Abstract
Prospective memory (PM) refers to the ability to remember to execute an intended action in the future. For successful PM performance, both top-down strategic monitoring and bottom-up spontaneous retrieval processes need to be appropriately recruited. We assessed PM performance and used fMRI to discover relevant neural correlates and possible predictors for PM performance in 25 postpartum and 26 nulliparous age- and education-matched women. Postpartum women showed decreased PM performance, a higher number of nocturnal awakenings, and lower estradiol level. The postpartum women had decreased functional connectivity (FC) in the right hippocampus and ventral frontoparietal networks (FPN) during retrieval-dominant PM trials relative to maintenance-dominant ongoing trials in the PM block. On multivariate analyses, decreased FC between the right hippocampus and ventral FPN and a higher number of nocturnal awakenings were independent predictors for poor PM performance after adjusting for age, education, estradiol level, and depressive symptoms. On mediation analyses, the estradiol level was found to have an indirect effect on PM accuracy via altered FC as a mediator. This suggests that decreased FC within the spontaneous retrieval-related regions including the right hippocampus and ventral FPN, disrupted sleep rhythms, and decreased estradiol level may contribute to poor PM performance in postpartum women.
Introduction
More than half of pregnant women perceive a decline in their cognitive abilities after pregnancy1. Postpartum women also complain of similar, but maybe a milder degree of cognitive change2. “Maternal amnesia”, “momnesia” or “pregnancy brain” are colloquial terms for this condition and the most frequent symptoms are forgetfulness and memory disturbances3,4. Recent meta-analysis studies show that these symptoms are not just subjective, but objective cognitive impairments2,5.
Prospective memory (PM) refers to the ability to remember to execute delayed intention in appropriate situations6. PM requires planning and forming an intention, maintaining the memory of the intention, detecting cues and retrieving the related intention, and then executing the intention while performing an ongoing activity7. PM is essential when performing daily tasks, so disturbances to its processes can cause simple inconveniences or more major consequences. For instance, forgetting to attend an important meeting can lead to a missed promotion at work, forgetting to take medication can result in serious health problems, and forgetting to turn off the gas can even cause fires. Increased background cognitive demands8, depressive symptoms9–11, and sleep disturbance have been suggested as factors that negatively affect PM12, while exposure to estrogen13,14 has been suggested to positively affect PM. Postpartum women might be especially vulnerable to PM dysfunction because they are dealing with additional demanding ongoing tasks (e.g., feeding their baby or changing their baby’s diaper every couple of hours without sleep), while possibly suffering from depressive symptoms15, and because they have decreased serum levels of estrogen. A few studies have reported decreased PM performance in pregnant or postpartum women using behavioral data4,16. However, as these studies mainly focused on pregnant women, there is little information about how PM is affected during the postpartum period. Moreover, no study has focused on revealing culprit regions related to PM performance in postpartum women using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). PM might be more crucial to postpartum women as they now have to be mothers on top of their other family life roles, and in cases when they have to return to their jobs, they must also be proficient at their work. So, we need to verify whether postpartum women truly have decreased PM, and if so, we need to define what brain regions and clinical factors are associated with this decreased PM ability to develop more strategies to improve PM performance.
Therefore, the present study aimed to assess PM performance in postpartum women by comparing PM performance between postpartum women and nulligravid women. PM task-based fMRI was used to find neural substrates associated with PM performance in postpartum women. We also obtained clinical information to establish possible predictors for PM performance in postpartum women.
Results
Participant characteristics
Initially, 25 women were recruited for the Postpartum group and 30 women were recruited for the Control group. From the Control group, one participant was excluded due to excessive imaging artifacts, two due to movements exceeding a prior maximum movement of 2 mm, and one due to an incidentally found old infarct. No participants were excluded from the Postpartum group. Consequently, 25 participants in the Postpartum group and 26 in the Control group were included for final analysis. The demographic and clinical data of the participants are summarized in Table 1. All of the Postpartum group were married, while only 23.1% of the Control group were married (P < 0.001). Compared with the Control group, the Postpartum group complained of severer subjective cognitive decline (higher Cognitive Failure Questionnaire [CFQ] scores; P = 0.021). Although the Postpartum group tended to sleep longer per day (P = 0.084), they awakened more frequently during the night (P < 0.001) than the Control group and no difference was found in the self-rated scale for lack of sleep. The degree of depressive symptoms tended to be severer (higher Beck Depression Inventory [BDI] score) in the Postpartum group (P = 0.092). The serum estradiol level (P = 0.004) was significantly lower in the Postpartum group. Among the cognitive assessment tests, the Postpartum group exhibited poorer performance only in the digit span sequencing test compared with the Control group (P = 0.033). After the false discovery rate (FDR) correction, significant differences were observed for marital status, number of nocturnal awakenings, and serum estradiol level between the Postpartum group and the Control group. Within the Control group, age was the only factor to differ according to marital status, with single women being younger than married women (28.7 ± 3.7 years vs. 33.5 ± 2.4 years; P = 0.006). Within the Postpartum group, compared to women who did not breastfeed, breastfeeding women awakened more frequently during the night (2.0 [1.0–2.3] vs. 0.0 [0.0–2.0]; P = 0.021) and complained of severer subjective cognitive decline (39.4 ± 13.5 vs. 29.2 ± 9.7; P = 0.046). However, the differences in each group were not significant after FDR correction.
Table 1.
Control group (n = 26) | Postpartum group (n = 25) | P value | |
---|---|---|---|
Demographic characteristics | |||
Age (y) | 29.8 ± 4.0 | 30.9 ± 3.0 | 0.267 |
Duration of education (y) | 19 (16–19) | 16 (16–19) | 0.160 |
Married | 6 (23.1%) | 25 (100%) | <0.001* |
Normal delivery/C-sec | N/A | 17/8 | N/A |
Interval between MRI scan and delivery date (d) | N/A | 103.7 ± 16.2 | N/A |
Breastfeeding (at night) | N/A | 14 (11) | N/A |
Self-report questionnaires | |||
Number of awakenings | 0.0 (0.0–0.0) | 1.5 (0.0–2.0) | <0.001* |
Total sleep time per day (h) | 6.0 (6.0–6.8) | 6.8 (6.0–8.0) | 0.084 |
Lack of sleep | 4.5 (2.0–7.0) | 5.0 (3.0–6.0) | 0.681 |
EPDS | 6.1 ± 4.0 | 7.3 ± 5.6 | 0.446 |
BDI | 6.2 ± 5.6 | 9.0 ± 6.0 | 0.092 |
CFQ | 27.5 ± 9.3 | 34.9 ± 12.9 | 0.021 |
Estradiol assay (pg/mL) | 87.0 (52.0–201.0) | 40.0 (24.8–65.5) | 0.004* |
Neuropsychological tests | |||
COWAT (Animal) | 21.0 ± 4.0 | 20.5 ± 5.8 | 0.725 |
15-item K-BNT | 14.5 (14.0–15.0) | 14.5 (14.0–15.0) | 0.573 |
Digit span forward | 15.0 (14.0–16.0) | 14.0 (13.0–15.0) | 0.127 |
Digit span backward | 12.4 ± 2.8 | 11.6 ± 2.8 | 0.269 |
Digit span sequencing | 9.5 ± 2.2 | 8.0 ± 2.5 | 0.033 |
Word List Memory | 25.9 ± 2.9 | 24.9 ± 3.6 | 0.281 |
Word List Recall | 90.0 (89.0–100.0) | 100.0 (90.0–100.0) | 0.141 |
Word List Recognition | 10.0 (10.0–10.0) | 10.0 (10.0–10.0) | 0.579 |
TMT_A | 20.0 (19.0–23.0) | 20.0 (17.8–28.0) | 0.962 |
TMT_B | 62.3 ± 26.7 | 54.1 ± 22.4 | 0.242 |
*Found to be significant after false discovery rate correction for multiple comparisons. Abbreviations: BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; CFQ, Cognitive Failure Questionnaire; COWAT, Controlled Oral Word Association test; EPDS, Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale; 15-item K-BNT, 15-item Korean version of the modified Boston Naming Test; and TMT, Trail Making Test. Data that have normal distribution are expressed as mean ± standard deviation; otherwise, data are expressed as medians with interquartile ranges in parentheses.
PM task behavioral performance
All participants successfully performed PM tasks for nearly all the trials, as measured by the proportion of correct responses (0.95 ± 0.05). The summary of correct responses and errors for the PM task are presented in Table 2. Compared to the Control group, the Postpartum group showed poorer performance in tasks requiring delayed intention. The Postpartum group gave fewer correct answers (P = 0.003) and had more omission errors (P = 0.003) with longer reaction times (P = 0.003) in PM trials, and fewer correct answers (P = 0.033) and more wrong errors (P = 0.033) in PM ongoing trials. Although, there were no significant differences in any of the performances for control (CTRL) ongoing trials, the Postpartum group tended to need more time to perform CTRL ongoing trials (P = 0.063). PM performance did not differ according to marital status within the Control group, nor did it differ according to breastfeeding within the Postpartum group.
Table 2.
PM Block | CTRL Block | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
PM trial | PM ongoing trial | CTRL ongoing trial | |||||||
Control | PP | P value | Control | PP | P value | Control | PP | P value | |
Correct response | 31.0 (30.0–32.0) | 29.0 (26.8–31.0) | 0.003 | 32.0 (31.0–32.0) | 31.0 (29.0–31.3) | 0.033 | 63.0 (62.0–64.0) | 62.0 (61.0–64.0) | 0.107 |
Type of error | |||||||||
Wrong | — | — | — | 0.0 (0.0–1.0) | 1.0 (0.8–3.0) | 0.033 | 1.0 (0.0–2.0) | 2.0 (0.0–3.0) | 0.107 |
Omission | 1.0 (0.0–0.2) | 3.0 (1.0–5.3) | 0.003 | — | — | — | — | — | — |
Commission | — | — | — | 0.0 (0.0–0.0) | 0.0 (0.0–1.0) | 0.361 | — | — | — |
Reaction time (ms) | 1248.0 ± 250.4 | 1460.7 ± 243.9 | 0.003 | 1829.8 ± 332.4 | 1948.3 ± 278.9 | 0.175 | 1132.8 ± 220.6 | 1248.6 ± 213.2 | 0.063 |
Abbreviations: PP, postpartum group. Data that have normal distribution are expressed as mean ± standard deviation; otherwise, data are expressed as medians with interquartile range in parentheses.
PM task-based fMRI Results
PM task-related activation analysis
In block and event-related design analyses, clusters including the bilateral rostrolateral prefrontal cortex (rlPFC), superior medial frontal gyrus, and lateral parietal cortex, areas which have been known to be associated with PM17, were found for the main effect of “task” (FWE-corrected P < 0.05; Fig. 1 and Supporting Information Table 1).
