Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Heart Rhythm. 2017 Sep 22;15(2):166–172. doi: 10.1016/j.hrthm.2017.09.036

Atrial Fibrillation and Cognitive Decline in the Framingham Heart Study

Arvind Nishtala 1,*, Ryan J Piers 2,3,4,*, Jayandra J Himali 3,4,5, Alexa S Beiser 3,4,5, Kendra L Davis-Plourde 4,5, Jane S Saczynski 6, David D McManus 6, Emelia J Benjamin 4,7,9, Rhoda Au 3,4,8,9
PMCID: PMC5881912  NIHMSID: NIHMS930486  PMID: 28943482

Abstract

BACKGROUND

There is a paucity of longitudinal research investigating the relations between AF and domain-specific cognitive performance.

OBJECTIVE

Our study investigated the association between AF and cognitive performance cross-sectionally and longitudinally.

METHODS

Eligible participants were dementia- and stroke-free at the time of baseline neuropsychological (NP) assessment, and underwent at least one additional NP assessment with a one year or greater inter-test interval. AF status was examined as a two-level variable (prevalent AF, no AF) in cross-sectional analyses, and then separately as a three-level variable (prevalent AF, interim AF, no AF) in longitudinal analyses. We examined the association between AF status and cognitive performance with linear regression. We first adjusted models for age and sex, and then for vascular risk factors and APOE4.

RESULTS

We studied 2682 participants of the Framingham Heart Study Original and Offspring cohorts. At the baseline neuropsychological assessment, 112 (4.2%) participants had AF (mean age=72±9 years, 32% women). After adjustment for vascular risk factors and APOE4, prevalent AF was significantly associated with poorer attention; sex differences were also noted with men performing worse on tests of abstract reasoning and executive function, while women did better on a measure of executive functioning. Prevalent AF was significantly associated with longitudinal decline in executive functioning in the Original cohort and interim AF was significantly associated with longitudinal decline in executive function in the Offspring cohort.

CONCLUSION

After accounting for vascular risk factor burden and APOE4, AF was associated with a vascular profile of change in cognitive function.

Keywords: atrial fibrillation, neuropsychological assessment, cognition, cognitive decline, Framingham Heart Study

Introduction

Atrial fibrillation (AF) is a serious cardiovascular condition that is associated with significant morbidity and mortality. The prevalence of AF is 16% in those 85–89 years of age,1 and the incidence and prevalence of AF is expected to more than double in the next three decades.2 AF is associated with an increased risk of stroke, heart failure, and all-cause mortality.3

The association of AF with cognitive and functional decline46 has been documented, however, the evidence is much more robust for AF’s association with an increased risk for dementia.79 In case control studies compared with sinus rhythm, AF was associated with significantly poorer performance on neuropsychological testing.10, 11 In a recent meta-analysis of more than 30,000 patients, Kalantarian et al. found that AF was associated with a 40% increased risk of cognitive impairment in participants without a history of stroke.12 Verbal memory, attention, and executive function may be particularly vulnerable in patients with AF.5, 10

Most community-based studies that have reported an association between AF and lowered cognitive performance have relied on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), a brief composite measure of cognitive status.13, 14 A post-hoc analysis of two randomized controlled trials with more than 30,000 patients found that patients with AF were significantly more likely to decline 3 or more points on the MMSE over a nearly 5-year follow-up period.4 However, the use of the MMSE to assess cognitive function has limitations and may be too crude and insensitive to detect subtle changes in the various domains of cognitive function, particularly in those with higher education.

The Framingham Heart Study (FHS) extended the study of AF and cognition beyond the MMSE to domain-specific tests and previously reported that within its middle to young old aged cohort, AF was associated with significantly lower scores on tests of reasoning, attention and spatial abilities.15 Among men, AF was associated with poorer performance in tests of global cognition, immediate and delayed memory, attention and executive function.6 These results were consistent with those of case-control studies, but were similarly limited by cross-sectional design and restricted age ranges of the study population.

There is a dearth of longitudinal studies that have investigated the association between AF and change in cognitive function. Studies of such a design may help identify specific domains of cognition that are preferentially affected and, in doing so, illuminate the potential mechanisms that underlie the relationship. For our study, we examined the association between prevalent and interim AF and concurrent level of cognitive function as well as longitudinal decline in cognitive function in a community-based sample with a broad age range. We hypothesized that AF is associated with poorer baseline cognitive performance and longitudinal decline in cognitive function.

