Skip to main content
BMJ Open logoLink to BMJ Open
. 2018 Mar 23;8(3):e019362. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-019362

Lifestyle and vascular risk effects on MRI-based biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease: a cross-sectional study of middle-aged adults from the broader New York City area

Lisa Mosconi 1,2,3, Michelle Walters 3, Joanna Sterling 4, Crystal Quinn 2, Pauline McHugh 2, Randolph E Andrews 5, Dawn C Matthews 5, Christine Ganzer 6, Ricardo S Osorio 2, Richard S Isaacson 1, Mony J De Leon 2, Antonio Convit 2
PMCID: PMC5875649  PMID: 29574441

Abstract

Objective

To investigate the effects of lifestyle and vascular-related risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease (AD) on in vivo MRI-based brain atrophy in asymptomatic young to middle-aged adults.

Design

Cross-sectional, observational.

Setting

Broader New York City area. Two research centres affiliated with the Alzheimer’s disease Core Center at New York University School of Medicine.

Participants

We studied 116 cognitively normal healthy research participants aged 30–60 years, who completed a three-dimensional T1-weighted volumetric MRI and had lifestyle (diet, physical activity and intellectual enrichment), vascular risk (overweight, hypertension, insulin resistance, elevated cholesterol and homocysteine) and cognition (memory, executive function, language) data. Estimates of cortical thickness for entorhinal (EC), posterior cingulate, orbitofrontal, inferior and middle temporal cortex were obtained by use of automated segmentation tools. We applied confirmatory factor analysis and structural equation modelling to evaluate the associations between lifestyle, vascular risk, brain and cognition.

Results

Adherence to a Mediterranean-style diet (MeDi) and insulin sensitivity were both positively associated with MRI-based cortical thickness (diet: βs≥0.26, insulin sensitivity βs≥0.58, P≤0.008). After accounting for vascular risk, EC in turn explained variance in memory (P≤0.001). None of the other lifestyle and vascular risk variables were associated with brain thickness. In addition, the path associations between intellectual enrichment and better cognition were significant (βs≥0.25 P≤0.001), as were those between overweight and lower cognition (βs≥-0.22, P≤0.01).

Conclusions

In cognitively normal middle-aged adults, MeDi and insulin sensitivity explained cortical thickness in key brain regions for AD, and EC thickness predicted memory performance in turn. Intellectual activity and overweight were associated with cognitive performance through different pathways. Our findings support further investigation of lifestyle and vascular risk factor modification against brain ageing and AD. More studies with larger samples are needed to replicate these research findings in more diverse, community-based settings.

Keywords: brain aging, brain Imaging, alzheimer’s disease, lifestyle, vascular risk


Strengths and limitations of this study.

  • One of the key strengths of this study is the availability of a well-characterised population of asymptomatic middle-aged healthy adults with multiple lifestyle and vascular risk measures, as well as neuroimaging.

  • Our statistical approach using confirmatory factor analysis and structural equation modelling allowed us to simultaneously discern the independent and inter-related effects of several lifestyle variables and vascular risk factors on brain atrophy and cognition.

  • A limitation of the study was its cross-sectional nature. Longitudinal studies with larger samples are necessary to test for temporal and causal inter-relationship between modifiable risk factors and future Alzheimer’s disease (AD).

  • We caution that participants were carefully screened research participants, which limits our ability to generalise results to the entire population. More studies are needed to replicate these research findings in community-based settings.

  • MRI measures are largely seen as biomarkers of neurodegeneration associated with AD. However, biomarkers specific to AD pathology (amyloid and tau pathology) are warranted to determine whether the associations between lifestyle and vascular risk are indeed related to ongoing AD.

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common form of dementia, affecting nearly 34 million people worldwide.1 Unless effective strategies for prevention are found, the prevalence of AD is expected to triple by 2050.

Given the current lack of disease-modifying treatment,2 and increasing awareness that AD pathology develops over many years prior to clinical symptoms,3 the potential for prevention is crucial to reduce AD risk and/or delay the onset of cognitive decline.

There is substantial epidemiological evidence linking modifiable risk factors, such as midlife hypertension, obesity, diabetes, poor diet, physical and intellectual inactivity, with increased risk of late-onset AD. Recent population-attributable models estimated that one in every three cases of AD may be accounted for by these lifestyle and vascular-related risk factors.1 4 As a result, trends in AD risk reduction research have focused on lifestyle interventions as well as vascular risk reduction.2 3 5

While the importance of lifestyle and vascular risk management in health is well understood, their specific effects on AD pathophysiology, and thus their effectiveness for AD prevention are less clear.6

Conflicting results in the literature may be due to the use of different patient populations, different lifestyle measures, different statistical constructs and an overall lack of AD biomarkers. Moreover, though a combined reduction in risk factors is projected to have a greater impact than any one factor alone,4 most studies have examined one or few variables at a time.7 Further, although there is consensus that preventative interventions have the highest chances of success when implemented in midlife,2 the majority of studies focused on the elderly, including those already experiencing cognitive impairment.8–14

Neuropathologically, AD is characterised by the presence of amyloid-beta (Aβ) plaques, neurofibrillary tangles and neuronal loss in selectively vulnerable brain regions.6 Neuronal loss in AD originates in the medial temporal lobes during the normal stages of cognition and spreads to cortical regions with disease progression.15 MRI has long been used to visualise neurodegeneration in vivo. Several MRI studies have shown that brain cortical thinning (ie, atrophy) in AD-vulnerable regions begins many years prior to dementia onset, making this biomarker an ideal candidate to monitor the effectiveness of preventative interventions.16

This study examines the simultaneous effects of multiple modifiable lifestyle and vascular risk factors on MRI-based cortical atrophy in a cohort of middle-aged healthy adults at risk for AD.

