood cells to supply the brain with oxygen;(33) thus, astaxanthin administration might be useful for a treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. It has also been reported that astaxanthin suppresses the extent of oxidative stress in cases of cerebral ischemia and has a neuro-protective effect.(34) Moreover, it was reported that astaxanthin administration in mice has a concentration-dependent effect of promoting neurogenesis in the hippocampus, which is related to learning and memory, suggesting that astaxanthin contributes to memory improvement.(35) Thus, several studies have verified the fact that astaxanthin is effective in improving the brain and neuronal function and is prophylactic against cerebral damage.
The age of the subjects in the present study was 45–64 years old. Because age may affect how astaxanthin administration influences cognitive function, all 54 participants were not only subjected to an effective analysis as a whole but also a subgroup analysis after dividing them into <55 years old and ≥55 years old groups. Cognitive functions were assessed using Word memory test, Verbal fluency test, and Stroop test.
The results of serum carotenoid level measurements (astaxanthin, adonirubin and adonixanthin) in the subjects of group A indicated that astaxanthin, adonirubin and adonixanthin were absorbed into blood after administration of astaxanthin supplement. Ohi et al.(36) reported that maximum plasma astaxanthin concentration of subjects taking a soft gel capsule as single dose containing astaxanthin of 9 mg derived from H. pluvialis was 0.077 ± 0.066 µg/ml, which was half lower than the concentration in group A of the present study (0.173 ± 0.058 µg/ml). It could be caused by the difference between single administration and repeated administration. Also, the carotenoid ratio in serum of subjects were astaxanthin:adonirubin:adonixanthin = 1:0.24:0.09 and that of astaxanthin supplement were astaxanthin:adonirubin: adonixanthin = 1:0.14:0.05. Consequently, the bioavailability of adonirubin and adonixanthin were superior to astaxanthin. Absorption process of carotenoids requires release of carotenoids from food matrix, diffusion in lipid emulsion, solubilization by pancreatic lipases and bile salts, formation of mixed micelles, movement across the microvilli, uptake of carotenoids by intestinal mucosal cells, incorporation into chylomicrons and circulation to lymphatic.(37) The difference of the molecular structures between astaxanthin, adonirubin and adonixanthin might affect the absorption process, which resulted in the shift of the ratio. We think the serum carotenoid levels in group A were maintained during this study.
We did not detect any significant intergroup differences in the amount of change in any outcome measure for the cognitive function tests among the 54 subjects, while group A showed significantly improvements from their baseline in more items of all the tests compared with group P. An analysis according to age group indicated that subjects aged <55 years old in group A had significantly larger increase in the number of recalled words after 5 min in the Word memory test than that in group P. These finding indicate a certain improvement in group A. It is likely that this result is related to the neurogenesis effect of astaxanthin in the hippocampus that was reported by Yook et al.(35) In addition, this may be reasonable because other substances which have antioxidant ability such as omega-3 fatty acids,(19) tocotrienol(20) and anthocyanins(21) are reported to be effective on cognitive function. The weight, BMI, systolic pressure and diastolic pressure of the subjects aged ≥55 years old were significantly higher than those of the subjects aged <55 years old. It was reported that high BP in middle age is a risk factor for late-life cognitive decline and dementia.(38) Hence, it might be difficult for people aged ≥55 years old to improve their cognitive function with astaxanthin supplement for a short period of time like 8 weeks due to the influence of higher BP. Currently, it remains unclear whether astaxanthin supplement administration by human for 4–8 weeks has an effect of suppressing oxidation in the brain and accumulation of amyloid-β; however, astaxanthin supplement may be effective for preventing dementia if ingested from a young age. Further investigation into the molecular and cellular mechanisms of astaxanthin that lead to these improvements in cognitive functions is required.
Previous clinical studies regarding the effect of astaxanthin administration on cognitive functions include the study by Katagiri et al.(23) involving astaxanthin derived from H. pluvialis. The results of that study suggested that astaxanthin administration improved cognitive function, despite the lack of significant differences between astaxanthin and placebo groups. Although the method of assessing cognitive functions used in the present study differ from that employed by Katagiri et al.,(23) because similar doses (the study of Katagiri et al.(23) vs the present study; asthaxanthin doses: 0, 6 and 12 mg/day vs 0 and 8 mg/day) and subjects (the subjects were aged 45–64 years old in both studies) were used, it is likely that the presence of statistically significant differences is related to the source of astaxanthin. Besides astaxanthin, adonirubin and adonixanthin are found between 20 and 30% in carotenoid derived from P. carotinifaciens, and can efficiently quench singlet oxygen.(18) Inoue et al.(39) reported that adonirubin and adonixanthin suppressed reactive oxygen species production and protected cells against light-induced damage as astaxanthin, and adonixanthin protected against light-induced cell damage through not only an anti-oxidative response but also through Nrf2 activation. In addition, it was reported that human neurodegenerative diseases have been associated with inflammation and dysregulation of the Nrf2 system.(40) Therefore, adonirubin and adonixanthin exhibit a multiplier effect with astaxanthin in terms of the antioxidant properties and biological activations like Nrf2, which may explain why a greater improvement in cognitive function was observed in the present study. Further investigation into the effect of different derivations is required.
Regarding safety, an investigation of adverse events indicated that subject ID 4161 (group P) developed acne vulgaris; however, the principal investigator determined this to be “likely unrelated” to the supplement. All other adverse events were determined to be “unrelated” to the supplement. There were no abnormalities in the physical measurements or other tests performed. Although there were some significant differences observed in several items compared with the baseline, under the conditions of the present study, we concluded that astaxanthin supplement was safe for administration. This conclusion is consistent with the findings of Katsumata et al.,(17) who reported that repeated administration of astaxanthin derived from P. carotinifaciens by rats over 13 weeks at a dose of 1,000 mg/day was not associated with negative effects.
Authors’ Contributions
MH and TI designed the study, and wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. TM contributed to interpretation of data, and assisted in the preparation of the manuscript.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank M. Fukuda for technical assistance with analysis of astaxanthin, adonixanthin and adonirubin.
Abbreviations
- BP
blood pressure
- bpm
beats per minutes
- HDS-R
physical tests and the Hasegawa Dementia Scale-Revised
Conflict of Interest
MH and TI are employees of JXTG Nippon Oil & Energy Corporation.
References
- 1.Gong M, Bassi A. Carotenoids from microalgae: a review of recent developments. Biotechnol Adv. 2016;34:1396–1412. doi: 10.1016/j.biotechadv.2016.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Nakajima Y, Inokuchi Y, Shimazawa M, Otsubo K, Ishibashi T, Hara H. Astaxanthin, a dietary carotenoid, protects retinal cells against oxidative stress in-vitro and in mice in-vivo. J Pharm Pharmacol. 2008;60:1365–1374. doi: 10.1211/jpp/60.10.0013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Nagaki A, Hayasaka S, Yamada T, Hayasaka Y, Sanada M, Uonomi T. Effects of astaxanthin on accommodation, critical flicker fusion, and pattern visual evoked potential in visual display terminal workers. J Trad Med. 2002;19:170–173. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Manabe E, Handa O, Naito Y, et al. Astaxanthin protects mesangial cells from hyperglycemia-induced oxidative signaling. J Cell Biochem. 2008;103:1925–1937. doi: 10.1002/jcb.21583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Aoi W, Naito Y, Takanami Y, et al. Astaxanthin improves muscle lipid metabolism in exercise via inhibitory effect of oxidative CPT I modification. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2008;366:892–897. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2007.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Aoi W, Naito Y, Sakuma K, et al. Astaxanthin limits exercise-induced skeletal and cardiac muscle damage in mice. Antioxid Redox Signal. 2003;5:139–144. doi: 10.1089/152308603321223630. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Tominaga K, Hongo N, Karato M, Yamashita E. Cosmetic benefits of astaxanthin on humans subjects. Acta Biochim Pol. 2012;59:43–47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kishimoto Y, Tani M, Uto-Kondo H, et al. Astaxanthin suppresses scavenger receptor expression and matrix metalloproteinase activity in macrophages. Eur J Nutr. 2010;49:119–126. doi: 10.1007/s00394-009-0056-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kuraji M, Matsuno T, Satoh T. Astaxanthin affects oxidative stress and hyposalivation in aging mice. J Clin Biochem Nutr. 2016;59:79–85. doi: 10.3164/jcbn.15-150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hussein G, Nakagawa T, Goto H, et al. Astaxanthin ameliorates features of metabolic syndrome in SHR/NDmcr-cp. Life Sci. 2007;80:522–529. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2006.09.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Yamashita E. Industrial production of astaxanthin using the green algae Haematococcus pluvialis and it use. Nippon Nogeikagaku Kaishi. 2015;76:740–743. (in Japanese) [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kitamura A, Hirai K, Yamashita E. Commercial production of astaxanthin using green algae in the family Haematococcus. Seibutsu-kogaku Kaishi. 2015;3:383–387. (in Japanese) [Google Scholar]
- 13.Johnson EA, Lewis M. Astaxanthin formation by the yeast Phaffia rhodozyma. J Gen Microbiol. 1979;115:173–183. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Andrewes AG, Starr MP. (3R,3'R)-astaxanthin from the yeast Phaffia rhodozyma. Phytochem. 1976;15:1009–1011. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Andrewes AG, Phaffia HJ, Starr MP. Carotenoids of Phaffia rhodozyma, a red-pigmented fermenting yeast. Phytochem. 1976;15:1003–1007. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Tsubokura A, Yoneda H, Mizuta H. Paracoccus carotinifaciens sp. nov., a new aerobic gram-negative astaxanthin-producing bacterium. Int J System Bacteriol. 1999;49 Pt 1:277–282. doi: 10.1099/00207713-49-1-277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Katsumata T, Ishibashi T, Kyle D. A sub-chronic toxicity evaluation of a natural astaxanthin-rich carotenoid extract of Paracoccus carotinifaciens in rats. Toxicol Rep. 2014;1:582–588. doi: 10.1016/j.toxrep.2014.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Maoka T, Yasui H, Ohmori A, et al. Anti-oxidative, anti-tumor-promoting, and anti-carcinogenic activities of adonirubin and adonixanthin. J Oleo Sci. 2013;62:181–186. doi: 10.5650/jos.62.