In the block design analysis, the right hippocampus and bilateral superior temporal gyri showed significant group x task interaction. In the event-related analysis, four clusters were found for the group x task interactions. The Postpartum group showed less activation increase in the right hippocampus and left precuneus in PM ongoing trials compared to CTRL ongoing trials. In contrast, the Postpartum group demonstrated greater activation increase in the left inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and right paracentral lobule in PM trials compared to PM ongoing trials (Fig. 2 and Supporting Information Table 2).
PM task-related FC analysis
As the right hippocampal clusters were found to be significant in group x task interactions on both block and event-related design analyses, this region was thought to play a critical role in the decreased PM performance observed in the Postpartum group. So, we conducted a conjunction analysis with two clusters and selected the overlapping region as a seed for a generalized Psycho-Physiological Interaction (gPPI) analysis (Fig. 3a). Moreover, meta-analysis results using the Neurosynth framework (http://neurosynth.org/) which is a platform for automated meta-analysis and available via a web interface showed that this region was selectively related with PM, supporting our assumption. The Postpartum group revealed less increase in FC in the bilateral IPL, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC)/anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in PM trials relative to PM ongoing trials (Fig. 3b and Supporting Information Table 3), which substantially overlapped with the retrieval-related ventral frontoparietal network (FPN) discussed in a recent meta-analysis study18. We extracted beta estimates, representing the strength of FC with the hippocampus seed, from the four main clusters (PCC, mPFC/ACC, right and left IPLs) included in this network, and revealed that the interaction was based on a significant decrease in FC in PM trials compared with PM ongoing trials in the Postpartum group (P < 0.001), while there was a significant increase in FC in PM trials compared with PM ongoing trials in the Control group (P = 0.005; Fig. 4c). In PM trials relative to CTRL ongoing trials, the Postpartum group showed less increase in FC mainly in the occipital and temporal areas. However, in PM ongoing trials relative to CTRL ongoing trials, the Postpartum group showed an increase in FC in the ventromedial PFC (Supporting Information Table 3).
Clinical and imaging variables associated with PM accuracy
Decreased PM accuracy was correlated with a higher BDI score (greater depressive symptoms, ρ = −0.320, P = 0.024), a higher number of nocturnal awakenings (ρ = −0.393, P = 0.005), a lower FC between the right hippocampus and retrieval-related ventral FPN in PM trials compared to PM ongoing trials (ρ = 0.306, P = 0.029), a lower score on the Digit span sequencing test (ρ = 0.322, P = 0.021), and a higher CFQ score (greater subjective cognitive decline; ρ = −0.268, P = 0.057). On the generalized linear model analysis, the number of nocturnal awakenings (odds ratio [OR] = 0.651 [0.525–0.809]; P < 0.001) and FC between the right hippocampus and ventral FPN in PM trials relative to PM ongoing trials (OR = 1.211 [1.034–1.419]; P = 0.018) were independent predictors for PM accuracy even after adjusting for age, duration of education, serum estradiol level, and the BDI score (Table 3). The mediation analyses for PM accuracy showed that a decreased serum level of estradiol was associated with decreased FC between the right hippocampus and ventral FPN in PM trials relative to PM ongoing trials that led to decreased PM accuracy. An increased number of nocturnal awakenings was associated with decreased PM accuracy without being mediated by altered FC. The BDI score did not significantly affect PM accuracy in both direct or indirect ways (Fig. 4 and Supporting Information Table 4).
Table 3.
Variable | Odds ratio | 95% CI | P value |
---|---|---|---|
Age (y) | 1.039 | 0.981–1.102 | 0.192 |
Duration of education (y) | 1.000 | 0.900–1.111 | 0.965 |
Estradiol (pg/mL) | 1.001 | 0.998–1.004 | 0.444 |
BDI score | 1.036 | 0.994–1.080 | 0.094 |
Number of awakenings | 0.651 | 0.525–0.809 | <0.001 |
FC_PM > PM ongoing | 1.211 | 1.034–1.419 | 0.018 |
Abbreviations: BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; CI, confidence interval; FC_PM > PM ongoing, functional connectivity between the right hippocampus and retrieval-related ventral fronto-parietal network in PM trials compared to PM ongoing trials.
Discussion
Our study revealed decreased PM performance and altered PM task-related brain activity in the Postpartum group. The right hippocampus failed to show significant increase in PM-related brain activation and had decreased FC with the ventral FPN in PM trials relative to PM ongoing trials in the Postpartum group. Furthermore, along with the number of nocturnal awakenings, FC between the right hippocampus and ventral FPN in PM trials relative to PM ongoing trials, which mediates the effect of the serum level of estradiol, was an independent predictor of PM performance. These findings suggest that the right hippocampus and its connectivity with the ventral FPN might be the culprit neural correlates for decreased PM performance in postpartum women.
According to the multiprocess framework, largely two kinds of processes may support PM: top-down strategic monitoring and bottom-up spontaneous retrieval processes19. Top-down strategic monitoring refers to the sustained attentional control process that maintains an intention active in the mind while a person is performing other ongoing tasks20 and that monitors the environment for intention relevant stimuli21. rlPFC and dorsal FPN (i.e., dorsolateral PFC, precuneus, and superior parietal lobule) have been suggested to subserve these processes17,18,22. Sometimes, the retrieval of PM intention is spontaneously and automatically triggered by certain PM cues without any efforts to keep the PM intention active. This is referred to as the bottom-up spontaneous retrieval process23. According to a recent meta-analysis study18, the ventral FPN (i.e., ventrolateral PFC, ACC, PCC, IPL, and insula) and ventromedial aspects of rlPFC might be neural correlates for this process.
In our study, we found that postpartum women had altered activation in the right hippocampus. Although inconclusive24,25, the medial temporal lobe including the hippocampus has also been suggested to be involved with the retrieval process, specifically in the more reflexive and automatic retrieval process recruited only by particular PM cues (i.e., focal PM cues, for which required cognitive processes are similar to those required during ongoing tasks)24,26. In the present study, we used focal PM cues (i.e., both PM and ongoing tasks required judgement on the direction of the hour hand of the clock), and the right hippocampus might have been consequentially recruited in the retrieval process, leading to group differences. Some studies have also reported that the right hippocampus, particularly its posterior portion as shown in our study, is associated with successful PM27 and future event construction28.
For successful PM performance, the regions relevant to each process must connect well at appropriate moments. Therefore, we conducted a gPPI analysis to evaluate PM-related FC patterns. Because intention retrieval is a more predominant process in the PM trial compared to the PM ongoing trial, FC between regions relevant to the retrieval process was expected to increase, as shown in the Control group. In contrast to the Control group, however, the Postpartum group showed decreased FC between retrieval-related regions in PM trials relative to PM ongoing trials, and this might be the reason behind poor PM performance in postpartum women. Supporting this assumption, FC between the right hippocampus and ventral FPN was an independent predictor for PM performance even after adjustment of possible confounding factors including age, duration of education, quality of sleep, serum estradiol level, and depressive symptoms.
Individuals rely on top-down strategic monitoring and bottom-up spontaneous retrieval processes to varying extents depending on the characteristics of PM and ongoing tasks as well as individual factors19,29,30. When the PM cue is focal and salient, it can be detected without much effort and when a person is performing very important and demanding ongoing tasks, there are not enough cognitive resources for sustained attentional control; thus, in these cases, individuals tend to depend more on the spontaneous retrieval process19,30. Given that postpartum women probably rely more on the spontaneous retrieval process for PM due to an increase in demanding ongoing tasks (e.g., taking care of their babies), disturbed neural correlates for the retrieval process might cause poor PM performance in their daily lives. Moreover, Hoekzema et al.31 have recently reported that postpartum women showed gray matter atrophy in the mPFC, precuneus, and bilateral temporal and frontal areas including the bilateral hippocampi which subserve social cognition and these areas show substantial overlap with the ventral FPN reported in our study. The authors also found more response in these regions when each woman was shown a picture of her own baby compared to when they were shown a picture of an unknown baby. Accordingly, the authors suggested that structural change might be an adaptive process for motherhood31. Therefore, we can also postulate that functional and structural alterations of the ventral FPN and hippocampus are critical changes for becoming a mother and PM cues, such as the unknown baby, might not be able to sufficiently stimulate these areas in postpartum women compared to signs of their own babies.
Although the serum estradiol level was not correlated with PM performance and was unable to predict PM performance in a direct way, it affected PM performance via altered FC during the PM task in our study.
This is consistent with previous results. It has been widely accepted that estrogen has neuroprotective effects on cognitive functions including working memory, verbal memory, and prospective memory in postmenopausal14,32 as well as premenopausal33,34 women. Estrogen has been suggested to be associated with cognitive function via modulation of brain activation35 and regional cerebral blood flow13,36 in particular regions including the inferior frontal gyrus, ACC, hippocampus, and IPL which overlap with the regions found in our study. Moreover, the presence of estrogen receptors in the hippocampus and cerebral cortex37–39 could be evidence of estrogen playing a role on cognition through brain functional modulation. Therefore, abrupt estrogen withdrawal after high serum levels of estrogen are sustained during pregnancy might contribute to decreased cognitive function including decreased PM in postpartum women. Unlike our results, some previous studies even found direct associations between estrogen and PM13,14. The use of different markers for estrogen (i.e., the Index for Cumulative Estrogen Exposure, which has been more consistently associated with cognition40, instead of the serum estrogen level measured at a single time point) and individual variability in the estrogen receptor gene which would result in different estrogen activity in the brain41 might be possible explanations for this discrepancy.
Several studies have demonstrated that individuals with sleep deprivation42 and bad sleeping habits12 have decreased PM performances. In line with previous results, a higher number of nocturnal awakenings was an independent predictor for poor PM performance and it affected PM performance without being mediated by altered FC during the PM task in our study. This finding is significant because sleep disturbance is a modifiable factor and appropriate interventions to improve quality of sleep might help postpartum women achieve better PM performance. It is worth noting that most breastfeeding women breastfed during the night (11/14, 78.6%) and had poorer sleep quality. Therefore, proper sleep education for this population could be the first step for better PM performance.
Depressive symptoms were also an important predictor of PM performance, although it failed to reach statistical significance. As we recruited only clinically non-depressed participants, the effects of depressive symptoms might have been underestimated.
Our study has several limitations. First, we only focused on event-based PM (i.e., retrieval of intention in response to an event) in this study as most neuroimaging research incorporates event-based PM tasks17,22,43, and there are already some hypotheses about the neurocognitive mechanism behind event-based PM19,30,44,45, which makes it easier to understand the significance of our results in relation to previous studies. Tasks for event-related PM are considered easier to perform than those for time-based PM (i.e. retrieval of intention after a defined amount of time which requires self-initiated processes)46–48, so our PM task was also easy to execute and most participants in our study were able to successfully perform the task for nearly all trials. Therefore, we could not evaluate brain activation patterns corresponding to error and correct trials, respectively. Second, we asked the study participants to only perform a PM task with focal PM cues and the attentional control process might not be required as much as the retrieval process for this task, causing false-negative findings. Therefore, studies using more cognitively demanding, non-focal or time-based PM tasks would be helpful to elucidate what brain regions are associated with error trials as well as the presence of altered top-down attention control-related brain activity during PM tasks in postpartum women. Finally, our cohort was not matched for marital status between the groups. However, we thought that marital status would not have a significant effect on PM as most studies researching the association between marital status and cognition have reported a positive long-term effect of marital status on mid- or later life cognition49–51. Also, there is little evidence of marital status having a relatively short-term effect on cognition including PM in young adults. Moreover, there was no difference in cognitive function according to marital status, including for PM accuracy within the Control group in our study. However, further studies with more well controlled cohorts are warranted for additional confirmative results.
In conclusion, the present study showed decreased PM behavioral performance in postpartum women compared to nulligravid control subjects. Decreased FC within spontaneous retrieval-related regions, serum estradiol level further affecting the altered FC, and disrupted sleep rhythms may contribute to poor PM performance in postpartum women.
Methods
Participants
Women aged 20–40 years in the 2nd–4th month after parturition (Postpartum group) with normal pregnancies, uncomplicated term vaginal or Caesarean deliveries, and healthy babies were recruited. Women who were matched to these patients for age and duration of education and who had never been pregnant (Control group) were recruited for comparison. Participants were excluded if they had any history or comorbidities that might account for cognitive dysfunction. All participants were right-handed, native Koreans with normal or corrected-to-normal vision.
Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents
This prospective study protocol was approved by the Yonsei University Severance Hospital ethical standards committee on human experimentation, and all participants provided written informed consent prior to all study procedures. All experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
Experimental tasks and procedures
Participants were asked to fill out self-report questionnaires for demographic and clinical information. Then, blood samples were drawn for the estradiol assay. After participants completed a shorter practice version of the fMRI scanning session using a laptop computer, PM task-based fMRI and a 3-dimensional (3D) T1-weighted image were acquired. Afterwards, a cognitive test battery was conducted to evaluate the objective cognitive status of the participants.
Self-report questionnaires
Self-report questionnaires were comprised of questions regarding demographic and clinical characteristics, total sleep time per day, number of nocturnal awakenings, and a scale of 10 to rate sleep deprivation, for which 0 = absence and 10 = very severe to indicate lack of sleep.
To assess depressive symptoms, both the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS)52 and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)53 were used, as EPDS, which was specifically developed and validated to measure depressive symptoms in postpartum women54,55, has not been validated for nulliparous women, while BDI, which is widely used to evaluate depressive symptoms, has not been validated for the detection of postpartum depression56. Moreover, previous studies have shown a strong correlation (r = 0.82) between EPDS and BDI scores57 and there were no significant differences in diagnostic performance for detecting postpartum depression when these two scales were used as continuous measures58; thus, we used both to quantify subjective depression for the Postpartum and Control groups while maintaining consistency between the two groups. Finally, the Cognitive Failure Questionnaire (CFQ)59 was included to assess subjective cognitive decline.
Estradiol assays
Blood samples were collected via venipuncture from the antecubital space and serum estradiol levels were measured by an enzyme chemiluminescent immunoassay using a UniCel DxI 800 automated analyzer (Beckman Coulter, Inc, Fullerton, CA). The minimum sensitivity and range of estradiol was 20–4800 pg/mL.
Neuropsychological tests
The cognitive test battery was comprised of the semantic Controlled Oral Word Association test (COWAT) and Trail Making Test (TMT) part B to assess executive function/verbal fluency60, the Digit Span subset of the Korean version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale: forward, backward, and sequencing recall tests to assess working memory61, the 15-item Korean version of the modified Boston Naming Test (K-BNT) to assess language function, the Word List Memory to assess verbal learning, the Word List Recall and Recognition to assess verbal learning/memory function, and the TMT part A to assess speed of information processing60.
fMRI paradigm for PM
The PM task in our study has the typical features of a PM paradigm43 and was inspired by tasks used in previous studies62,63. The PM task was designed with alternating blocks of PM and CTRL conditions, and each block was repeated 4 times (Fig. 5). The stimuli consisted of an arrow and a clock presented in the left and right side of the screen, respectively. For the ongoing task, participants were instructed to respond by pressing a keypad with the right index or third finger depending on whether the arrow and the hour hand of the clock indicated the same quadrant or not. Both the arrow and the clock could indicate a direction from 1 to 11 o’clock. The CTRL blocks consisted of only ongoing trials (CTRL ongoing) where participants did not have to maintain PM intentions (uncontaminated ongoing trials; not affected by PM intention). In the PM blocks, participants were asked to remember particular times (1, 4, 7, 10 or 2, 5, 8, 11; each set was the target of 2 PM blocks), and to press a third button with their ring finger whenever they saw that particular time on the clock (PM trial) while performing the ongoing task (PM ongoing; contaminated ongoing trials; affected by PM intention). Each block consisted of 16 trials, and half of the trials were PM trials for the PM block. At the beginning of each block, an instruction page was presented for 4 s which gave directions on the tasks of that certain block. Participants were given 4 s to perform each following task trial. Responses were categorized into correct response, wrong error, omission error, and commission error. Correct response was defined as pressing the correct button in each trial, wrong error as failure to respond or pressing the wrong button in ongoing trials, omission error as failure to press the third button in PM trials, and commission error as failure to inhibit PM response in ongoing trials. The PM task was programmed using the Cogent 2000 toolbox (www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent.php) and MATLAB 7.12.0 (The MathWorks).
Image acquisition
All scans were acquired by using a 3 T MR imaging unit (Discovery MR750; GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, WI) with an 8-channel head coil. fMRI data were acquired with a gradient-echo single-shot echoplanar imaging sequence with the following parameters: TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, FOV = 240 × 240 mm2, voxel size = 3.75 × 3.75 × 4.0 mm3, flip angle = 90°, 33 axial slices tilted 30° from the AC–PC plane, no gap, interleaved, scan time = 9 m 14 s including a dummy scan for 10 s. During each scan, stimuli were projected onto a black background screen at the end of the scanner, and participants viewed stimuli by way of a mirror mounted on the head coil. For coregistration and normalization purposes, we also performed a 3D-T1-turbo field echo sequence with the following parameters: sagittal acquisition with TR = 8.3 ms, TE = 3.3 ms, FOV = 198 × 220 mm2, voxel size = 0.77 × 0.86 × 1.0 mm3, 216 slices, flip angle = 12°, no gap.
Image analysis
The fMRI data were preprocessed and analyzed using SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, U.K.). Slice-timing correction was done by resampling all slices relative to the middle slice (i.e., 17th slice) in temporal order. The EPI data for each subject was realigned to the first volume for motion correction and only data sets with ≤ 2 mm maximal displacement during the entire scan were included in this study. Next, the functional images were coregistered to the T1-weighted image and spatially normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template provided with SPM8, then resampled into 3 × 3 × 3 mm voxels, followed by spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel with a full width at half maximum (FWHM) of 8 mm. A high-pass filter of 1/128 Hz was used to remove low-frequency noise, and an AR (1) + white noise model was used for temporal autocorrelation.
PM task-related activation analysis
General linear model analyses were then performed for both block and event-related designs to examine not only sustained but transient brain activity differences between the groups. In block design analysis, regressors were created for PM blocks and CTRL blocks by convolving the boxcar function with the hemodynamic response function (HRF) implemented in SPM. The length of each block in the regressors was 68 s which included 4 s to present the instruction page. In event-related analysis, regressors were created for each trial type by convolving neural input functions with the HRF. All error trials were separately modeled with a single regressor and excluded from further analyses. Six movement parameters extracted from the realignment process were included as confounders, as well as a single covariate representing the mean session effect. Random-effects group analyses were performed in full-factorial design with within-subject factors (block design: PM vs. CTRL block; event-related design: PM vs. PM ongoing vs. CTRL ongoing trial) and between-subject factors (group: Postpartum vs. Control) using beta estimates for each regressor.
PM task-related functional connectivity (FC) analysis
To identify group differences in PM task-related FC, a gPPI analysis64 was conducted. A right hippocampus seed was obtained from the overlapping area between significant clusters with group x task interaction from the block- and event-related design analyses. The physiological variable was made by deconvolving the mean BOLD signal within the seed, and the psychological variable was created by convolving each task regressor (i.e., PM, PM ongoing, and CTRL ongoing trials) with the HRF. The physiological variable was multiplied by the psychological regressors to form the interaction term. Individual beta images for each PPI regressor were then entered into group-level analysis with procedures identical to those used in event-related design analyses. To identify specific patterns of connectivity across conditions in regions which showed significant differences between the Postpartum and Control groups, ROI analyses were conducted using the MarsBar plug-in for SPM (http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/). The ROIs were converted to 4 mm sphere masks centered at the peak, and percent signal changes which were extracted from each ROI for each participant were entered into the second level analysis.
Unless stated otherwise, all statistical analyses were corrected for multiple comparisons based on Monte Carlo simulation corresponding to an alpha level of P < 0.0565.
Clinical data analysis
The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to determine whether data were normally distributed. Accordingly, quantitative data with normal distribution were presented as means ± standard deviations (SDs), and were compared by using a two-sample t-test. Otherwise, data were presented as medians with ranges and the Mann-Whitney U-test was used. Qualitative data were analyzed by using the χ2 test or Fisher’s exact test. False discovery rate correction was performed to correct for multiple comparisons in clinical variables and neuropsychological test results66. Two-tailed Spearman correlation analyses were performed to assess the relationship between PM accuracy and clinical and imaging results which either showed significant group differences or were suggested to be related to PM performance (e.g., depressive symptoms). We also conducted a univariate generalized linear model analysis to evaluate the association between PM accuracy and the significantly correlated variables in the above-mentioned Spearman correlation analysis. Then, a multivariate generalized linear model was used to identify independent factors for PM accuracy. Variables significantly associated with PM accuracy in the univariate model (P < 0.05), other variables of interest (e.g., serum level of estradiol and depressive symptoms), and possible confounding factors (e.g., age and duration of education) were included in the multivariate model. Additionally, to evaluate whether altered FC during the PM task mediated the effects of the clinical variables, mediation analyses were performed after controlling for age and duration of education using a generalized linear model.
Statistical analyses were performed by using SPSS, Version 24.0 (IBM, Armonk, New York), and a two-tailed P < 0.05 was considered significant.
Data availability
The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Kyunghwa Han for her statistical advice. This study was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea Grant funded by the Korean Government (NRF-2014R1A1A2055116) and faculty research grants from Yonsei University College of Medicine (4-2013-0111).
Author Contributions
Na-Young Shin, Sanghoon Han, and Seung-Koo Lee designed the study; Na-Young Shin and Yoonjin Nah performed the study; Dong Joon Kim, Se Joo Kim, Jong Eun Lee, and Sang-Guk Lee advised on design, execution of experiments and interpretation of the results; and Na-Young Shin and Yunjin Bak wrote and edited the manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Change history
9/21/2018
A correction to this article has been published and is linked from the HTML and PDF versions of this paper. The error has not been fixed in the paper.
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-018-23875-5.
References
- 1.Brett M, Baxendale S. Motherhood and memory: a review. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2001;26:339–362. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(01)00003-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Anderson MV, Rutherford MD. Cognitive reorganization during pregnancy and the postpartum period: an evolutionary perspective. Evol Psychol. 2012;10:659–687. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Parsons C, Redman S. Self-reported cognitive change during pregnancy. Aust J Adv Nurs. 1991;9:20–29. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Casey P, Huntsdale C, Angus G, Janes C. Memory in pregnancy. II: Implicit, incidental, explicit, semantic, short-term, working and prospective memory in primigravid, multigravid and postpartum women. J Psychosom Obstet Gynaecol. 1999;20:158–164. doi: 10.3109/01674829909075590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Henry JD, Rendell PG. A review of the impact of pregnancy on memory function. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29:793–803. doi: 10.1080/13803390701612209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Einstein GO, McDaniel MA. Normal aging and prospective memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1990;16:717–726. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.16.4.717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Marsh RL, Hicks JL, Watson V. The dynamics of intention retrieval and coordination of action in event-based prospective memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2002;28:652–659. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.4.652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Einstein GO, Smith RE, McDaniel MA, Shaw P. Aging and prospective memory: the influence of increased task demands at encoding and retrieval. Psychol Aging. 1997;12:479–488. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.3.479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Li YR, Weinborn M, Loft S, Maybery M. Cognitive control processes underlying time-based prospective memory impairment in individuals with high depressive symptomatology. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2014;149:18–23. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Li YR, Loft S, Weinborn M, Maybery MT. Event-based prospective memory deficits in individuals with high depressive symptomatology: problems controlling attentional resources? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2014;36:577–587. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.918090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Altgassen M, Kliegel M, Martin M. Event-based prospective memory in depression: The impact of cue focality. Cognition & Emotion. 2009;23:1041–1055. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Fabbri M, Tonetti L, Martoni M, Natale V. Sleep and prospective memory. Biological Rhythm Research. 2014;45:115–120. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Maki PM, Resnick SM. Longitudinal effects of estrogen replacement therapy on PET cerebral blood flow and cognition. Neurobiol Aging. 2000;21:373–383. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(00)00123-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hesson J. Cumulative estrogen exposure and prospective memory in older women. Brain Cogn. 2012;80:89–95. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.O’Hara MW, McCabe JE. Postpartum depression: current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2013;9:379–407. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Rendell PG, Henry JD. Prospective-memory functioning is affected during pregnancy and postpartum. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2008;30:913–919. doi: 10.1080/13803390701874379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Burgess PW, Gonen-Yaacovi G, Volle E. Functional neuroimaging studies of prospective memory: what have we learnt so far? Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:2246–2257. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Cona G, Scarpazza C, Sartori G, Moscovitch M, Bisiacchi PS. Neural bases of prospective memory: a meta-analysis and the “Attention to Delayed Intention” (AtoDI) model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;52:21–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.McDaniel MA, Einstein GO. Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied cognitive psychology. 2000;14:S127–S144. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Guynn MJ. A two‐process model of strategic monitoring in event‐based prospective memory: Activation/retrieval mode and checking. International Journal of Psychology. 2003;38:245–256. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Smith RE. The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2003;29:347–361. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.3.347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Burgess PW, Quayle A, Frith CD. Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:545–555. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00149-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.McDaniel MA, Guynn MJ, Einstein GO, Breneiser J. Cue-focused and reflexive-associative processes in prospective memory retrieval. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2004;30:605–614. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.3.605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Beck SM, Ruge H, Walser M, Goschke T. The functional neuroanatomy of spontaneous retrieval and strategic monitoring of delayed intentions. Neuropsychologia. 2014;52:37–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.McDaniel MA, Lamontagne P, Beck SM, Scullin MK, Braver TS. Dissociable neural routes to successful prospective memory. Psychol Sci. 2013;24:1791–1800. doi: 10.1177/0956797613481233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gordon BA, Shelton JT, Bugg JM, McDaniel MA, Head D. Structural correlates of prospective memory. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:3795–3800. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Poppenk J, Moscovitch M, McIntosh AR, Ozcelik E, Craik FI. Encoding the future: successful processing of intentions engages predictive brain networks. Neuroimage. 2010;49:905–913. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL. Remembering the past and imagining the future: common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1363–1377. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Scullin MK, McDaniel MA, Shelton JT. The Dynamic Multiprocess Framework: evidence from prospective memory with contextual variability. Cogn Psychol. 2013;67:55–71. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Einstein GO, et al. Multiple processes in prospective memory retrieval: factors determining monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2005;134:327–342. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.3.327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hoekzema E, et al. Pregnancy leads to long-lasting changes in human brain structure. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20:287–296. doi: 10.1038/nn.4458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Duff SJ, Hampson E. A beneficial effect of estrogen on working memory in postmenopausal women taking hormone replacement therapy. Horm Behav. 2000;38:262–276. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2000.1625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Grigorova M, Sherwin BB, Tulandi T. Effects of treatment with leuprolide acetate depot on working memory and executive functions in young premenopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31:935–947. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Maki PM, Rich JB, Rosenbaum RS. Implicit memory varies across the menstrual cycle: estrogen effects in young women. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:518–529. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00126-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Toffoletto S, Lanzenberger R, Gingnell M, Sundstrom-Poromaa I, Comasco E. Emotional and cognitive functional imaging of estrogen and progesterone effects in the female human brain: a systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;50:28–52. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Resnick SM, Maki PM, Golski S, Kraut MA, Zonderman AB. Effects of estrogen replacement therapy on PET cerebral blood flow and neuropsychological performance. Hormones and Behavior. 1998;34:171–182. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Osterlund MK, Grandien K, Keller E, Hurd YL. The human brain has distinct regional expression patterns of estrogen receptor alpha mRNA isoforms derived from alternative promoters. J Neurochem. 2000;75:1390–1397. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2000.0751390.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Osterlund MK, Gustafsson JA, Keller E, Hurd YL. Estrogen receptor beta (ERbeta) messenger ribonucleic acid (mRNA) expression within the human forebrain: distinct distribution pattern to ERalpha mRNA. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2000;85:3840–3846. doi: 10.1210/jcem.85.10.6913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Perlman WR, et al. Expression of estrogen receptor alpha exon-deleted mRNA variants in the human and non-human primate frontal cortex. Neuroscience. 2005;134:81–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.03.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Smith CA, et al. Lifelong estrogen exposure and cognitive performance in elderly women. Brain Cogn. 1999;39:203–218. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1999.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Sundermann EE, Maki PM, Bishop JR. A review of estrogen receptor alpha gene (ESR1) polymorphisms, mood, and cognition. Menopause. 2010;17:874–886. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e3181df4a19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Grundgeiger T, Bayen UJ, Horn SS. Effects of sleep deprivation on prospective memory. Memory. 2014;22:679–686. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2013.812220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Burgess PW, Scott SK, Frith CD. The role of the rostral frontal cortex (area 10) in prospective memory: a lateral versus medial dissociation. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:906–918. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00327-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Burgess PW, Dumontheil I, Gilbert SJ. The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:290–298. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Burgess PW, Gilbert SJ, Dumontheil I. Function and localization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2007;362:887–899. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Einstein GO, McDaniel MA, Richardson SL, Guynn MJ, Cunfer AR. Aging and prospective memory: examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1995;21:996–1007. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.21.4.996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Khan A, Sharma NK, Dixit S. Cognitive load and task condition in event- and time-based prospective memory: an experimental investigation. J Psychol. 2008;142:517–531. doi: 10.3200/JRLP.142.5.517-532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Park DC, Hertzog C, Kidder DP, Morrell RW, Mayhorn CB. Effect of age on event-based and time-based prospective memory. Psychol Aging. 1997;12:314–327. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.2.314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Hakansson K, et al. Association between mid-life marital status and cognitive function in later life: population based cohort study. BMJ. 2009;339:b2462. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b2462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Feng L, et al. Marital Status and Cognitive Impairment among Community-Dwelling Chinese Older Adults: The Role of Gender and Social Engagement. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2014;4:375–384. doi: 10.1159/000358584. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Mousavi-Nasab SM, Kormi-Nouri R, Sundstrom A, Nilsson LG. The effects of marital status on episodic and semantic memory in healthy middle-aged and old individuals. Scand J Psychol. 2012;53:1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2011.00926.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150:782–786. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4:561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Murray L, Carothers AD. The validation of the Edinburgh Post-natal Depression Scale on a community sample. Br J Psychiatry. 1990;157:288–290. doi: 10.1192/bjp.157.2.288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Gibson J, McKenzie-McHarg K, Shakespeare J, Price J, Gray R. A systematic review of studies validating the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale in antepartum and postpartum women. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2009;119:350–364. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01363.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.O’Hara MW, Rehm LP, Campbell SB. Postpartum depression. A role for social network and life stress variables. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1983;171:336–341. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Beck CT, Gable RK. Further validation of the Postpartum Depression Screening Scale. Nurs Res. 2001;50:155–164. doi: 10.1097/00006199-200105000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Chaudron LH, et al. Accuracy of depression screening tools for identifying postpartum depression among urban mothers. Pediatrics. 2010;125:e609–617. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Broadbent DE, Cooper PF, FitzGerald P, Parkes KR. The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. Br J Clin Psychol. 1982;21(Pt 1):1–16. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1982.tb01421.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Lee JH, et al. Development of the Korean version of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Packet (CERAD-K): clinical and neuropsychological assessment batteries. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57:P47–53. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.1.p47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Yeom, T., Park, Y., Oh, K., Kim, J. & Lee, Y. K-Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Korea Guidance, 1992).
- 62.Braver TS. The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16:106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Scullin MK, McDaniel MA, Einstein GO. Control of cost in prospective memory: evidence for spontaneous retrieval processes. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2010;36:190–203. doi: 10.1037/a0017732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.McLaren DG, Ries ML, Xu G, Johnson SC. A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): a comparison to standard approaches. Neuroimage. 2012;61:1277–1286. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Slotnick SD, Moo LR, Segal JB, Hart J., Jr. Distinct prefrontal cortex activity associated with item memory and source memory for visual shapes. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2003;17:75–82. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(03)00082-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the royal statistical society. Series B (Methodological) 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.