Methods

Participants

Participants were enrolled in observational brain imaging studies of clinically and cognitively normal middle-aged adults conducted at two research centres affiliated with the Alzheimer’s disease Coordinating Center of New York University (NYU) School of Medicine between 2010 and 2016. Details about the studies have previously been published.17–21 All participants provided informed consent to participate in this study.

Briefly, participants were derived from multiple community sources, including individuals interested in research participation, family members and caregivers of impaired patients. Subjects were 30–60 years old, with ≥12-year education, Clinical Dementia Rating=0, Global Deterioration Scale<2, Hamilton depression scale <16 and normal cognitive test performance for age and education.22 Because our goal was to examine the influence of lifestyle and vascular risk factors in healthy middle-aged participants without overt cardiovascular and/or cerebrovascular disease, we excluded individuals with Modified Hachinski Ischaemia scale score >4. Those with medical conditions or history of conditions that may affect brain structure or function (eg, stroke, poorly controlled diabetes, head trauma, any neurodegenerative diseases, depression, hydrocephalus, intracranial mass and infarcts on MRI) and those taking steroids or psychoactive medications were excluded. Only participants with clinical, laboratory, neuropsychological, volumetric MRI examinations and lifestyle questionnaires within 6 months of each other were included.

A family history of AD that included at least one first-degree relative whose AD onset was after age 60 was elicited using standardised questionnaires.17 Apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotypes were determined using standard quantitative PCR procedures.17

Cognitive measures

The neuropsychological battery of tests was previously described.22 Briefly, three cognitive domains were assessed from the following tests: memory (immediate and delayed recall of a paragraph and immediate and delayed recall of paired associates), executive function (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) digit symbol substitution) and language (WAIS vocabulary).

We modelled memory as a latent factor composed of standardised versions of immediate and delayed recall scores using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) (alpha=0.84). The three cognitive domains were examined as outcome variables (see Statistical analysis).

Vascular risk measures

Vascular risk factors included in the model were: (1) overweight, measured using the body mass index (BMI, kg/m2); (2) presence of hypertension, conservatively determined based on either current antihypertensive treatment or blood pressure assessments; (3) elevated plasma cholesterol and/or elevated plasma homocysteine, as obtained after overnight fasting using standard laboratory procedures and (4) insulin sensitivity, measured with the Quantitative Insulin Sensitivity Check Index,23 where lower scores reflect greater insulin resistance and higher scores reflect insulin sensitivity.

Lifestyle measures

Diet

Dietary data regarding average food consumption over the prior year were obtained using the Block or Harvard food frequency questionnaire, as previously described.19 Briefly, food items were categorised into food groups based on similarities in food and nutrient composition, and intake of each food group was calculated by summing the intakes of food group items. Mediterranean diet (MeDi) scores were generated as the sum of caloric intake-adjusted daily gram intake dichotomised relative to the sex-specific median for each beneficial (fruits, vegetables, legumes, cereals and fish; monounsaturated to saturated fat ratio; mild to moderate alcohol consumption) versus detrimental (meat and dairy products) component.19 24 Greater score indicate greater MeDi adherence.

Physical activity

The Baecke and Minnesota leisure time physical activity questionnaires were used to estimate the current level of physical activity.25 26 For each activity, information was collected on the frequency and duration of engagement over the past year. Frequency and duration data were multiplied with an activity-specific intensity code indicating calorie expenditure. We standardised and summed the activity-dependent scores from each test to obtain the overall annual intensity of physical activity per person, which was converted to metabolic equivalents.

Intellectual enrichment

Intellectual activity and years of education were used as measures of intellectual enrichment as described below. Intellectual activity throughout life was assessed using a validated 25-item interview in which participants were asked to report how often they engaged in common cognitively demanding activities that depend minimally on socioeconomic status, such as reading books or newspapers, writing letters or e-mails, going to the library and playing games at different age epochs.9 27 Previous studies described this instrument in detail and reported high internal consistency and positive associations of intellectual activity with educational and cognitive performance.27

MRI acquisition and processing

All subjects received three-dimensional volumetric T1-weighted MRI on a 3T scanner according to published protocols.17–21 MRIs were acquired and preprocessed as described.19 28 Volumetric segmentation, cortical surface reconstruction and parcellation were performed using the FreeSurfer V.5.3 software package.29 30 Cortical thickness measures were obtained for a subset of a priori-defined regions of interest (ROIs) known to show atrophic changes early in AD and in association with lifestyle7: entorhinal cortex (EC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), orbitofrontal cortex, inferior and middle temporal gyrus. FreeSurfer was also used to derive total intracranial volumes (TIV) to adjust for individual variability.

A latent factor of brain structure was generated by combining the ROI thickness measures using CFA to represent that construct. Instead of predicting each of these ROIs individually, the latent factor predicts their shared variance (ie, intercorrelation). EC was set as the marker variable. All ROIs were strongly correlated with each other (alpha >0.78, see online supplementary etable 1).

Supplementary file 1

bmjopen-2017-019362supp001.pdf (39.5KB, pdf)

Statistical analysis

We constructed structural equation models (SEMs) (MPlus V.731) to evaluate interdependent relationships among the four sets of variables: lifestyle (diet, physical activity and intellectual enrichment), vascular-related risk (overweight, hypertension, insulin resistance, elevated plasma cholesterol and homocysteine levels), MRI biomarkers (latent variable of brain structure) and cognitive measures (latent variable of memory, executive function and language).