Methods

Study Sample

The FHS is a single-site, community-based, prospective cohort study that was founded in 1948 to investigate risk factors for cardiovascular disease. The Original study cohort consisted of 5209 participants aged 28 through 74 years residing in Framingham, MA, U.S.A. between 1948 and 1953. The Framingham Offspring Study cohort is a community-based sample of 5124 participants (or their spouses) who had at least one biological parent who was a member of the Original cohort who have been followed since 1971.16 The cohorts had clinic examinations approximately every two (Original cohort) or four-eight (Offspring cohort) years. The sample for the present study consists of members from both the Original and Offspring cohorts. To be considered for the study, participants had to be dementia-free at the time of baseline neuropsychological (NP) assessment between 1999 and 2001 (examination 26 for the Original cohort and examination 7 for the Offspring cohort), and had to undergo at least one additional NP assessment with at least one year inter-test interval. The mean follow-up time in years (median Q1,Q3) for Original cohort was 3 years (2,3) and for Offspring cohort 6 years (6,7). The protocol for diagnosing participants in the Framingham Heart Study with dementia and stroke have been previously described.17 Participants younger than 40 years were excluded from the analyses (n=16). All participants with a history of clinical stroke were further excluded.

The study was in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975. The Boston University Institutional Review Board approved the study protocol and all participants provided written informed consent.

Atrial Fibrillation Assessment

The presence of AF (or atrial flutter) is determined by asking participants about AF during examinations and biennial health history updates, which include routine questions about AF. If AF is reported, records are sought. Presence of AF among FHS participants is confirmed from multiple sources: 12-lead electrocardiograms obtained at each FHS exam, and from all cardiovascular disease-related hospitalizations and clinician visits. Cases of suspected new-onset AF undergo rigorous adjudication by two FHS cardiologists.

AF was examined as a two-level variable (prevalent AF, no AF) in cross-sectional analyses. Separately, it was analyzed as a three-level variable (prevalent AF, interim AF, no AF) in longitudinal analyses, with no AF as referent. Prevalent AF was defined as AF at the baseline examination; participants who developed AF between the baseline examination and the follow-up NP assessment were classified as having interim AF.

Neuropsychological Measures

The test battery is administered using standardized testing protocols and scoring procedures, details of which have been described previously.18 The battery is comprised of tests measuring performance across major cognitive domains (see Table 1). The tests included Visual Reproduction (Immediate and Delayed Recall), which tests visual memory; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Test Similarities, which tests abstract reasoning; Hooper Visual Organization, which tests visuospatial organization and executive function; and Trail Making Test – A and – (B-A), which test attention and executive function respectively. For the Trail Making Test – (B-A), a difference score was calculated. This derived score may reflect a purer measure of the executive abilities required to complete the Trail Making Test – B by subtracting out the simple sequencing and psychomotor demands common to both Trail Making Test – A and – B. The Hooper Visual Organization test, Trail Making Test – A, and Trail Making Test – (B-A) were natural log transformed to normalize their skewed distributions. To aid consistency of interpretation, these values were re-signed such that higher scores reflect superior task performance.

Table 1.

Description of neuropsychological tests.

Test Description Cognitive Domain
Visual Reproductions: Immediate & Delayed Recall Participants are shown a drawing for 10 seconds and asked to draw them from memory and then asked to draw them again after a time delay. Visual memory
Similarities Participants are asked how a list of paired items are alike. Abstract reasoning
Hooper Visual Organization Test Participants are asked to recognize pictures of objects that have been cut up and rearranged. Visuoperceptual organization
Trail Making Test – A As quickly as possible, participants are asked to draw a line connecting numbers that are randomly placed on a page in sequence. Attention
Trail Making Test – B As quickly as possible, participants are asked to draw a line alternating between numbers and letters that are randomly placed on a page in sequence. Executive function

Statistical Analysis

The cross-sectional association between AF and cognitive function at baseline was assessed using multivariable linear regression relating AF to each of the aforementioned tests of the NP test battery. The first model was adjusted for age, sex, education, time between baseline examination and NP testing, and cohort. The second model was additionally adjusted for systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, antihypertensive medications, smoking, myocardial infarction, heart failure, and apolipoprotein ε4 (APOE4) status. Secondary analyses were performed using interaction terms to assess effect modification by cohort and sex.

