Abstract
Objectives
We aimed to examine whether there are sex and age differences in psychosocial risk factors of marijuana use during adolescence.
Methods
Data were drawn from 57,767 adolescents (8th and 10th graders) from the 2012–2013 Monitoring the Future study. We examined the association between socio-demographic and behavioral correlates with different frequencies of past-year marijuana use (non-use, occasional use: <10 time, frequent use: 10–39 times, and regular use: 40+ times). We further investigated whether these associations were similar for boys and girls of different ages.
Results
Overall, 20.6% of the adolescents reported past-year marijuana use: 12.1% occasional use, 4.3% frequent use, and 3.8% regular use. Girls were less likely to be frequent and regular marijuana users (frequent use: OR=0.83 [0.75, 0.93]; regular use: OR=0.41 [0.36, 0.48]) while no sex difference was noted for occasional use. Also, the odds of deviant behaviors were higher as the frequencies of marijuana use were higher. Compared to younger girls, older boys and girls had higher association between all levels of marijuana use and low self-esteem, low perceived harm, peer influence and perceived easy access. Besides, younger boys were more likely than younger girls to report an association between regular marijuana use with low self-esteem, peer influence, and perceived easy access but not with perceived low harm.
Conclusions/Importance
Findings suggest the relationship between these psychosocial correlates and frequency of marijuana involvement varies across sex and age groups. These variations ask for a nuanced approach to prevention of marijuana involvement in different groups of youth.
Keywords: marijuana use, problem behaviors, age difference, sex difference, prevention
Introduction
Marijuana is one of the most commonly abused psychoactive substances in the United States and around the world (1–4). Accumulating evidence suggests that there may be long-term effects on brain maturation during key developmental periods following heavy marijuana use in adolescence. Magnetic resonance imaging studies have found grey matter volume reduction among those who persistently use marijuana before age 17 (5). Declines of IQ scores have also been found among heavy marijuana users who started in their teens and continued into adulthood (6). In addition, some reports have suggested links between marijuana use and some mental disorders (7–11) and respiratory complications (12). In 2013, an estimated 1.7 million adolescents in the US were current marijuana users (4). Between 2008 and 2013, past-month marijuana use increased from 5.8% to 7.0% among 8th graders, from 13.8% to 18.0% among 10th graders, and from 19.4% to 22.7% among 12th graders (13). There is a need to remain vigilant and revisit risk factors and correlates of marijuana use as many countries, including the U.S., have seen changes in policies that sanction marijuana use for medical purposes and changes in social norms that support legalizing it for recreational use. Information on sex and age variations in psychosocial factors which are putative risk factors of marijuana use are important to monitor because they are instrumental for the development of targeted marijuana prevention and intervention programs.
Marijuana involvement among youth is often captured by an assay of the frequency and quantity of use. A previous study demonstrated that frequency of marijuana use is a stronger proxy than quantity of use for predicting problematic use (marijuana use disorder), and this association varied significantly by age (14). In addition, a dose-response relationship between frequency of cannabis use and psychosis liability has been proposed (15). Studies have also found that psychosocial correlates (such as decision-making) vary among individuals with different levels of marijuana use (16); so could there be sex and age differences in the relationship between a person’s socio- demographic and psychosocial characteristics and frequency of marijuana use during adolescence?
In this paper we examined the association of a number of established psychosocial and behavioral risk factors of marijuana use with frequency of marijuana use, accounting for sex and age differences in these associations. We used national samples of youth drawn from the Monitoring the Future (MTF) study (17). The risk factors examined included low self-esteem, low perceived harm, peer influence and perceived high drug availability of marijuana. These variables were selected based on past literature and behavioral models such as Problem Behavior Theory (18, 19) and the Theory of Planned Behavior (20). Both of these models find that attitudes, norms and perceived behavioral control influence behaviors. Several studies of teenage substance use behaviors have found supporting evidence for both of these theoretical models (20–23).
A number of past studies using MTF data have found strong associations between marijuana use in adolescents and self-esteem (24), perceived harm (25), peer influence (26) and perceived drug availability (27). Research also suggests that the association of these factors with marijuana use varies across sex and age groups. For instance, Cohn et. al. found differences in risk-perception across different age groups (28), and Steinberg et. al. demonstrated that adolescents of different ages held different capacities to resist peer influence (29). Also, sex was found to interact with psychosocial characteristics of marijuana use (30). Other research suggests that decision-making processes may vary among individuals with different level of marijuana use (16). Nevertheless, past research has not investigated these psychosocial factors as a function of different levels of marijuana use among boys and girls of different ages.
To address these gaps in knowledge, we analyzed data from the MTF to a) examine the socio-demographic and behavioral characteristics of adolescents in the 8th and 10th grades according to levels (as measured by frequency) of marijuana use, and b) assess how the relationship between psychosocial factors and levels of marijuana use differ by sex/age groups.
Methods
Study sample
Combined annual data from the MTF public use data files for the years 2012 and 2013 (N= 57,767) were analyzed. Since 1975, the MTF administers classroom surveys to a nationally representative sample of school attending American students in 8th and 10th graders. The survey uses a multistage, stratified sampling design with the first stage being 108 primary geographic areas and the second stage, the schools. There are four interconnected versions of the survey instrument so that a wide array of data can be collected while minimizing respondent burden. More details about MTF can be found elsewhere (3, 17). About 50–70% of originally selected schools participate in the MTF. For schools that do not participate, replacements are carefully chosen to be as similar as possible to the school being replaced. Response rates for surveys in 2012–2013 were 90–91% (8th grade) and 87–88% (10th grade). Half of the sampled adolescents (50.9%) were female, 53.9% were White, 10.5% Black, 16.6% Hispanic, and 19.0% from other races and ethnicities (percentages calculated using sample weights).
Measures
Frequency of marijuana use in the past 12 months
Frequency of marijuana use was assessed by the following question: “On how many occasions (if any) have you used marijuana (weed, pot) or hashish (hash, hash oil) … during the last 12 months?” The respondents could choose from different numbers of occasions including 0, 1–2, 3–4, 6–9, 10–19, 20–39, and ≧ 40. Responses were re-categorized into the following of levels of marijuana use: no use (0 times), occasional use (1–9 times), frequent use (10–39 times), and regular use (more than 40 times).
Socio-demographic characteristics
Since the MTF does not ask about age, we used grade level as a proxy for age with 8th grade (ages 13–14) categorized as the young age for this study, and 10th grade (ages 15–16 years) categorized as older age. We further categorized our participants into four groups by sex and age (younger girls, younger boys, older girls, older boys). Other socio-demographic variables for the analyses included race/ethnicity (Non-Hispanic White, Black, Hispanic, Other), region (Northeast, Northcentral, West, South) and survey year.
Past-year deviant behaviors
Past-year deviant behaviors included gang fights, hurting others, stealing things, trespassing and selling drugs. Participants who reported taking part in a fight where a group of friends were against another group were categorized as having been in a gang fight. Similarly, hurting others, stealing things, trespassing and selling drugs were defined by having “hurt someone badly enough to need bandages or a doctor?”, “taken something not belonging to you worth over $50?”, “gone into some house or building when you weren’t supposed to be there?” and “sold an illegal drug?”, respectively. Those who reported yes to any one of the questions were categorized as having deviant behaviors in the past year.
Psychosocial factors: self-esteem, peer influence, perceived harm and perceived drug availability
Self-esteem was assessed by eliciting level of agreement with the statement “I feel that my life is not very useful.” Low self-esteem was operationalized by the response “agree or mostly agree” to this question. Perceived harm of marijuana was assessed by the question: “How much do you think people risk harming themselves (physically or in other ways) if they smoke marijuana regularly? (1) no risk, (2) slight risk, (3) moderate risk, (4) great risk.” Perceived low harm was operationalized by the response “no or slight risk” to this question. Peer influence was assessed by the question: “How many of your friends would you estimate … smoke marijuana or hashish? (1) none, (2) a few, (3) some, (4) most, (5) all.” High peer influence was operationalized by the response “most or all” to this question. Perceived drug availability was assessed by the question: “How difficult do you think it would be for you to get each of the following types of drugs, if you wanted some marijuana? (1) probably impossible, (2) very difficult, (3) fairly difficult, (4) fairly easy, (5) very easy.” Perceived easy access was operationalized by the response “fairly easy” or “very easy” to this question.
Statistical analyses
Analyses accounted for the complex multistage sample design, and data were weighted to adjust for differential selection probabilities using Stata 12.0 (31). We used Taylor series estimation methods to obtain proper standard error estimates for the cross-tabulations and logistic regressions. All percentages reported are weighted by study weights.
First, we created basic contingency tables and conducted multivariate logistic regression analyses to examine the association between different levels of marijuana use and socio-demographic variables, and deviant behaviors. Next the associations between different levels of marijuana use and psychosocial correlates (low self-esteem, peer influence, perceived low harm and perceived easy access) were examined. Then developmental differences (sex and age subgroups) in the association between these psychosocial factors and different levels of marijuana use were explored. This work was based on analyses of publicly available de-identified data deemed exempt from review by the Institutional Review Board.
Results
Characteristics of participants by frequency of marijuana use
The sample consisted of 57,767 adolescents. Overall, 20.6% of the adolescents had used marijuana in the past year: 12.1% (n=7,160) reported occasional marijuana use, 4.3% (n=2,524) frequent use, and 3.8% (n=2,221) regular use. We found distinct socio-demographic and behavioral profiles according to the level of past-year marijuana use. As seen in in Table 1, compared to non-marijuana users, marijuana users were generally more likely to be older (10th grade). Girls were less likely than boys to use marijuana frequently or regularly while no sex difference was seen for occasional use. Participants who reported occasional use were more likely to be Black or Hispanic compared to Whites, while no racial/ethnic differences were observed among frequent or regular users.
Table 1.
No use (0 occasion) (N=45,862) | Occasional use (1–9 occasions) (N=7,160) | Frequent use (10–39 occasions) (N=2,524) | Regular use (≧40 occasions) (N=2,221) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||
% | % | OR (95% CI) | % | OR (95% CI) | % | OR (95% CI) | |
| |||||||
Sex | |||||||
Male | 48.1 | 48.2 | 1.00 | 52.6 | 1.00 | 69.1 | 1.00 |
Female | 51.9 | 51.8 | 0.99(0.91,1.09) | 47.4 | 0.83(0.75,0.93)+ | 30.9 | 0.41(0.36,0.48)± |
| |||||||
Grade level | |||||||
8th | 56.8 | 35.7 | 1.00 | 26.0 | 1.00 | 26.0 | 1.00 |
10th | 43.2 | 64.4 | 2.37(2.01,2.79)± | 74.0 | 3.75(2.86,4.92)± | 74.0 | 4.93(3.53,6.90)± |
| |||||||
Race | |||||||
White | 67.1 | 59.3 | 1.00 | 62.3 | 1.00 | 66.8 | 1.00 |
Black | 13.2 | 15.0 | 1.28(1.02,1.61)* | 13.4 | 1.10(0.79,1.53) | 12.0 | 0.91(0.66,1.26) |
Hispanic | 19.8 | 25.7 | 1.47(1.14,1.91)* | 24.4 | 1.33(0.56,1.85) | 21.3 | 1.08(0.83,1.41) |
| |||||||
Region | |||||||
Northeast | 18.5 | 17.4 | 1.00 | 20.2 | 1.00 | 17.0 | 1.00 |
Northcentral | 24.1 | 20.2 | 0.89(0.78,1.01) | 20.0 | 0.76(0.67,0.86)+ | 22.3 | 1.00(0.85,1.18) |
South | 35.1 | 36.5 | 1.10(0.95,1.28) | 34.3 | 0.89(0.72,1.10) | 36.4 | 1.12(0.78,1.62) |
West | 22.3 | 25.9 | 1.23(1.11,1.37)+ | 25.4 | 1.04(0.89,1.22) | 24.3 | 1.18(0.89,1.22) |
| |||||||
Gang Fight | |||||||
No | 88.4 | 79.0 | 1.00 | 67.4 | 1.00 | 51.5 | 1.00 |
Yes | 11.6 | 21.0 | 2.73(2.41,3.09)± | 32.6 | 3.40(2.99,3.87)± | 48.5 | 5.06(3.69,6.93)± |
| |||||||
Attacked Others | |||||||
No | 91.0 | 79.7 | 1.00 | 74.7 | 1.00 | 87.8 | 1.00 |
Yes | 9.0 | 20.3 | 2.56(1.96,3.47)± | 25.3 | 3.42(2.73,4.28)± | 12.2 | 6.30(4.28,8.25)± |
| |||||||
Trespassed buildings | |||||||
No | 84.4 | 63.9 | 1.00 | 55.0 | 1.00 | 52.2 | 1.00 |
Yes | 15.6 | 36.2 | 3.06(2.66,3.53)± | 45.0 | 4.42(3.66,5.36)± | 47.8 | 4.96(3.98,6.17)± |
| |||||||
Stole things worth >$50 | |||||||
No | 96.1 | 87.2 | 1.00 | 79.4 | 1.00 | 69.2 | 1.00 |
Yes | 3.9 | 12.8 | 3.60(3.16,4.09)± | 20.6 | 6.37(4.62,8.78)± | 30.8 | 10.94(7.92,15.11)± |
| |||||||
Sold drugs | |||||||
No | 99.0 | 89.3 | 1.00 | 68.2 | 1.00 | 46.1 | 1.00 |
Yes | 1.0 | 10.7 | 12.78(5.19,11.00)± | 31.8 | 49.82(35.89,69.16)± | 53.9 | 124.88(79.45,196.30)± |
p<0.05,
p <0.01,
p< 0.001
Generally speaking, we found the odds of reporting deviant behaviors were higher as the frequencies of marijuana use were higher. For example, the odds ratio for the association of stealing and marijuana use almost doubled for each increase in level of use compared to non-use (occasional use: 3.60[3.16, 4.09], frequent use: 6.37[4.62, 8.78], regular marijuana use: 10.94[7.92, 15.11]). Similar patterns were observed for reports of gang fights, attacking others, and trespassing. Notably, the odds of selling drugs were exceptionally high among participants for all levels of marijuana use.
Sex and age differences by frequency of marijuana use
We examined levels of frequency of marijuana use based on four sex and age groups (younger girls, younger boys, older girls, and older boys), as illustrated in Figure 1. For younger girls and boys, the prevalence of occasional and frequent marijuana use was similar; in contrast, regular marijuana use was almost twice as common in younger boys as in younger girls [2.0% vs. 1.1%]. Occasional marijuana use was similarly common across the sexes and a similar pattern of higher prevalence with age was noted for both boys and girls. The prevalence of occasional use was almost two times higher in older girls (16.4%) compared with younger girls (8.3%) and in older boys (15.7%) compared to younger boys (8.3%). Also, the prevalence of regular marijuana use was exceptionally high in older boys (8.8%), followed by older girls (3.6%), and younger boys (2.0%) and girls (2.1%).
Association of low self-esteem, peer influence, perceived low harm and perceived easy access with frequency of marijuana use
Low self-esteem, perceived low harm, peer influence and perceived easy access were significantly associated with all levels of marijuana use (Figure 2 and Appendix Table 1). In adjusted logistic regression models (Appendix Table 1), individuals with low self-esteem reported slightly increased odds of all levels of marijuana use compared to non-users. Nevertheless, in perceived low harm, the adjusted odds ratio increased across a gradient with the frequency of marijuana use. A similar pattern was observed in the measures of peer influence and perceived easy access.
Association of low self-esteem, peer influence, perceived low harm and perceived easy access with frequency of marijuana use by sex and age
We have observed a general pattern of the association between different levels of marijuana use and psychosocial correlates in adolescents of different sex and age (Table 2). Compared to younger girls, older boys and girls were more likely to report association between low self-esteem, low perceived harm, peer influence and perceived easy access with all levels of marijuana use. Younger boys were more likely than younger girls to report an association between low self-esteem, peer influence, and perceived easy access with regular marijuana use but not in perceived low harm.
Table 2.
Occasional use (1–9 occasions) | Frequent use (10–39 occasions) | Regular use (≧40 occasions) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||
Wt% | aOR | Wt% | aOR | Wt% | aOR | |
| ||||||
Low self-esteem | ||||||
Younger girls | 25.0 | 1.00 | 19.7 | 1.00 | 9.7 | 1.00 |
Younger boys | 15.5 | 1.08(0.67,1.76) | 9.2 | 0.91(0.46,1.80) | 16.2 | 3.33(1.18,9.43)* |
Older girls | 36.5 | 2.14(1.71,2.68)± | 39.7 | 3.37(1.67,6.80)+ | 27.1 | 5.85(3.11,10.98)± |
Older boys | 23.0 | 2.00(1.14,3.50)± | 31.5 | 3.73(2.50,5.57)± | 47.1 | 13.30(4.87,36.33)+ |
| ||||||
Perceived low harm | ||||||
Younger girls | 16.4 | 1.00 | 12.0 | 1.00 | 7.1 | 1.00 |
Younger boys | 17.7 | 0.98(0.78,1.23) | 13.2 | 0.87(0.67,1.13) | 12.6 | 1.53(0.79,2.95) |
Older girls | 32.2 | 2.43(1.85,3.20)+ | 33.5 | 3.06(1.93,4.86)+ | 22.5 | 3.65(2.58,5.17)± |
Older boys | 33.1 | 1.89(1.39,2.58)+ | 41.2 | 2.94(2.03,4.25)± | 57.9 | 7.33(5.55,9.67)± |
| ||||||
Peer influence | ||||||
Younger girls | 18.8 | 1.00 | 13.8 | 1.00 | 6.2 | 1.00 |
Younger boys | 12.6 | 0.98(0.88,1.08) | 11.7 | 0.96(0.64,1.43) | 11.7 | 2.72(1.83,4.03)+ |
Older girls | 40.7 | 1.41(1.30,1.53)± | 37.2 | 1.50(1.11,2.04)* | 25.1 | 2.65(1.83,3.84)+ |
Older boys | 28.0 | 1.12(0.88,1.44) | 37.4 | 1.83(1.29,2.59)+ | 57.0 | 7.47(4.75,11.75)± |
| ||||||
Easy Access | ||||||
Younger girls | 16.9 | 1.00 | 12.1 | 1.00 | 6.9 | 1.00 |
Younger boys | 15.1 | 1.01(0.91,1.13) | 11.8 | 0.99(0.77,1.27) | 12.9 | 2.05(1.22,3.45)* |
Older girls | 36.5 | 1.66(1.32,2.09)+ | 36.1 | 2.20(1.73,2.79)± | 24.1 | 2.68(1.66,4.33)+ |
Older boys | 31.6 | 1.63(1.26,2.12)+ | 40.1 | 2.86(2.18,3.74)± | 56.1 | 7.15(4.20,12.17)± |
Adjusted logistic regression was adjusted for race, education, region, deviant behaviors and year.
p<0.05,
p <0.01,
p< 0.001
Discussion
There were three main findings from this study. First, there were distinct socio-demographic and behavioral profiles of adolescents based on the frequency of past-year marijuana use. For instance, older adolescents were generally more likely than younger ones to use marijuana at all levels. Also, boys were more likely to use marijuana frequently or regularly as compared to girls while there was no gender difference observed in occasional use. Second, prevalence of perceived low harm, peer influence and perceived easy access increased as frequency of marijuana use goes up. No such gradient was found in low self-esteem. Third, older adolescents were more likely than younger girls to have association between the examined psychosocial correlates (low self-esteem, peer influence, perceived low harm, perceived easy access) and all levels of marijuana use. However, younger boys were more likely than younger girls to have association between regular marijuana use and low self-esteem, peer influence, and perceived easy access.
The finding of differences in socio-demographic profiles of adolescents who use marijuana according to levels of use offers potentially significant clinical and public health implications. A previous study indicated male-female differences in prevalence of marijuana use decreased from 1999 to 2013 (32); our study using the latest MTF data further showed that boys were more likely to engage in heavy marijuana use (frequent and regular use) than girls, while no sex difference was observed in occasional users. As animal studies showed female rats more sensitive to the rewarding effects of marijuana (33) and human studies showed females developed marijuana use disorder more quickly than males after their marijuana initiation (34), future study to continue monitoring of occasional users of both sexes to identify the risk factors for heavier marijuana use is recommended.
Past research suggests that Black and Hispanic adolescents had surpassed Whites in reporting past-year marijuana use and marijuana use disorder (35). Our findings further suggest that minority groups were more likely to than White youth to engage in occasional use, but not in frequent and regular use. However, as previous research has found that early adolescent substance use is more strongly associated with later psychiatric disorders among Black than White adolescents (36), further study to understand the etiology of such racial differences in occasional marijuana use is crucial to prevent worse health outcomes in minority groups.
Consistent with previous studies (37), low self-esteem, perceived harm, peer influence, and perceived drug availability were all associated with marijuana use. We further found evidence suggesting that these factors played different roles in different levels of marijuana use. For instance, low self-esteem was only slightly increased in adolescents who engaged in any levels of marijuana use compared to non-users; whereas, perceived low harm, peer influence and perceived easy access had a dose-response relationship with frequency of marijuana use. Our subgroup analyses further revealed that older adolescents had higher association between low self-esteem, low perceived harm, peer influence, perceived easy access and all levels of marijuana use than young girls. In addition, younger boys were more likely than younger girls to have association between regular marijuana use with low self-esteem, peer influence, and perceived easy access but not with perceived low harm.
These findings offer potentially valuable information for the development of substance prevention or early intervention programs. Adolescents of different sex and age for whom the associations of specific psychosocial correlates with marijuana use were stronger may most benefit from interventions targeting those factors. For instance, social resistance skills programs or Life Skills Training which enhances coping abilities to peer influence (38), and normative education approaches which aims at correcting the perceived low harm and perceived easy access may be particularly helpful in reducing marijuana use among older boys and girls. In contrast, programs targeting at perceived low harm may not be as effective as strategies targeting at other psychosocial factors in preventing younger boys’ regular marijuana use. Over all, our results, in resonance with previous studies, indicate the need for an age-specific or sex-specific substance prevention program (32, 35).
Consistent with the National Survey on Drug Use and Health report (4), we found marijuana users at all frequency levels were more likely to report problematic behaviors than non-users. Furthermore, a dose-response relationship between the level of marijuana use and the odds of reporting delinquency (attacking others, gang fighting, trespassing, stealing, and selling drugs) was found, with selling drugs showing the strongest association. As drug selling is found to be linked to violence (39) and even lethal violence (40), our finding suggests that violence risk should be evaluated among adolescents with heavier marijuana use.
Several limitations to this study and of the MTF data should be noted. First, due to the cross-sectional nature of the data, no causal inferences can be made. Low self-esteem may be a risk factor contributing to marijuana use but it may also be a consequence of marijuana use and its detrimental effects on academic productivity. Second, the MTF was a school-based survey; thus school dropouts, who might have more severe substance use problems or poorer social outcome, were not included. Third, the factors were assessed using single questions, which may impact the reliability of these ratings. Furthermore, self-ratings are prone to recall or social-desirability bias. Fourth, this study did not account for the influence of socio-economic status because this information was not available in the MTF. Fifth, due to the relatively low prevalence of selling drugs in non-marijuana use population, the odds ratios for the association of this behavior with different levels of marijuana use were extremely high. Finally, grade was used as a proxy for age, and it is likely that some students in each grade were older than their peers because they were held back.
Conclusion
In the context of the above limitations, this study found distinct socio-demographic and behavioral profiles for different levels of marijuana users. Furthermore, the study found sex and age subgroup variations in the relationship between marijuana involvement and psychosocial factors including self-esteem, perceived harm, peer influence, and drug availability.
Early detection and targeted interventions have been shown to positively impact adolescents’ behavioral trajectory, steering them away from problem behaviors, such as drug use, and towards more positive behaviors (23, 38). Programs aimed at prevention of and early intervention for marijuana use can benefit from knowledge regarding socio-demographic differences in psychosocial correlates across levels of marijuana use in adolescents of different aged and sexes. For instance, targeting low perceived-harm may not be as effective as targeting other psychosocial correlates to curb regular marijuana use in younger boys.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Funding source: This study was supported by the U.S. National Institutes of Health- National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) K24 DA023186 (P.I.: Dr. Strain) and R01DA037866 (P.I.: Dr. Martins) and also by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child and Human Development R01HD060072 (P.I.: Dr. Martins). NIDA and NICHD have no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the paper for publication.
The authors gratefully acknowledge Katherine Keyes, associate professor in Department of Epidemiology in Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University, for her technical assistance. Dr. Keyes reported no conflict of interests.
Abbreviations
- MTF
Monitoring The Future
- NSDUH
National Survey on Drug Use and Health
Footnotes
Contributor’s statement
Dr. Chen conceptualized and designed the study, conducted the analyses, drafted the initial manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. Drs. Martins, Storr, Strain and Mojtabai interpreted the study results, reviewed and revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted.
Acquisition of data: The data reported herein come from the 2012–2013 Monitoring The Future (MTF) public data files available at the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Data Archive and the Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research, which are sponsored by the Office of Applied Studies, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration.
Financial disclosure: Dr. Strain has received consulting fees for medical-legal cases, and from The Oak Group, Egalet Corporation, Indivior Pharmaceuticals, and Pinney Associates. Dr. Martins has received consulting fees from Purdue Pharma in 2014. All other authors declare they have no conflicts of interest.
Potential conflict of Interest Disclosures: Other authors have no conflicts of interest relevant to this article to disclose except for the above-mentioned disclosures.
References
- 1.Goodman LGA, Brunton L, et al. Goodman & Gilman’s: The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics. 11. New York: NY: McGraw Hill; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Degenhardt L, Hall W. Extent of illicit drug use and dependence, and their contribution to the global burden of disease. Lancet. 2012;379(9810):55–70. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)61138-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Miech RA, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future national results on drug use: 1975–2012: Overview, Key Findings on Adolescent Drug Use. 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 4.SAMHSA. Results from the 2013 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Summary of National Findings. Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wilson W, Mathew R, Turkington T, Hawk T, Coleman RE, Provenzale J. Brain morphological changes and early marijuana use: a magnetic resonance and positron emission tomography study. J Addict Dis. 2000;19(1):1–22. doi: 10.1300/J069v19n01_01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Meier MH, Caspi A, Ambler A, Harrington H, Houts R, Keefe RS, et al. Persistent cannabis users show neuropsychological decline from childhood to midlife. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109(40):E2657–64. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206820109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bovasso GB. Cannabis abuse as a risk factor for depressive symptoms. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(12):2033–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.12.2033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Swain-Campbell N. Cannabis use and psychosocial adjustment in adolescence and young adulthood. Addiction. 2002;97(9):1123–35. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00103.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Moore TH, Zammit S, Lingford-Hughes A, Barnes TR, Jones PB, Burke M, et al. Cannabis use and risk of psychotic or affective mental health outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet. 2007;370(9584):319–28. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61162-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Zvolensky MJ, Lewinsohn P, Bernstein A, Schmidt NB, Buckner JD, Seeley J, et al. Prospective associations between cannabis use, abuse, and dependence and panic attacks and disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2008;42(12):1017–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Malone DT, Hill MN, Rubino T. Adolescent cannabis use and psychosis: epidemiology and neurodevelopmental models. Br J Pharmacol. 2010;160(3):511–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2010.00721.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Brook JS, Adams RE, Balka EB, Johnson E. Early adolescent marijuana use: risks for the transition to young adulthood. Psychol Med. 2002;32(1):79–91. doi: 10.1017/s0033291701004809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Miech RA, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future Survey, Overview of Findings 2013. 2013 [updated 2013; cited 2014 9/21]; Available from: http://www.drugabuse.gov/monitoring-future-survey-overview-findings-2013.
- 14.Chen K, Kandel DB, Davies M. Relationships between frequency and quantity of marijuana use and last year proxy dependence among adolescents and adults in the United States. Drug and alcohol dependence. 1997;46(1–2):53–67. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(97)00047-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ruiz-Veguilla M, Barrigon ML, Hernandez L, Rubio JL, Gurpegui M, Sarramea F, et al. Dose-response effect between cannabis use and psychosis liability in a non-clinical population: evidence from a snowball sample. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47(8):1036–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gonzalez R, Schuster RM, Mermelstein RM, Diviak KR. The role of decision-making in cannabis-related problems among young adults. Drug and alcohol dependence. 2015;154:214–21. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.06.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Miech RA, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future national results on drug use: 1975–2013: Overview, Key Findings on Adolescent Drug Use. 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 18.SL, JRJ . Problem behavior and psychosocial development: a longitudinal study of youth. New York: Academic Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Jessor R, Jessor S. A social-psychological framework for studying drug use. NIDA Res Monogr. 1980;30:102–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 1991;50(2):179–211. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Jessor R. Problem-behavior theory, psychosocial development, and adolescent problem drinking. Br J Addict. 1987;82(4):331–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1987.tb01490.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hames A, Evangeli M, Harrop C, di Forti M. Understanding cannabis use in first-episode psychosis: an application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2012;6(1):38–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-7893.2011.00316.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Malmberg M, Overbeek G, Vermulst AA, Monshouwer K, Vollebergh WA, Engels RC. The theory of planned behavior: precursors of marijuana use in early adolescence? Drug and alcohol dependence. 2012;123(1–3):22–8. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Luhtanen RK, Crocker J. Alcohol use in college students: effects of level of self-esteem, narcissism, and contingencies of self-worth. Psychol Addict Behav. 2005;19(1):99–103. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.1.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Swaim RC. Individual and school level effects of perceived harm, perceived availability, and community size on marijuana use among 12th-grade students: a random effects model. Prev Sci. 2003;4(2):89–98. doi: 10.1023/a:1022922231605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Dishion TJ, Loeber R. Adolescent marijuana and alcohol use: the role of parents and peers revisited. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 1985;11(1–2):11–25. doi: 10.3109/00952998509016846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Crost B, Guerrero S. The effect of alcohol availability on marijuana use: evidence from the minimum legal drinking age. J Health Econ. 2012;31(1):112–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2011.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cohn LD, Macfarlane S, Yanez C, Imai WK. Risk-perception: differences between adolescents and adults. Health Psychol. 1995;14(3):217–22. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.14.3.217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Steinberg L, Monahan KC. Age differences in resistance to peer influence. Dev Psychol. 2007;43(6):1531–43. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Schepis TS, Desai RA, Cavallo DA, Smith AE, McFetridge A, Liss TB, et al. Gender differences in adolescent marijuana use and associated psychosocial characteristics. Journal of addiction medicine. 2011;5(1):65–73. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0b013e3181d8dc62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 12. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Johnson RM, Fairman B, Gilreath T, Xuan Z, Rothman EF, Parnham T, et al. Past 15-year trends in adolescent marijuana use: Differences by race/ethnicity and sex. Drug and alcohol dependence. 2015;155:8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.08.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Fattore L, Spano MS, Altea S, Angius F, Fadda P, Fratta W. Cannabinoid self-administration in rats: sex differences and the influence of ovarian function. British journal of pharmacology. 2007;152(5):795–804. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjp.0707465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hernandez-Avila CA, Rounsaville BJ, Kranzler HR. Opioid-, cannabis- and alcohol-dependent women show more rapid progression to substance abuse treatment. Drug and alcohol dependence. 2004;74(3):265–72. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Wu LT, Swartz MS, Brady KT, Hoyle RH, Workgroup NA. Perceived cannabis use norms and cannabis use among adolescents in the United States. J Psychiatr Res. 2015;64:79–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.02.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gil AG, Wagner EF, Tubman JG. Associations between early-adolescent substance use and subsequent young-adult substance use disorders and psychiatric disorders among a multiethnic male sample in South Florida. American journal of public health. 2004;94(9):1603–9. doi: 10.2105/ajph.94.9.1603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.von Sydow K, Lieb R, Pfister H, Hofler M, Wittchen HU. What predicts incident use of cannabis and progression to abuse and dependence? A 4-year prospective examination of risk factors in a community sample of adolescents and young adults. Drug and alcohol dependence. 2002;68(1):49–64. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(02)00102-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Griffin KW, Botvin GJ. Evidence-based interventions for preventing substance use disorders in adolescents. Child and adolescent psychiatric clinics of North America. 2010;19(3):505–26. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2010.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Little M, Steinberg L. Psychosocial Correlates of Adolescent Drug Dealing in the Inner City: Potential Roles of Opportunity, Conventional Commitments, and Maturity. The Journal of research in crime and delinquency. 2006;43(4):357–86. doi: 10.1177/0022427806291260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.McLaughlin CR, Daniel J, Joost TF. The relationship between substance use, drug selling, and lethal violence in 25 juvenile murderers. Journal of forensic sciences. 2000;45(2):349–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.