The three cognitive domains were modelled as a function of lifestyle variables, vascular variables and MRI biomarkers (see online supplementary efigure 1). Lifestyle and vascular variables were specified as exogenous variables. Brain structure formed an intermediate layer of endogenous variables, and the cognitive domains were the final downstream endogenous variables. As such, exogenous variables (lifestyle and vascular risk measures) predicted brain variables, and brain variables in turn predicted cognition. We regressed brain structure on the lifestyle and vascular variables. We then regressed cognitive domains on all other variables in the model. Additionally, ran a separate model in which we replace the latent variable of brain structure with the two limbic regions (ie, EC and PCC) based on evidence of their earlier predictive capacity.

Supplementary file 2

bmjopen-2017-019362supp002.pdf (223.7KB, pdf)

Since variables in the two exogenous domains, lifestyle and vascular risk, were only marginally related to each other (see Results), the model was further broken down into the following three submodels: (1) we first modelled the relationships between lifestyle, vascular risk and brain, excluding cognition from the model; (2) we modelled the relationships between lifestyle, brain and cognition, excluding vascular risk and (3) we modelled the relationships between vascular risk, brain and cognition, excluding lifestyle from the model.

All models were adjusted for the intercorrelations between lifestyle variables, vascular-risk variables, brain structure and cognitive measures as appropriate. Age, gender and APOE status were examined as covariates. Brain measures were evaluated with and without adjustment for TIV.

For each analysis, we first fit the full models, estimating all paths and then assessed the reduced models suggested by the primary analysis (eg, with non-significant paths removed). We used χ2 statistic, comparative fit index (CFI) and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) goodness of fit tests to indicate model fits for the full models. Each of these measures incorporates unique criteria to assess fit so a summary of all three measures provides a more comprehensive estimate. After the path coefficients were derived, the paths were thresholded to achieve a second, more parsimonious model, by eliminating paths with P values >0.05. Path elimination was monitored via the χ2, CFI and RMSEA. Good model fit can be reflected by a χ2 to df ratio <2.0,32 a CFI>0.8533 and an RMSEA<0.06.34 The CFI and RMSEA are among the measures least affected by sample size and perform very well at all sample sizes.32 As such, CFI and RMSEA were our primary reporting criteria for the reduced models.

Some of the reduced models were saturated (eg, these models accounted for all possible relationships that could exist among variables in our dataset), making overall model fit statistics not applicable. In this case, we focused on specific paths and path significance rather than omnibus measures model fits. The standardised beta coefficients (βs) can be interpreted as partial correlations. Given that all of the paths are standardised, one can judge meaningfulness by their raw and comparative path weights. All analyses used maximum likelihood estimation in the MPlus package.31

Results

Participants

A total of 116 participants were included in the analysis (table 1). Participants were on average 50 years old (range 25–60), 62% women, with education ≥12 years. Of these, 38% had a family history of AD, and 40% had at least one copy of the APOE4 allele (APOE4 carriers).

Table 1.

Participants’ demographic and clinical characteristics

n 116
Age, years 50 (6), range 30–60
Sex, % female 62
Education, years 16 (2), range 12–22
Family history of LOAD, % positive 38
APOE ε4 carriers, % positive 40
Ethnicity, % white 68
Subjective complaints, % positive 44
Laboratory findings
 BMI (kg/cm2) 25 (4), range 18–37
 Hypertension, % positive 14
 QUICKI score (unitless) 0.32 (0.03)
 Plasma cholesterol/HDL ratio (unitless) 3.3 (0.8)
 Plasma homocysteine (µmol/L) 7.9 (6.2)
Lifestyle measures
 Mediterranean diet (unitless) 4.3 (1.9), range 1–8
 Physical activity (metabolic equivalent/hour) 9.8 (6.3), range 1–37
 Intellectual activity (unitless) 3.7 (0.7), range 1.8–4.9*

Values are mean (SD) unless otherwise specified.

*Data were not available for 20 out of 116 patients.

APOE, apolipoprotein E; BMI, body mass index; HDL, high-density lipoprotein; LOAD, late-onset Alzheimer’s disease; QUICKI, Quantitative Insulin Sensitivity Check Index.

Structural equation modelling

In the full model, age was associated with risk of hypertension (βs=0.18, P=0.049) and, although it tended to be negatively related to brain structure (βs=−0.10 to −0.19, ns), it was not significantly associated with the lifestyle or cognitive variables.

Men, as expected, showed higher vascular risk than women, as reflected in a higher frequency of hypertension, elevated cholesterol and homocysteine levels (P<0.04). Women showed better executive function performance than men (P<0.001). Sex was not significantly or marginally related to lifestyle variables or brain structure.

APOE status was not associated with lifestyle or cognitive variables but was associated with plasma cholesterol (P=0.033) and marginally associated with brain structure (P=0.07). These effects were driven by APOE4 carriers exhibiting higher cholesterol levels and reduced cortical thickness than non-carriers.

Given these relationships, all subsequent analyses were performed with and without adjusting for age, sex and APOE, as appropriate.

Among lifestyle variables, with and without controlling for the above confounds, participants with higher intellectual enrichment showed higher MeDi adherence (βs≥0.27, P≤0.028). Otherwise, lifestyle variables were not significantly related to one another.

Among vascular variables, BMI was negatively associated with insulin sensitivity (βs=−0.27, P<0.01) and positively associated with cholesterol levels and hypertension (βs=0.32 and βs=0.29, P<0.001). Cholesterol levels and hypertension were associated with each other (βs=0.24, P<0.05).

