Abstract
The importance of early experiences for mental health across the lifespan is well recognized. In particular, there is a strong association between adverse caregiving experiences and mental illness. However, relative to studies assessing outcomes in adults, there are far fewer studies assessing the earlier emerging manifestations of caregiving adversity during development. This lack of developmental research limits an understanding of the mechanisms that link adversity with mental illness. Adoption of a developmental approach to research in this field will yield greater insights into the factors that tie adversity to poor emotion function across a lifespan. In this review, we focus on recent findings that have used a developmental approach in the examination of mental health and early adversity. These studies are notable in that, across numerous species, they converge on the idea that early adversity leads to accelerated maturation of emotion circuits in the brain and in the behaviors supported by these regions. We propose that these ‘stress acceleration’ effects are evidence of early system adaptation.
Introduction
It has long been recognized that early experiences establish the foundation for life long mental health. Arguably the most important of these early experiences is the ongoing parent-child relationship. Indeed, being born into and brought up within a stable home can be a powerful factor contributing to mental wellbeing [e.g., 1]. However, the opposite is also true; dysfunctional early caregiving (such as neglect, parental mental illness, and familial violence) is strongly associated with mental illness, accounting for up to 45% of child-onset, and 32% of adult-onset mental health disorders (including anxiety, mood and psychotic disorders) [2, 3]. While the relationship between early experiences of adversity and mental illness is often observed, the mechanisms linking these experiences are not well articulated. Hence, understanding how early experiences get ‘under the skin’ to influence an individual across his/her lifespan is one of the central goals of research in the affective neurosciences.
One predominant perspective on the link between early adversity and mental illness is that such early experiences impair brain function, as children with histories of adversity reliably perform more poorly on tests of language and cognition [4-6]. However, numerous theories now predict that, rather than leading to general deficits or delays, adversity may in fact lead to a reprioritization of developmental strategy away from a neotenous state (which favors a slow developmental pace and prolonged childhood [7]) and towards more adult-like functioning within fear/stress-related domains [8-13]. For example, many theories operating within an evolutionary biology framework (including Life History Theory, Psychosocial Acceleration Hypothesis, Child Development Theory, and the Adaptive Calibration Model) posit that our evolutionary history selected for plasticity in developmental trajectories, such that early experiences could program the length of childhood and the age of pubertal transition [9, 10, 12, 13]. Specifically, these theories suggest that slower developmental strategies would be advantageous in conditions of low stress, allowing the individual to absorb information from the social and family environment early in life that may increase survival and parenting skills before transitioning into the reproductive stage of development. In contrast, faster developmental strategies allowing for earlier reproduction are proposed to increase reproductive success and fitness in contexts of adversity where long-term survival is uncertain. Indeed, in support of these theories, there is considerable evidence demonstrating that the age of pubertal maturation is negatively associated with levels of stress in the early environment [e.g., reviewed in 9].
The frameworks discussed above are persuasive in their description of accelerated patterns of development as adaptations to contexts of early adversity (as opposed to traditional views that stress diminishes functionality). However, the mechanisms behind how such altered trajectories are related to the higher rates of mental illness seen in adversity-exposed populations remain elusive. The fact that caregiving adversity is associated with both accelerated development of emotion behavior/neurobiology, as well as increased risk for mental illness compellingly suggests that the two outcomes may be mechanistically related. Our goal in the current paper is to provide a neurobiological framework (the Stress Acceleration Hypothesis) within which to conceptualize such stress-induced accelerations, particularly those that occur within stress and fear systems. Using his framework, we then consider whether the early changes in trajectories of affective neurocircuitry are associated with later emergences of mental health disorders. Although these links have yet to be established within a single study over developmental time, the theory attempts to provide a mechanistic link between early adversity and mental illness (e.g., depression, anxiety) that is established in the literature [2]. We begin the paper by reviewing behavioral, then neural, studies of stress acceleration effects on fear systems, before putting forward the Stress Acceleration Hypothesis and discussing how accelerated outcomes may be mechanistically linked to mental health/illness.
Behavioural Studies Demonstrating Stress Acceleration Effects
While the development of emotional learning and emotion regulation has been investigated for some time in rodent studies [e.g., 18], there are far fewer examples of research explicitly investigating the interaction between rearing environments and the maturation of emotion learning in any species. Interestingly, those studies that have focused on that interaction report that rearing adversity has significant consequences for two forms of emotion learning with clear clinical implications – fear conditioning and extinction. In the first studies to investigate the effects of rearing adversity on fear learning, standard-reared infant pups were compared to pups raised in an impoverished home cage with reduced nesting material [16]. This impoverished environment affects maternal behaviors, leading to fragmented care, and often, pup maltreatment [19]. When the non-stressed pups were later exposed to threat conditioning (odor-shock pairings) as infants (i.e., on postnatal day [P] 10), they exhibited an age appropriate paradoxical approach response towards to the shock-paired odor (a behavior consistent with attachment). By P12, a developmental transition occurs whereby non-stressed pups begin to exhibit avoidance responses to shock-associated odors. Critically, that behavioral transition from odor approach to odor avoidance occurred much earlier in development if pups had been exposed to rearing adversity (reduced nesting material), suggesting that rearing adversity accelerated the developmental trajectory of threat responses displayed towards learned aversive stimuli [16].
While the studies just described examined the effect of rearing adversity on the expression of threat behavior to conditioned aversive cues, other studies have reported that early life adversity also affects the retention of such conditioned associations. For example, rats exposed to aversive early rearing (involving either maternal-separation, repeated foot-shock, hypothermia, or restraint) before explicit fear conditioning in infancy exhibited longer lasting fear associations than their non-stressed peers [20, 21]. Indeed, evidence also exists for accelerated development of appetitive memory retention [e.g., 20], but extensive review of this evidence is beyond the scope of the review. Enhanced retention for multiple conditioned associations in young animals is particularly noteworthy, as memory in early life is typically characterized by very rapid forgetting – a phenomenon known as infantile amnesia [see 22, for a review]. Hence, it appears that early adversity accelerated the trajectory of memory development beyond the period of infantile amnesia, allowing for adult-like memory retention in infancy.
Inhibitory learning too appears to develop faster following early exposure to stress. While fear inhibition (i.e., extinction) of learned fear responses appears to be relatively ‘adult-like’ in early life, relapse behavior following extinction develops more gradually, becoming apparent only at post-weaning ages [see 23, for a review]. In rats exposed to maternal separation rearing, however, or those reared by a mother treated with the stress hormone corticosterone, the developmental trajectory of extinction-relapse was shifted to the left [24-26]. In other words, stressed rats began to exhibit contextually mediated relapse (i.e., renewal and reinstatement) of extinguished fear as early as infancy. Importantly, context mediation of fear extinction is known to involve interactions between the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and amygdala [for reviews see 27, 28] – neural interactions that are not involved in extinction under typical rearing conditions in infancy [for a review see 23]. This brings up the intriguing possibility that early adversity leads to premature development of neural circuits involved in mature forms of emotional responding – particularly amygdala-mPFC interactions, possibly via stress prematurely activating these structures Indeed, there is now a growing body of evidence in humans and rodents (reviewed in the next section) suggesting that this is true.
In sum, the behavioral literature suggests that emotional development in individuals exposed to early adversity, especially parental deprivation, appears to be fast-tracked, such that independent forms of emotion regulation (i.e., not parent regulated) occur earlier in development. Broadly defined, these data fit with many of the theories that use the evolutionary-biology framework by suggesting that individual developmental trajectories respond to environmental context is ways that may confer a survival advantage to that individual. More specifically, however, these data fit with the stress-acceleration framework by demonstrating how mature forms of emotion learning and expression emerge following adversities known to impact the stress response system – parental deprivation. The impact of such adversity on the neural regions involved in stress and emotion is reviewed in the next section.
Neural Studies Demonstrating Stress Acceleration Effects
Some of the first studies to investigate the effect of early adversity on the neural circuits involved in fear learning and extinction focused on the amygdala. Critically, across many species it was shown that rearing adversity had dramatic effects of amygdala structure and function that appear to indicate accelerated development. In terms of amygdala function, previous studies have shown that stress leads to precocious involvement of the amygdala in fear learning. For example, the transition from approach to avoidance responses to shock-paired odors that occurs across typical rat development is paralleled by a change in amygdala involvement in threat conditioning [16]. Critically, early adversity accelerated not only the behavioral responses to conditioning (i.e., from approach to avoidance), but also the involvement of the amygdala, such that after stress, odor-shock learning in infancy elicited precocious avoidance and amygdala activation [16]. In terms of structural development following stress, studies in juvenile rodents have shown that stress leads to precocious myelination of axons in the basolateral amygdala [29] and studies in humans have shown that maternal deprivation in early in life (i.e., previous institutionalization; PI) is associated with atypically large amygdala volumes in children (5-16 years) that appeared to reflect earlier structural maturation [30]. Similarly, 10-year-old children exposed to maternal depression since birth had larger amygdale than same aged peers without depressed mothers [31]. Also, high rates of childhood adversity were associated with a larger (i.e., more mature) left amygdala volume in early adolescence (mean age 12.64 years) [32]. However, some studies have also reported smaller amygdala volumes following adversity (e.g., abuse, institutional neglect) in adolescents (mean age 11-12 years) [33, 34] and in preschoolers (3-6 years) [35]. Hence, further studies are required to determine the precise manner in which different adversities may affect amygdala volume across development.
In addition to affecting structural and functional development of the amygdala, the emotion center of the brain, rearing adversity is also know to affect the development of neural regions that can regulate amygdala activity – the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and hippocampus. For example, studies in humans have shown that parental deprivation accelerates the functional development of the mPFC in children, such that amygdala-mPFC interactions are more adult-like following deprivation experiences [17]. Specifically, previous studies across typical development had shown that functional connectivity between amygdala and mPFC changes from being positive in childhood (6-10 years), to becoming negative inadolescence and adulthood (11-17 years) [36]. That switch in connectivity valence occurred earlier following previous institutionalization, such that the negative connectivity pattern was evident already in childhood in the previously institutionalized group [36]. Critically, within the previously institutionalized group, those exhibiting the more mature pattern of connectivity appeared to have less anxiety than those with the immature pattern, suggesting that early maturation of amygdala-mPFC functional connectivity may confer emotion regulation advantages, at least in the short-term. Although fear learning and extinction have not been explicitly assessed in the PI youths, one might speculate, based on the neural acceleration patterns reported, that emotion-learning behaviors might also look more ‘adult-like’ in this group. Interestingly, studies that have examined structural changes in the prefrontal cortex following adversity have often reported smaller volumes in children (11-12 years) [33, 34], demonstrating a possible dissociation of the effects of stress on structural and functional development (i.e., accelerated functional maturation, possibly at the cost of impaired structural integrity).
In terms of the hippocampus, it appears that rearing adversity may have differing effects on structural and functional development. For example, numerous studies have reported that children and adolescents (3-12 years) exposed to neglect, abuse, conditions of poverty and depressive maternal symptomatology from birth had similar or smaller hippocampal volumes to typically reared children/adolescents [31, 35, 37]. In contrast, studies in rodents have shown that the experience of early social isolation causes an early transition in the molecular signaling cascade for long-term potentiation (LTP) in the hippocampal CA1 region in infancy [38]. Importantly, LTP is the cellular substrate underlying learning and memory, and hippocampal CA1 activity is known to be particularly involved in learning contextual associations [39]. Hence, although effects of early adversity on hippocampal structure appear to involve reduced volume (that may indicate delayed development or even damage), the cellular mechanisms supporting adultlike emotion learning, as well as the emergence of numerous forms of learning known to rely on hippocampal function (i.e., contextual conditioning, contextually mediated renewal of extinguished aversions; [24, 25, 40]) do appear to be developmentally accelerated by stress.
Similar to the behavioral data demonstrating ‘acceleration’ phenotypes following early adversity, the neural data just discussed also supports the idea that development is faster paced following adversity. Specifically, brain regions important for detecting and responding to stress and threat (the amygdala, prefrontal cortex, and hippocampus) all appear to be more mature (either functionally and/or structurally) following adversity. The fact that adversity in the rearing environment has been linked to both accelerated developmental patterns as well as mental illness across numerous literatures provides strong initial support for the proposal that a mechanistic link exists between the two processes. In the next section we discuss our hypothesis that the accelerated developmental patterns are causal in creating mental health risk.
The Stress Acceleration Hypothesis
The studies discussed above converge on the findings that, across species, early life caregiving stress prematurely activates and accelerates the maturation of various neural structures, functional profiles, and molecular compositions, in brain regions important for emotion expression, and associative learning and memory. We propose that accelerated phenotypes emerge because stress experienced early in life may prematurely activate the core circuitry of emotional learning and reactivity [11]. That is, the acceleration of limbic development following early stress may rely on an activity-dependent process. Importantly, we hypothesize that this accelerated development, while meeting immediate emotional demands (i.e., emotional regulation in parental absence), may have long-term consequences on the circuit integrity and functioning by altering developmental plasticity. This adaptation, may increase the vulnerability of the individual to developing fear-related psychopathology in adulthood. Indeed, decreased plasticity in adulthood between amygdala and prefrontal regions has recently been shown to increase the risk for an anxious phenotype in a rodent model [44]. Taken together, the argument follows that prematurely terminating the plasticity afforded by a developmental sensitive period for emotional behaviors may have significant consequences on emotion regulation abilities in adulthood.
At a behavioral level, we argue that this accelerated maturation may reflect a reprioritization of developmental trajectories in favor of emotional systems, and associative learning and memory, in ways that confer a survival advantage to the developing offspring. Specifically, it may be adaptive to move from a state of parent-regulation to self-regulation earlier in environments characterized by absent or inconsistent parental care. Put another way, the quantity and quality of parental care may act as an environmental signal for the pace of offspring maturation within circumscribed emotion systems [8, 41].
Consistent with an adaptation interpretation, it has been shown that an accelerated amygdala-mPFC phenotype following parental deprivation was associated with less anxiety during childhood [17]. However, whether that short-term benefit is maintained across time, and whether there are costs associated with early emotion system maturation, remain relatively unknown. Importantly, a long childhood period has been proposed as critical for the development and practice of behavioral skills and strategies during play that can later be used to aid survival and reproduction [42]. Also, a prolonged period of cortical development is associated with greater general intelligence [43]. In terms of emotion circuitry, we have recently suggested that a period of immaturity is required so that parent-assisted emotion regulation can help mold the circuitry into its stable adult state [41]. In this hypothesis, early termination of the immature period would shift the circuit into a stable state of function before it had acquired the characteristics needed for adequate self-regulatory control. In support of this hypothesis, evidence now exists in rodents demonstrating that the emotion circuitry of young individuals is less responsive to environmental regulation following early adversity [16]. Hence, although early emergence of self-regulation may be advantageous in contexts of parental absence, there are likely numerous costs to this developmental trajectory. Further research is required to understand how such latent vulnerabilities may become manifest, both at the time of the stressor as well as later in development.
Conclusion
The studies discussed in this review strongly suggest that accelerated development of emotion behaviors and circuits is a core outcome following the experience of rearing adversity. We suggest that such accelerated development may have implications for the integrity of emotion circuits and emotion regulation later in life. It remains possible that these neural adaptations are in fact indicators of longer-term mental health resilience – a solid understanding of these adaptations continues to be investigated. However, findings from several literatures suggest that anxiety is a common long-term outcome associated with “growing up” quickly [45], an example being the increased risk for internalizing disorders in adults with a history of exhibiting early parentification behaviors due to having parents that were unable to provide optimal parenting [46]. The veracity of the model proposed in this paper will require very long-term follow-up data to fully address.
Although a comprehensive review of the mechanisms behind these acceleration effects are outside the scope of the current review, evolutionary-biology theories and data on pubertal development following adversity would indicate that both stress and sex hormones are likely to play a role. Future studies aimed at understanding the biological and psychological pathways that lead to acceleration of emotional development following rearing adversity will have a large impact on the treatment and prevention of mental illness. Although this review focused largely on cross-sectional studies, the use of longitudinal designs in future studies should go a long way in elucidating the neurobiology of these stress-accelerated effects. Such designs will also be useful in determining whether accelerated brain development is predictive of psychopathology risk or resilience. We hope that the current paper can provide the scaffolding and motivation for future research aimed at addressing stress effects on brain development.
Highlights.
Early caregiving adversity is associated with poor mental health outcomes
Across species, adversity accelerates the developmental trajectory of emotion circuits
Accelerated development may reflect an adaptive strategy for low parental care contexts
Changes to the pace of development may change the nature of mature phenotypes
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant R01MH091864 (to N. Tottenham), by the Dana Foundation (to N. Tottenham), and National Health and Medical Research Council Early Career Fellowship 1091571 (to B. Callaghan).
Footnotes
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest to report.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Vanderbilt-Adriance E, et al. Child, family, and community protective factors in the development of children's early conduct problems. Family Relations. 2015;64:64–79. doi: 10.1111/fare.12105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Green JG, et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67(2):113–123. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.McLaughlin KA, et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication II: associations with persistence of DSM-IV disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67(2):124–132. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Van Ijzendoorn MH, Juffer F, Poelhuis CWK. Adoption and cognitive development: a meta-analytic comparison of adopted and nonadopted children's IQ and school performance. Psychological bulletin. 2005;131:301–316. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hostinar CE, et al. Associations between early life adversity and executive function in children adopted internationally from orphanages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109:7208–17212. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121246109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ayoub C, et al. Cognitive skill performance among young children living in poverty: Risk, change, and the promotive effects of Early Head Start. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2009;24:289–305. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gould SJ. Ontogeny and phylogeny. USA: Harvard University Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
- 8**.Frankenhuis WE, de Weerth C. Does Early-Life Exposure to Stress Shape or Impair Cognition? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2013;22:407–412. This paper clearly expresses how early life exposures to stress can, in certain situations, lead to changes in cognition that are better characterised as ‘adaptations’ rather than ‘impairments’. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ellis BJ. Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: an integrated life history approach. Psychological bulletin. 2004;130:920–958. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Belsky J. The development of human reproductive strategies progress and prospects. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2012;21:310–316. [Google Scholar]
- 11.McEwen BS, Gianaros PJ. Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1186:190–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05331.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Del Giudice M, Ellis BJ, Shirtcliff EA. The adaptive calibration model of stress responsivity. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2011;35:1562–1592. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Belsky J, Steinberg L, Draper P. Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child development. 1991;62:647–670. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01558.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14*.Callaghan BL, Richardson R. Early experiences and the development of emotional learning systems in rats. Biology of Mood and Anxiety Disorders. 2013;3(8) doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-3-8. Within this paper, a comprehensive review of human and non-human animal studies supporting the stress-acceleration hypothesis is provided. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Li S, Callaghan BL, Richardson R. Infantile amnesia: forgotten but not gone. Learning & Memory. 2014;21(3):135–139. doi: 10.1101/lm.031096.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Moriceau S, et al. Early-life stress disrupts attachment learning: the role of amygdala corticosterone, locus ceruleus corticotropin releasing hormone, and olfactory bulb norepinephrine. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(50):15745–15755. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4106-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17**.Gee D, et al. Early Developmental Emergence of Mature Human Amygdala-Prefrontal Phenotype following Maternal Deprivation: Evidence of Stress-Induced Acceleration. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110:15638–15643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1307893110. This paper provides a clear example of stress leading to acceleration of connectivity between neural regions which support adult-like emotion regulation – the amygdala and prefrontal cortex network; an effect which was shown to be mediated by individual levels of the stress hormone – corticosterone. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Campbell BA, Campbell EH. Retention and extinction of learned fear in infant and adult rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1962;55(1):1–8. doi: 10.1037/h0049182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ivy AS, et al. Dysfunctional nurturing behavior in rat dams with limited access to nesting material: a clinically relevant model for early-life stress. Neuroscience. 2008;154:1132–1142. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.04.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Haroutunian V, Riccio DC. Reduction of ontogenetic retention decrements in rats by pretraining stressful experiences. Journal of comparative and physiological psychology. 1979;93:501–511. doi: 10.1037/h0077567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Callaghan B, Richardson R. The effect of adverse rearing environments on persistent memories in young rats: removing the brakes on infant fear memories. Translational Psychiatry. 2012;2(7):e138. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Callaghan BL, Richardson R. The elusive engram: What can infantile amnesia tell us about memory? Trends in Neuroscience. 2014;37:47–53. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kim JH, Richardson R. New findings on extinction of conditioned fear early in development: theoretical and clinical implications. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67:297–303. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Callaghan BL, Richardson R. Maternal separation results in early emergence of adult-like fear and extinction learning in infant rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2011;125(1):20–28. doi: 10.1037/a0022008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Cowan CSM, Callaghan B, Richardson R. Acute early-life stress results in early emergence of adult-like fear retention and extinction relapse in infant rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2013;125:703–711. doi: 10.1037/a0034118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Callaghan BL, Richardson R. Early emergence of adult-like fear extinction in the rat following chronic corticosterone treatment of mother or pups. Behavioural Neuroscience. doi: 10.1037/bne0000009. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bouton ME, et al. Contextual and temporal modulation of extinction: behavioral and biological mechanisms. Biological psychiatry. 2006;60:352–360. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kalisch R, et al. Context-dependent human extinction memory is mediated by a ventromedial prefrontal and hippocampal network. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:9503–9511. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2021-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ono M, et al. Early weaning induces anxiety and precocious myelination in the anterior part of the basolateral amygdala of male Balb/c mice. Neuroscience. 2008;156:1103–1110. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.07.078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Tottenham N, et al. Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Developmental Science. 2010;13(1):46–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lupien SJS, Parent ea. Larger amygdala but no change in hippocampal volume in 10-year-old children exposed to maternal depressive symptomatology since birth. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(34):14324–14329. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105371108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32*.Whittle S, et al. Childhood Maltreatment and Psychopathology Affect Brain Development During Adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2013;52(9):940–952.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.06.007. This study demonstrates that stress acceleration effects may also be evident in early adolescence following exposure to childhood treatment and subsequent psychopathology. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hanson JL, et al. Early Stress Is Associated with Alterations in the Orbitofrontal Cortex: A Tensor-Based Morphometry Investigation of Brain Structure and Behavioral Risk. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:7466–7472. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0859-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hanson JL, et al. Structural variations in prefrontal cortex mediate the relationship between early childhood stress and spatial working memory. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:7917–7925. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0307-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Luby J, et al. The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: the mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA pediatrics. 2013;167:1135–1142. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.3139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gee DG, et al. A developmental shift from positive to negative connectivity in human amygdala-prefrontal circuitry. J Neurosci. 2013;33(10):4584–93. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3446-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hanson JL, et al. Behavioral problems after early life stress: contributons of the hippocampus and amygdala. Biological Psychiatry. 2015;4:314–323. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Huang CC, et al. Neonatal isolation accelerates the developmental switch in the signalling cascades for long-term potentiation induction. The Journal of Physiology. 2005;569(3):789–799. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.098160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Maren S, Phan KL, Liberzon I. The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning extinction and psychopathology. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2013;14:417–428. doi: 10.1038/nrn3492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Callaghan BL, Richardson R. Early emergence of adult-like fear renewal in the developing rat after chronic corticosterone treatment of the dam or the pups. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;128(5):594. doi: 10.1037/bne0000009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41**.Callaghan B, Tottenham N. The Neuro-Environmental Loop of Plasticity: A cross-species analysis of parental effects on emotion circuitry development following typical and adverse caregiving. Neuropsychopharmacology Reviews, Accepted. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.204. In this paper we expand on the ideas presented here to speak in greater depth about the cue of ‘child independence from parents’ acting as a primary signal in a feedback loop for plasticity in emotion circuits. We argue that a small degree of parental independence opens plasticity in neural circuits implicated in emotion regulation. It is then argued that the input of parent-assisted regulation during this period of plasticity tones the circuit, allowing for increased self-regulation in the child and eventually closing the period of plasticity. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Pellegrini AD, Dupuis D, Smith PK. Play in evolution and development. Developmental Review. 2007;27:261–276. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Shaw P, Greenstein D, Lerch J, Clasen L, Lenroot R, Gogtay NEEA, Evans A, Rapaport J, Giedd J, et al. Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature. 2006;440:676–679. doi: 10.1038/nature04513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Fragale JE, Khariv V, Gregor DM, Smith IM, Jiao X, Elkabes S, Servatius RJ, Pang KC, Beck KD. Dysfunction in amygdala-prefrontal plasticity and extinction-resistant avoidance: A model for anxiety disorder vulnerability. Experimental Neurology. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2015.11.002. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Zehr JL, et al. An association of early puberty with disordered eating and anxiety in a population of undergraduate women and men. Hormones and Behavior. 2007;52:427–435. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Hooper LM, et al. Characterizing the magnitude of the relation between self - reported childhood parentification and adult psychopathology: a meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2011;67:1028–1043. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]