Summary
Objective
The objective of this study was to determine whether there is an association between under‐reporting of body weight and social desirability as is found with self‐reports of energy intake.
Methods
Twenty‐seven lean individuals (mean body mass index ± standard deviation = 21.6 ± 2.0 kg m−2) and 26 individuals with obesity (mean body mass index = 35.4 ± 4.8 kg m−2) were e‐mailed a questionnaire on which they had to state their body weight and conduct a home food inventory. The next day, research team members went to their homes to weigh the participants, conduct their own food inventory and administer the Marlowe–Crowne scale for social desirability.
Results
Among individuals with obesity, lower social desirability scores were associated with a greater degree of under‐reporting body weight (r = +0.48, p < 0.02). Among lean individuals, the correlation was negative but statistically non‐significant (p = −0.22, p > 0.10). Nine individuals with obesity were extreme under‐reporters (2.27 kg or more), and eight of these had social desirability scores in the bottom half of the Marlowe–Crowne scale (p < 0.01). Six under‐reported on the home food inventory by three or more items.
Conclusions
Individuals with obesity and low social desirability scores are more likely than others to be extreme under‐reporters of body weight, possibly due to a lack of awareness of their own weight.
Keywords: Body weight, home food inventory, self‐reports, social desirability
Introduction
Nearly 40% of adults in the USA are obese (body mass index [BMI] > 30 kg m−2), and another third are overweight (BMI > 25 kg m−2) 1. The negative health outcomes are numerous and common 2. Lifestyle modification (including caloric restriction), recommendations to increase physical activity and behavioural modification are the most frequently used approaches to weight loss.
To effectively deal with this problem, health professionals rely heavily on self‐reports of diet and exercise. However, in a thorough review of the literature, Archer et al. 3 concluded that information provided through self‐reports of energy intake (e.g. questionnaires and interviews) have little resemblance to actual intake. Self‐reported data in human nutrition research have been called implausible 4. People frequently under‐report energy intake by 30% or more 5, 6, 7, with some individuals under‐reporting to such an extent that they are referred to as ‘extreme under‐reporters’ 8. Studies additionally show that individuals tend to under‐report their body weights 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 17, 18 and over‐report their heights 9, 12 and the amount of time engaged in exercise 19.
Archer et al. 3 have demonstrated that memory‐based dietary assessments are prone to several ‘intentional and unintentional distorting factors’ (p. 3) including false memories (unintentional); response bias due to social desirability (intentional); and perceptual, coding and retrieval errors. Several studies have found an association between under‐reporting of energy intake and social desirability 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, i.e. ‘the need of [individuals] to obtain approval by responding in a culturally appropriate manner’ (28, p. 353). People who have high social desirability may deceive others in self‐reports of behaviour in order to make themselves look better 29. Individuals with obesity are particularly prone to under‐report their energy intake 5, 8, 25, 30, 31, 32, 33. The extent of the under‐reporting becomes greater with each incremental increase in BMI 31 and is specific to high‐fat and high‐sugar foods; protein consumption is over‐reported 34, 35. King et al. 36 found that unlike lean individuals, many individuals with obesity even under‐report when taking an inventory (not from recall) of high‐calorie foods in their homes.
Self‐reports of body weight might be of particular interest to both researchers and practitioners because of the ease of checking accuracy. For self‐reports of body weight, errors can be due to faulty recall, not having weighed oneself in a while and differences in the calibration of the scales used by the participants and the researchers, or errors can be deliberate because of social desirability. However, little is known about the relationship between social desirability and misreporting of weight. In a review of data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, Burke and Carman 9 concluded that misreporting of body weight provided ‘robust evidence of social desirability bias’ (p. 198), but they did not actually measure social desirability. In the only study that has, Larson 12 found that the discrepancy between actual weight and self‐reported weight was significantly correlated with social desirability scores in young normal‐weight women but not in men. Individuals with obesity were not included in the study. The present study differs from the Larson study and previous studies of energy intake in that it directly compared lean individuals (BMI < 25 kg m−2) with individuals who have obesity (BMI > 30 kg m−2).
Particular focus was given to those individuals who were extreme under‐reporters of body weight. Extreme under‐reporting of body weight is unlikely to be due to chance or scale calibration differences. Previous studies found that self‐reports of body weight were lower than actual weights by a mean of about 1 to 1.8 kg 10, 11, 14, with only 20% under‐reporting by more than 2 kg 18. For the present study, under‐reporting was considered to be extreme if the difference was 2.27 kg (5 pounds) or more.
Methods
Participants
Participants were 53 college students attending a south‐east public university enrolled in a junior‐level human sexuality course. The study included two groups: lean individuals (BMI < 25 kg m−2) and individuals with obesity (BMI > 30 kg m−2). Lean participants (N = 27) were aged 19 to 29 years (20.8 ± 1.9) and included 19 women and 8 men (22 Caucasians and 5 African–Americans). Participants with obesity (N = 26) were aged 19 to 25 years plus one 45‐year‐old participant (21.6 ± 3.0) and included 15 women and 11 men (20 Caucasians, 4 African–Americans and 2 Asian Americans). All participants lived in a house or apartment (no dormitory or group housing) and did not use a meal plan and were paid $30 for participating in the study.
Procedure
The study consisted of two parts. In part 1, the participants were e‐mailed a questionnaire, which included their body weight and an inventory of high‐calorie foods kept in the home. These had to be completed before the start of part 2, a home visit the next day by two or more members of the research team. Team members weighed the participants on a calibrated scale, administered their own inventory of high‐calorie foods in the home and also administered a questionnaire to measure social desirability. The questionnaire was not identified as a measure of social desirability. Body weights were measured without the participants wearing shoes, jackets or other heavy clothing. Participants were not told the purpose of the home visit in advance. None chose to withdraw from the study. The study was approved by the university's Institutional Review Board.
Measures
Social desirability was ascertained by use of the 33‐question (18 keyed as true and 15 as false) Marlowe–Crowne scale developed by Crowne and Marlowe 28. High scores indicate a high level of social desirability, whereas low scores indicate lower levels of social desirability. This is a frequently used scale (cited over 5,280 times) for which the behaviours are ‘culturally sanctioned and approved but which are improbable of occurrence’ and have ‘minimal pathological or abnormal implications’ (28, p. 350). Example question: ‘I am always courteous, even to people who are disagreeable’.
The home food inventory was developed by King et al. 36 as an easy‐to‐administer, quick (12–20 min) inventory. It includes seven categories of common, high‐calorie foods (45 total) plus the presence of alcohol. Inter‐rater reliability was determined to be 0.93.
Results
Mean BMI (±standard deviation) was 21.6 ± 2.0 kg m−2 for the lean group and 35.4 ± 4.8 kg m−2 for the group with obesity. For 13 of the individuals with obesity, BMI was 35 kg m−2 or greater. The mean scores (±standard deviation) on the Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale were 17.7 ± 4.2 for lean participants (range: 4–25) and 15.4 ± 5.3 for the participants with obesity (range: 4–29) (t = 1.62, p > 0.10). The overall mean was 16.5 ± 4.9. Difference scores (in pounds) were calculated between self‐reported body weights and weights measured by research team members (negative signs were assigned to difference scores resulting from self‐reported weights that were less than those recorded by the research team, and positive numbers were assigned to difference scores resulting from self‐recorded weights that were greater than those recorded by the team). For the lean group, there was no statistically significant relation between the body‐weight difference scores and scores on the Marlowe–Crowne scale (r = −0.22, p > 0.10). However, for the participants with obesity, the correlation was significant: low scores on the Marlowe–Crowne scale were associated with a greater degree of under‐reporting (r = +0.48, p < 0.02).
Among the lean participants, 16 of the 27 under‐reported their body weight by 0.45 kg or more, but only three were extreme under‐reporters (−2.27, −3.12 and −5.90 kg; Table 1). Among the participants with obesity, 18 under‐reported their body weight, and nine of these were extreme under‐reporters (−2.72, −2.72, −3.63, −3.63, −4.99, −6.80, −8.16, −13.61 and −16.33 kg). Dividing the Marlowe–Crowne scale into two halves, 1–16 (below the overall mean) and 17–33 (above the overall mean), eight of the nine extreme under‐reporters among the group with obesity were in the bottom half of the scale (χ 2 = 11.87, d.f. = 3, p < 0.01, ɸ = 0.47; six women and two men). Of the 11 individuals with obesity whose score on the Marlowe–Crowne scale was in the range 17–29, seven also under‐reported their body weights, but five of them by only 0.91 kg or less. Three lean individuals over‐reported their body weight by 2.27 kg or more (+3.63, +4.54 and +4.99 kg). Among individuals with obesity, only five over‐reported their body weight, and none were extreme over‐reporters (+0.45, +0.73, +1.36, +1.81 and +1.81 kg).
Table 1.
Lean participants (N = 27) | Participants with obesity (N = 26) | |
---|---|---|
Body weight | ||
Under‐reporters (extreme)* | 16 (3) | 18 (9) |
Accurate reporters | 4 | 3 |
Over‐reporters (extreme)* | 7 (3) | 5 (0) |
Home food inventory | ||
Under‐reporters | 6 | 14 |
Accurate reporters | 3 | 1 |
Over‐reporters | 18 | 11 |
Extreme misreporting was defined as a deviation of 2.27 kg or more from a participant's actual body weight.
Errors occurred in six of the home food inventories (one team member under‐reported), and thus, it was not possible to conduct a full statistical analysis as was performed with body weight. However, among the 27 lean participants, only six (22.2%) under‐reported the number of high‐calorie food items in their homes, and only two of these under‐reported by more than two items (−3 and −5). Among the 26 participants with obesity, 14 (53.8%) under‐reported (χ 2 = 5.64, d.f. = 1, p < 0.025, ɸ = 0.33), and nine of these under‐reported by more than two items (−3, −3, −3, −4, −5, −6, −6, −10 and −11). These nine included six who had been extreme under‐reporters for body weight.
Discussion
The direction and magnitude of the correlation between accuracy of self‐reported body weights and social desirability among the lean participants (−0.22, higher social desirability scores associated with a greater degree of under‐reporting) was consistent with what has been reported previously for accuracy of self‐reports of energy intake and social desirability 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27. The latter studies included participants with a full range of BMI scores, from lean individuals to individuals with obesity (mean BMI scores ranged from 23.6 to 28.7 kg m−2), and the strongest reported correlation was −0.34 26, with others reporting correlations of −0.25 or less 20. The present results were not statistically significant, possibly due to sample size.
Larson 12 found a correlation of +0.51 between misreporting of body weight and Marlowe–Crowne scores among young, healthy women (mean BMI = 22.4 kg m−2). Misreported weights were calculated as (actual weight minus reported weight), and thus, under‐reporting resulted in a positive value, whereas in the present study, under‐reported weights had negative values in calculating the correlation coefficient. Therefore, the direction of the associations between misreported body weight and social desirability scores in the present study for lean individuals and for the Larson study were the same.
In the present study, mean BMI among individuals with obesity was 35.4 ± 4.80 kg m−2. Both the direction and magnitude of the correlation (+0.48; lower social desirability scores associated with a greater degree of under‐reporting) among individuals in this group was unexpected, not only because of the correlations found in previous studies with energy intake 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27 but also particularly so because previous studies had also found (as was found here) that the extent of under‐reporting energy intake was greater with increasing BMI 5, 8, 25, 30, 31, 32, 33. The present results cannot be due to the assessment tool for measuring social desirability as most previous studies also used the Marlowe–Crowne scale 20, 21, 22, 24, 25, 26, 27, 37, 38. Previous studies reported that the strength of the relationship between social desirability and misreporting energy intake and body weight was stronger in women than men 12, 20, 38 and that results were also influenced by level of education 21, 35. However, in the present study, the two samples did not differ in terms of education and were very similar with regard to age, gender and ethnicity. While many studies have found an association between under‐reporting of energy intake and both BMI 5, 8, 25, 30, 31, 32, 33 and social desirability 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, there is little evidence to support a relationship between BMI and social desirability. One study reported a correlation of −0.21 23 and another reported +0.18 22. No study has reported an overall statistically significant difference in social desirability between lean individuals and individuals with obesity. In the present study, the mean Marlowe–Crowne scores for the two groups (17.7 ± 4.2 for lean individuals and 15.4 ± 5.3 for individuals with obesity) did not significantly differ, and the range of scores was very similar.
Researchers have assumed that on self‐reports, individuals high in social desirability may ‘overestimate desirable traits and behaviors and underestimate undesirable ones’ 23 or ‘provide answers believed to be socially accepted’ 37. Factor analysis has revealed that misreporting because of high social desirability can result from two factors: (i) self‐deception, i.e. the respondent truly believes his or her self‐reports because he or she has an overly positive impression of himself or herself or (ii) impression management, i.e. the respondent deliberately over‐reports or under‐reports in order to deceive others 29. In a review of studies of self‐reported body weight, Polivy et al. 16 concluded that women who were overweight under‐reported their body weight primarily because of the former, i.e. that they truly believed themselves to be thinner than they actually were. However, neither self‐deception nor impression management is helpful in explaining the present results found for individuals with obesity. The magnitude of the positive correlation between misreporting of body weight and social desirability was due largely to eight of the nine extreme under‐reporters having social desirability scores in the lower half of the Marlowe–Crowne scale.
For energy intake, the implicit assumption has always been that the socially desirable goal is to eat less. If true, then a related socially desirable goal would be not to gain excess weight, and a reasonable conclusion is that individuals high in social desirability would weigh themselves somewhat regularly and have a good idea of their body weight. Conversely, individuals low in social desirability may not have weighed themselves for a considerable time and have little idea of how much weight they have gained. The research team members noted that most of the individuals with obesity were reluctant to have team members weigh them during the home visit. It should also be noted that at the time of the home visit, two individuals with obesity revealed that they had recently begun to diet. Both had social desirability scores in the upper half of the Marlowe–Crowne scale (scores of 19 and 24), and both gave accurate self‐reported body weights.
Conclusion
Individuals with obesity are more likely than others to be extreme under‐reporters of body weight. We concur with Brestoff et al. 39 and Burke and Carman 9 that this is likely due to these individuals being unaware of their body weight. In their examination of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey data, Burke and Carman 9 concluded that 41% of the female respondents and over 50% of the male respondents were ‘weight unaware’. The present results suggest that among individuals with obesity, the lack of awareness is often the result of relatively low social desirability.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declared no conflict of interest.
King, B. M. , Cespedes, V. M. , Burden, G. K. , Brady, S. K. , Clement, L. R. , Abbott, E. M. , Baughman, K. S. , Joyner, S. E. , Clark, M. M. , and Pury, C. L. S. (2018) Extreme under‐reporting of body weight by young adults with obesity: relation to social desirability. Obesity Science & Practice, 4: 129–133. doi: 10.1002/osp4.153.
References
- 1. Hales CM, Carroll MD, Fryar CD, Ogden CL. Prevalence of obesity among adults and youth: United States, 2015–2016. NCHS Data Brief 2017; no. 288. [PubMed]
- 2. Institute of Medicine . Accelerating progress in obesity prevention: solving the weight of the nation. 2012. http://www.nationalacademies.org/hmd/Reports/2012/Accelerating-Progress-in-Obesity-Prevention.aspx. Accessed September 8, 2017. [PubMed]
- 3. Archer E, Pavela G, Lavie CJ. The inadmissibility of ‘what we eat in America’ and NHANES dietary data in nutrition and obesity research and the scientific formulation of national dietary guidelines. Mayo Clin Proc 2015; 90: 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Ioannidis JPA. Implausible results in human nutrition research. BMJ 2013; 347: f6698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Archer E, Hand GA, Blair SN. Validity of U.S. nutritional surveillance: National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey caloric energy intake data, 1971–2010. PLoS One 2013; 8: e76632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Lissner L, Troiano RP, Midthune D, et al. OPEN about obesity: recovery biomarkers, dietary errors and BMI. Int J Obes (Lond) 2007; 31: 956–961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Subar AF, Kipnis V, Troiano RP, et al. Using intake biomarkers to evaluate the extent of dietary misreporting in a large sample of adults: the OPEN study. Am J Epidemiol 2003; 158: 1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Ferrari P, Slimani N, Ciampi A, et al. Evaluation of under‐ and overreporting of energy intake in the 24‐hour diet recalls in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC). Public Health Nutr 2002; 5: 1329–1345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Burke MA, Carman KG. You can be too thin (but not too tall): social desirability bias in self‐reports of weight and height. Econ Hum Biol 2017; 27: 198–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Christian NJ, King WC, Yanovski SZ, Courcoulas AP, Belle SH. Validity of self‐reported weights following bariatric surgery. JAMA 2013; 310: 2454–2456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Connor Gorber S, Tremblay M, Moher D, Gorber B. A comparison of direct vs. self‐report measures for assessing height, weight, and body mass index: a systematic review. Obes Rev 2007; 8: 307–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Larson MR. Social desirability and self‐reported weight and height. Int J Obes (Lond) 2000; 24: 663–665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Merrill RM, Richardson JS. Validity of self‐reported height, weight, and body mass index: findings from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Prev Chronic Dis 2009; 6: A121. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Nyholm M, Gullberg B, Merlo J, Lundqvist‐Persson C, Råstam L, Lindblad U. The validity of obesity based on self‐reported weight and height: implications for population studies. Obesity 2007; 15: 197–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Palta M, Prineas RJ, Berman R, Hannan P. Comparison of self‐reported and measured height and weight. Am J Epidemiol 1982; 115: 223–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Polivy J, Herman CP, Trottier K, Sidhu R. Who are you trying to fool: does weight underreporting by dieters reflect self‐protection or self‐presentation? Health Psychol Rev 2014; 8: 319–338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Stewart AW, Jackson RT, Ford MA, Beaglehole R. Underestimation of relative weight by use of self‐reported height and weight. Am J Epidemiol 1987; 125: 122–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Villanueva EV. The validity of self‐reported weight in US adults: a population based cross‐sectional study. BMC Public Health 2001; 1: 11–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Lichtman SW, Pisarska K, Berman ER, et al. Discrepancy between self‐reported and actual caloric intake and exercise in obese subjects. N Engl J Med 1992; 327: 1893–1898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Hebert JR, Clemow L, Pbert L, Ockene IS, Ockene JK. Social desirability bias in dietary self‐report may compromise the validity of dietary intake measures. Int J Epidemiol 1995; 24: 389–398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Hebert JR, Peterson KE, Hurley TG, et al. The effect of social desirability trait on self‐reported dietary measures among multi‐ethnic female health center employees. Ann Epidemiol 2001; 11: 417–427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Hebert JR, Ebbeling CB, Matthews CE, et al. Systematic errors in middle‐aged women's estimates of energy intake: comparing three self‐report measures to total energy expenditure from doubly labeled water. Ann Epidemiol 2002; 12: 577–586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Klesges LM, Baranowski T, Beech B, et al. Social desirability bias in self‐reported dietary, physical activity and weight concerns measures in 8‐ to 10‐year‐old African–American girls: results from the Girls health Enrichment Multisite Studies (GEMS). Prev Med 2004; 38: S78–S87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Scagliusi FB, Polacow VO, Artioli GG, Benatti FB, Lancha AH. Selective underreporting of energy intake in women: magnitude, determinants, and effect of training. J Am Diet Assoc 2003; 103: 1306–1313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Scagliusi FB, Ferriolli E, Pfrimer K, et al. Characteristics of women who frequently under report their energy intake: a doubly labelled water study. Eur J Clin Nutr 2009; 63: 1192–1199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Taren DL, Tobar M, Hill A, et al. The association of energy intake bias with psychological scores of women. Eur J Clin Nutr 1999; 53: 570–578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Tooze JA, Subar AF, Thompson FE, Troiano R, Schatzkin A, Kipnis V. Psychosocial predictors of energy underreporting in a large doubly labeled water study. Am J Clin Nutr 2004; 79: 795–804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Crowne DP, Marlowe D. A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. J Consult Psychol 1960; 24: 349–354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Paulhus DL. Two‐component models of socially desirable responding. J Pers Soc Psychol 1984; 46: 598–609. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Bandini LG, Schoeller DA, Cyr HN, Dietz WH. Validity of reported energy intake in obese and nonobese adolescents. Am J Clin Nutr 1990; 52: 421–425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Braam LAJLM, Ocké MC, Bueno‐de‐Mesquita HB, Seidell JC. Determinants of obesity‐related underreporting of energy intake. Am J Epidemiol 1998; 147: 1081–1086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Klesges RC, Eck LH, Ray JW. Who underreports dietary intake in a dietary recall? Evidence from the second National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. J Consult Clin Psychol 1995; 63: 438–444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Price GM, Paul AA, Cole TJ, Wadsworth MEJ. Characteristics of the low‐energy reporters in a longitudinal national dietary survey. Br J Nutr 1997; 77: 833–851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Heitmann BL, Lissner L. Dietary underreporting by obese individuals – is it specific or nonspecific? Br Med J 1995; 311: 986–989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Lafay L, Mennen L, Basdevant A, et al. Does energy intake underreporting involve all kinds of food or only specific food items? Results from the Fleurbaix Laventie Ville Santé (FLUS) study. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2000; 24: 1500–1506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. King BM, Ivester AN, Burgess PD, et al. Adults with obesity underreport high‐calorie foods in the home. Health Behav Policy Rev 2016; 3: 439–443. [Google Scholar]
- 37. Poínhos R, Oliveira BMPM, Correia F. Eating behavior in Portuguese higher education students: the effect of social desirability. Nutrition 2015; 31: 310–314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Hebert JR, Ma Y, Clemow L, et al. Gender differences in social desirability and social approval bias in dietary self‐report. Am J Epidemiol 1997; 146: 1046–1055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Brestoff JR, Perry IJ, Van den Broeck J. Challenging the role of social norms regarding body weight as an explanation for weight, height, and BMI misreporting biases: development and application of a new approach to examining misreporting and misclassification bias in surveys. BMC Public Health ; 11: 331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]