Vascular and lifestyle variables were not associated with each other, except for higher intellectual enrichment correlating with lower risk of hypertension (βs=−0.22, P<0.05), and insulin sensitivity was marginally, though not significantly, associated with diet (βs=0.12, P=0.18). The model was therefore broken down in the following three submodels:

Lifestyle, vascular risk and MRI-based biomarkers

Figure 1 represents the path diagrams from model 1, which exhibited adequate fit, χ2(41)≥95.37, RMSEA<0.05, CFI≥0.85, P<0.001. Controlling for age, diet and insulin sensitivity were the only factors positively associated with brain structure (βs≥0.26 and βs≥0.58, respectively, P≤0.008, table 2). The reduced model fit the data well (RMSEA <0.01, CFI≥0.90) reflecting the fact that diet and insulin sensitivity both significantly and independently predicted brain structure (diet: βs=0.20, P=0.017 and insulin sensitivity: βs=0.57, P<0.001), consistent with the pattern of significant and non-significant paths obtained in full model 1. Including sex and APOE status as covariates did not significantly influence these relationships (table 2).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Assessing the impact of lifestyle and vascular risk factors on brain ageing and cognition. Results of the following SEMs. (A) Relationships between lifestyle, vascular risk and brain, excluding cognition from the model. (B) Relationships between lifestyle, brain and cognition, excluding vascular risk from the model. (C) Relationships between vascular risk, brain and cognition, excluding lifestyle from the model. Red lines indicate the significant effects observed in our data. Purple lines indicate significant alternate paths. Blue lines indicate significant correlational effects. Grey lines indicate associations which were tested but found to be non-significant. Numbers beside the lines are standardised beta coefficients and corresponding P values: *P<0.01; **P<0.001. Not shown here, all brain ROI variables were correlated with one another (see online supplementary etable 1). Direct associations between lifestyle and vascular risk factors are found in online supplementary etable 2. Variables in squares are measured variables (eg, one variable represents this construct). Variables in circles are latent variables (multiple variables were averaged using a CFA to represent that construct). Straight lines indicate causal relationships, curved lines indicate correlational relationships. Age was entered as a covariate. Brain biomarkers were examined with and without adjusting for total intracranial volume. CFA, confirmatory factor analysis; ROI, region of interest; SEMs, structural equation models.

Table 2.

Results of structural equation modelling for model 1: lifestyle, vascular risk and brain

βs SE P value
Associations between lifestyle and brain MRI biomarkers
 Diet → Brain 0.260 0.099 0.009
Diet → Brain 0.204 0.086 0.018
 Physical activity → Brain −0.130 0.108
Physical activity → Brain −0.058 0.047
 Intellectual enrichment → Brain −0.192 0.098
Intellectual enrichment → Brain −0.190 0.102
Associations between vascular risk and brain MRI biomarkers
 Overweight → Brain −0.067 0.101
Overweight → Brain −0.116 0.173
 Insulin sensitivity → Brain 0.559 0.081 <0.001
Insulin sensitivity → Brain 0.029 0.006 <0.001
 Plasma cholesterol/HDL → Brain −0.038 0.101
Plasma cholesterol/HDL → Brain −0.010 0.037
 Plasma homocysteine → Brain 0.177 0.315
Plasma homocysteine → Brain 0.175 0.372
 Hypertension → Brain 0.076 0.106
Hypertension → Brain 0.032 0.042

The table consists of standardised betas (βs), their SEs and P values of the estimates from the full model with age entered as a covariate. The βs can be interpreted as partial correlations. Only significant P values (two tailed) are reported. Paths in italics are adjusted for sex and APOE status as covariates.

APOE, apolipoprotein E; HDL, high-density lipoprotein.

Restricting analysis to limbic structures fit the data well (RMSEA <0.05, CFI=1.0), confirming that insulin sensitivity was positively associated with both EC and PCC (βs=0.42 and βs=0.38, P<0.001), and diet was associated with PCC (βs=0.23, P=0.004) and marginally with EC (βs=0.13, P=0.12) (figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Assessing the impact of lifestyle and vascular risk factors on limbic brain structures and cognition. Results of the following SEMs. (A) Relationships between lifestyle, vascular risk and limbic brain, excluding cognition from the model. (B) Relationships between lifestyle, limbic brain and cognition, excluding vascular risk from the model. (C) Relationships between vascular risk, limbic brain and cognition, excluding lifestyle from the model. See legend to figure 1. Age was entered as a covariate. Brain measures were examined with and without adjusting for total intracranial volume. *P<0.01; **P<0.001. EC, entorhinal cortex; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; SEMs, structural equation models.

Lifestyle, MRI-based biomarkers and cognition

Model 2 exhibited adequate fit, χ2(64)≥108.51, RMSEA <0.05, CFI≥0.90, P<0.001. The reduced models were completely saturated (eg, all possible relationships that could exist among variables in the dataset were accounted for). As such, we focused on specific paths and path significance rather than omnibus measures of model fit.

As in model 1, with and without adjusting for age and TIV, diet was the only lifestyle factor positively associated with brain structure (βs≥0.27, P≤0.02, table 3 and figure 1). Diet and physical activity were not associated with cognitive function, whereas intellectual enrichment was positively associated with cognition (P<0.05, table 3). Specifically, intellectual activity was positively associated with memory and executive function (βs=0.28 and βs=0.39, respectively, P<0.02), and marginally with language (βs=0.25, P=0.08). Education positively predicted memory, executive function and language (βs=0.21–0.33, P≤0.038).

Table 3.