The longitudinal association between AF and cognitive function was assessed using multivariable linear regression relating AF to annualized change in each of the NP tests. The first model was adjusted for age, sex, education, time between baseline examination and NP testing, and cohort. The second model was additionally adjusted for systolic and diastolic blood pressure, antihypertensive medications, smoking, myocardial infarction, heart failure, and APOE4 status. Secondary analyses were performed using interaction terms to assess effect modification by sex and cohort.

Two-sided significance was set at p<0.10 for analyses assessing effect modification and at p<0.05 for all other analyses. All data were analyzed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC).

Results

Table 2 describes the baseline characteristics for participants in the NP study sample (N=2682) by AF status. At time of baseline neuropsychological assessment, 112 (4.2%) participants had AF (mean age=72±9 years, 32% women). Compared to those without AF at baseline, participants with prevalent AF were older, more likely to be men, had higher prevalence of cardiovascular risk factors and disease, but did not have significantly different Apoe4 ε4 levels.

Table 2.

Baseline characteristics of NP study sample by atrial fibrillation status and cohort (Framingham Heart Study Original & Offspring cohorts ages 40 years or more).

Characteristic Original p Offspring p


Atrial Fibrillation (N=29) No Atrial Fibrillation (N=215) Atrial Fibrillation (N=83) No Atrial Fibrillation (N=2355)
Age, years, mean±SD* 83±3 83±3 0.4991 68±7 61±9 <0.0001
Female, n (%)* 13 (45) 140 (65) 0.0339 23 (28) 1296 (55) <0.0001
Education, n (%) 0.6961 0.0856
 No HS degree 3 (10) 39 (18) 6 (7) 76 (3)
 HS degree only 13 (45) 93 (43) 29 (35) 659 (28)
 Some college 8 (28) 45 (21) 21 (25) 719 (31)
 College degree 5 (17) 38 (18) 27 (33) 901 (38)
Medical History, n (%)
 Myocardial infarction*,‡ 4 (14) 16 (7) 0.2719** 22 (27) 72 (3) <0.0001**
 Heart Failure*,‡ 4 (14) 6 (3) 0.0206** 13 (16) 7 (0.3) <0.0001**
Systolic Blood Pressure, mmHg mean±SD* 137±20 140±20 0.4207 131±21 126±18 0.0234
Diastolic Blood Pressure, mmHg, mean±SD* 66±10 69±12 0.1216 70±9 74±10 <0.0001
Treatment with Antihypertensives, n (%)* 19 (66) 116 (54) 0.2500 49 (59) 701 (30) <0.0001
Current smoking, n (%)* 1 (3) 10 (5) 1.000** 4 (5) 299 (13) 0.0324
Apoe4 ε4, n (%) 5 (17) 41 (19) 0.8046 14 (17) 521 (23) 0.2601
Time between neuropsych measures, years median (Q1,Q3) 2 (1,3) 3 (2,3) 0.1915 7 (6,7) 6 (6,7) 0.6119
Visual Reproductions: Immediate* 5±3 6±3 0.4832 7±4 9±3 <0.0001
Visual Reproduction: Delayed Recall* 4±3 4±3 0.6536 7±4 8±3 <0.0001
Similarities (abstract reasoning)* 13±5 14±4 0.1673 15±4 17±4 <0.0001
Hooper Visual Organization test, median, (Q1,Q3)* 21 [17,23] 21 [17,23] 0.5117 25 [22,26] 26 [24,27] 0.0004
Trail Making Test - A, median (Q1,Q3)* 1 (1,1) 1 (1,1) 0.1809 1 (0,1) 1 (0,1) <0.0001
Trail Making Test - (B-A), median (Q1,Q3)* 1 (1,3) 2 (1,2) 0.9545 1 (1,1) 1 (0,1) 0.0075
*

IQR=interquartile range; variable log transformed for testing

**

using Fisher’s exact test

Table 3A summarizes the multivariable linear regression results for the cross-sectional association between prevalent AF and neuropsychological test performance. In Model 1, prevalent AF was associated with poorer performance on the Visual Reproduction: Immediate Recall, Similarities, and Trail Making Test – A tests. After adjustment for several vascular risk factors and APOE4 (Model 2), prevalent AF remained associated with poorer performance on the Similarities (β±SE= −0.96±0.33, p=0.004) and Trail Making Test – A (β±SE= −0.07±0.03, p=0.03). In Model 2 analyses, a sex interaction was observed for Similarities (p=0.01), and Trail Making Test – A (p=0.007). There were no significant cohort interactions with Model 2 analyses.