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lim SY, Kim EJ, Kim A, Lee HJ, Choi HJ, Yang SJ. Nutritional factors affecting mental health. Clin Nutr Res. 2016;5:143–152. doi: 10.7762/cnr.2016.5.3.143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kaneai N, Sumitani K, Fukui K, Koike T, Takatsu H, Urano S. Tocotrienol improves learning and memory deficit of aged rats. J Clin Biochem Nutr. 2016;58:114–121. doi: 10.3164/jcbn.15-52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Krikorian R, Shidler MD, Nash TA, et al. Blueberry supplementation improves memory in older adults. J Agric Supplement Chem. 2010;58:3996–4000. doi: 10.1021/jf9029332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Santos RF, Galduróz JC, Barbieri A, Castiglioni ML, Ytaya LY, Bueno OF. Cognitive performance, SPECT, and blood viscosity in elderly non-demented people using Ginkgo biloba. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2003;36:127–133. doi: 10.1055/s-2003-41197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Katagiri M, Satoh A, Tsuji S, Shirasawa T. Effects of astaxanthin-rich Haematococcus pluvialis extract on cognitive function: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. J Clin Biochem Nutr. 2012;51:102–107. doi: 10.3164/jcbn.D-11-00017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Nakagawa K, Kiko T, Miyazawa T, et al. Antioxidant effect of astaxanthin on phospholipid peroxidation in human erythrocytes. Br J Nutr. 2011;105:1563–1571. doi: 10.1017/S0007114510005398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Fukuda M, Ishibashi T, Maoka T. Analysis of adonixanthin and adonirubin in human Serum by ultra performance liquid chromatography (UPLC)-MS/MS. Carotenoid Sci. 2017;21:42–46. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Imamura Y. Manual for the Clinical Assessment of Higher Brain Function 2000, revised 2nd edition. Tokyo: Shinkoh Igaku Shuppansha Co., Ltd.; 2001. pp. 17–18. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Uemura K. Simple and effective assessment of posttraumatic higher brain function disorders with special reference to the prefrontal area. In: Kanno T, Kato Y, editors. Minimally Invasive Neurosurgery and Multidisciplinary Neurotraumatology. Tokyo: Springer; 2006. pp. 293–309. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Clark DG, McLaughlin PM, Woo E, et al. Novel verbal fluency scores and structural brain imaging for prediction of cognitive outcome in mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2016;2:113–122. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2016.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hakoda Y, Watanabe M. New Stroop Test II. Fukuoka: Toyo Physical Co., Ltd.; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Nouchi R, Taki Y, Takeuchi H, et al. Brain training game boosts executive functions, working memory and processing speed in the young adults: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One. 2013;8:e55518. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30:507–514. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ikeda Y, Tsuji S, Satoh A, Ishikura M, Shirasawa T, Shimizu T. Protective effects of astaxanthin on 6-hydroxydopamine-induced apoptosis in human neuroblastoma SH-SY5Y cells. J Neurochem. 2008;107:1730–1740. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05743.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kiko T, Nakagawa K, Satoh A, et al. Amyloid β levels in human red blood cells. PLoS One. 2012;7:e49620. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lee DH, Lee YJ, Kwon KH. Neuroprotective effects of astaxanthin in oxygen-glucose deprivation in SH-SY5Y cells and global cerebral ischemia in rat. J Clin Biochem Nutr. 2010;47:121–129. doi: 10.3164/jcbn.10-29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Yook JS, Okamoto M, Rakwal R, et al. Astaxanthin supplementation enhances adult hippocampal neurogenesis and spatial memory in mice. Mol Nutr Food Res. 2016;60:589–599. doi: 10.1002/mnfr.201500634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Ohi Y, Kitamura A, Tsukahara H, et al. Pharmacokinetic of astaxanthin after oral administration of a soft gel capsule preparation. Rinshoiyaku. 2011;27:297–303. (in Japanese) [Google Scholar]
- 37.Saini RK, Nile SH, Park SW. Carotenoids from fruits and vegetables: chemistry, analysis, occurrence, bioavailability and biological activities. Food Res Int. 2015;76 (Pt 3):735–750. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2015.07.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Qiu C, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. The age-dependent relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2005;4:487–499. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70141-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Inoue Y, Shimazawa M, Nagano R, et al. Astaxanthin analogs, adonixanthin and lycopene, activate Nrf2 to prevent light-induced photoreceptor degeneration. J Pharmacol Sci. 2017;134:147–157. doi: 10.1016/j.jphs.2017.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sandberg M, Patil J, D'Angelo B, Weber SG, Mallard C. NRF2-regulation in brain health and disease: implication of cerebral inflammation. Neuropharmacology. 2014;79:298–306. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]