Results of structural equation modelling for model 2: lifestyle, brain and cognition

βs SE P value
Associations between lifestyle and brain MRI biomarkers
 Diet → Brain 0.253 0.098 0.010
Diet → Brain 0.253 0.099 0.010
 Physical activity → Brain −0.129 0.107
Physical activity → Brain −0.132 0.126
 Intellectual enrichment → Brain
  Intellectual activity → Brain −0.113 0.125
  Education → Brain −0.164 0.099
Intellectual enrichment → Brain
  Intellectual activity → Brain −0.112 0.124
  Education → Brain −0.168 0.099
Associations between brain and cognitive measures
 Brain → Memory 0.050 0.118
Brain → Memory 0.064 0.094
 Brain → Executive function −0.034 0.101
Brain → Executive function −0.041 0.100
 Brain → Language −0.314 0.094
Brain → Language −0.318 0.094
Associations between lifestyle and cognitive measures
 Diet → Memory 0.033 0.113
Diet → Memory 0.023 0.092
 Diet → Executive function 0.023 0.099
Diet → Executive function 0.042 0.099
 Diet → Language 0.068 0.107
Diet → Language 0.063 0.107
 Physical activity → Memory 0.070 0.113
Physical activity → Memory 0.052 0.094
 Physical activity → Executive function 0.002 0.099
Physical activity → Executive function 0.007 0.098
 Physical activity → Language 0.002 0.102
Physical activity → Language −0.009 0.102
 Intellectual enrichment → Memory 0.278 0.114 0.014
 Intellectual activity → Memory
 Education → Memory 0.213 0.102 0.038
Intellectual enrichment → Executive function
 Intellectual activity → Executive function 0.385 0.101 <0.001
 Education → Executive function 0.329 0.084 <0.001
Intellectual enrichment → Language
 Intellectual activity → Language 0.251 0.135
 Education → Language 0.325 0.084 <0.001
Intellectual enrichment → Memory 0.184 0.094 0.050
Intellectual enrichment → Executive function 0.327 0.084 0.001
Intellectual enrichment → Language 0.326 0.084 0.001

The table consists of standardised betas (βs), their SEs, and P values of the estimates from the full model with age entered as a covariate. The βs can be interpreted as partial correlations. Only significant P values (two tailed) are reported. Paths in italics are adjusted for sex and APOE status as covariates.

Brain structure was not significantly associated with cognition. However, when analyses were restricted to limbic structures, diet showed positive associations with PCC (βs=0.27, P=0.006) and EC (βs=0.17, P=0.08), which in turn positively, though marginally, predicted memory (βs=0.19, P=0.078) (figure 2).

Including sex and APOE status as covariates did not influence the other relationships of interest in any of the models (table 3).

Vascular risk, MRI-based biomarkers and cognition

Model three exhibited adequate fit, χ2(78)≥151.54, RMSEA <0.05, CFI≥0.89, P<0.001. The reduced models were completely saturated and overall model fit statistics were not applicable. Below, we focus on path significance rather than omnibus measures of model fit.

As in model 1, insulin sensitivity was the only vascular factor associated with brain structure (βs=0.58, P<0.01, table 4 and figure 1). Brain structure was not significantly associated with cognition. However, when analysis was restricted to limbic structures, insulin sensitivity showed positive associations with EC (βs=0.30, P<0.001) and EC in turn positively predicted memory (βs=0.39, P<0.001) (figure 2). Additionally, BMI was negatively associated with memory and executive function (βs=−0.22 and βs=−0.27, P≤0.021, figure 2).

Table 4.

Results of structural equation modelling for model 3: vascular risk, brain and cognition

βs SE P value
Associations between vascular risk and brain MRI
biomarkers
 Overweight → Brain −0.067 0.101
Overweight → Brain −0.116 0.173
 Insulin sensitivity → Brain 0.559 0.081 <0.001
Insulin sensitivity → Brain 0.029 0.006 <0.001
 Plasma cholesterol/HDL → Brain −0.038 0.101
Plasma cholesterol/HDL → Brain −0.010 0.037
 Plasma homocysteine → Brain 0.177 0.315
Plasma homocysteine → Brain 0.175 0.372
 Hypertension → Brain 0.076 0.106
Hypertension → Brain 0.032 0.042
Associations between brain and cognitive measures
 Brain → Memory 0.032 0.125
Brain → Memory 0.042 0.123
 Brain → Executive function −0.032 0.101
Brain → Executive function −0.037 0.101
 Brain → Language −0.325 0.095
Brain → Language −0.327 0.095
Associations between vascular risk and cognitive measures
 Overweight → Memory −0.195 0.119
Overweight → Memory −0.191 0.119
 Overweight → Executive function −0.230 0.091 0.012
Overweight → Executive function −0.229 0.091 0.012
 Overweight → Language −0.006 0.095
Overweight → Language −0.008 0.095
 Insulin sensitivity → Memory 0.082 0.116
Insulin sensitivity → Memory 0.041 0.124
 Insulin sensitivity → Executive function −0.072 0.097
Insulin sensitivity → Executive function −0.064 0.096
 Insulin sensitivity → Language −0.480 0.075 <0.001
Insulin sensitivity → Language −0.490 0.074 <0.001
 Plasma homocysteine → Memory −0.298 0.164
Plasma homocysteine → Memory −0.183 0.196
 Plasma homocysteine → Executive function −0.190 0.140
Plasma homocysteine → Executive function −0.188 0.122
 Plasma homocysteine → Language −0.218 0.338
Plasma homocysteine → Language −0.358 0.294
 Cholesterol/HDL → Memory −0.051 0.115
Cholesterol/HDL → Memory −0.042 0.112
 Cholesterol/HDL → Executive function −0.138 0.097
Cholesterol/HDL → Executive function −0.030 0.101
 Cholesterol/HDL → Language −0.057 0.096
Cholesterol/HDL → Language −0.052 0.095
 Hypertension → Memory −0.084 0.129
Hypertension → Memory −0.084 0.130
 Hypertension → Executive function −0.092 0.099
Hypertension → Executive function −0.073 0.098
 Hypertension → Language −0.176 0.095
Hypertension → Language −0.179 0.094

The table consists of standardised betas (βs), their SEs, and P values of the estimates from the full model with age entered as a covariate. The βs can be interpreted as partial correlations. Only significant P values (two tailed) are reported. Paths in italics are adjusted for sex and APOE status as covariates.