Table 3A.

Association between prevalent atrial fibrillation and neuropsychological test performance.

Cognitive Measure Model 1* Model 2** Sex Interaction p Cohort Interaction p
β SE p β SE p
Visual Reproduction: Immediate Recall −0.67 0.28 0.02 −0.40 0.30 0.17 0.12 0.98
Visual Reproduction: Delayed Recall −0.51 0.30 0.09 −0.29 0.31 0.36 0.16 0.93
Similarities (abstract reasoning) −1.02 0.31 0.001 −0.96 0.33 0.004 0.01 0.48
Hooper Visual Organization test*** −0.04 0.05 0.41 −0.03 0.05 0.57 0.15 0.89
Trail Making Test – A*** −0.09 0.03 0.007 −0.07 0.03 0.03 0.42 0.79
Trail Making Test – (B-A)*** −0.02 0.02 0.35 −0.009 0.02 0.70 0.007 0.84
*

Adjusted for age, sex, education, time between baseline examination and NP testing, and cohort.

**

Additional adjustments for systolic BP, diastolic BP, antihypertensive medications, myocardial infarction and heart failure, smoking status, and ApoE ε4.

***

measures natural log transformed

Table 3B summarizes the association between prevalent AF and neuropsychological test performance stratified by sex for the tests that demonstrated a significant sex interaction. After adjustment for several vascular risk factors and APOE4, prevalent AF was associated with better test performance on Trail Making Test – (B-A) in women (β±SE= 0.08±0.04, p<0.04). However, prevalent AF was associated with poorer performance on the Similarities (β±SE= −1.60±0.42, p<0.001) and Trail Making Test – (B-A) (β±SE= −0.07±0.03, p=0.02) in men.

Table 3B.

Association between prevalent atrial fibrillation and neuropsychological test performance by sex.

Women* Men*

Cognitive Measure β SE p β SE p
Similarities (abstract reasoning) 0.22 0.54 0.69 −1.60 0.42 <0.001
Trail Making Test – (B-A)** 0.08 0.04 0.04 −0.07 0.03 0.02
*

Adjusted for age, education, time between baseline examination and NP testing, cohort, systolic BP, diastolic BP, antihypertensive medications, myocardial infarction and heart failure, smoking status, and ApoE ε4.

**

this was natural log transformed.

Table 4A describes the multivariable linear regression results for the longitudinal association between AF and cognitive function, comparing prevalent and interim AF to the referent category, no AF. In Model 1, prevalent AF was associated with greater decline in performance on Trail Making Test – (B-A). In Model 2, prevalent (β±SE= −0.31±0.06, p<0.001) and interim (β±SE= −0.10±0.04, p=0.03) AF was associated with greater decline in performance on Trail Making Test – (B-A).

Table 4A.

Association between prevalent and interim atrial fibrillation and annual change in neuropsychological test performance (Interim AF N=140: Original=31; Offspring=109).

Cognitive Measure Model 1* Model 2** Cohort Interaction p
β SE p β SE p
Visual Reproduction: Immediate Recall
 noAF Ref Ref 0.76
 prevAF 0.01 0.07 0.856 −0.01 0.08 0.897
 intAF −0.009 0.06 0.878 −0.03 0.06 0.573
Visual Reproduction: Delayed Recall
 noAF Ref Ref 0.86
 prevAF −0.07 0.08 0.371 −0.10 0.08 0.221
 intAF −0.04 0.06 0.454 −0.07 0.06 0.267
Similarities (abstract reasoning)
 noAF Ref Ref 0.098
 prevAF −0.06 0.09 0.503 −0.004 0.10 0.964
 intAF 0.05 0.07 0.467 0.02 0.07 0.737
Hooper Visual Organization test
 noAF Ref Ref 0.13
 prevAF −0.07 0.07 0.367 −0.02 0.08 0.802
 intAF −0.11 0.06 0.050 −0.09 0.06 0.129
Trail Making Test – A
 noAF Ref Ref 0.66
 prevAF −0.03 0.02 0.075 −0.01 0.02 0.548
 intAF 0.008 0.01 0.514 0.009 0.01 0.463
Trail Making Test – (B-A)
 noAF Ref Ref <0.001
 prevAF −0.25 0.06 <0.001 −0.31 0.06 <0.001
 intAF −0.08 0.04 0.071 −0.10 0.04 0.03
*

Adjusted for age, sex, education, time between baseline examination and NP testing, and cohort.

**

Additional adjustments for systolic BP, diastolic BP, antihypertensive medications, myocardial infarction and heart failure, smoking status, and ApoE ε4.

A significant cohort interaction was observed for Similarities and Trail Making Test – (B-A) in Model 2 analyses (Similarities: p=0.098; Trail Making Test – (B-A): p<0.001).

Table 4B describes the multivariable linear regression results for the longitudinal association between AF and cognitive function stratified by cohort for the tests that demonstrated a significant cohort interaction. There was no significant association between AF and annual change in NP performance on the Similarities test in either cohort. Prevalent AF was associated with significant decline in performance on Trail Making Test – (B-A) (β±SE= −1.24±0.33, p<0.001) in the Original cohort and interim AF was associated with significant decline in performance on Trail Making Test – (B-A) (β±SE= −0.09±0.03, p<0.001) in the Offspring cohort.

Table 4B.

Association between atrial fibrillation and annual change in neuropsychological test performance by cohort (The Framingham Heart Study Original and Offspring cohorts ages 40 years or more).

ORIGINAL OFFSPRING

Model 1* Model 1*
Cognitive Measure β SE p β SE p
Similarities (abstract reasoning)
 noAF Ref Ref
 prevAF −0.15 0.42 0.72 −0.03 0.08 0.72
 intAF 0.54 0.39 0.16 −0.06 0.06 0.29
Trail Making Test – (B-A)
 noAF Ref Ref
 prevAF −1.03 0.32 0.002 0.006 0.03 0.87
 intAF −0.04 0.27 0.88 −0.09 0.02 <0.001
ORIGINAL OFFSPRING

Model 2** Model 2**
Cognitive Measure β SE p β SE p
Similarities (abstract reasoning)
 noAF Ref Ref
 prevAF −0.21 0.42 0.62 0.03 0.08 0.68
 intAF 0.20 0.39 0.62 −0.04 0.06 0.53
Trail Making Test – (B-A)
 noAF Ref Ref
 prevAF −1.24 0.33 <0.001 −0.008 0.04 0.82
 intAF −0.16 0.28 0.58 −0.09 0.03 <0.001
*

Adjusted for age, sex, education, and time between baseline examination and NP testing.

**

Additional adjustments for systolic BP, diastolic BP, antihypertensive medications, myocardial infarction and heart failure, smoking status, and ApoE ε4.

Discussion

Atrial fibrillation status was predominantly associated with poorer performance on tests of executive function in the Framingham Original and Offspring cohorts. In the cross-sectional analyses, prevalent AF was significantly associated with poorer attention. When looking at performance by sex, prevalent AF in men was associated with poorer abstract reasoning and executive function skills whereas women demonstrated better executive function. Prevalent AF and interim AF were associated with greater decline in executive function but these results were cohort specific; prevalent AF was a significant predictor in the Original cohort, whereas interim AF was a significant predictor in the Offspring cohort. Attention, executive function, and abstract reasoning are cognitive domains associated with frontal lobe atrophy, stroke, and increased white matter hypertensities.1925 Taken together, and in the absence of other cognitive deficits, the impact on these domains reflects a vascular profile of change.

In our sample, participants with AF had a significantly greater burden of vascular risk factors, were older, and were more likely to be men. The stronger cross-sectional association of AF to cognitive deficits in men compared to women is consistent with a prior cross-sectional FHS study of AF in men,6 however, it should be noted that there was no analysis looking at women in that study. These results should be interpreted with caution due to the small sample size of female participants with AF, and could be due to chance or residual confounding. While it has not been described in individuals with AF, a higher cardiovascular risk was associated with a greater decline in reasoning in men but not women in the Whitehall II study.26 To our knowledge, there has been no previous literature that identifies this sex interaction on the association of atrial fibrillation with cognition.

