Abstract
Object conceptual processing has been localized to distributed cortical regions that represent specific attributes. A challenging question is how object semantic space is formed. We tested a novel framework of representing semantic space in the pattern of white matter (WM) connections by extending the representational similarity analysis (RSA) to structural lesion pattern and behavioral data in 80 brain-damaged patients. For each WM connection, a neural representational dissimilarity matrix (RDM) was computed by first building machine-learning models with the voxel-wise WM lesion patterns as features to predict naming performance of a particular item and then computing the correlation between the predicted naming score and the actual naming score of another item in the testing patients. This correlation was used to build the neural RDM based on the assumption that if the connection pattern contains certain aspects of information shared by the naming processes of these two items, models trained with one item should also predict naming accuracy of the other. Correlating the neural RDM with various cognitive RDMs revealed that neural patterns in several WM connections that connect left occipital/middle temporal regions and anterior temporal regions associated with the object semantic space. Such associations were not attributable to modality-specific attributes (shape, manipulation, color, and motion), to peripheral picture-naming processes (picture visual similarity, phonological similarity), to broad semantic categories, or to the properties of the cortical regions that they connected, which tended to represent multiple modality-specific attributes. That is, the semantic space could be represented through WM connection patterns across cortical regions representing modality-specific attributes.
Author summary
One of the most challenging questions in cognitive neuroscience is how semantic knowledge, for example, that “scissors” and “knives” are related in meaning, can emerge from primary sensory dimensions such as visual forms. It is often assumed that in the human brain, semantics are stored in regions of the brain cortex, where distinct types of modality-specific information are transferred to and bind together. We tested an alternative hypothesis—“representation by connection”—in which higher-order semantic information could be coded by means of connection patterns between cortical regions. Combining data from behavior and brain imaging of 80 patients with brain lesions, we applied machine learning to construct the mapping models between the lesion patterns on axonal tracts (white matter) and item-specific object-naming performances. We found that specific white matter lesions produced deficits in object naming associated with the object’s semantic space, but not relevant to its primary dimension. The naming performances of semantically related objects were better predicted from white matter lesion-pattern models. That is, the higher-order semantic space could be coded in patterns of brain connections by linking cortical areas that do not necessarily contain such information.
Introduction
One of the most challenging questions in cognitive neuroscience is how abstract knowledge emerges from more basic dimensions of information, such as visual shapes and patterns of motor action. How do we proceed from the visual shape of a pair of scissors to the knowledge that they can be used to cut things and that they are semantically related to an axe, which looks different and is manipulated differently from scissors? Research on the neural basis of semantic memory—the storage of general knowledge about the world—has revealed widely distributed brain regions supporting modality-specific attributes of objects, such as shape, color, and motion (e.g., [1,2]; see review in [3]). Nonetheless, such attribute-specific knowledge and its simple pairings are not adequate to explain the actual semantic space of objects that have quite different sensory/motor attributes but that may nonetheless be considered to be semantically similar (e.g., [4–7]). To achieve such a semantic space, various steps of binding and abstraction are assumed to occur at specific gray matter (GM) regions [6,8–11].
Although past research on semantic representation has focused on the roles of cortical regions, specific white matter (WM) tracts have been found to be necessary for semantic processing, including the left inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), the left uncinate fasciculus (UF), and the left anterior thalamic radiation. Damage to these tracts is associated with semantic deficits in patients [12–17]. WM is classically assumed to relay information [18–20]. In accord with this general notion, these WM tracts that are necessary for semantic processing are assumed to relay distributed information to particular GM regions (e.g., the anterior temporal lobe or angular gyrus) for binding, where concepts are represented and the “deep structures” of semantic space are formed [6,7,21]. The nature of the potential information carried by WM has never been discussed or examined.
Herein, we present results for a new notion that the WM connections, being natural binding structures, provide an alternative basis to achieve semantic representation. Distributed GM regions that represent different attribute dimensions (e.g., shape, color, manner of interaction) of the same object are connected by WM. The WM linking pattern itself would then contain multiple dimensions of information in these GM regions and, importantly, additional information about the manner of mapping among various attributes. The incorporation of these elements has been argued to be necessary for the “higher-order” semantic similarity relationships, which are not explained by attribute-specific spaces, to emerge (e.g., [7, 22]).