HDL, high-density lipoprotein.

Including sex and APOE status as covariates did not significantly influence the other relationships of interest (table 4).

Discussion

The major conclusions of this study were the following: (1) among lifestyle and vascular risk factors, diet and insulin sensitivity explained variability in brain cortical thickness in cognitively healthy, middle-aged adults; (2) MRI measures of limbic structures in turn explained variability in memory performance; (3) intellectual enrichment and increased BMI explained variability in cognitive performance through different pathways.

Our results indicate that diet and insulin sensitivity may be among the earliest modifiable risk factors to influence the expression of AD biomarkers, suggesting these modifiable risk factors may alter risk of AD pathophysiology during middle age. These associations were independent of age, sex and APOE genotype.

Findings of associations between lower MeDi adherence and increased brain atrophy are consistent with the literature in the elderly.19 24 35 36 Likewise, we and others have described strong associations between insulin resistance and limbic cortex atrophy in both adolescents and non-demented elderly.20 28 37 Our data extend prior observations to a population of middle-aged healthy adults and offers a comprehensive view of how the interplay of lifestyle and vascular factors influences possible AD risk. Indeed, besides being linked with lower risk of dementia, the MeDi was shown to support healthier insulin regulation and cardiovascular health.38 Given the known increased AD risk associated with prediabetes and type 2 diabetes,39 our findings fit with increased cardiovascular risk being a driver of increased brain ageing and AD.

The statistical approach we employed allowed us to simultaneously examine several lifestyle and vascular risk factors and discern their independent as well as intercorrelated effects on brain atrophy and cognition. Other studies that used SEM reported minimal or null associations between physical and intellectual activities and AD biomarkers in non-demented elderly, although biomarkers independently predicted cognitive function.12 40 These studies, however, did not take into account diet or vascular risk. Our study in a younger cohort (mean age 50 vs 78–82 years12 40) indicates that physical and intellectual activity do not impact brain ageing as much as diet does during the normal stages of cognition. When vascular risk factors were accounted for in the model, insulin sensitivity influences cortical thickness and its effects on limbic regions had significant effects on memory performance. These effects were present accounting for the impact of exercise, intellectual activity and additional vascular risk factors.

In our cohort, insulin resistance was strongly associated with increased BMI (ie, overweight). Although BMI was not directly associated with MRI measures, it negatively impacted memory and executive function through other pathways. It has long been known that midlife obesity affects cognitive performance,41 possibly by promoting cardiovascular disease (CVD) and Aβ deposition.42

Additionally, intellectual enrichment was associated with better cognitive performance, suggesting a protective effect on AD risk. It is possible that continued intellectual stimulation may lower the risk or delay the onset of AD by enhancing cognitive reserve, as suggested by studies in the elderly.9 11–14 40

Several issues require further consideration. First, our results are cross-sectional and do not allow for determination of causality or temporal relationships between lifestyle, vascular risk, brain biomarkers and cognitive status. Studies with larger samples and longitudinal follow-ups are needed to examine lifestyle and vascular risk factors as possible AD risk modifiers. From a statistical perspective, even though our model fits were adequate and the observed relationships were supported by the data, the associations were not so strong to yield an ideal model fit. To better evaluate the impact of modifiable risk factors on brain ageing, we performed additional linear regression analyses to estimate R-squared values for the predictors identified in the reduced SEM models. R-squared is the ‘per cent of variance explained’ by the model (eg, the fraction by which the variance of the errors is less than the variance of the dependent variable). As with SEM analysis, diet and insulin sensitivity were both significant predictors of brain structure, yielding a combined R-squared=0.28 (age adjusted), P<0.001. In other words, the combination of diet and insulin sensitivity explained 28% of the variance in brain MRI measures. Additionally, intellectual enrichment and BMI explained 11% and 8% of the variance in cognitive measures, respectively (P≤0.022). As such, these modifiable risk factors were fairly good predictors after adjusting for all other variables in the model. The associations were nonetheless modest, which is not unexpected in a relatively young, healthy population. That said, caution should be exerted in interpreting these data. For instance, some of the null associations may depend on sample size limitations. However, null effects observed in our cohort are consistent with negative findings from large-scale studies in the elderly.12 40 Therefore, we offer that the strongest arguments of the study are the significant findings which manifest themselves despite these limitations. Overall, our data suggest that lifestyle and vascular risk have an impact on brain ageing during midlife, thus supporting further investigation of modifiable risk factors for healthy brain ageing and AD risk reduction.

As with other studies in asymptomatic at-risk individuals,18 43 44 imaging biomarkers were only modestly associated with cognitive measures, most likely because our patients were all cognitively normal and younger than 60. Previous studies have demonstrated that associations between brain biomarkers and cognition are evident in clinical populations, such as those with clear brain pathology but not among normal populations.45 Nonetheless, associations between limbic MRI measures and memory were significant, indicating that modifiable risk factors may impact cognitive health before old age.

We cannot exclude that our screening criteria may have biased effect estimates.46 For instance, participation was limited to healthy, middle-aged individuals without severe cardiac and CVD. Our goal was to identify possible vascular, metabolic and lifestyle correlates of brain health, prior to severe disease and at a young enough age for potential interventions to be impactful. However, this makes our population restricted in the variability of cardiometabolic disease. As such, our results are only relevant to middle-aged, cognitively normal, healthy men and women without active CVD, stroke or diabetes. Nonetheless, our results are in keeping with prospective, community-based studies of non-diabetic populations showing that insulin resistance and increased blood sugar levels, even at levels considered normal in standard glucose tests, increase AD risk.47

Another limitation of the study pertains to the characterisation of lifestyle habits. We used self-report questionnaires of diet and lifestyle, which are vulnerable to error and may not have captured relevant dimensions of lifestyle activities that influence cognitive functioning. Additionally, given the cross-sectional nature of our study and synchronous timing of lifestyle and MRI assessments, we cannot exclude the possibility that dietary adherence or physical activity levels were short-term choices in this cohort, which are less likely to impact brain biomarkers than long-term choices. However, 90% of participants reported living the lifestyle assessed in the surveys for 5 years or more.

Although MRI measures of neurodegeneration are sensitive to early AD changes, they are believed to emerge after changes in neuronal activity and downstream to Aβ accumulation.16 As such, MRI measures are not specific to AD and offer limited information on whether the observed associations were due to AD or to other causes of cortical thinning. Amyloid and tau biomarkers specific to AD are warranted to investigate the potential of lifestyle and vascular risk intervention for AD prevention. However, Aβ deposition is an age-dependent phenomenon, with 0% of cognitively normal individuals aged 45–49 years and less than 6% of those in the fifth decade of life testing positive for Aβ.48 Considering that all our participants were cognitively normal and between 30 and 60 years of age, very few (if any) would have had substantial amyloid burden, making this cohort an ideal population for testing of primary prevention strategies.

We caution that present results were found in small numbers of carefully screened patients who were evaluated under controlled clinical conditions. Longitudinal studies with larger samples are needed to replicate and assess the generalisability of these preliminary findings in community-based populations with higher variability in socioeconomic and medical status, as well as to incorporate other AD biomarkers. Should preventative studies prove successful, work will be needed to estimate the effects of increased longevity on dementia burden in such an increasingly older, although healthier, population.

In conclusion, our results suggest that a well-rounded lifestyle that incorporates a healthy diet (such as the MeDi), reduces vascular risk factors (especially insulin resistance and overweight) and promotes intellectual activity might be neuroprotective during ageing. More studies are needed to evaluate midlife lifestyle and vascular risk factor modification for AD prevention.

Supplementary Material

Reviewer comments
Author's manuscript

Footnotes

Contributors: LM, JS and RI: study concept and design. LM, CQ, PM, MJDL, AC and RSO: acquisition of data. LM, MW, JS, CQ, REA, DCM, CG and AC: analysis and interpretation. All authors: critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. AC: study supervision.

Funding: This study was supported by NIH/NIA grants AG035137, AG13616, 2P01AG026572 and NIH/NIDDK DK083537; funding from the Department of Neurology at Weill Cornell Medical College and philanthropic support of the Alzheimer’s Prevention Clinic, Weill Cornell Memory Disorders Program.

Competing interests: None declared.

Patient consent: Obtained.

Ethics approval: This study was approved by NYU School of Medicine Institutional Review Board.

Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

Data sharing statement: All relevant data have been included in the paper. Technical appendix, statistical code and dataset will be made available on request.

References

  • 1. Barnes DE, Yaffe K. The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. Lancet Neurol 2011;10:819–28. 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70072-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Andrieu S, Coley N, Lovestone S, et al. Prevention of sporadic Alzheimer’s disease: lessons learned from clinical trials and future directions. Lancet Neurol 2015;14:926–44. 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00153-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Sperling RA, Karlawish J, Johnson KA. Preclinical Alzheimer disease: the challenges ahead. Nat Rev Neurol 2013;9:54–8. 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Norton S, Matthews FE, Barnes DE, et al. Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based data. Lancet Neurol 2014;13:788–94. 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70136-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Biessels GJ. Capitalising on modifiable risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol 2014;13:752–3. 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70154-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Mattson MP. Late-onset dementia: a mosaic of prototypical pathologies modifiable by diet and lifestyle. NPJ Aging Mech Dis 2015;1 10.1038/npjamd.2015.3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Mosconi L, McHugh PF. Let food be thy medicine: diet, nutrition, and biomarkers’ risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Nutr Rep 2015;4:126–35. 10.1007/s13668-014-0111-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Head D, Bugg JM, Goate AM, et al. Exercise engagement as a moderator of the effects of APOE genotype on amyloid deposition. Arch Neurol 2012;69:636–43. 10.1001/archneurol.2011.845 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Landau SM, Marks SM, Mormino EC, et al. Association of lifetime cognitive engagement and low β-amyloid deposition. Arch Neurol 2012;69:623–9. 10.1001/archneurol.2011.2748 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Liang KY, Mintun MA, Fagan AM, et al. Exercise and Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers in cognitively normal older adults. Ann Neurol 2010;68:311–8. 10.1002/ana.22096 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Merrill DA, Siddarth P, Raji CA, et al. Modifiable risk factors and brain positron emission tomography measures of amyloid and tau in nondemented adults with memory complaints. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2016;24:729–37. 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.05.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Vemuri P, Lesnick TG, Przybelski SA, et al. Effect of intellectual enrichment on AD biomarker trajectories: longitudinal imaging study. Neurology 2016;86:1128–35. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Vemuri P, Lesnick TG, Przybelski SA, et al. Association of lifetime intellectual enrichment with cognitive decline in the older population. JAMA Neurol 2014;71:1017–24. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.963 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Wirth M, Haase CM, Villeneuve S, et al. Neuroprotective pathways: lifestyle activity, brain pathology, and cognition in cognitively normal older adults. Neurobiol Aging 2014;35:1873–82. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.02.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Morrison JH, Hof PR. Life and death of neurons in the aging brain. Science 1997;278:412–9. 10.1126/science.278.5337.412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Jack CR, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, et al. Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol 2013;12:207–16. 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70291-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Mosconi L, Brys M, Switalski R, et al. Maternal family history of Alzheimer’s disease predisposes to reduced brain glucose metabolism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007;104:19067–72. 10.1073/pnas.0705036104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Mosconi L, Murray J, Davies M, et al. Nutrient intake and brain biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease in at-risk cognitively normal individuals: a cross-sectional neuroimaging pilot study. BMJ Open 2014;4:e004850 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-004850 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Mosconi L, Murray J, Tsui WH, et al. Mediterranean diet and magnetic resonance imaging-assessed brain atrophy in cognitively normal individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. J Prev Alzheimers Dis 2014;1:23–32. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Convit A, Wolf OT, Tarshish C, et al. Reduced glucose tolerance is associated with poor memory performance and hippocampal atrophy among normal elderly. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2003;100:2019–22. 10.1073/pnas.0336073100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Frosch OH, Yau PL, Osorio RS, et al. Insulin resistance among obese middle-aged is associated with decreased cerebrovascular reactivity. Neurology 2017;89:249–55. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. De Santi S, Pirraglia E, Barr W, et al. Robust and conventional neuropsychological norms: diagnosis and prediction of age-related cognitive decline. Neuropsychology 2008;22:469–84. 10.1037/0894-4105.22.4.469 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Katz A, Nambi SS, Mather K, et al. Quantitative insulin sensitivity check index: a simple, accurate method for assessing insulin sensitivity in humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2000;85:2402–10. 10.1210/jcem.85.7.6661 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Gu Y, Brickman AM, Stern Y, et al. Mediterranean diet and brain structure in a multiethnic elderly cohort. Neurology 2015;85:1744–51. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Taylor HL, Jacobs DR, Schucker B, et al. A questionnaire for the assessment of leisure time physical activities. J Chronic Dis 1978;31:741–55. 10.1016/0021-9681(78)90058-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Baecke JA, Burema J, Frijters JE. A short questionnaire for the measurement of habitual physical activity in epidemiological studies. Am J Clin Nutr 1982;36:936–42. 10.1093/ajcn/36.5.936 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Wilson R, Barnes L, Bennett D. Assessment of lifetime participation in cognitively stimulating activities. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2003;25:634–42. 10.1076/jcen.25.5.634.14572 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Yau PL, Castro MG, Tagani A, et al. Obesity and metabolic syndrome and functional and structural brain impairments in adolescence. Pediatrics 2012;130:e856–e864. 10.1542/peds.2012-0324 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2000;97:11050–5. 10.1073/pnas.200033797 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Holland D, Brewer JB, Hagler DJ, et al. Subregional neuroanatomical change as a biomarker for Alzheimer’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009;106:20954–9. 10.1073/pnas.0906053106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 6th edn Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Ullman JB. Structural equation modeling Tabachnik LSF BG, Using multivariate statistics. needham heights. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull 1990;107:238–46. 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Hu Li‐tze, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 1999;6:1–55. 10.1080/10705519909540118 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Luciano M, Corley J, Cox SR, et al. Mediterranean-type diet and brain structural change from 73 to 76 years in a Scottish cohort. Neurology 2017;88:449–55. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003559 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Mayeux R, et al. Mediterranean diet and mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 2009;66 10.1001/archneurol.2008.536 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Gold SM, Dziobek I, Sweat V, et al. Hippocampal damage and memory impairments as possible early brain complications of type 2 diabetes. Diabetologia 2007;50:711–9. 10.1007/s00125-007-0602-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Esposito K, Maiorino MI, Bellastella G, et al. A journey into a mediterranean diet and type 2 diabetes: a systematic review with meta-analyses. BMJ Open 2015;5:e008222 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-008222 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Whitmer RA, Sidney S, Selby J, et al. Midlife cardiovascular risk factors and risk of dementia in late life. Neurology 2005;64:277–81. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000149519.47454.F2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Vemuri P, Lesnick TG, Przybelski SA, et al. Effect of lifestyle activities on Alzheimer disease biomarkers and cognition. Ann Neurol 2012;72:730–8. 10.1002/ana.23665 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Baumgart M, Snyder HM, Carrillo MC, et al. Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population-based perspective. Alzheimers Dement 2015;11:718–26. 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Gottesman RF, Schneider AL, Zhou Y, et al. Association between midlife vascular risk factors and estimated brain amyloid deposition. JAMA 2017;317:1443–50. 10.1001/jama.2017.3090 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Reiman EM, Chen K, Liu X, et al. Fibrillar amyloid-beta burden in cognitively normal people at 3 levels of genetic risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009;106:6820–5. 10.1073/pnas.0900345106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Vlassenko AG, Mintun MA, Xiong C, et al. Amyloid-beta plaque growth in cognitively normal adults: longitudinal [11C]Pittsburgh compound B data. Ann Neurol 2011;70:857–61. 10.1002/ana.22608 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Van Petten C. Relationship between hippocampal volume and memory ability in healthy individuals across the lifespan: review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 2004;42:1394–413. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Weuve J, Proust-Lima C, Power MC, et al. Guidelines for reporting methodological challenges and evaluating potential bias in dementia research. Alzheimers Dement 2015;11:1098–109. 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.06.1885 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Crane PK, Walker R, Hubbard RA, et al. Glucose levels and risk of dementia. N Engl J Med 2013;369:540–8. 10.1056/NEJMoa1215740 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Morris JC, Roe CM, Xiong C, et al. APOE predicts amyloid-beta but not tau Alzheimer pathology in cognitively normal aging. Ann Neurol 2010;67:122–31. 10.1002/ana.21843 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary file 1

bmjopen-2017-019362supp001.pdf (39.5KB, pdf)

Supplementary file 2

bmjopen-2017-019362supp002.pdf (223.7KB, pdf)

Reviewer comments
Author's manuscript

Articles from BMJ Open are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES