Abstract
In nocturnal rodents, voluntary wheel-running activity (WRA) represents a self-reinforcing behavior. We have previously demonstrated that WRA is markedly reduced in mice with a region-specific deletion of the transcription factor Pou4f1 (Brn3a), which leads to an ablation of the dorsal medial habenula (dMHb). The decrease in WRA in these dMHb-lesioned (dMHbCKO) mice suggests that the dMHb constitutes a critical center for conveying reinforcement by exercise. However, WRA also represents a prominent output of the circadian system and the possibility remains that the dMHb is a source of input to the master circadian pacemaker located in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) of the hypothalamus. To test this hypothesis, we assessed the integrity of the circadian system in dMHbCKO mice. Here we show that developmental lesion of the dMHb reduces WRA under both a light-dark (LD) cycle and constant darkness, and increases the circadian period of WRA, but has no effect on the circadian amplitude or period of home cage activity or the daily amplitude of sleep stages, suggesting that the lengthening of period is a result of the decreased WRA in the mutant mice. Polysomnographic sleep recordings show that dMHbCKO mice have an overall unaltered daily amplitude of sleep stages but have fragmented sleep and an overall increase in total (rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. Photoresponsiveness is intact in dMHbCKO mice but, compared to control animals, they re-entrain faster to a 6-h abrupt phase delay protocol. Circadian changes in WRA of dMHbCKO mice do not appear to emerge within the central pacemaker, as circadian expression of the clock genes Per1 and Per2 within the SCN is normal. We do find some evidence for fragmented sleep and an overall increase in total REM sleep, supporting a model in which the dMHb is part of the neural circuitry encoding motivation and involved in the manifestation of some of the symptoms of depression.
Manuscript Keywords: habenula, sleep, physical activity, depression, motivation, REM sleep
Introduction
Circadian pacemakers have an endogenous period close to 24 h in the absence of environmental cues and can be reset by entraining to daily environmental cycles. In mammals, behaviors such as wheel-running activity (WRA) often provide temporal feedback to the circadian system to reset the clock or regulate the period of the pacemaker (Edgar and Dement 1991; Edgar et al. 1991; Mistlberger 1991; Yamada et al. 1988). We have recently shown that the dorsal medial habenula (dMHb) is essential for normal levels of voluntary WRA, and that reduced WRA in dMHb-lesioned (dMHbCKO) mice may be accounted for by a lack of perceived reinforcement (Hsu et al. 2014). However, a reduction in the amplitude of circadian WRA can result from decreased in vivo suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) electrical activity and clock gene expression in mice (Han et al. 2012; Nakamura et al. 2011). Thus, the reduced WRA seen in dMHbCKO mice could also be a result of reduced activity feedback to the circadian system and/or decreased amplitude in the circadian circuitry involving the SCN and the habenula, and not solely due to reduced motivation.
The habenula is located on the dorsal surface of the thalamus; this paired structure has been implicated in the regulation of a wide variety of behaviors including motivation, reward, learning and memory, circadian rhythms and sleep (Aizawa et al. 2013; Proulx et al. 2014; Valjakka et al. 1998; Velasquez et al. 2014; Zhao et al. 2015). The habenula neurons project to the interpeduncular nucleus (IP) through the fasciculus retroflexus (FR). Both transection of the FR in hamsters (Paul et al., 2011) as well as dMHb ablation in mice (Hsu et al., 2014) reduce the amount of WRA and change its 24-h temporal distribution. In contrast, there is no change in locomotion in an open field or in the home cage between control and dMHbCKO mice (Hsu et al., 2014). Interestingly, the circadian periods of both WRA and home cage activity (HCA) were longer in FR lesioned hamsters compared to control animals (Paul et al. 2011). These findings, together with the fact that the habenula exhibits autonomous rhythms of gene expression and electrical activity (Guilding et al. 2010; Sakhi et al. 2014; Tavakoli-Nezhad and Schwartz 2006; Zhao and Rusak 2005), have suggested that the habenula contains an extra-SCN circadian oscillator involved in the regulation of circadian rest-activity cycles. However, the functions of the main subregions of the habenula as part of the circadian system remain to be determined.
In the present study, we addressed these questions by first measuring voluntary WRA and HCA of intact and dMHb-lesioned mice under both 12:12 LD cycle and constant darkness (DD), and determining whether the lesion affects circadian period. We also examined photoresponsiveness by looking at light-induced phase-shifting of WRA and re-entrainment after abrupt LD cycle phase shifts. Finally, we measured the circadian expression of SCN Per1 and Per2, as well as electroencephalographic (EEG)-recorded sleep stages to determine if the central pacemaker and another of its prominent circadian outputs were affected by the lesion. Together, our results show that genetic ablation of the dMHb has minimal impact on the circadian regulation of locomotor activity and sleep. Although the circadian period of WRA is lengthened in dMHbCKO mice, this change appears to be a consequence of the decrease in WRA that may result from reduced motivation. On the other hand, ablation of the dMHb leads to increases in sleep fragmentation and in rapid eye movement (REM). These sleep abnormalities, together with reduced perceived reinforcement of locomotor activity and a range of both normal and abnormal depression-related behavioral tests we have previously described (Hsu et al. 2016; Hsu et al. 2014), suggest that the loss of dMHb function leads to a specific subset of the canonical manifestations of depression.
Materials and Methods
Generation of transgenic mice
All experiments with animals were performed in accordance with animal protocols approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Washington and the Seattle Children’s Research Institute.
Mice with a tissue-specific null mutation of Pou4f1 in the dMHb and control littermates used in this study were generated and genotyped as previously described (Hsu et al. 2014). Three transgenic strains of mice were used: Pou4f1tlacZ, in which a functionally null allele replacing the Pou4f1 coding sequence with a β-galactosidase expression cassette (Quina et al. 2005) ; Pou4f1flox, in which the principal coding exon of Pou4f1 is flanked by loxP sequences (Gerfen et al. 2013; Hsu et al. 2014); and Syt6Cre, STOCK Tg(Syt6-Cre)KI148Gsat/Mmcd, a BAC transgenic line obtained from the Gensat project. Experimental mice were generated by crossing mice with the genotype Pou4f1tlacZ/+, Syt6Cre/Cre with Pou4f1flox/flox mice to yield the genotypes Pou4f1tlacZ/flox, Syt6Cre/+ (dMHbCKO) and Pou4f1flox/+, Syt6Cre/+ (control) mice in equal ratios. The dMHbCKO mice show a profound loss of neurons in the dMHb due to postnatal cell death, while control mice do not show detectable loss of dMHb neurons (Hsu et al. 2014). Pou4f1+/− and Pou4f1+/+ mice were generated by crossing mice with the genotype Pou4f1tlacZ/+, Syt6Cre/Cre with C57BL/6NCrl mice (Charles River Laboratories). All mice used were backcrossed >6 generations to C57BL/6NCrl mice. Behavioral assays were performed on male mice 2–8 months of age.
Behavioral measurements and analyses
For WRA, mice were individually housed in cages equipped with a horizontal wire running wheel and maintained in a ventilated, light-tight room under a 12:12 LD cycle with 200-lux intensity or constant darkness (DD). WRA was monitored using Clocklab software (Actimetrics) and analyzed using El Temps software (Dr. Toni Diéz-Noguera, Universidad de Barcelona) as previously described (Han et al. 2012). Data were analyzed in 10-min or 60-min bins for double-plotted WRA actograms or WRA waveforms, respectively.
To determine if light would induce phase shift of circadian WRA, lights were turned off following assessment of LD WRA and mice remained in DD throughout a 24-h period. Mice remained in their home cages and were moved to another chamber, exposed to a 30-min pulse of light (500-lux) at circadian time (CT) 16 (with CT12 defined as the onset of WRA and each circadian hour approximated to 60-min), and transferred back to the chamber in DD. To quantify the light-induced phase delay, a line through the onset of WRA was drawn on the plotted actograms by three independent observers for the 6–8 cycles after exposure to the light pulse, and then extrapolated back to the day of the pulse; the delay was measured as the difference between the activity onset on the day of the pulse and the extrapolated line.
To determine the effect of abrupt LD phase shifts on the phase of the circadian WRA rhythm, a delay and advance “jetlag” protocol was employed (Han et al. 2012). After mice were entrained to a 12:12 LD cycle, a delay jetlag was initiated by extending the light phase by 6 h, i.e. delaying the time of lights off by 6 h. The mice stayed under this new 12:12 LD cycle until they were re-entrained. The advance jetlag was initiated by shortening the light phase by 6 h, i.e. advancing the time of lights off by 6 h. For delays, we counted the number of days for the activity offset to reach the same phase relationship—relative to the LD cycle—as before the LD shift. For advances, we counted the number of days for activity onset to reach a same phase relationship as before the LD shift. Three independent observers assessed both delay and advance shifts, and the average day count was used.
To determine the phase angle of entrainment, the time of onset of WRA after mice were released into DD was subtracted from the time of lights off before the release. A positive phase angle of entrainment implies the mouse started WRA prior to time of lights off.
HCA was measured by housing mice singly in cages equipped with an infrared (IR) beam detector (Gardtec Gardscan MX PIR, RS Components Ltd., Northamptonshire, United Kingdom) and without a running wheel.
A single cohort of control and dMHbCKO mice was used for all circadian behavioral rhythms measured using WRA. The order of the tests conducted was as follows: LD WRA measurements, release into DD and CT16 light-induced phase shift, DD WRA and period measurements, re-entrainment to the LD cycle, and delay jetlag followed by advance jetlag. Following administration of the CT16 light pulse, mice remained in DD until WRA had stabilized before assessments of DD WRA began (Fig. 1A, arrowhead). A second cohort of control and dMHbCKO mice was used to examine the phase angle of entrainment; both genotypes of this second cohort had undergone a sucrose preference test, a rotarod test, a Catwalk test and an open field test reported elsewhere (Hsu et al. 2014). A third cohort was used to measure HCA. A fourth cohort of Pou4f1+/− and Pou4f1+/+ mice was used to assess the effect of Pou4f1 haploinsufficiency on WRA.
Sleep
Sleep stages were recorded as previously described with some modifications (Cambras et al. 2007; Lee et al. 2009). Mice were anaesthetized with isoflurane and placed into a stereotaxic device where isoflurane anesthesia continued throughout the surgery protocol. Each mouse was implanted with EEG electrodes, consisting of dental screws (Pinnacle Technology, Lawrence, KS, # 8209: 0.10”). A midline incision was made above the skull. Recording electrodes were screwed through cranial holes as follows: over the left frontal cortex (1.5 mm lateral and 2 mm anterior to bregma), over the right parietal cortex (1.5 mm lateral and 2 mm posterior to bregma), a ground electrode was placed over the visual cortex (1.5 mm lateral and 4.0 mm posterior to bregma), and a reference electrode placed over the cerebellum (1.5 mm lateral and 6.5 mm posterior to bregma). Electromyogram (EMG) signals were obtained by placing a pair of silver wires into the neck muscles. The screws were connected, through silver wires, to a common six-pin connector compatible with our recording device (Pinnacle Technology). The screws and connector were fixed to the skull with dental cement. After surgery, mice were housed in single recording cages under a 12:12 LD cycle.
After a recovery period of 5–7 days, the mice were connected to the data-recording device. To allow mice to acclimate to the preamplifier and tether, we waited another two days after connecting them to start recording data. Two days of EEG and EMG activity were recorded under a 12:12 LD cycle. The EEG and EMG signals were sampled at 400 Hz with an EEG low-pass filter of 25 Hz and a low-pass EMG filter of 100 Hz. Each 10-sec epoch was classified as either wake, non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, or REM sleep using an automatic scoring algorithm written in R version 3.3.0. First, the power spectrum of the EEG and EMG signals is calculated using an overlap window of 50%. Then, for each-10 sec epoch the following parameters are generated: a) Delta power: the sum of EEG power at frequencies ranging between 0.5–4 Hz; b) Theta power: the sum of EEG power at frequencies ranging between 6–12 Hz; and c) EMG RMS: the root mean square of the raw EMG signal. For each 10-sec epoch, we determined the brain state using the following algorithm: if EMG RMS was higher than a threshold visually determined for each 24-h recording session, the epoch was classified as wake, otherwise, the epoch was defined as sleep. Then, sleep stage was classified as REM whenever the theta to delta power ratio during the epoch was higher than the average theta to delta ratio—over the whole recording 24-h session—plus 1 SD. The remaining epochs were classified as NREM sleep. This scoring method was based on previous studies (Fenzl et al. 2007; Louis et al. 2004; Weber et al. 2015), executed though a newly developed R code (Ben-Hamo et al., unpublished) and validated by two independent experimenters that visually scored 10-sec bins as previously described (Cambras et al. 2007; Lee et al. 2016; Lee et al. 2009), with a minimum of 90% score agreement in both cases. The 10-sec scored epochs were further binned into 20- or 60-min bins, for which we calculated the percentage of each brain state. A 20- or 60-min bin was classified as REM when more than 20% of the bin was spent in REM sleep. If REM sleep was lower than 20% during the bin, it was classified as either wake or NREM sleep depending on which of the two brain states contributed to a higher percentage of the 20 or 60-min bin. Hypnograms were generated using the 10-min bin sleep scores to feed a custom-made “R” code (Ben-Hamo et al. unpublished). REM intensity was calculated as the product of REM percentage and theta power within each 20- or 60-min bin. Statistical analysis was performed both for 20- and 60-min bins and yielded similar results; only 60-min bin results are reported.
In situ hybridization
Mice were housed in cages provided with a running wheel under DD conditions and sacrificed every 4 h over a 24-h period at the indicated circadian times and in situ hybridization for mPer1 and mPer2 was performed with radiolabeled riboprobes as previously described (de la Iglesia 2007). Autoradiographic images were generated by exposing hybridized brain slices to Ultramax film (Kodak). Images were scanned at high resolution, and optical densities (OD) of the autoradiographic hybridization signals were measured with ImageJ software (National Institutes of Health) using a template for the whole SCN.
General statistical methods
Statistical analyses were conducted using unpaired or paired two-tailed t-tests, two-way ANOVA or a mixed-model two-way ANOVA, in which one factor (time) was used as a repeated measure. Post-hoc comparisons were done with Sidak’s post-hoc analyses when appropriate. To measure sleep-stage fragmentation we used survival analysis (Lee et al. 2016; Norman et al. 2006). For each individual, we identified all periods consisting of contiguous sleep stages (total sleep, REM or NREM sleep) in the 10-sec epoch sleep scores. Following the identification of the contiguous sleep epochs for each individual, we fitted a survival curve for all control and dMHbCKO mice, and used Kaplan-Meier estimates of survival to compare the two groups. The 24-h distribution of REM intensity was compared between phenotypes by a 2-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, as the data was not normally distributed. All tests were done with GraphPad Prism 6 (GraphdPad Software, Inc., CA, USA) or “R version 3.1.2.
Results
dMHbCKO mice have reduced voluntary wheel running and longer free-running period
Our previous study has shown that mice with lesions of the dMHb exhibit a reduction in voluntary WRA while entrained to a 12:12 LD cycle (Hsu et al. 2014). To assess the possibility that the reduced WRA may be a result of changes in its circadian regulation, we measured WRA in control and dMHbCKO mice while they were free-running under constant darkness (Fig. 1). Both genotypes entrained to the 12:12 LD cycle and dMHbCKO mice exhibited less WRA compared to control mice (interaction: F(23,575) = 10.96, P <0.0001; genotype: F(1,25) = 15.73, P = 0.0005; time: F(23,575) = 65.66, P < 0.0001; Fig. 1B), especially during the first half of the night between Zeitgeber time (ZT) 12 and ZT17 (with ZT12 the time of lights off). In constant darkness (DD), this phenotype persisted in dMHbCKO mice (interaction: F(23,483) = 14.42, P < 0.0001; genotype: F(1,21) = 15.38, P = 0.0008; time: F(23,483) = 63.83, < 0.0001; Fig. 1C) and, as under LD conditions, post-hoc analyses indicated that the dMHbCKO mice exhibited less WRA during the early subjective night, between CT12 and CT19. In addition to the reduced WRA, dMHbCKO mice free-run with a longer period compared to control mice (see below for two-way ANOVA results).
Because of the nature of the genetic model used, the dMHbCKO mice used in this experiment have both copies of the Pou4f1 gene excised in the dMHb but are hemizygous for Pou4f1 elsewhere, bearing one copy of the constitutive null Pou4f1tlacZ allele and one of the Pou4f1flox allele. To test whether the reduced WRA could be due to haploinsufficiency of Pou4f1, rather than the dMHb lesion observed in dMHbCKO mice, we also examined voluntary WRA on a cohort of matched Pou4f1+/− and Pou4f1+/+ mice. No difference in WRA was observed between Pou4f1+/− and Pou4f1+/+ mice (interaction: F(23,299) = 0.27, P = 0.99; genotype: F(1,13) = 0.23, P = 0.64; time: F(23,299) = 47.92, P < 0.0001; Fig. 1D). Thus, we conclude that the developmental loss of the dMHb, and not reduced expression of Pou4f1 elsewhere in the brain, mediates the reduced WRA observed in dMHbCKO mice.
Ablation of dMHb neurons affects home cage activity minimally
For this experiment, we measured HCA as interruptions of an IR beam in the home cage (Fig. 2A). Confirming our previous finding (Hsu, et al., 2014), there was no difference in IR crossings between control and dMHbCKO mice entrained to a 12:12 LD cycle, although the 24-h temporal distribution of HCA differed between genotypes (interaction: F(23,414) = 1.57, P = 0.047; genotype: F(1,18) = 2.81, P = 0.11; time: F(23,414) = 66.14, P < 0.0001; Fig. 2B). There was also no difference in HCA between genotypes under DD (interaction: F(23,391) = 1.04, P = 0.41; genotype: F(1,17) = 0.93, P = 0.35; time: F(23,391) = 30.79, P < 0.0001; Fig. 2C).
We compared the WRA and HCA period of control and dMHbCKO mice (Fig. 2D). There was an effect of genotype (F(1,42) = 16.01, P = 0.0003) and method of recording activity (F(1,42) = 14.61, P = 0.0004) but no effect of the interaction (F(1,42) = 3.67, P = 0.062). Post-hoc comparisons indicated that circadian periods were different between control and dMHbCKO for WRA but not for HCA, suggesting that the effect of genotype emerged from WRA period differences.
dMHbCKO mice have normal amounts of total sleep but fragmented sleep and increased REM sleep
To determine if lesions of the dMHb affect outputs other than WRA, and given the proposed role of the habenula on the regulation of sleep, we were able to examine the sleep patterns in a limited number of control and dMHbCKO mice under a 12:12 LD cycle (Fig. S1). Throughout the 24-h LD cycle (Fig. S1A), the time spent in wake or non-REM sleep did not differ between genotypes (wake: t(5) = 0.69, P = 0.52; non-REM sleep: t(5) = 0.46, P = 0.67). In contrast, total REM sleep was higher in dMHbCKO mice than controls (t(5) = 2.68, P = 0.0436; Fig. S1A). We also looked at wake and sleep duration during either the light or the dark phase (Fig. S1B). As expected, both genotypes displayed more wakefulness during the dark phase (interaction: F(1,5) = 0.04, P = 0.84; genotype: F(1,5) = 0.48, P = 0.52; time: F(1,5) = 33.84, P = 0.0021) and more non-REM sleep during the light phase (interaction: F(1,5) = 0.02, P = 0.89; genotype: F(1,5) = 0.21, P = 0.67; time: F(1,5) = 35.46, P = 0.0019; Fig. S1B). While both genotypes displayed more REM sleep during the light phase, there was a significant effect of genotype (interaction: F(1,5) = 0.29, P = 0.61; genotype: F(1,5) = 7.21, P = 0.0436; time: F(1,5) = 23.11, P = 0.0049; Fig. 3B) with dMHbCKO mice showing higher levels of REM sleep.
Although total sleep throughout the 24-h LD cycle did not differ between genotypes, the possibility exists that sleep fragmentation is affected by ablation of the dMHb. Indeed, inspection of 10-min-binned hypnograms from individuals from each genotype suggests that sleep is more fragmented in dMHbCKO mice (Fig. S1C). We performed survival analysis and confirmed that total-sleep bouts have shorter duration in dMHbCKO mice (Fig. S1D). Interestingly, survival analysis of non-REM and REM sleep indicated that while non-REM bouts were shorter in dMHbCKO than control mice, an opposite trend, although not statistically significant (P = 0.091), was observed for REM sleep (Fig. S1D). In fact, this effect was statistically significant for both sleep stages in the first 24 h of sleep recordings (P < 0.001 and P = 0.03 for NREM and REM sleep respectively). Thus, the overall increase in REM sleep found in dMHbCKO mice relative to controls is likely accounted for by an increase in REM bouts and a longer duration of these bouts. Finally, the distribution of REM intensity over the 24-h LD cycle did not differ between the two genotypes (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, P = 0.063; Fig. S1E).
dMHbCKO mice have intact circadian photoresponsiveness
We examined photoresponsiveness first by looking at the response of WRA to a phase-shifting light pulse. A brief light pulse administered during the early subjective night is known to cause a delay in the circadian pacemaker (Johnson et al. 2003). Both genotypes were exposed to a 30-min, 500-lux light pulse at CT16, 4 h after the activity onset after 24 h in darkness (Fig. 1A, red asterisk). The mean phase delay for WRA onset was not significantly different for the control and dMHbCKO mice (116.6 ± 16.34 vs. 105.1 ± 12.39 min, respectively; t(25) = 0.56, P = 0.58). We also examined re-entrainment to abrupt LD cycle phase shifts (Fig. 3A). dMHbCKO mice re-entrained faster to a 6-h phase delay compared to control mice (t(25) = 2.36, P = 0.027; Fig. 3B) but in response to a 6-h phase advance both genotypes were fully re-entrained after a similar number of days (t(25) = 0.45, P = 0.66; Fig. 3C). Finally, the phase angle of entrainment was more variable in dMHbCKO mice (F tests for variances; F(27,22) = 4.67, P = 0.0005) but did not differ between genotypes (Mann-Whitney test, P = 0.1469).
dMHbCKO mice have normal expression of clock genes in the SCN
We have shown that reduced WRA in dMHbCKO mice is likely to result from a lack of motivation or reinforcement rather than a motor deficit that prevents wheel running (Hsu et al. 2014). However, the possibility remains that the reduced amplitude in circadian WRA of dMHbCKO mice results from a master circadian pacemaker that, due to reduced locomotor activity feedback, exhibits lower amplitude oscillation in its core molecular components. To test this hypothesis, we looked at the circadian expression of clock genes Per1 and Per2 by in situ hybridization in SCN slices from control and dMHbCKO mice sacrificed every 4 h over a 24-h period under DD conditions. The expression of mPer1 was rhythmic in both control and dMHbCKO mice, and oscillated with a peak between CT4 and CT8 (interaction: F(5,38) = 1.23, P = 0.32; genotype: F(2,38) = 1.22, P = 0.28; time: F(5,38) = 23.31, P < 0.0001; Fig. 4A,C). A similar pattern was observed for mPer2 but with approximately a 4-h delay (interaction: F(5,38) = 1.53, P = 0.21; genotype: F(1,38) = 0.70, P = 0.41; time: F(5,38) = 19.58, P < 0.0001; Fig. 4B,D). We concluded that clock gene expression in the SCN is not affected by the dMHb lesion.
Discussion
The present study confirms our previous findings that dMHb-lesioned mice exhibit reduced voluntary WRA under a 12:12 LD cycle (Hsu et al. 2014), and shows that the phenotype persists under constant darkness. In contrast, the lesion has virtually no effect on HCA measured under either 12:12 LD or constant conditions. These results strengthen our interpretation that dMHb neurons are essential for the reinforcement of wheel running and suggest that their ablation has no effect on the amplitude of the circadian pacemaker driving locomotor activity. This interpretation is consistent with our finding that dMHbCKO mice have normal expression of Per1 and Per2 in the SCN and normal 24-h temporal regulation of sleep. dMHbCKO mice exhibit a longer circadian period than control mice for WRA but not for HCA. Intense activity like WRA can induce both phase shifts and shortening of the free-running period of mice (Edgar et al. 1991; Yamada et al. 1988). Thus, our results suggest that the lower levels of running-wheel activity, which result from reduced motivation in dMHbCKO mice, fail to exert the normal levels of locomotor activity feedback necessary to change the period of the central circadian pacemaker. In contrast, reduced wheel-running does not seem to affect the amplitude of the pacemaker, as rhythmic clock gene expression within the SCN remained unaltered in dMHbCKO mice.
The longer circadian period found in dMHbCKO mice was consistent with the fact that they re-entrained faster, relative to control mice, in response to an abrupt delay of the LD cycle. In contrast, dMHbCKO mice did not show slower re-entrainment to abrupt advances of the LD cycle. This apparent paradox may be due to the fact that entrainment of the circadian system by advances appears to rely more on the discrete effects of light on the phase of the circadian pacemaker whereas entrainment by delays more on the continuous effect of light on the pacemaker period (Schwartz et al. 2011).
The longer circadian period of WRA in dMHbCKO mice is consistent with lengthening of circadian period in hamsters bearing FR transections (Paul et al. 2011), and suggests that the effects of FR lesion may be specifically due to the destruction of dMHb efferent projections. However, Paul et al. (2011) also found that FR transection increased the HCA circadian period, which was unaffected by our dMHb lesion. Furthermore, whereas FR transection led to a decrease in the amount of WRA, it had the opposite effect on HCA (Paul et al. 2011). Because FR transections sever axons emerging from neurons in both the lateral and medial habenula, discrepancies between FR transection and genetic ablation of the dMHb suggest that the two subregions in the habenula may have different roles on the regulation of WRA, HCA and their circadian regulation. However, species differences between hamsters and mice in habenula function cannot be excluded.
We believe that reduced WRA in dMHbCKO mice reflects a decrease in motivation rather than a motor deficit, as dMHbCKO mice also display a reduction in sucrose preference, a model of anhedonia (Hsu et al. 2014). It seems that the dMHb is required to convey multiple types of reinforcement, and in turn for the maintenance of mood. This interpretation is consistent with the observation that wheel-running is an elective behavior that is even displayed by mice living in their natural environment (Meijer and Robbers 2014). The role of the dMHb in conveying reinforcing signals is further supported by intracranial self-stimulation experiments in which dMHb neurons are selectively activated using optogenetics (Hsu, et al., 2014). In contrast, in tests of “behavioral despair” such as the forced swim test and tail suspension test, dMHbCKO mice do not show depression-like behavior (Hsu et al. 2016; Hsu et al. 2014). Here we show that reduced WRA is associated with lengthening of the circadian period. Moderate lengthening of the circadian pacemaker period can lead to a prominent delay in the phase of entrainment (Johnson et al. 2003), which in turn has been associated with depression (Emens et al. 2009; Lewy et al. 2006). The phase angle of entrainment in dMHbCKO mice was not statistically different from that of controls, but it did show more variability. In summary, the dMHb contribution to mood stability that we propose likely relies on its ability to reinforce rewarding stimuli and does not appear to be associated with a delayed phase of entrainment. Finally, the resilience of dMHbCKO mice in some models of depression-like behavior (Hsu et al. 2016; Hsu et al. 2014) also supports the idea that these models do not reflect a single functional pathway.
Importantly, while dMHb ablation does not seem to affect the daily timing of sleep it leads to increased sleep fragmentation and increased levels of REM sleep. Both sleep fragmentation and prolonged REM sleep bouts that lead to an overall increase in REM sleep are signature sleep symptoms in depressed patients (Palagini et al. 2013). Cui and collaborators (Cui et al. 2014) have recently shown that mice with increased activity of lateral habenula (LHb) neurons, after local genetic KO of the glutamate transporter GLT-1, show both a depressive phenotype and an increase in REM sleep. Furthermore, lesions of the FR (Valjakka et al. 1998) or of the LHb (Aizawa et al. 2013) lead to deficits in REM sleep. dMHbCKO mice display phenotypic traits of depression including lack of motivation for wheel-running behavior and anhedonia, sleep fragmentation and increased REM sleep. Thus, the different, and sometimes opposing, roles that are emerging for the LHb and the dMHb are not just limited to the regulation of reinforcement of rewarding stimuli (Hsu et al. 2014) and circadian modulation of locomotor activity, but also to the regulation of sleep.
The neural pathways by which the dMHb increases motivation for WRA, and in turn changes in the circadian period, remain to be determined. Non-photic stimuli, including WRA, that modulate the phase and period of circadian rhythms, can exert their effect through direct projections from the thalamic intergeniculate leaflet of the thalamus and the serotonergic raphe nucleus (RN) to the SCN (Yannielli and Harrington 2004). Whereas both tract-tracing and physiological studies have characterized a direct projection from the LHb to the RN ((Aghajanian and Wang 1977; Kalen et al. 1989; Nishikawa and Scatton 1985; Wang and Aghajanian 1977), the connectivity between the dMHb—its principal target the lateral interpeduncular nucleus—and centers regulating mood, circadian and sleep physiology remain to be determined.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by NIH Awards DA035838 and MH093667 to E.E.T and NSF Award IOS0909716 and NIH Award NS094211 to HOD. W.A.H. was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health Training Award F32MH098498. JJG was supported by the Gates Millennium Scholarship, a Washington Research Foundation Fellowship and a McNair Fellowship. JGP was supported by the Mary Gates Undergraduate Fellowship, and the Levinson and Sargent Awards. MBH was supported by the Israeli Committee for Higher Education Postdoctoral Fellowship for Women and by the Washington Research Foundation Funds for Innovation in Neuroengineering. BAC transgenic mice were generated by the Gene Expression Nervous System Atlas (GENSAT) Project, NINDS Contracts N01NS02331 & HHSN271200723701C to The Rockefeller University (New York, NY). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of any of the funding agencies.
References
- Aghajanian GK, Wang RY. Habenular and other midbrain raphe afferents demonstrated by a modified retrograde tracing technique. Brain Res. 1977;122(2):229–242. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90291-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aizawa H, Yanagihara S, Kobayashi M, Niisato K, Takekawa T, Harukuni R, McHugh TJ, Fukai T, Isomura Y, Okamoto H. The synchronous activity of lateral habenular neurons is essential for regulating hippocampal theta oscillation. J Neurosci. 2013;33(20):8909–8921. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4369-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cambras T, Weller JR, Angles-Pujoras M, Lee ML, Christopher A, Diez-Noguera A, Krueger JM, de la Iglesia HO. Circadian desynchronization of core body temperature and sleep stages in the rat. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(18):7634–7639. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702424104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cui W, Mizukami H, Yanagisawa M, Aida T, Nomura M, Isomura Y, Takayanagi R, Ozawa K, Tanaka K, Aizawa H. Glial dysfunction in the mouse habenula causes depressive-like behaviors and sleep disturbance. J Neurosci. 2014;34(49):16273–16285. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1465-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Iglesia HO. In situ hybridization of suprachiasmatic nucleus slices. Methods Mol Biol. 2007;362:513–531. doi: 10.1007/978-1-59745-257-1_41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edgar DM, Dement WC. Regularly scheduled voluntary exercise synchronizes the mouse circadian clock. The American journal of physiology. 1991;261(4 Pt 2):R928–933. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1991.261.4.R928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edgar DM, Martin CE, Dement WC. Activity feedback to the mammalian circadian pacemaker: Influence on observed measures of rhythm period length. J Biol Rhythms. 1991;6(3):185–199. doi: 10.1177/074873049100600301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emens J, Lewy A, Kinzie JM, Arntz D, Rough J. Circadian misalignment in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2009;168(3):259–261. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenzl T, Romanowski CP, Flachskamm C, Honsberg K, Boll E, Hoehne A, Kimura M. Fully automated sleep deprivation in mice as a tool in sleep research. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;166(2):229–235. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerfen CR, Paletzki R, Heintz N. Gensat bac cre-recombinase driver lines to study the functional organization of cerebral cortical and basal ganglia circuits. Neuron. 2013;80(6):1368–1383. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guilding C, Hughes AT, Piggins HD. Circadian oscillators in the epithalamus. Neuroscience. 2010;169(4):1630–1639. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.06.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han S, Yu FH, Schwartz MD, Linton JD, Bosma MM, Hurley JB, Catterall WA, de la Iglesia HO. Nav1.1 channels are critical for intercellular communication in the suprachiasmatic nucleus and for normal circadian rhythms. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(6):E368–377. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1115729109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsu YW, Morton G, Guy EG, Wang SD, Turner EE. Dorsal medial habenula regulation of mood-related behaviors and primary reinforcement by tachykinin-expressing habenula neurons. eNeuro. 2016;3(3) doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0109-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsu YW, Wang SD, Wang S, Morton G, Zariwala HA, de la Iglesia HO, Turner EE. Role of the dorsal medial habenula in the regulation of voluntary activity, motor function, hedonic state, and primary reinforcement. J Neurosci. 2014;34(34):11366–11384. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1861-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson CH, Elliott JA, Foster R. Entrainment of circadian programs. Chronobiol Int. 2003;20(5):741–774. doi: 10.1081/cbi-120024211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalen P, Strecker RE, Rosengren E, Bjorklund A. Regulation of striatal serotonin release by the lateral habenula-dorsal raphe pathway in the rat as demonstrated by in vivo microdialysis: Role of excitatory amino acids and gaba. Brain Res. 1989;492(1–2):187–202. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)90901-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee ML, Katsuyama AM, Duge LS, Sriram C, Krushelnytskyy M, Kim JJ, de la Iglesia HO. Fragmentation of rapid eye movement and nonrapid eye movement sleep without total sleep loss impairs hippocampus-dependent fear memory consolidation. Sleep. 2016;39(11):2021–2031. doi: 10.5665/sleep.6236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee ML, Swanson BE, de la Iglesia HO. Circadian timing of rem sleep is coupled to an oscillator within the dorsomedial suprachiasmatic nucleus. Curr Biol. 2009;19(10):848–852. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewy AJ, Lefler BJ, Emens JS, Bauer VK. The circadian basis of winter depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(19):7414–7419. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602425103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Louis RP, Lee J, Stephenson R. Design and validation of a computer-based sleep-scoring algorithm. J Neurosci Methods. 2004;133(1–2):71–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.09.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meijer JH, Robbers Y. Wheel running in the wild. Proc Biol Sci. 2014;281(1786) doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mistlberger RE. Effects of daily schedules of forced activity on free-running rhythms in the rat. J Biol Rhythms. 1991;6(1):71–80. doi: 10.1177/074873049100600108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura TJ, Nakamura W, Yamazaki S, Kudo T, Cutler T, Colwell CS, Block GD. Age-related decline in circadian output. J Neurosci. 2011;31(28):10201–10205. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0451-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nishikawa T, Scatton B. Inhibitory influence of gaba on central serotonergic transmission. Involvement of the habenulo-raphe pathways in the gabaergic inhibition of ascending cerebral serotonergic neurons. Brain Res. 1985;331(1):81–90. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90717-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norman RG, Scott MA, Ayappa I, Walsleben JA, Rapoport DM. Sleep continuity measured by survival curve analysis. Sleep. 2006;29(12):1625–1631. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.12.1625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palagini L, Baglioni C, Ciapparelli A, Gemignani A, Riemann D. Rem sleep dysregulation in depression: State of the art. Sleep Med Rev. 2013;17(5):377–390. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paul MJ, Indic P, Schwartz WJ. A role for the habenula in the regulation of locomotor activity cycles. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;34(3):478–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07762.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Proulx CD, Hikosaka O, Malinow R. Reward processing by the lateral habenula in normal and depressive behaviors. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(9):1146–1152. doi: 10.1038/nn.3779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quina LA, Pak W, Lanier J, Banwait P, Gratwick K, Liu Y, Velasquez T, O’Leary DD, Goulding M, Turner EE. Brn3a-expressing retinal ganglion cells project specifically to thalamocortical and collicular visual pathways. J Neurosci. 2005;25(50):11595–11604. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2837-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakhi K, Belle MD, Gossan N, Delagrange P, Piggins HD. Daily variation in the electrophysiological activity of mouse medial habenula neurones. J Physiol. 2014;592(Pt 4):587–603. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.263319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz WJ, Tavakoli-Nezhad M, Lambert CM, Weaver DR, de la Iglesia HO. Distinct patterns of period gene expression in the suprachiasmatic nucleus underlie circadian clock photoentrainment by advances or delays. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(41):17219–17224. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1107848108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tavakoli-Nezhad M, Schwartz WJ. Hamsters running on time: Is the lateral habenula a part of the clock? Chronobiol Int. 2006;23(1–2):217–224. doi: 10.1080/07420520500521947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valjakka A, Vartiainen J, Tuomisto L, Tuomisto JT, Olkkonen H, Airaksinen MM. The fasciculus retroflexus controls the integrity of rem sleep by supporting the generation of hippocampal theta rhythm and rapid eye movements in rats. Brain Res Bull. 1998;47(2):171–184. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(98)00006-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Velasquez KM, Molfese DL, Salas R. The role of the habenula in drug addiction. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:174. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang RY, Aghajanian GK. Physiological evidence for habenula as major link between forebrain and midbrain raphe. Science. 1977;197(4298):89–91. doi: 10.1126/science.194312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weber F, Chung S, Beier KT, Xu M, Luo L, Dan Y. Control of rem sleep by ventral medulla gabaergic neurons. Nature. 2015;526(7573):435–438. doi: 10.1038/nature14979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamada N, Shimoda K, Ohi K, Takahashi S, Takahashi K. Free-access to a running wheel shortens the period of free-running rhythm in blinded rats. Physiol Behav. 1988;42(1):87–91. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(88)90265-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yannielli P, Harrington ME. Let there be “more” light: Enhancement of light actions on the circadian system through non-photic pathways. Prog Neurobiol. 2004;74(1):59–76. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao H, Rusak B. Circadian firing-rate rhythms and light responses of rat habenular nucleus neurons in vivo and in vitro. Neuroscience. 2005;132(2):519–528. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhao H, Zhang BL, Yang SJ, Rusak B. The role of lateral habenula-dorsal raphe nucleus circuits in higher brain functions and psychiatric illness. Behav Brain Res. 2015;277C:89–98. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.