To investigate the information coded in WM connections, we extended representational similarity analysis (RSA) [23], a highly productive method that tests the nature of representation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of cortical regions [24–26], to lesion data and WM connections. RSA examines the relationship between the representational dissimilarity matrix (RDM) derived from neural patterns and RDMs based on various types of stimulus information as a measure of information representation. The conventional neural RDM is measured by the dissimilarity of brain activity patterns induced by stimulus conditions. Here, we compute the neural RDMs with a machine-learning model using the voxel-wise lesion patterns as features to predict behavioral performance in patients with brain damage (see Fig 1). The performance in picture naming of 100 object items and the structural MRI data of 80 patients were collected. For each WM connection, a training model was built for each item (e.g., scissors) using the support vector machine (SVM) classifier with patients’ voxel-wise lesion patterns as predictive features and the naming performances of that item as labels (0, incorrect; 1, correct). The correlation between the predicted score using the classifier from that item and the actual scores of another item (e.g., axe) was taken as the neural similarity basis of these two items, based on the assumption that if this connection pattern contains certain aspects of information shared by the naming process of these two items, models trained with one item (useful features relevant for such information) should also predict naming accuracy of the other item. Once the neural RDMs are obtained from various WM connections or GM regions using this method, they can be correlated with behavioral RDMs of various object property dimensions, including the semantic RDM and four modality-specific attribute RDMs (shape, manipulation, color, and motion). Neural RDMs that are correlated with the semantic RDM even after controlling for the attribute RDMs are considered to contain “higher-order” semantic information.
Results
Behavioral RDMs: Semantic and modality-specific attributes
Behavioral RDMs for the semantic, shape, manipulation, color, and motion features of 100 objects (20 animals, 20 fruits and vegetables, 20 tools, 20 non-tool small objects, and 20 large non-manipulable objects) were generated using a multi-arrangement method [29]. In this task, 20 college students were instructed to arrange the items by a particular dimension of interest on a computer screen, and the distance among items was derived, resulting in an RDM (see Fig 2A).
The semantic RDM was visually clustered into three domains: animals, fruits and vegetables, and man-made objects (tools, small non-tool objects, large non-manipulable objects; see Fig 2B & 2C). Visualization of the semantic RDM using multidimensional scaling (Fig 2C) further revealed that within each category, words with closer semantics tended to share similar function (e.g., scissors and knife), share certain distinct features, or belong to finer subordinate categories (e.g., peanut and potato). The semantic RDM and the four modality-specific attribute RDMs were intercorrelated to various degrees (Fig 2D; semantic with shape: r = 0.35; with manipulation: r = 0.47; with color: r = 0.23; with motion: r = 0.27; p < 10−9).
WM neural RDMs: Lesion-naming model decoding
Neural RDMs were generated for each of the 688 WM connections (S1A Fig) that were identified through deterministic tractography across 90 automated anatomical labeling (AAL) regions based on the diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) data of 48 healthy controls [27]. To generate the neural RDM for each WM connection, we performed lesion-naming model decoding using voxel-wise lesion patterns and item-level naming responses. For 80 patients with brain damage, lesion patterns in each WM connection (with each voxel in the WM connections labeled as “lesion” or “intact”) for each patient were obtained by overlapping the manually traced lesion mask (converted to the MNI) space) with the WM mask (see Fig 1). A total of 680 out of 688 WM connections with adequate lesion coverage (see Materials and methods; see also S1E Fig for the lesion distribution map) were included in the following analyses. The patients’ naming performances for each of the 100 pictures were collected (performance distribution in S1B Fig).
WM neural RDMs were generated using item-based lesion-naming prediction models. For 197 connections, the lesion-naming models had successful within-item prediction averaged across all items (Bonferroni p < 0.05; diagonal in Fig 1D). That is, they yielded successful naming prediction models and were the connections that we considered in the following analyses. Of these connections, 185 were located in the left hemisphere and 12 in the right hemisphere (S1C Fig). For each of these WM connections, we computed the correspondence between the predicted scores using SVM classifiers built using the training patients’ lesion patterns and the naming scores of one item and the actual naming score of another item in the testing samples across testing iterations. This between-item correlation was taken as the similarity value for this item pair in the neural RDM, based on the assumption that if this connection pattern contains certain aspects of information shared by the naming process of these two items being captured by the SVM model, models trained with one item should also predict naming accuracy of the other item. Worth clarifying is that this procedure does not depend fully on the correlation between the actual naming accuracies across item pairs but also to what degree the potentially shared underlying properties for their naming process are supported by each WM connection (as captured by the SVM models). For example, for connections supporting phonological processing, the SVM models may pick up phonological properties and result in higher correlation between phonologically related pairs; those supporting semantic processing may pick up semantic properties and result in correlation between semantically related pairs. The resulting 100 × 100 (-item) lesion-naming prediction similarity matrix was transformed to be the neural RDM of this connection (1-prediction similarity, Fig 1D).
RSA results: Semantic representation in WM connections
Using RSA, the correlations between the WM neural RDMs and the semantic RDM were assessed. Significantly positive correlations were obtained in 60 WM connections (r = 0.03–0.11, false discovery rate [FDR] q < 0.05; see S1D Fig). These WM connections connected widely distributed regions across the left hemisphere, and approximately half (31/60) of the connections had at least one of the connected nodes located in the temporal lobe. The most densely connected regions (degree z-score > 1) were the middle temporal gyrus (MTG), superior temporal gyrus (STG), orbital part of middle frontal gyrus, inferior parietal lobule (IPL), and precentral gyrus.
What about semantic effects that could not be explained by modality-specific attributes, peripheral factors, or broad semantic categorical effects? We controlled for the effects of all four modality-specific attributes, two peripheral variables (the early visual and phonological) and semantic category matrix (labeling within-category pairs 1 and between-category pair 0) using partial correlation. The semantic effect was consistently significant in eight WM connections (r = 0.03–0.07, FDR q < 0.05; Fig 3A–3C). Table 1 presents the detailed statistical results before and after, including these variables as covariates. These eight connections were considered to represent (relatively) higher-order semantic space. Five of them were located in the left ventral visual pathway and connected occipital regions (middle occipital gyrus, calcarine sulcus, and lingual gyrus) and temporal regions (STG, MTG, superior anterior temporal lobe [ATL], and middle ATL). The three remaining WM connections were located in the right hemisphere, connecting the postcentral gyrus with the thalamus, lingual gyrus, and parahippocampal gyrus. These reconstructed connections are shown in Fig 3B and S2 Fig.
Table 1. The RSA results of the WM connections showing significant effects of higher-order semantic space.
Analysis aspect | Control variables | WM connections | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MTG_L—CAL_L | MTG_L—LING_L | MidATL_L—MTG_L | SupATL_L—MOG_L | STG_L—CAL_L | PoCG_R -LING_R | PoCG_R—PHG_R | Thalamus_R—PoCG_R | ||
Higher-order semantic RSA | |||||||||
(control for modality-specific attributesa and the other relevant matrices#) | |||||||||
0.032(0.024*) | 0.049(<0.001*) | 0.073(<1 × 10−6*) | 0.051(<0.001*) | 0.065(<0.001*) | 0.04(0.005*) | 0.034(0.017*) | 0.043(0.002*) | ||
(control for modality-specific attributesb and the other relevant matrices#) | |||||||||
0.021(0.147) | 0.029(0.045) | 0.065(<0.001*) | 0.044(0.002*) | 0.05(<0.001*) | 0.036(0.012*) | 0.032(0.026) | 0.036(0.011*) | ||
Higher-order semantic RSA: Effect of modality-specific attributes | |||||||||
(control for modality-specific attributesa) | |||||||||
0.045(0.001*) | 0.066(<0.001*) | 0.065(<0.001*) | 0.046(0.001*) | 0.065(<0.001*) | 0.067(<0.001*) | 0.052(<0.001*) | 0.059(<0.001*) | ||
Higher-order semantic RSA: Types of lesion | |||||||||
(using only stroke patients, control for modality-specific attributesa and the other relevant matrices#) | |||||||||
0.056(<0.001*) | 0.081(<1 × 10−6*) | 0.077(<1 × 10−6*) | 0.067(<0.001*) | 0.085(<1 × 10−6*) | 0.017(0.245) | 0.017(0.234) | −0.004 | ||
(using only patients with unilateral left hemispheric lesions, control for modality-specific attributesa and the other relevant matrices#) | |||||||||
0.031(0.028) | 0.049(<0.001*) | 0.039(0.007*) | 0.049(<0.001*) | 0.05(<0.001*) | - | - | - | ||
Higher-order semantic RSA: Effect of the GM nodes | |||||||||
(control for modality-specific attributesa and the other relevant matrices# and the neural RDMs of two GM nodes being connected) | |||||||||
0.038(0.007*) | 0.057(<0.001*) | 0.096(<1 × 10−6*) | 0.058(<0.001*) | 0.062(<0.001*) | 0.026(0.063) | 0.005(0.723) | −0.038 | ||
Raw semantic RSA | |||||||||
(control for the other relevant matrices# and not modality-specific attributes) | |||||||||
0.049(<0.001*) | 0.06(<0.001*) | 0.111(<1 × 10−6*) | 0.085(<1 × 10−6*) | 0.089(<1 × 10−6*) | 0.019(0.193) | 0.035(0.013*) | 0.028(0.045) | ||
Object category RSA | |||||||||
(control for the low-level visual and phonological matrices) | |||||||||
0.016(0.25) | 0.017(0.243) | 0.014(0.323) | 0.011(0.431) | 0.005(0.739) | 0.001(0.943) | 0.005(0.747) | −0.001 |
*Positive correlation values that survived FDR correction (q < 0.05). When controlling for the modality-specific attributes, two ways (a and b, below) of dealing with items not having certain modality ratings were both considered.
aSetting those cells as being most dissimilar, “1.”
bSetting them as missing values, “NaN.”
#Low-level visual, phonological, category.
Abbreviations: CAL, calcarine sulcus; FDR, false discovery rate; GM, gray matter; L, left; LING, lingual gyrus; MidATL, middle anterior temporal lobe; MOG, middle occipital gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; NaN, not a number; PHG, parahippocampal gyrus; PoCG, postcentral gyrus; R, right; RDM, representational dissimilarity matrix; RSA, representational similarity analysis; STG, superior temporal gyrus; SupATL, superior anterior temporal lobe.
To examine the degree to which the semantic effects we observed on these WM connections reflect effects of broad semantic category, we also checked the RSA effect of the category matrix (correlating the neural RDM and the category RDM) and found that none of these connections had significant effects of the semantic category (p > 0.05, Table 1).
Validation analyses
To consolidate the main results above, we further performed validation analyses to test the following concerns: (1) The WM mask we adopted was constructed using DTI data acquired on a scanner with a low magnetic field (1.5 T) and 32 directions. Was the WM connection construction accurate and unaffected by crossing-fiber problems? (2) To maximize power, we included patients with multiple etiologies (84% stroke and 16% traumatic brain injury [TBI]) and lesion distributions (37.5% lesion in the left hemisphere only, 43.8% lesion in bilateral hemispheres, and 18.8% in the right hemisphere only). Were the results systematically affected by disease type or hemispheric differences?
The quality of the WM fiber tracking
We reconstructed WM connections using diffusion data from a public state-of-the-art connectome database—the Human Connectome Project (HCP)—to ensure that the WM connections we adopted [27] were not false connections. The HCP data were acquired with a high angular resolution diffusion imaging (HARDI) sequence and therefore allowed for complex diffusion models to handle the cross-fiber issues. For the eight WM connections with higher-order semantic representation that we observed in the main results above, the WM connections reconstructed from the HCP data and our DTI data were visually very similar when projected onto the 3D brain (see S2 Fig).
Controlling for the effects of patient disease type and lesion hemisphere
There was no systematic difference in the naming scores between stroke and TBI patients (t78 = −1.30, p = 0.20). We computed the neural RDMs in WM connections using only data from the 67 stroke patients, and the RSA results across the five left higher-order semantic WM connections remained highly consistent with those in the results using all patients, but the effects in right hemisphere diminished (r = −0.004–0.017, FDR q > 0.05). Using only patients with unilateral left hemispheric lesions (30 patients), we also obtained results that were generally consistent with the main results: all but one higher-order semantic WM connections retained significance (FDR q < 0.05, except for the one connecting the MTG and calcarine sulcus, r = 0.03, uncorrected p < 0.05, see Table 1).
RSA results: The representation content of GM nodes that are connected by semantic WM connections
What types of representations are linked by the WM connections that represent the semantic space? Do the WM connections simply relay semantic information that has already been encoded in the GM nodes, or do they contain information that cannot be accounted for by representation in the GM nodes? We tested the representational contents of the seven GM nodes that were connected by the five higher-order semantic WM connections whose effects remained robust in the validation tests (see Table 1). Four GM regions had successful within-item naming prediction and were considered in the RSA analysis: superior ATL, middle ATL, MTG, and STG. The neural RDM for each GM node was constructed using the same method as with the neural RDMs of the WM connections.
We found that the higher-order semantic representation in the five semantic WM connections cannot be simply explained by GM information (Fig 3D; S1 Table): when correlating the GM neural RDMs with the semantic RDM (controlling for peripheral and categorical matrices), only the superior ATL reached significance (r = 0.04, FDR q < 0.05). However, this effect could be explained by modality-specific attribute representations. After controlling for the four modality-specific attribute matrices, none of the four GM nodes significantly correlated with the semantic RDM at either the conventional threshold (FDR q < 0.05) or a less stringent threshold (uncorrected p < 0.05, see S1 Table). Additionally, when testing the higher-order semantic representation in the five WM connections by further adding the neural RDMs of the two GM nodes being connected as additional confounding variables, the results remained unchanged (see Table 1). We further constrained our WM connection mask with a WM mask constructed by T1 segmentation (conducted using SPM8 in MNI T1 template, default parameters) to offer a clear WM boundary, i.e., containing only WM voxels. We then recomputed the higher-order semantic RSA in these WM connections using only the voxels within the WM mask and found that the effects in all five WM connections remained significant (FDR q < 0.05, r = 0.03–0.07, SD = 0.01).
If not semantic, do these GM nodes code modality-specific attributes? We correlated the neural RDM of each GM node with each of the four modality-specific attribute RDMs (shape, manipulation, color, and motion; Fig 3D & S1 Table; the three control matrices—low-level visual, phonological, category—were controlled for). The superior ATL, MTG, and STG were significantly correlated with the shape and manipulation RDMs (shape: r = 0.04–0.08, manipulation: r = 0.12–0.16, FDR q < 0.05). The middle ATL was significantly correlated with the shape and color RDMs (shape: r = 0.04, color: r = 0.06, FDR q < 0.05).
Finally, we conducted a whole-brain analysis across all 90 AAL GM nodes. In addition to superior ATL, the neural RDMs of the left IPL, precentral gyrus, and postcentral gyrus were significantly correlated with the semantic RDM (r = 0.04–0.05, FDR q < 0.05), but none of these or any other GM regions retained significance after controlling for the four modality-specific attribute matrices (FDR q < 0.05).
Discussion
To test the potential WM basis of semantic representation, we developed a structural-property-pattern-based RSA approach by applying machine learning to lesion and behavioral data in patients to derive item-based neural RDMs for WM connections. We found that a set of WM connections connecting occipital/middle temporal regions and anterior temporal regions represented a semantic space that was not explained by broad semantic categories or the effects of modality-specific attributes and, hence, was addressed as higher-order semantic representation. Such semantic effects were not fully explained by the properties of the GM nodes that were connected. Although the neural RDM of a connecting node—the superior ATL—correlated with the semantic RDM, such effect diminished after controlling for modality-specific attributes. Instead, these GM nodes tended to represent modality-specific attributes, including shape and manipulation in the superior ATL, MTG, and STG and shape and color in the middle ATL.
First, it should be noted that we inferred semantic effects to be higher-order when they were not explained by linear combinations of the classical modality-specific attributes for objects. The potential effects of some untested modalities or certain nonlinear combinations across various modalities could not be fully excluded. Also, subjectively judged semantic distance might be a rather composite measure that is driven by multiple semantic dimensions, which may have different neural bases (e.g., [30]). Under the current (conventional) operation, these WM connections that represent higher-order semantics tend to lie in several major pathways that have been associated with semantic processing using univariate lesion-behavior correlation or intraoperative stimulation [12,16,21,27,31]. These connections partly belong to IFOF, and the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF) (the overlapped voxels with the Johns Hopkins University WM template: IFOF [32%], ILF [71%], and minimally on the minor forceps [6%] and superior longitudinal fasciculus [8%]). Lesion or atrophy in IFOF is associated with semantic deficit severity in patients with stroke and in patients with semantic dementia [12,27,32]. A similar result was also found with ILF in semantic dementia [16,33]. Additionally, direct intraoperative stimulation of IFOF induces semantic errors [34,35]. Our current findings based on multivariate RSA demonstrate that the organization of specific connections among these large WM tract bundles represent the fine-grained semantic space. Items closer in semantic space are represented by more similar WM patterns in these specific connections. Note that it is well known that patients’ specific naming errors may vary from session to session [36]. The WM lesion pattern observed here is likely associated with some aspects of semantic space rather than with specific items. The damage of such specific aspects of semantic space would result in noisy/impaired representation for a range of items sharing that space, resulting in potentially different outputs at different time points. Such semantic space was nonetheless much finer than broad semantic categories, however, as the RSA results were robust after controlling for the categorical matrix. It is also well known that patients may make different types of errors, such as phonological and semantic paraphasias, which may be originated from different cognitive stages. Our approach here pulled all types of naming errors together, and the RSA results of correlating the neural RDM with different RDMs (semantic versus phonological/visual) presumably reflect the neural basis of different error types, which should be directly examined in future research.
What is the relationship between the WM representations and the nature of the GM regions that they connect? First, we indeed observed that one of the seven linked GM regions was related to semantic space—superior ATL. The finding that lesion-pattern-behavior (neural) RDM in the superior ATL correlated with semantic space before regressing out the effects of modality-specific attributes converges nicely with the accumulated evidence about the cortical representation of semantics from fMRI and neuropsychological studies. ATL is the region with the strongest atrophy in patients with semantic dementia, which is marked by semantic deficits [6,7,31,37,38] and is sensitive to multiple modalities of object attributes [39,40]. Unlike the WM connections related to higher-order semantic space, the semantic effect in the superior ATL could be explained by the effects of modality-specific attributes. Worth noting is that ventral ATL was not scrutinized because it was not a node in the AAL parcellation we used but was included in the fusiform and inferior temporal nodes. What should be highlighted, however, is that the positive effects of higher-order semantic representation in the WM connections are significant and are not simply inheriting the properties of the connected GM nodes. Several higher-order semantic WM connections observed here connected ATL with other regions, inviting further questions about whether it is the integrity of ATL or of the ATL-related WM connections that make stronger contributions to the semantic deficits in semantic dementia patients.
While our results certainly do not argue against the possibility that there are specific GM regions supporting semantic representation, we found that the GM nodes being connected by the WM connections obtained here tended to represent multiple modality-specific object properties. Of the four GM regions we could test, the MTG, STG, and superior ATL represented shape and manipulation properties, and the middle ATL represented shape and color properties. These results converge nicely with the fMRI literature studying the sensitivity of these regions for object attributes. For instance, the effects of various attributes were recently tested using parametric modulation analyses [2], which found that the posterior MTG was sensitive to both shape and manipulation knowledge. Coutanche and Thompson-Schill [39] found that the ATL codes the integration of color and shape, and Peelen and Caramazza [40] found that the ATL codes both manipulation and location. The STG was sensitive to motion properties in Fernandino et al. [2] but not in our study, perhaps due to different parcellation scales regarding the finer structure within this region. Note that many studies about the attribute-specific property representations have revealed results in sensory and motor cortices (e.g., shape in the lateral occipital/temporal cortex: [26,41]; color in the ventromedial occipital cortex such as lingual gyrus: [42–44]). However, these regions could not be tested in our data given their chance-level lesion-naming prediction performance, which could either be due to low lesion distribution in these regions (see S1E Fig for lesion distributions) or because the specific dimensions they represent are unnecessary for object picture-naming behavior. It may also be the case that higher-order semantic space is formed by binding multiple, rather than single, pairs of attributes. Consistent with this speculation, it has been shown that computation simulation models with a convergent architecture, in which intermediate units code multiple types of dimension pairings, were better at capturing the “deep” structure of conceptual space and promoting generalizations across semantically related items that were not apparently similar along single dimensions [22].
What is the mechanism of coding higher-order semantic information in WM that connects multiple modality-specific attributes? One potential mechanism could be through synchronized firing of specific sensory and motor patterns for objects. Consider when people use a pair of scissors: the neurons that represent the attributes across various modalities—e.g., shape, haptics, ways of grasping and manipulating it, seeing the consequence of using it (things being cut)—fire together. Such functional co-activation across a wide range of attributes occurs often when we see or use scissors, which enhances the structural connection between neurons within and across dimensions of the same object. WM provides a basis for such synchronization between distant cortical regions [45]. These synchronizations also lead to the building and tuning of WM connections, because neuronal activity traveling through axons can affect the properties of myelin sheaths in the active circuit; for example, electrical activity in the axon induces myelination [46,47]. This interactive process results in the WM basis of a multidimensional representation of “scissors,” which is closer in the higher-order semantic space to concepts such as “axe” or “paper.” The formation and modulation of the WM microstructure underlying these representations can be affected by our experiences, which is the basis of acquiring new concepts and of the coloring of existing concepts. Ample evidence describes how WM is affected by experience. Early-life experiential deprivation in animals and humans leads to decreased myelin sheath thickness and WM volume [48,49], whereas these parameters increase when the organism is placed in a rich experiential environment [50]. Reading training [51] and music practice [52,53] during childhood lead to increased fractional anisotropy in WM. The acquisition of motor skills changes the WM microstructure [54,55]. The exact relationship between WM microstructure and the functional coupling between cortical regions for various representational dimensions warrants further studies.
A final methodological note is that the approach we developed here—building neural RDMs using machine learning with structural lesion data and condition-specific performances—could be easily adapted to other cognitive issues and all kinds of brain structural integrity measurements, including DTI indices (e.g., fractional anisotropy, mean diffusivity) or voxel-based morphometry measures for both patient and healthy populations. For the current study, we chose to focus on manually traced lesion on the T1 image (with reference to T2) because it captures the structural damage in our specific patient group (mostly stroke) in a most straightforward fashion. RSA, an approach that connects major branches of systems neuroscience—brain-activity measurement, behavioral measurement, and computational modeling [23]—could now be extended to an additional branch, i.e., brain structural measurement.
In conclusion, using a structural-property-pattern-based RSA approach, we found that the WM structures mainly connecting occipital/middle temporal regions and anterior temporal regions represent fine-grained higher-order semantic information. Such semantic relatedness effects were not attributable to modality-specific attributes (shape, manipulation, color, and motion) or to the representation contents of the cortical regions that they connected and were above and beyond the broad categorical distinctions. By connecting multiple modality-specific attributes, higher-order semantic space can be formed through patterns of these connections.
Materials and methods
Participants
Eighty patients with brain damage participated in the present study. The patient group (60 males, 20 females) was recruited from the China Rehabilitation Research Center with at least 1 month post-onset (mean = 6.09; SD = 11.69; range: 1–86 months) and premorbidly right-handed. The majority suffered from stroke (n = 67) and others suffered from TBI (n = 13). The patients’ mean age was 45 years (SD = 13; range: 19–76 years) and mean years of formal education was 13 (SD = 3; range: 2–19). Twenty additional college students (10 males; mean age = 22.9, SD = 2.45, range = 19–27) participated in the multi-arrangement experiment for the behavioral RDMs. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University (IORG0004944), adhering to the Declaration of Helsinki for research involving human subjects. All participants gave informed written consent.
MRI data collection and preprocessing
Each subject was scanned using a 1.5T GE SIGNA EXCITE scanner with an 8-channel split head coil at the China Rehabilitation Research Center. We collected two types of images: (1) high-resolution 3D T1-weighted MPRAGE images in the sagittal plane with a matrix size = 512 × 512, voxel size = 0.49 × 0.49 × 0.70 mm3, repetition time (TR) = 12.26 ms, echo time (TE) = 4.2 ms, inversion time = 400 ms, field of view (FOV) = 250 × 250 mm2, flip angle = 15°, and slice number = 248; and (2) FLAIR T2-weighted images in the axial plane with a matrix size = 512 × 512, voxel size = 0.49 × 0.49 × 5 mm3, TR = 8,002 ms, TE = 127.57 ms, inversion time = 2 s, FOV = 250 × 250 mm2, flip angle = 90°, and slice number = 28. To improve the image quality, the T1 image was scanned twice. The two scans were then co-registered and averaged for the following analyses. All imaging data can be found at the Open Science Framework database (URL: https://osf.io/h7upk/?view_only=52b8f86cffa14ed4844e4a1b9cd429cb).
Materials, neuropsychological testing, and behavioral RDM construction
Materials
One hundred colored photographs of objects, with an equal number of items from five semantic categories (animals, fruits and vegetables, tools, small non-tool artifacts, and large non-manipulable objects), were used in the neuropsychological testing and behavioral RDM construction.
Neuropsychological testing
Patients underwent an oral picture-naming test outside the scanner. They were asked to name each object on a computer screen. The first complete response was scored. Responses were scored as 1 if correct or 0 if wrong.