There are several potential explanations for our results. AF may cause cerebral hypoperfusion or small cerebral emboli, which may impact certain vascular beds and subsequently lead to smaller brain volumes.27, 28 It should also be noted that AF and brain atrophy share similar predisposing factors, including hypertension and diabetes. Hypertension has been associated with smaller total brain and hippocampal volumes29 whereas diabetes has been linked to reduced total brain and grey matter volumes.30 Therefore, the insignificance of associations following adjustment for vascular risk may be explained by confounding due to these comorbid risk factors. A recent study within the FHS Offspring cohort found that AF was associated with smaller frontal lobe volumes.31 Past research has found a link between frontal lobe volume, specifically prefrontal cortex volume, and executive function task performance19 as well as complex cognitive behavior and decision making.20 This could propose an explanation for why participants with AF did worse on tests of abstract reasoning (Similarities) and executive function (Trail Making Test – (B-A)), all tests associated with the frontal lobes. We posit that the longitudinal association between AF and decline in executive function further corroborates cross-sectional findings that the association between AF and cognition may be mediated by decrease in frontal lobe volumes.

The strengths of our study are its prospective, community-based design, with multiple cognitive assessment measures at baseline and follow-up. Few studies have investigated the association of AF with domain-specific neuropsychological test performance. For this study, we examined the association between baseline AF and concurrent level of cognitive function and longitudinal decline in cognitive function across a community-based sample with a broader age range than what has been published previously.15 Additionally, our study included multiple cognitive assessment measures. We adjusted for several cardiovascular risk factors, which have been associated with cognitive decline in young, middle-aged, and elderly individuals.32 We also adjusted for APOE4, which independently has been linked to sporadic late onset Alzheimer’s disease,33 as well as smaller brain volumes, greater brain atrophy, and increased white matter hyperintensities.34

There are, however, several limitations. The study participants were predominantly of European ancestry and most had at least some college education, which may limit the generalizability of the study. We were not able to adjust for all major cardiovascular risk factors, including diabetes, body mass index, depression and anxiety, alcohol use, and homocysteine levels. The prevalence of AF in our sample was low (about 4.2%), which may have limited power to detect additional associations. Further, since we examined prevalent AF at the time of neuropsychological assessment, we are unable to account for the duration of AF exposure. We were also unable to report on other markers of chronicity of AF (such as left atrial size or reduced left atrial appendage velocities) because they were not measured at baseline. We were not able to characterize AF status based on type of AF, particularly as certain types of AF have been more strongly associated with global brain atrophy,35 and additionally, were not able to classify participants by anticoagulation status. We cannot exclude misclassification of AF status, as AF may be clinically unrecognized. Lastly, with an observational cohort-based study design, we cannot exclude residual confounding and we cannot infer causality from our results.

In conclusion, AF was significantly associated with poorer performance on tests of executive function, particularly in men. Furthermore, AF was associated with significant longitudinal decline in executive function when compared to those without AF. Future research is warranted to further investigate the sex disparity in cognitive impairment identified by our study in addition to research that utilizes neuroimaging and biomarkers of brain aging to characterize the mechanisms by which atrial fibrillation is associated with executive dysfunction.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Framingham Heart Study’s National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute contract (N01-HC-25195; HHSN268201500001I), by grants (R01-AG016495, R01-AG008122, R01-AG033040) from the National Institute on Aging, and by grant (R01-NS017950) from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, and 1R01HL128914; 2R01HL092577; 3R01HL092577-06S1. The authors thank the extraordinary participants and families of the Framingham Heart Study who made our work possible. We also acknowledge the great work of all the research assistants and study staff.

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest: There are no conflicts of interest to declare.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Piccini JP, Hammill BG, Sinner MF, Jensen PN, Hernandez AF, Heckbert SR, Benjamin EJ, Curtis LH. Incidence and prevalence of atrial fibrillation and associated mortality among Medicare beneficiaries, 1993–2007. Circulation: Cardiovascular Quality and Outcomes. 2012;5:85–93. doi: 10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.111.962688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Colilla S, Crow A, Petkun W, Singer DE, Simon T, Liu X. Estimates of current and future incidence and prevalence of atrial fibrillation in the U.S. adult population. American Journal of Cardiology. 2013;112:1142–7. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2013.05.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Piccini JP, Hammill BG, Sinner MF, Hernandez AF, Walkey AJ, Benjamin EJ, Curtis LH, Heckbert SR. Clinical course of atrial fibrillation in older adults: the importance of cardiovascular events beyond stroke. European Heart Journal. 2014;35:250–256. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/eht483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Marzona I, O’Donnell M, Teo K, Gao P, Anderson C, Bosch J, Yusuf S. Increased risk of cognitive and functional decline in patients with atrial fibrillation: results of the ONTARGET and TRANSCEND studies. Canadian Medical Association Journal. 2012;184:E329–E336. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.111173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Knecht S, Oelschläger C, Duning T, Lohmann H, Albers J, Stehling C, Heindel W, Breithardt G, Berger K, Ringelstein EB, Kirchhof P, Wersching H. Atrial fibrillation in stroke-free patients is associated with memory impairment and hippocampal atrophy. European Heart Journal. 2008;29:2125–2132. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehn341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Elias MF, Sullivan LM, Elias PK, Vasan RS, D’Agostino RB, Sr, Seshadri S, Au R, Wolf PA, Benjamin EJ. Atrial Fibrillation Is Associated With Lower Cognitive Performance in the Framingham Offspring Men. Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases. 2006;15:214–222. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2006.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.de Bruijn RF, Heeringa J, Wolters FJ, Franco OH, Stricker BH, Hofman A, Koudstaal PJ, Ikram MA. Association between atrial fibrillation and dementia in the general population. JAMA Neurology. 2015;72:1288–1294. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.2161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Santangeli P, Di Biase L, Bai R, Mohanty S, Pump A, Cereceda Brantes M, Horton R, Burkhardt JD, Lakkireddy D, Reddy YM, Casella M, Dello Russo A, Tondo C, Natale A. Atrial fibrillation and the risk of incident dementia: a meta-analysis. Heart Rhythm. 2012;9:1761–1768. doi: 10.1016/j.hrthm.2012.07.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bunch TJ, Weiss JP, Crandall BG, May HT, Bair TL, Osborn JS, Anderson JL, Muhlestein JB, Horne BD, Lappe DL, Day JD. Atrial fibrillation is independently associated with senile, vascular, and Alzheimer’s dementia. Heart Rhythm. 2010;7:433–437. doi: 10.1016/j.hrthm.2009.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Farina E, Magni E, Ambrosini F, Manfredini R, Binda A, Sina C, Mariani C. Neuropsychological deficits in asymptomatic atrial fibrillation. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica. 1997;96:310–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1997.tb00289.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.O’Connell JE, Gray CS, French JM, Robertson IH. Atrial fibrillation and cognitive function: case-control study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 1998;65:386–389. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.65.3.386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kalantarian S, Stern TA, Mansour M, Ruskin JN. Cognitive Impairment Associated With Atrial Fibrillation. Annals of Internal Medicine. 2013;158:338–346. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-158-5-201303050-00007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kilander L, Andrén B, Nyman H, Lind L, Boberg M, Lithell H. Atrial fibrillation is an independent determinant of low cognitive function: a cross-sectional study in elderly men. Stroke. 1998;29:1816–1820. doi: 10.1161/01.str.29.9.1816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ott A, Breteler MM, de Bruyne MC, van Harskamp F, Grobbee DE, Hofman A. Atrial fibrillation and dementia in a population-based study. The Rotterdam Study. Stroke. 1997;28:316–321. doi: 10.1161/01.str.28.2.316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Elias MF, Sullivan LM, D’Agostino RB, Elias PK, Beiser A, Au R, Seshadri S, DeCarli C, Wolf PA. Framingham stroke risk profile and lowered cognitive performance. Stroke. 2004;35:404–409. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000103141.82869.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Feinleib M, Kannel WB, Garrison RJ, McNamara PM, Castelli WP. The Framingham offspring study. Design and preliminary data Preventive Medicine. 1975;4:518–525. doi: 10.1016/0091-7435(75)90037-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Seshadri S, Beiser A, Au R, Wolf PA, Evans DA, Wilson RS, Petersen RC, Knopman DS, Rocca WA, Kawas CH, Corrada MM, Plassman BL, Langa KM, Chui HC. Operationalizing diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer’s disease and other age-related cognitive impairment-Part 2. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2011;7:35–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2010.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Au R, Seshadri S, Wolf PA, Elias M, Elias P, Sullivan L, Beiser A, D’Agostino RB. New norms for a new generation: cognitive performance in the framingham offspring cohort. Experimental Aging Research. 2004;30:333–58. doi: 10.1080/03610730490484380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Gunning-Dixon FM, Raz N. Neuroanatomical correlates of selected executive functions in middle- aged and older adults: A prospective MRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:1929–1941. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00129-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Yang Y, Raine A. Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research. 2009;174:81–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Zinn S, Bosworth HB, Hoenig HM, Swartzwelder HS. Executive function deficits in acute stroke. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2007;88:173–80. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2006.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Au R, Massaro JM, Wolf PA, Young ME, Beiser A, Seshadri S, D’Agostino RB, DeCarli C. Association of white matter hyperintensity volume with decreased cognitive functioning: the Framingham Heart Study. Archives of Neurology. 2006;63:246–50. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.2.246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hochstenbach J, Mulder T, van Limbeek J, Donders R, Schoonderwaldt H. Cognitive decline following stroke: A comprehensive study of cognitive decline following stroke. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1998;20:503–517. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.4.503.1471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Rasquin SM, Verhey FR, Lousberg R, Lodder J. Cognitive performance after first ever stroke related to progression of vascular brain damage: a 2 year follow up CT scan study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 2005;76:1075–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.055541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Prins ND, Scheltens P. White matter hyperintensities, cognitive impairment and dementia: an update. Nature Reviews Neurology. 2015;11:157–65. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2015.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kaffashian S, Dugravot A, Nabi H, Batty GD, Brunner E, Kivimäki M, Singh-Manoux A. Predictive utility of the Framingham general cardiovascular disease risk profile for cognitive function: evidence from the Whitehall II study. European Heart Journal. 2011;32:2326–32. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehr133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Muller M, van der Graaf Y, Visseren FL, Vlek AL, Mali WP, Geerlings MI SMART Study Group. Blood pressure, cerebral blood flow, and brain volumes. The SMART-MR study. Journal of Hypertension. 2010;28:1498–1505. doi: 10.1097/HJH.0b013e32833951ef. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Stirling J, Muramatsu K, Shirai T. Cerebral Embolism as a cause of stroke and transient ischemic attack. Echocardiography. 1996;13:513–518. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-8175.1996.tb00929.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wiseman RM, Saxby BK, Burton EJ, Barber R, Ford GA, O’Brien JT. Hippocampal atrophy, whole brain volume, and white matter lesions in older hypertensive subjects. Neurology. 2004;63:1892–1907. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000144280.59178.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Espeland MA, Bryan RN, Goveas JS, Robinson JG, Siddiqui MS, Liu S, Hogan PE, Casanova R, Coker LH, Yaffe K, Masaki K, Rossom R, Resnick SM WHIMS-MRI Study Group. Influence of type 2 diabetes on brain volumes and changes in brain volumes: results from the Women’s Health Initiative Magnetic Resonance Imaging studies. Diabetes Care. 2013;36:90–97. doi: 10.2337/dc12-0555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Piers RJ, Nishtala A, Preis SR, DeCarli C, Wolf PA, Benjamin EJ, Au R. Association between Atrial Fibrillation and Volumetric MRI Brain Measures: Framingham Offspring Study. Heart Rhythm. 2016;13:2020–2024. doi: 10.1016/j.hrthm.2016.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Joosten H, van Eersel ME, Gansevoort RT, Bilo HJ, Slaets JP, Izaks GJ. Cardiovascular risk profile and cognitive function in young, middle-aged, and elderly subjects. Stroke. 2013;44:1543–9. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.000496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Sadigh-Eteghad S, Talebi M, Farhoudi M. Association of apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 allele with sporadic late onset Alzheimer’s disease, a meta-analysis. Neurosciences. 2012;17:321–326. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.DeCarli C, Reed T, Miller BL, Wolf PA, Swan GE, Carmelli D. Impact of apolipoprotein E epsilon4 and vascular disease on brain morphology in men from the NHLBI twin study. Stroke. 1999;30:1548–1553. doi: 10.1161/01.str.30.8.1548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Stefansdottir H, Arnar DO, Aspelund T, Sigurdsson S, Jonsdottir MK, Hjaltason H, Launer LJ, Gudnason V. Atrial Fibrillation is Associated With Reduced Brain Volume and Cognitive Function Independent of Cerebral Infarcts. Stroke. 2013;44:1020–1025. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.12.679381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES