Abstract
The experience of traumatizing events and resulting posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptomology relates to a range of impulsive behaviors. While both PTSD and impulsivity are heterogeneous and multidimensional constructs, no research has used person-centered approaches to examine subgroups of individuals based on these response endorsements. Hence, our study examined PTSD-impulsivity typologies and their construct validity in two samples: university students (n = 412) and community participants recruited through Amazon’s MTurk (n = 346). Measures included the Stressful Life Events Screening Questionnaire (PTEs), PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PTSD severity), UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale (negative urgency, lack of premeditation, lack of perseverance, sensation seeking). Dimensions of Anger Reaction Scale (anger), and the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (depression). For both samples, results of latent profile analyses indicated a best-fitting 3-class solution: High, Moderate, and Low PTSD-Negative Urgency. Negative urgency was the most distinguishing impulsivity facet. Anger and depression severity significantly predicted membership in the more severe symptomatology classes. Thus, individuals can be meaningfully categorized into three subgroups based on PTSD and impulsivity item endorsements. We provide some preliminary evidence for a negative urgency subtype of PTSD characterized by greater depression and anger regulation difficulties; and underscore addressing emotional regulation skills for these subgroup members.
Keywords: PTSD, impulsivity, latent profile analysis, depression, anger
1. Introduction
The experience of potentially traumatizing events (PTEs) is associated with several health and behavioral problems including Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) and pathological impulsive behaviors including substance use (Breslau, 2009; 2010), suicidal attempts (Belik, Stein, Asmundson, & Sareen, 2009), sexual risk taking behavior (Cavanaugh, Hansen, & Sullivan, 2010), and aggressive acts (Orth & Wieland, 2006). PTSD and impulsivity are both heterogeneous and multidimensional constructs (Armour, Mullerova, & Elhai, 2016; Berg, Latzman, Bliwise, & Lilienfeld, 2015). While extensive evidence has linked PTSD severity to impulsivity (Jakšić, Brajković, Ivezić, Topić, & Jakovljević, 2012), no study to our knowledge has used person-centered approaches to examine subgroups of people based on their endorsement of PTSD and impulsivity items. Thus, our study examined PTSD-impulsivity typologies (latent subgroups of individuals based on endorsement patterns), and their construct validity in two distinct samples: university students’ and a community sample.
PTSD is a multidimensional constellation of four symptom clusters: intrusions, avoidance of internal and external trauma reminders, negative alternations in cognitions and mood (NACM), and alternations in arousal and reactivity (AAR) following the experience of a PTE (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). This is not surprising given the heterogeneous nature of PTSD (Galatzer-Levy & Bryant, 2013), and its high comorbidity with psychopathology (Kessler, Sonnega, Bromet, Hughes, & Nelson, 1995). Examples of PTSD subtypes include subgroups of individuals characterized by predominant dissociative symptoms (dissociative subtype; Műllerová, Hansen, Contractor, Elhai, & Armour, 2016; Wolf, Miller, Reardon, et al., 2012), and predominant depression symptoms (depression subtype; Armour, et al., 2015; Contractor, Roley-Roberts, Langdon, & Armour, 2017). Such subgroupings have construct validity and are distinct in terms of psychopathology covariates (Contractor, et al., 2017; M.W. Miller & Resick, 2007).
We were most interested in the interaction of the personality trait of impulsivity and PTSD symptoms in defining PTSD subgroupings (possibly an impulsive subtype of PTSD). We consider impulsivity as multidimensional, most widely assessed by the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale (Whiteside & Lynam, 2001). The assessed four facets of impulsivity include lack of premeditation (tendency to act without careful thought), negative urgency (tendency to engage in impulsive behaviors in the context of intense negative emotions), sensation seeking (tendency to seek excitement), and lack of perseverance (difficulty completing tasks and a tendency for boredom). The UPPS conceptualization of impulsivity offers a pragmatic lens to understanding impulsivity as it relates to different symptoms (e.g., of PTSD) in forming heterogeneous subgroups. From a UPPS perspective, while the overt behavior of impulsivity may appear the same, the underlying mechanisms for this behavior may be different (Whiteside & Lynam, 2001); possibly depending on PTSD symptoms. Our study, thus, examined how impulsivity and PTSD facets might group together among people who have experienced PTEs for the following reasons.
First, substantial evidence supports an externalizing subtype of PTSD characterized by high negative emotionality and low constraint (impulsivity), as distinct from the internalizing subtype of PTSD characterized by low positive emotionality and high negative emotionality (Carleton, Mulvogue, & Duranceau, 2015; M. W. Miller, 2003; Wolf, Miller, & Harrington, 2012). Externalizing class members are more likely to demonstrate aggression and substance use, while internalizing class members are more likely to experience depression, and anxiety (Castillo, et al., 2014; Forbes, Elhai, Miller, & Creamer, 2010; M. W. Miller, 2003). Thus, evidence indicates that impulsivity plays a role in defining PTSD subtypes, specifically the externalizing subtype of PTSD.
Second, there is a strong empirical and theoretical relation between PTSD severity and impulsivity (Contractor, Armour, Forbes, & Elhai, 2016; Jakšić, et al., 2012). According to a disinhibition perspective, PTSD severity may interfere with one’s ability to inhibit behaviors when perceiving rewarding situations (Casada & Roache, 2005). This perspective aligns with UPPS’s lack of premeditation facet, which strongly relates to PTSD’s NACM severity (Roley, Contractor, Weiss, Armour, & Elhai, 2017). From an emotional regulation perspective, engagement in impulsive behaviors may functionally help to reduce PTSD’s negative affect (Marshall-Berenz, Vujanovic, & MacPherson, 2011; Weiss, Tull, Sullivan, Dixon-Gordon, & Gratz, 2015). This perspective aligns with UPPS’s negative urgency facet, which strongly relates to PTSD severity (mainly NACM and AAR; Contractor, Armour, Forbes, et al., 2016; Roley, et al., 2017). Further, the compulsive re-exposure theory posits that people with PTSD severity engage in sensation-seeking activities (UPPS’s sensation seeking facet) to mimic the biological arousal experienced during a PTE (Joseph, Dalgleish, Thrasher, & Yule, 1997). Research indicates that sensation seeking predicts all PTSD clusters’ severity (mainly NACM and AAR; Contractor, Armour, Forbes, et al., 2016; Roley, et al., 2017). Lastly, a cognitive explanation (Ben-Zur & Zeidner, 2009) highlights attentional difficulties (attributed to intrusive thoughts), and a restriction in information processing capacity following trauma. Consequently, one may behave impulsively to redirect attention, or distract from intrusive thoughts (UPPS lack of perseverance; Billieux, 2012; Roberts, Pullig, & Manolis, 2015; Wu, Cheung, Ku, & Hung, 2013). Lack of perseverance predicts PTSD’s intrusion severity (Roley, et al., 2017).
Despite the aforementioned, there is little understanding of how the different PTSD and impulsivity facets interact in creating subgroups. Person-centered approaches of latent class (LCA) and latent profile (LPA) analyses can extend this line of research on PTSD subtypes by identifying latent subgroupings of people based on endorsed response patterns. LCA assesses categorical symptom indicators while LPA assesses continuous indicators; the obtained subgroups are compared qualitatively and quantitatively (Kline, 2011; McCutcheon, 1987). Using two different samples, our current study examined the best-fitting latent class solution in categorizing participants based on PTSD and impulsivity endorsements, and the construct validity of the optimal class-solution. Drawing from prior research on latent subgroups based on posttraumatic responses (PTSD and other co-occurring symptoms), we hypothesized finding an optimal three- or four-class solution (Armour, et al., 2015; Contractor, Armour, Shea, Mota, & Pietrzak, 2016; Contractor, et al., 2017).
We examined the covariates of gender and age in predicting latent class membership in both samples. Consistently, evidence indicates that females have greater PTSD severity (Tolin & Foa, 2006); however, there are mixed findings regarding the relation between age and PTSD severity (Green, et al., 1991; Norris, Kaniasty, Conrad, Inman, & Murphy, 2002). Regarding impulsivity’s multidimensional construct, Cross et al. (2011) concluded through a meta-analysis that gender differences were contingent on the type of measure used to assess impulsivity and the theory underlying the impulsivity construct. This meta-analysis reflected more sensation seeking in males, more negative urgency in females, and no gender differences in lack of perseverance and lack of premeditation facets. While some evidence indicates a decline in impulsivity with age (Steinberg, 2010; Steinberg, et al., 2008), sensation seeking has a curvilinear relation with age (strongest relation between ages 10 and 15; Steinberg, et al., 2008). Thus, we hypothesized that being female would increase the chance of being in a subgroup characterized by higher PTSD severity, but did not hypothesize how gender would relate to impulsivity facets. Further, we explored the relationship between age and latent class membership.
We further examined the covariates of anger and depression (each variable available in one sample) in establishing the construct validity of the optimal class solution. Evidence indicates PTSD’s co-occurrence with depression (Rytwinski, Scur, Feeny, & Youngstrom, 2013) and difficulties in regulating anger (Orth & Wieland, 2006). Further, impulsivity is highly related to depression (Swann, Steinberg, Lijffijt, & Moeller, 2008) and difficulties in regulating anger (Contractor, Armour, Wang, Forbes, & Elhai, 2015). Thus, we hypothesized that a subgroup characterized by higher impulsivity and PTSD severity would have greater depression and anger severity compared to other subgroups.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants and Procedure
2.1.1. University Sample
A university’s Institutional Review Board approved all procedures. The sample included 911 undergraduates recruited from 2011 through 2013. Self-report measures were administered on Psychdata.com following informed consent. Participants were provided course credit for study participation. Of 911 participants, we excluded participants not endorsing at least one PTE. Of the remaining 427 participants, we further excluded 15 participants missing more than 30% of items on the PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5; >/=6; Weathers, et al., 2013), the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale (UPPS; >/= 13 items; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001), or the Dimensions of Anger Reaction Scale–5 (DARS-5; > 2 items; Forbes, et al., 2004). In the final sample of 412 participants, 27 participants were missing between 1–2 PCL-5 items; 79 participants were missing between 1–4 UPPS items; and 12 participants were missing one DARS-5 item. The sample of 412 participants had a mean age of 20.06 years (SD = 4.42), with the majority being female (n = 278, 74.50%). Table 1 has detailed descriptive information.
Table 1.
Descriptive information on demographics.
University sample (n = 412) | MTurk Sample (n = 346) | |
---|---|---|
M (SD) | ||
Age | 20.06 (4.42) | 33.60 (9.52) |
Years of Schooling | 12.70 (1.30) | 15.31 (2.43) |
n (%)* | ||
Female | 278 (74.50%) | 199 (57.70%) |
Employment Status | ||
Part time | 198 (48.20%) | 59 (17.10%) |
Full time | 27 (6.60%) | 227 (65.80%) |
Unemployed | 20 (4.90%) | 44 (12.80%) |
Unemployed Student | 166 (40.40%) | 8 (2.30%) |
Retired | N/A | 7 (2%) |
Relationship Status | ||
Single | 331 (80.30%) | 123 (35.50%) |
Living with significant other | 58 (14.10%) | 51 (14.70%) |
Married | 18 (4.40%) | 149 (43.10%) |
Divorced, separated, or widowed | 5 (1.20%) | 23 (6.60%) |
Racial Status | ||
White | 297 (72.10%) | 288 (83.20%) |
Asian | 17 (4.10%) | 36 (10.40%) |
African American | 106 (25.70%) | 22 (6.40%) |
American Indian or Alaskan Native | 15 (3.60%) | 16 (4.60%) |
Native Hawaiian/other Pacific Islander | 3 (.70%) | 5 (1.40%) |
Ethnicity | ||
Hispanic or Latino | 19 (4.70%) | 39 (11.30%) |
Not Hispanic or Latino | 368 (90.40%) | 296 (86%) |
Income | ||
Less than $15,000 | 141 (34.90%) | 40 (11.60%) |
$15,000 – $24,999 | 44 (10.90%) | 47 (13.60%) |
$25,000 – $34,999 | 39 (9.70%) | 55 (15.90%) |
$35,000 – $49,999 | 34 (8.40%) | 53 (15.30%) |
$50,000 – $64,999 | 48 (11.90%) | 53 (15.30%) |
$65,000 – $79,999 | 29 (7.20%) | 34 (9.80%) |
$80,000 and higher | 69 (17.10%) | 64 (18.50%) |
Note.
All reported percentages are valid percentages to account for missing data.
2.1.2. MTurk sample
A university’s Institutional Review Board approved all procedures. The original sample included 499 participants recruited through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) platform (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). Participants 18 years and older were screened on four inclusionary criteria, including experiencing a PTE. Participants completed the survey hosted on Psychdata.com following informed consent, and were compensated 75 cents for study participation. Among the 499 respondents who completed the survey, 19 participants attempted the questionnaire twice/thrice and were excluded (n = 480). We further excluded participants who (1) did not meet one or more of the inclusionary criteria (n = 120); (2) were missing data on all measures (n = 11); and (3) were missing more than 30% item-level data (n = 3) on the PCL-5 (>/=6 items; Weathers, et al., 2013), the UPPS scale (>/=13 items; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001), or the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9; >/=3 items; Kroenke & Spitzer, 2002). In the final sample of 346 participants, 45 participants were missing between 1–3 PCL-5 items; 73 participants were missing between 1–4 UPPS items; and 19 participants were missing one PHQ-9 item. The average age in this sample was 33.60 years (SD = 9.52), and approximately half were female (n = 199, 57.70%). Table 1 has detailed descriptive information.
2.2. Assessments
2.2.1. Demographic information
Information on gender, age, years of schooling, employment status, ethnic and racial background, relationship status, and socio-economic status was obtained in both samples.
2.2.2. Potentially traumatic experiences
Information on exposure to 13 PTEs was obtained using the Stressful Life Events Screening Questionnaire (SLESQ; Goodman, Corcoran, Turner, Yuan, & Green, 1998) in both samples. The SLESQ uses dichotomous response options (yes/no). We added three additional items to address changes in DSM-5 criteria for a Criterion A PTE (Elhai, et al., 2012). The SLESQ has a good test-retest reliability, and concurrent and convergent validity (Goodman, et al., 1998). For participants who endorsed more than one PTE, we wished to ensure that they referenced their index PTE while endorsing questions on PTSD severity; hence, they specified their most distressing PTE.
2.2.3. PTSD symptoms
Severity of DSM-5 PTSD symptoms experienced in the past month was assessed using the PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5; Weathers, et al., 2013) in both samples. The PCL-5, a 20-item self-report measure, uses response options ranging from 0 (Not at all) to 4 (Extremely). Participants completed the PCL-5 in reference to their most distressing event endorsed on the SLESQ. The PCL-5 has excellent psychometric properties (Blevins, Weathers, Davis, Witte, & Domino, 2015; Bovin, et al., 2016; Wortmann, et al., 2016). The item-level responses were summed to create four subscale scores: intrusions, avoidance, NAMC, and AAR. The Cronbach’s α for the intrusions, avoidance, NACM, and AAR subscales were .86, .87, .88, and .87 respectively in the university sample, and .90, .89, .92, .87 respectively in the MTurk sample.
2.2.4. Impulsivity Facets
The UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale (UPPS; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001), a 45-item self-report measure, was used to assesses four impulsivity facets in both samples. The four facets were lack of premeditation (11 items), negative urgency (12 items), sensation seeking (12 items), and lack of perseverance (10 items). Response options range from 1 (Agree Strongly) to 4 (Disagree Strongly). The subscales have good internal consistency, and convergent and divergent validity (Smith, et al., 2007; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001; Whiteside, Lynam, Miller, & Reynolds, 2005). Cronbach’s α for the lack of premeditation, negative urgency, sensation seeking, and lack of perseverance subscales were .85, .88, .82, and .83 respectively in the university sample; and .88, .90, .90, and .87 respectively in the MTurk sample. For both samples, we used a past-month timeline of inquiry, for consistency with PTSD ratings.
2.2.5. Anger reactions
In the university sample, the Dimensions of Anger Reaction Scale – 5 (DARS-5; Forbes, et al., 2004), a 5-item self-report questionnaire, assessed one’s disposition towards anger in response to stressful situations (Forbes, et al., 2004). Responses are given on a 5-point Likert rating scale ranging from 1 (none of the time) to 4 (all of the time) referencing the past month. This measure has good convergent validity (Forbes, et al., 2004), and an internal consistency coefficient of .85 in the university sample.
2.2.6. Depression
In the MTurk sample, the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9; Kroenke & Spitzer, 2002), a 9-item self-report measure, assessed the severity of depression symptoms within the past two weeks. Response options range from 0 (“Not at all”) to 3 (“Nearly every day”). The PHQ-9 has excellent psychometric properties (Kroenke & Spitzer, 2002; Kroenke, Spitzer, & Williams, 2001). Internal consistency in the MTurk sample was .91.
2.3. Data Analyses
We conducted a latent profile analysis (LPA) with Mplus 7 using PCL-5 subscales (intrusions, avoidance, NACM, AAR), and UPPS subscales (lack of premeditation, negative urgency, sensation seeking, lack of perseverance) as indicators. We used Maximum Likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (ML) for the LPA, analyzing one through four-class models based on prior studies with PTSD symptoms (e.g., Armour, et al., 2015; Ayer, et al., 2011; Contractor, et al., 2017). The optimal class solution had the lowest Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) and sample-size adjusted BIC values (SSABIC), a significant Lo–Mendell–Rubin Adjusted Likelihood Ratio Test value (LMR), a significant Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test (BLRT) p value, good entropy values (quality of classification), parsimony, and interpretative meaning (DiStefano & Kamphaus, 2006; Nylund, Asparouhov, & Muthén, 2007; Nylund, Bellmore, Nishina, & Graham, 2007). A model with a 10-point lower BIC value has a 150:1 likelihood to be the better fitting model (Raftery, 1995). In comparing a K-class with a K-1 class model, a significant LRT test indicates the optimal model as the one with K-1 classes (Nylund, Asparouhov, et al., 2007). We estimated missing values with ML.
After identifying the best-fitting class solution, we tested differences across classes on the PTSD and impulsivity subscales with one-way ANOVA tests (effect sizes of partial eta square). Posterior class probabilities were imported into SPSS for these analyses. Next, we tested the effect of covariates (anger for the university sample; depression for the MTurk sample; gender and age for both samples) on latent class membership. We used the Mplus three-step approach (multinomial logistic regression) by estimating class membership in relation to auxiliary variables of interest (covariates) while accounting for misspecification bias (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2014; Vermunt, 2010). For the 3-step analysis, Mplus uses list-wise deletion to deal with missing data; thus, reducing our sample to 361 participants in the university sample, and 326 participants in the MTurk sample.
3. Results
3.1. University Sample
Table 2 indicates the LPA results. Based on recommended guidelines, a 3-class solution was found to be the optimal class solution (DiStefano & Kamphaus, 2006; Nylund, Asparouhov, et al., 2007). According to LMR value guidelines, the 3-class solution would be the best-fitting model. Despite a continuous decrease in BIC and SSABIC values, the difference between the 3- and 4-class solutions is minimal compared to the difference across the other class solutions. We additionally considered parsimony and interpretative meaning. Further, entropy values were quite similar for all the models tested.
Table 2.
Fit indices of the latent-class models.
Model | AIC | BIC | SSABIC | Entropy | LMR (p) | BLRT (p) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
University sample | ||||||
1 class | 19510.638 | 19574.975 | 19524.203 | – | – | – |
2 class | 18652.214 | 18752.739 | 18673.409 | .89 | 860.544 (p < .001) | p < .001 |
3 class | 18421.960 | 18558.675 | 18450.786 | .90 | 243.755 (p = .0007) | p < .001 |
4 class | 18325.815 | 18498.719 | 18362.271 | .88 | 112.077 (p = .35) | p < .001 |
MTurk sample | ||||||
1 class | 16756.938 | 16818.481 | 16767.724 | – | – | – |
2 class | 15972.645 | 16068.806 | 15989.499 | .90 | 787.330 (p < .001) | p < .001 |
3 class | 15738.681 | 15869.460 | 15761.602 | .89 | 247.265 (p = .0005) | p < .001 |
4 class | 15646.992 | 15812.389 | 15675.981 | .86 | 107.643 (p = .09) | p < .001 |
Note. AIC is Akaike Information Criterion, BIC is Bayesian Information Criterion, SSABIC is sample-size adjusted BIC, LMR is Lo–Mendell–Rubin Adjusted Likelihood Ratio Test value, BLRT is Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test.
See Figure 1 for a graphical depiction of the 3-class solution. The class descriptors reference the negative urgency facet rather than the impulsivity construct based on the predominant between-class comparative differences in negative urgency rather than most other impulsvity facets. Class 3 was characterized by relatively higher PTSD subscale severity (particularly NACM and AAR) and negative urgency scores (High PTSD-Negative Urgency). In comparison, Class 2 was characterized by lower PTSD subscale severity and negative urgency scores (Low PTSD-Negative Urgency). Class 1 was characterised by relatively moderate PTSD subscale severity and negative urgency scores (Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency). Table 3 indicates ANOVA test results for class differences in the PTSD and UPPS subscale severity. All classes differed significantly on all PTSD subscale severity. However, among the UPPS subscales, all classes differed significantly only on the negative urgency subscale score; and some classes differed significantly (excluding Class 1-Class 3 comparison) on the lack of perseverance subscale score. Where there were significant differences, Class 3 had the highest scores followed by Class 1, and then Class 2.
Figure 1. Latent profiles of participants based on responses to PTSD and impulsivity indicators (university sample).
Note. Class 1 is Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency, Class 2 is Low PTSD-Negative Urgency, Class 3 is High PTSD-Negative Urgency, NACM is PTSD’s negative alterations in mood and cognitions subscale; AAR is PTSD’ alterations in arousal and reactivity subscale.
Table 3.
Results of ANOVA tests comparing classes on PTSD and UPPS subscale scores.
Full Sample | Class 1 | Class 2 | Class 3 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variables | M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | F | Partial η2 |
University Sample | ||||||
Lack of Premeditation | 21.44 (5.28) | 21.89 (5.39) | 21.16 (5.10) | 21.27 (5.66) | .82 | .004 |
Negative Urgency | 30.29 (7.36) | 31.77 (6.92)>2**, Cohen’s d = .58; <3**, Cohen’s d = .64 | 27.82 (6.59)<3**, Cohen’s d = 1.23 | 36.26 (7.11) | 36.11** | .16 |
Sensation Seeking | 32.04 (6.88) | 31.78 (7.07) | 32.06 (6.51) | 32.76 (7.78) | .36 | .002 |
Lack of Perseverance | 19.66 (4.99) | 20.28 (4.97) >2*, Cohen’s d = .35 | 18.59 (4.80) <3**, Cohen’s d = .73 | 22.06 (4.71) | 11.87** | .06 |
Intrusions | 5.67 (4.62) | 7.66 (3.18)>2**, Cohen’s d = 1.83; <3**, Cohen’s d = 1.38 | 2.49 (2.41) <3**, Cohen’s d = 3.11 | 12.57 (3.90) | 302.22** | .60 |
Avoidance | 2.80 (2.42) | 4.21 (1.90) >2**, Cohen’s d = 2.02; <3**, Cohen’s d = .84 | 1.00 (1.21) <3**, Cohen’s d = 3.25 | 5.70 (1.65) | 290.58** | .59 |
NACM | 7.26 (6.44) | 10.02 (3.67) >2**, Cohen’s d = 2.43; <3**, Cohen’s d = 2.14 | 2.27 (2.63) <3**, Cohen’s d = 4.78 | 18.09 (3.87) | 596.30** | .75 |
AAR | 5.95 (5.73) | 8.42 (3.88) >2**, Cohen’s d = 2.16; <3**, Cohen’s d = 1.96 | 1.66 (2.11) <3**, Cohen’s d = 4.90 | 15.60 (5.73) | 509.85** | .72 |
MTurk sample | ||||||
Lack of Premeditation | 21.22 (5.73) | 20.25 (5.08) <3*, Cohen’s d = .36 | 21.15 (5.15) | 22.75 (7.59) | 3.42* | .02 |
Negative Urgency | 29.56 (7.68) | 25.95 (7.73) <2**, Cohen’s d = .53; <3**, Cohen’s d = 1.13 | 29.68 (6.21) <3**, Cohen’s d = .71 | 34.63 (7.57) | 31.19** | .16 |
Sensation Seeking | 28.63 (8.28) | 28.75 (7.70) | 28.39 (8.04) | 28.98 (9.83) | .13 | .001 |
Lack of Perseverance | 20.37 (5.66) | 18.24 (4.95) <2**, Cohen’s d = .55; <3**, Cohen’s d = .74 | 21.11 (5.47) | 22.35 (6.16) | 14.22** | .08 |
Intrusions | 8.84 (5.43) | 3.35 (2.77) <2**, Cohen’s d = 2.28; <3**, Cohen’s d = 4.44 | 10.09 (3.13) <3**, Cohen’s d = 1.88 | 15.64 (2.77) | 407.19** | .71 |
Avoidance | 3.80 (2.50) | 1.34 (1.58) <2**, Cohen’s d = 1.94; <3**, Cohen’s d = 3.42 | 4.52 (1.69) <3**, Cohen’s d = 1.22 | 6.41 (1.38) | 247.99** | .60 |
NACM | 10.89 (7.60) | 3.07 (2.89) <2**, Cohen’s d = 2.64; <3**, Cohen’s d = 5.35 | 12.50 (4.15) <3**, Cohen’s d = 2.18 | 21.20 (3.82) | 537.66** | .77 |
AAR | 9.27 (6.47) | 2.55 (2.59) <2**, Cohen’s d = 2.66; <3**, Cohen’s d = 4.78 | 10.81 (3.55) <3**, Cohen’s d = 1.93 | 17.77 (3.68) | 475.12** | .75 |
Note. University Sample: Class 1 is Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency, Class 2 is Low PTSD-Negative Urgency, Class 3 is High PTSD-Negative Urgency; MTurk sample: Class 1 is Low PTSD-Negative Urgency, Class 2 is Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency, and Class 3 is High PTSD-Negative urgency; NACM is PTSD’s negative alterations in mood and cognitions subscale; AAR is PTSD’ alterations in arousal and reactivity subscale;
p < .05;
p < .001.
Table 4 indicates results of the multinomial logistic regression analyses (n = 361). Anger was a significant predictor for all class comparisons. Anger significantly predicted the Low versus the Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency class memberships (B = −.15, p < .001, OR = .86), the High versus the Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency class memberships (B = .13, p = .001, OR = 1.14), and the High versus the Low PTSD-Negative Urgency class memberships (B = .28, p = .001, OR = 1.32). Gender was a significant predictor when comparing the Low versus the Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency class memberships (B = −.65, p = .04, OR = .52).
Table 4.
Results of multinomial logistic regression analyses.
OR (95% CI) | |||
---|---|---|---|
University Sample | |||
Class 2 (Low) vs. 1 (Moderate)# | Class 3 (High) vs. 1 (Moderate)# | Class 3 (High) vs. 2 (Low)# | |
Age | 1.03 (.97 – 1.11) | 1.05 (.97 – 1.14) | 1.02 (.96 – 1.08) |
Gender | .52 (.28 – .97)* | .98 (.41 – 2.35) | 1.87 (1.01 – 3.45) |
Anger | .86 (.80 – .92)** | 1.14 (1.05 – 1.23)p= .001 | 1.32 (1.23 – 1.43) p= .001 |
MTurk sample | |||
Class 2 (Moderate) vs. 1 (Low)# | Class 3 (High) vs. 1 (Low)# | Class 3 (High) vs. 2 (Moderate)# | |
Age | .98 (.94 – 1.01) | 1.02 (.95 – 1.09) | 1.00 (.95 – 1.06) |
Gender | .54 (.27 – 1.09) | .52 (.16 – 1.68) | .98 (.38 – 2.55) |
Depression | 1.37 (1.26 – 1.50)** | 2.08 (1.63 – 2.66)** | 1.51 (1.21 – 1.89)** |
Note. University Sample: Class 1 is Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency, Class 2 is Low PTSD-Negative Urgency, Class 3 is High PTSD-Negative Urgency; MTurk sample: Class 1 is Low PTSD-Negative Urgency, Class 2 is Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency, and Class 3 is High PTSD-Negative urgency;
p < .05;
p < .001;
indicates the reference class.
3.2. MTurk sample
Consistent with the results of the university sample (see Table 2), the 3-class solution was deemed optimal based on the recommended guidelines (DiStefano & Kamphaus, 2006; Nylund, Asparouhov, et al., 2007). Figure 2 provides a graphical depiction of the 3-class solution. Again, class descriptors referenced the negative urgency facet based on that being the most distinguishing of all classes among the UPPS subscales. Class 3 was characterized by relatively higher PTSD subscale severity (particularly NACM and AAR), and negative urgency scores (High PTSD-Negative Urgency). In comparison, Class 1 was charcterized by relatively lower PTSD and negative urgency scores (Low PTSD-Negative Urgency). Class 2 was characterised by moderate PTSD and negative urgency scores (Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency). Results of the ANOVA tests (see Table 3) indicated that all classes differed significantly on PTSD subscale severity. Further, all classes differed significantly on the negative urgency score, and while some classes differed significantly on the lack of predmeditation (Class 1 vs. Class 3) and lack of perseverance (excluding Class 2 vs. Class 3) subscale scores. Where there were significant differences, Class 3 had the highest scores followed by Class 2, and then Class 1.
Figure 2. Latent profiles of participants based on responses to PTSD and impulsivity indicators (MTurk sample).
Note. Class 1 is Low PTSD-Negative Urgency, Class 2 is Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency, and Class 3 is High PTSD-Negative Urgency, NACM is PTSD’s negative alterations in mood and cognitions subscale; AAR is PTSD’ alterations in arousal and reactivity subscale.
Results of the multinomial logistic regression analyses (see Table 4; n = 326) indicated that depression was a significant predictor for all class comparisons. Depression severity was significant in predicting the Moderate versus the Low PTSD-Negative Urgency class memberships (B = .32, p < .001, OR = 1.37), the High versus the Low PTSD-Negative Urgency class memberships (B = .73, p < .001, OR = 2.08), and the High versus the Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency class memberships (B = .41, p < .001, OR = 1.51). Age and gender were not significant covariates.
4. Discussion
Using two distinct samples (university students and community), we examined the nature and construct validity of latent subgroups of participants based on their responses to PTSD and impulsivity items. Results indicated an optimal 3-class solution, with classes differing mainly in severity of the indicators of PTSD subscales and negative urgency predominantly, and with construct validity. Negative urgency was the predominant impulsivity facet that helped differentiate subgroups of people.
Both samples differed on some demographic characteristics. Comparatively, the MTurk sample had an older and more diverse age sample, and included a larger percentage of employed participants. While in both samples the most prevalent PTE was the unexpected death of a family member/close friend, the samples differed in the prevalence of other PTEs experienced. The second most prevalent PTE for the student sample was childhood sexual molestation, compared to the experience of a life-threatening accident in the MTurk sample. Despite differences in the samples, we found evidence for three latent subgroupings of participants based on their responses to PTSD and impulsivity items. We labelled them High PTSD-High negative urgency, Moderate PTSD-Moderate negative urgency, and Low PTSD-Low negative urgency.
While all latent classes differed in all PTSD subscale severity, the negative urgency facet of impulsivity was the most distinguishing for all classes. Negative urgency references a tendency to act impulsively under conditions of intense negative affect (Whiteside & Lynam, 2001), with the possible function of alleviating the distressing negative emotions. Theoretical and empirical evidence indicates that people with greater PTSD symptom severity may engage in impulsive behaviors to regulate their mood, mainly to reduce negative affect (Marshall-Berenz, et al., 2011; Weiss, et al., 2015). Berg, et al. (2015) found that negative urgency (compared to other UPPS facets) consistently had the largest effect size in relation to several categories of psychopathology. Our findings suggest that their results might generalize to PTSD as well.
In both samples, the UPPS lack of perseverance facet (related to distractibility) differentiated the Low from both the Moderate and High PTSD-Negative Urgency classes. Our findings also suggested that the UPPS lack of premeditation facet (difficulty in reflecting on consequences before engaging in an act), though often considered the prototypical definition of impulsivity, did not differentiate classes beyond the Low vs. High PTSD-Negative Urgency class comparisons (only MTurk sample). Further, the UPPS sensation-seeking facet did not differentiate latent classes in either sample. Our findings align with prior empirical research linking lack of perseverance to PTSD intrusion severity (Roley, et al., 2017), and with the cognitive explanation of the relation between PTSD symptoms and impulsivity (Ben-Zur & Zeidner, 2009). While our findings support the role of lack of perseverance in subtyping diagnostic patterns, this facet did not consistently differentiate all subgroups of people (especially those at higher levels of post-trauma severity). Thus, while there is evidence for the usefulness of the UPPS model of impulsivity, our results suggest that in regards to PTSD subtypes, the most relevant facet is negative urgency, followed by lack of perseverance, and lack of premeditation.
The class solution had construct validity. Depression (university sample) significantly predicted membership in the more severe class. Our results align with empirical evidence indicating PTSD’s co-occurrence with depression (Rytwinski, et al., 2013). The causality explanation of PTSD-depression comorbidity indicates that PTSD may be a causal risk factor for depression, or depression may be a causal risk factor for PTE, and subsequent PTSD (Breslau, 2009; Strander, Thomsen, & Highfill-McRoy, 2014). Alternatively, the common factors explanation highlights shared environmental and genetic risk/buffering factors underlying these disorders (Breslau, 2009; Strander, et al., 2014). Further, our results align with empirical evidence relating impulsivity to depression (d’Acremont & Van der Linden, 2007; Swann, et al., 2008). Impulsive individuals may use ineffective emotion regulation strategies when experiencing stressful life events, which may lead to increased depression and emotional stress (d’Acremont & Van der Linden, 2007). Such an explanation highlights the role of the negative urgency facet in relation to depression. Indeed, suicide attempts among people who are depressed are linked to impulsivity (Corruble, Benyamina, Bayle, Falissard, & Hardy, 2003; McGirr & Turecki, 2007; Simon, et al., 2001).
Further, anger regulation difficulties (MTurk sample) significantly predicted membership in the more severe class. PTSD relates to difficulties in suppressing, regulating, inhibiting, and appropriately expressing anger (Olatunji, Ciesielski, & Tolin, 2010; Orth & Wieland, 2006). The survival mode theory indicates that anger has survival value in threatening situations; however becomes maladaptive when threat schemas are activated in non-threatening situations (which occurs in individuals with PTSD). Consequently, people react with hostile appraisal, arousal, and antagonistic behavior, failing to regulate anger (Chemtob, Novaco, Hamada, Gross, & Smith, 1997; Novaco & Chemtob, 2002). Alternatively, the fear avoidance theory states that anger represents an avoidant coping mechanism to deal with trauma-related fear (Foa, Riggs, Massie, & Yarczower, 1995). Further, our results align with evidence linking impulsivity to anger regulation difficulties (Ahmed, Kingston, DiGiuseppe, Bradford, & Seto, 2012; Contractor, Armour, et al., 2015; Milligan & Waller, 2001). The experience of negative affect such as anger could trigger impulsive behaviors including self-mutilation (Herpertz, Sass, & Favazza, 1997) and overt aggression (Shorey, Brasfield, Febres, & Stuart, 2011). Indeed, negative urgency has been shown to explain the relation between PTSD and physical aggression (Weiss, Connolly, Gratz, & Tull, 2017).
Age was not a significant covariate in the models. Further research should investigate whether our results would hold true in samples with predominantly younger or older participants. Gender was not a consistent predictor of class membership across samples. The only significant result was that being female decreased the chances of being in the Low vs. Moderate PTSD-Negative Urgency classes by 48% (university sample). While research strongly portrays that females have greater PTSD severity compared to males (Tolin & Foa, 2006), it is possible that mixed findings regarding gender differences in impulsivity (Cross, et al., 2011) could be driving our obtained pattern of relationships.
4.1. Implications, Limitations, and Future Research
Our study results have several implications. When assessing PTSD and impulsivity, we find three meaningful subgroups of people with intra-group homogeneity and inter-group heterogeneity. Although no study has examined this research question, the results of an optimal three-class solution has been found in other studies of posttraumatic responses, such as PTSD and depression (Contractor, et al., 2017), and PTSD, depression, and anxiety (Contractor, Elhai, et al., 2015). Thus, we could conceptualize an impulsive (negative urgency) subtype of PTSD characterized by high PTSD severity and the negative urgency facet of impulsivity. It would be helpful to reference negative urgency in lieu of the general construct of impulsivity when examining the relation between PTSD and impulsivity.
Further, our findings align with studies indicating a strong relation between negative urgency and PTSD cluster severity (Contractor, Armour, Forbes, et al., 2016; Roley, et al., 2017); and the mediating role of negative urgency in the relation between PTSD severity and risky behaviors (Weiss, et al., 2015). In the clinical realm, targeting emotion regulation skills needs to be an important component of treatment for those experiencing greater PTSD severity and negative urgency (Cloitre, Koenen, Cohen, & Han, 2002; Ford, Courtois, Steele, Hart, & Nijenhuis, 2005).
NACM and arousal cluster severity mainly paralleled negative urgency severity. NACM symptoms in particular are quite representative of PTSD’s inherent emotional distress (Contractor, et al., 2014; Elhai, et al., 2015), and engaging in impulsive behaviors (e.g., substance use, suicidal behaviors) may reduce this emotional distress (McLean, et al., 2017; Weiss, Tull, Viana, Anestis, & Gratz, 2012). Further, it is possible that a tendency to act impulsively when experiencing intense negative emotions associated with NACM and AAR symptoms may exacerbate PTSD symptom severity in the long-run (Mirhashem, et al., 2017). Clinically, it may be beneficial to assess for and target impulsive tendencies among individuals with increased NACM and AAR severity to prevent the development of negative behavior cycles.
Lastly, we found a pattern of increasing depression severity and anger regulation difficulties with increasing PTSD severity and impulsivity. In addition to supporting pre-existing theoretical models of these constructs and alignment with empirical research, our results support a clinical focus on depression and anger management for clients who report PTSD severity and impulsivity. Future efforts should focus on developing and evaluating the efficacy of integrated PTSD treatment approaches, including investigating the role of depression, anger, and impulsivity as underlying mechanisms of PTSD symptoms.
Like any study based solely on self-report for data collection, there may have been response biases and social desirability effects. Further, we did not use item-level data as LPA indicators because of model convergence problems. We recommend such an approach in future research with a larger sample to capture more heterogeneity and identify item-level data patterns contributing to diagnostic subgrouping. Additionally, future research needs to examine the generalizability of results to more culturally diverse samples with higher levels of clinical severity. Despite these limitations, using two different samples, we have indicated some preliminary evidence for a negative urgency subtype of PTSD (mainly impulsivity when experiencing negative affect) categorized by greater depression and difficulties regulating anger. This may be a more refined manner of categorizing the externalizing subtype of PTSD (M. W. Miller, 2003).
Highlights.
We examined PTSD-impulsivity typologies and their construct validity.
We used latent profile analyses on data from a student and a community sample.
Results indicated an optimal 3-class solution, with classes differing in severity.
Negative urgency was the predominant impulsivity facet differentiating classes.
Anger and depression predicted membership in the high severity subgroup.
Acknowledgments
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflict of Interest: None
References
- Ahmed AG, Kingston DA, DiGiuseppe R, Bradford JM, Seto MC. Developing a clinical typology of dysfunctional anger. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2012;136:139–148. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Armour C, Contractor AA, Elhai JD, Stringer M, Lyle G, Forbes D, Richardon JD. Identifying latent profiles of Posttraumatic Stress and Major Depression symptoms in Canadian veterans: Exploring differences across profiles in health related functioning. Psychiatry Research. 2015;228:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armour C, Mullerova J, Elhai JD. A systematic literature review of PTSD’s latent structure in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV to DSM-5. Clinical Psychology Review. 2016;44:60–74. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three-step approaches using Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2014;21:1–13. [Google Scholar]
- Ayer L, Danielson CK, Amstadter AB, Ruggiero K, Saunders B, Kilpatrick D. Latent classes of adolescent posttraumatic stress disorder predict functioning and disorder after 1 year. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2011;50:364–375. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belik SL, Stein MB, Asmundson GJ, Sareen J. Relation between traumatic events and suicide attempts in Canadian military personnel. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;54:93–104. doi: 10.1177/070674370905400207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Zur H, Zeidner M. Threat to life and risk-taking behaviors: A review of empirical findings and explanatory models. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2009;13:109–128. doi: 10.1177/1088868308330104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berg JM, Latzman RD, Bliwise NG, Lilienfeld SO. Parsing the heterogenity of impulsivity: A meta-analyttic review of the behavioral implications of the UPPS for psychopathology. Psychological Assessment. 2015;27:1129–1146. doi: 10.1037/pas0000111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billieux J. Problematic use of the mobile phone: a literature review and a pathways model. Current Psychiatry Reviews. 2012;8:299–307. [Google Scholar]
- Blevins CA, Weathers FW, Davis MT, Witte TK, Domino JL. The Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5): Development and initial psychometric evaluation. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2015;28:489–498. doi: 10.1002/jts.22059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bovin MJ, Marx BP, Weathers FW, Gallagher MW, Rodriguez P, Schnurr PP, Keane TM. Psychometric properties of the PTSD Checklist for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–Fifth Edition (PCL-5) in veterans. Psychological Assessment. 2016;28:1379–1391. doi: 10.1037/pas0000254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breslau N. The epidemiology of trauma, PTSD, and other posttrauma disorders. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2009;10:198–210. doi: 10.1177/1524838009334448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2011;6:3–5. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carleton RN, Mulvogue MK, Duranceau S. PTSD personality subtypes in women exposed to intimate-partner violence. Psychological Trauma Theory Research Practice and Policy. 2015;7:154–161. doi: 10.1037/tra0000003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casada JH, Roache JD. Behavioral inhibition and activation in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2005;193:102–109. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000152809.20938.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castillo DT, Joseph JS, Tharp AT, Baca JC, Torres-Sena LM, Qualls C, Miller MW. Externalizing and internalizing subtypes of posttraumatic psychopathology and anger expression. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2014;27:108–111. doi: 10.1002/jts.21886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanaugh CE, Hansen NB, Sullivan TP. HIV sexual risk behavior among low-income women experiencing intimate partner violence: The role of posttraumatic stress disorder. AIDS and Behavior. 2010;14:318–327. doi: 10.1007/s10461-009-9623-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chemtob CM, Novaco RW, Hamada RS, Gross DM, Smith G. Anger regulation deficits in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1997;10:17–36. doi: 10.1023/a:1024852228908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cloitre M, Koenen KC, Cohen LR, Han H. Skills training in affective and interpersonal regulation followed by exposure: A phase-based treatment for PTSD related to childhood abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:1067–1074. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.5.1067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Contractor AA, Armour C, Forbes D, Elhai JD. PTSD’s underlying dimensions and their relation with impulsivity facets. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease. 2016;204:20–25. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Contractor AA, Armour C, Shea MT, Mota N, Pietrzak RH. Latent profiles of DSM-5 PTSD symptoms and the “Big Five” personality traits. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2016;37:10–20. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Contractor AA, Armour C, Wang X, Forbes D, Elhai JD. The mediating role of anger in the relationship between PTSD symptoms and impulsivity. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy. 2015;7:138–145. doi: 10.1037/a0037112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Contractor AA, Durham TA, Brennan JA, Armour C, Wutrick HR, Frueh CB, Elhai JD. DSM-5 PTSD’s symptom dimensions and relations with major depression’s symptom dimensions in a primary care sample. Psychiatry Research. 2014;215:146–153. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Contractor AA, Elhai JD, Fine TH, Tamburrino MB, Cohen GH, Shirley E, Chan PK, Liberzon I, Galea S, Calabrese JR. Latent profile analyses of posttraumatic stress disorder, depression and generalized anxiety disorder symptoms in trauma-exposed soldiers. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2015;68:19–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Contractor AA, Roley-Roberts ME, Langdon S, Armour C. Heterogeneity in patterns of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder and depression symptoms: Latent profile analyses. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2017;212:17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.01.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corruble E, Benyamina A, Bayle F, Falissard B, Hardy P. Understanding impulsivity in severe depression? A psychometrical contribution. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2003;27:829–833. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(03)00115-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cross CP, Copping LT, Campbell A. Sex differences in impulsivity: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2011;137:97–130. doi: 10.1037/a0021591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- d’Acremont M, Van der Linden M. How is impulsivity related to depression in adolescence? Evidence from a French validation of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Journal of Adolescence. 2007;30:271–282. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiStefano C, Kamphaus RW. Investigating subtypes of child development: A comparison of cluster analysis and latent class cluster analysis in typology creation. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2006;66:778–794. [Google Scholar]
- Elhai JD, Contractor AA, Tamburrino M, Fine TH, Cohen G, Shirley E, Chan PK, Liberzon I, Calabrese JR, Galea S. Structural relations between DSM-5 PTSD and Major Depression symptoms in military soldiers. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2015;175:373–378. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.01.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elhai JD, Miller ME, Ford JD, Biehn TL, Palmieri PA, Frueh BC. Posttraumatic stress disorder in DSM-5: Estimates of prevalence and symptom structure in a nonclinical sample of college students. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2012;26:58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foa EB, Riggs DS, Massie ED, Yarczower M. The impact of fear activation and anger on the efficacy of exposure treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder. Behavior Therapy. 1995;26:487–499. [Google Scholar]
- Forbes D, Elhai JD, Miller MW, Creamer M. Internalizing and externalizing classes in posttraumatic stress disorder: A latent class analysis. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2010;23:340–349. doi: 10.1002/jts.20526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forbes D, Hawthorne G, Elliott P, McHugh T, Biddle D, Creamer M, Novaco RW. A concise measure of anger in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2004;17:249–256. doi: 10.1023/B:JOTS.0000029268.22161.bd. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ford JD, Courtois CA, Steele K, Hart OVD, Nijenhuis ER. Treatment of complex posttraumatic self‐dysregulation. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2005;18:437–447. doi: 10.1002/jts.20051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galatzer-Levy IR, Bryant RA. 636,120 ways to have posttraumatic stress disorder. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2013;8:651–662. doi: 10.1177/1745691613504115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman LA, Corcoran C, Turner K, Yuan N, Green BL. Assessing traumatic event exposure: General issues and preliminary findings for the Stressful Life Events Screening Questionnaire. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1998;11:521–542. doi: 10.1023/A:1024456713321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green BL, Korol M, Grace MC, Vary MG, Leonard AC, Gleser GC, Smitson-Cohen S. Children and disaster: Age, gender, and parental effects on PTSD symptoms. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1991;30:945–951. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199111000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herpertz S, Sass H, Favazza A. Impulsivity in self-mutilative behavior: Psychometric and biological findings. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1997;31:451–465. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(97)00004-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jakšić N, Brajković L, Ivezić E, Topić R, Jakovljević M. The role of personality traits in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) Psychiatria Danubina. 2012;24:256–266. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joseph S, Dalgleish T, Thrasher S, Yule W. Impulsivity and post-traumatic stress. Personality and Individual Differences. 1997;22:279–281. [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, Nelson CB. Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1995;52:1048–1060. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240066012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khoury L, Tang YL, Bradley B, Cubells JF, Ressler KJ. Substance use, childhood traumatic experience, and posttraumatic stress disorder in an urban civilian population. Depression and Anxiety. 2010;27:1077–1086. doi: 10.1002/da.20751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 3rd. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Kroenke K, Spitzer RL. The PHQ-9: a new depression diagnostic and severity measure. Psychiatric Annals. 2002;32:509–515. [Google Scholar]
- Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. The PHQ 9 validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2001;16:606–613. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshall-Berenz EC, Vujanovic AA, MacPherson L. Impulsivity and alcohol use coping motives in a trauma-exposed sample: The mediating role of distress tolerance. Personality and Individual Differences. 2011;50:588–592. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.11.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCutcheon AL. Latent Class Analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- McGirr A, Turecki G. The relationship of impulsive aggressiveness to suicidality and other depression-linked behaviors. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2007;9:460–466. doi: 10.1007/s11920-007-0062-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLean CP, Zang Y, Zandberg L, Bryan CJ, Gay N, Yarvis JS, Foa EB, STRONG STAR Consortium Predictors of suicidal ideation among active duty military personnel with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2017;208:392–398. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller MW. Personality and the etiology and expression of PTSD: A three-factor model perspective. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2003;10:373–393. [Google Scholar]
- Miller MW, Resick PA. Internalizing and externalizing subtypes in female sexual assault survivors: Implications for the understanding of complex PTSD. Behavior Therapy. 2007;38:58–71. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milligan RJ, Waller G. Anger and impulsivity in non-clinical women. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:1073–1078. [Google Scholar]
- Mirhashem R, Allen HC, Adams ZW, van Stolk-Cooke K, Legrand A, Price M. The intervening role of urgency on the association between childhood maltreatment, PTSD, and substance-related problems. Addictive Behaviors. 2017;69:98–103. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.02.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Műllerová J, Hansen M, Contractor AA, Elhai JD, Armour C. Dissociative features in posttraumatic stress disorder: A latent profile analysis. Psychological Trauma Theory Research Practice and Policy. 2016;8:601–608. doi: 10.1037/tra0000148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norris FH, Kaniasty K, Conrad ML, Inman GL, Murphy AD. Placing age differences in cultural context: A comparison of the effects of age on PTSD after disasters in the United States, Mexico, and Poland. Journal of Clinical Geropsychology. 2002;8:153–173. [Google Scholar]
- Novaco RW, Chemtob CM. Anger and combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2002;15:123–132. doi: 10.1023/A:1014855924072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nylund K, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A monte carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling. 2007;14:535–569. [Google Scholar]
- Nylund K, Bellmore A, Nishina A, Graham S. Subtypes, severity, and structural stability of peer victimization: What does latent class analysis say? Child Development. 2007;78:1706–1722. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01097.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olatunji BO, Ciesielski BG, Tolin DF. Fear and loathing: A meta-analytic review of the specificity of anger in PTSD. Behavior Therapy. 2010;41:93–105. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2009.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orth U, Wieland E. Anger, hostility, and posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults: A meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006;74:698–706. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.4.698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raftery AE. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology. 1995;25:111–163. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts JA, Pullig C, Manolis C. I need my smartphone: A hierarchical model of personality and cell-phone addiction. Personality and Individual Differences. 2015;79:13–19. [Google Scholar]
- Roley ME, Contractor AA, Weiss NH, Armour C, Elhai JD. Impulsivity facets’ predictive relations with DSM-5 PTSD symptom clusters. Psychological Trauma Theory Research Practice and Policy. 2017;9:76–79. doi: 10.1037/tra0000146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rytwinski NK, Scur MD, Feeny NC, Youngstrom EA. The co‐occurrence of major depressive disorder among individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta‐analysis. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2013;26:299–309. doi: 10.1002/jts.21814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shorey RC, Brasfield H, Febres J, Stuart GL. The association between impulsivity, trait anger, and the perpetration of intimate partner and general violence among women arrested for domestic violence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2011;26:2681–2697. doi: 10.1177/0886260510388289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon TR, Swann AC, Powell KE, Potter LB, Kresnow MJ, O’Carroll PW. Characteristics of impulsive suicide attempts and attempters. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior. 2001;32:49–59. doi: 10.1521/suli.32.1.5.49.24212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith GT, Fischer S, Cyders MA, Annus AM, Spillane NS, McCarthy DM. On the validity and utility of discriminating among impulsivity-like traits. Assessment. 2007;14:155–170. doi: 10.1177/1073191106295527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L. A dual systems model of adolescent risk‐taking. Developmental psychobiology. 2010;52:216–224. doi: 10.1002/dev.20445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L, Albert D, Cauffman E, Banich M, Graham S, Woolard J. Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: Evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental psychology. 2008;44:1764–1778. doi: 10.1037/a0012955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strander VA, Thomsen CJ, Highfill-McRoy RM. Etiology of depression comorbidity in combat-related PTSD: A review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review. 2014;34:87–98. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swann AC, Steinberg JL, Lijffijt M, Moeller FG. Impulsivity: Differential relationship to depression and mania in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2008;106:241–248. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.07.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tolin DF, Foa EB. Sex differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder: A quantitative review of 25 years of research. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:959–992. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vermunt JK. Latent class modeling with covariates: Two improved three-step approaches. Political Analysis. 2010;18:450–469. [Google Scholar]
- Weathers FW, Litz BT, Keane TM, Palmieri PA, Marx BP, Schnurr PP. The PTSD Checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5) Scale available from the National Center for PTSD. 2013 at www.ptsd.va.gov.IN.
- Weiss NH, Connolly KM, Gratz KL, Tull MT. The role of impulsivity dimensions in relation between probable posttraumatic stress disorder and aggressive behavior among substance users. Journal of Dual Diagnosis. 2017:1–10. doi: 10.1080/15504263.2017.1293310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss NH, Tull MT, Sullivan TP, Dixon-Gordon KL, Gratz KL. Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and risky behaviors among trauma-exposed inpatients with substance dependence: The influence of negative and positive urgency. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2015;155:147–153. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.07.679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss NH, Tull MT, Viana AG, Anestis MD, Gratz KL. Impulsive behaviors as an emotion regulation strategy: Examining associations between PTSD, emotion dysregulation, and impulsive behaviors among substance dependent inpatients. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2012;26:453–458. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiteside SP, Lynam DR. The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:669–689. [Google Scholar]
- Whiteside SP, Lynam DR, Miller JD, Reynolds SK. Validation of the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale: A four-factor model of impulsivity. European Journal of Personality. 2005;19:559–574. [Google Scholar]
- Wolf EJ, Miller MW, Harrington KM. Personality-based latent classes of posttraumatic psychopathology: Personality disorders and the internalizing/externalizing model. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2012;121:256–262. doi: 10.1037/a0023237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf EJ, Miller MW, Reardon AF, Ryabchenko KA, Castillo D, Freund R. A Latent Class Analysis of dissociation and posttraumatic stress disorder. Evidence for a dissociative subtype. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;7:698–705. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wortmann JH, Jordan AH, Weathers FW, Resick PA, Dondanville KA, Hall-Clark B, Foa EB, Young-McCaughan S, Yarvis J, Hembree EA, Mintz J, Peterson AL, Litz BT. Psychometric analysis of the PTSD Checklist-5 (PCL-5) among treatment-seeking military service members. Psychological Assessment. 2016;28:1392–1403. doi: 10.1037/pas0000260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu AM, Cheung VI, Ku L, Hung EP. Psychological risk factors of addiction to social networking sites among Chinese smartphone users. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2013;2:160–166. doi: 10.1556/JBA.2.2013.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]