Behavioral RDM construction
The semantic RDM was based on a multi-arrangement method [29]. Each subject judged the semantic distance among 100 objects in the oral picture-naming task by arranging them on a computer screen. The distance between any two objects on the screen reflected their semantic distance. The subjects were instructed to “arrange objects according to how similar they are in meaning; for instance, the meaning of ‘rock–cell phone’ has little in common so they should be dragged far apart; ‘rock–sand’ has high similarity in meaning so they should be dragged close together; please consider only the aspect of ‘semantic similarity’ and disregard other aspects such as object size, color, materials, or pure associations (e.g., dog–bone).” The instruction was adapted from classical behavioral studies using semantic similarity ratings (e.g., [56]). To optimally estimate the dissimilarity matrix, all 100 images were only shown together in the first trial, and a subset was selected in every subsequent trial (see [27] for details). Modality-specific attribute (shape, manipulation, color, and motion) RDMs were based on the same arrangement method using different instructions (e.g., “Please arrange these objects according to their color/shape/manipulation/motion similarity”). Because some attributes may not be salient for some categories (e.g., it is not sensible to ask for the manipulation of a tiger or the motion of a monument), only those categories with explicit and lucid attributes were selected for a given attribute (i.e., shape for all five categories, small non-tool artifacts and tools for manipulation, fruits/vegetables and animals for color, and tools and animals for motion). All modality-specific attribute RDMs were then mapped to a complete 100 × 100 matrix by setting missing values to 1 (i.e., items without certain type of salient properties were labeled as being most dissimilar with other items on this property type; we also carried out an analysis, setting such missing values to NaN and the result pattern remained largely unchanged, see Table 1). Confounding variable RDMs were constructed based on the visual, phonological, and category properties of the items. We computed the low-level visual RDMs based on image silhouettes, because this method offers an effective prediction of the activation patterns in the early visual cortex [25]. The image pixels were binarized according to whether the pixel belonged to the object (pixel value = 1) or to the background (pixel value = 0). The dissimilarity between images was computed by 1 minus Jaccard similarity. For the phonological RDM, the dissimilarity of two-item names was measured by 1 minus the proportion of shared sub-syllabic units (onset or rhyme), regardless of position (e.g., [57]). The sub-syllabic units for a given syllable were defined based on the phonetic transcript of Chinese characters (the “pinyin” system), which transcribes each syllable with an onset consonant (“shengmu”) and a rhyme vowel or vowel-consonant (“yunmu”). The categorical RDM was constructed based on five object categories, with item pairs within the same category labeled 0 and other cells labeled 1.
Neural RDM construction
We used structural-property-pattern (lesion)-based RSA to investigate semantic and modality-specific attribute representation in WM connections and GM regions. Similar to the conventional RSA, which is a highly fruitful method to research the neural representation in cortical regions using functional imaging data, the structural-property-pattern (lesion)-based RSA computes the relationship between the neural RDMs and behavioral or theoretical RDMs. The main difference is that the neural RDMs in this study were constructed by machine-learning models based on performances on neuropsychological tests and patients’ brain structural lesion patterns. The main rationale for this neural similarity measure is that if a WM connection pattern contains certain aspects of information shared by the naming process of two items (e.g., some semantic features), models trained with one item should also be able to predict naming accuracy of the other item to some degree. We first extracted the lesion features, balanced item labels by bootstrapping, input the lesion features and balanced labels into SVM training and testing to obtain the neural RDM, and used permutation to estimate the significance level of the neural RDM. The full pipeline is shown in Fig 1 and the details for each of these steps are described below in turn. The scripts of the full pipeline can be found at https://osf.io/h7upk/?view_only=52b8f86cffa14ed4844e4a1b9cd429cb.
Extracting the lesion features
As shown in Fig 1B, we first obtained the lesion mask (manually traced in T1 image) for each patient, then converted to MNI space, which was then overlapped with a WM connection template constructed from a healthy population [27], to extract the voxel-wise lesion pattern for each patient on each WM connection. We here focused on the structural (lesion) imaging data instead of performing analyses directly on patients’ DTI data (e.g., analyzing fractional anisotropy [FA] values or performing tractography), mainly because lesions from structural imaging (T1 and T2) are most straightforward in capturing brain structure damage properties in our specific patient type (mostly chronic stroke).
For lesion identification, in each patient, a lesion mask was constructed from manually traced lesion contours on averaged T1 images slice-by-slice with reference to T2 images (see [27] for details). Lesion mapping in patients with brain damage is a challenging task and various automatic methods have been developed, with supervised or nonsupervised algorithms [58–61], but manual drawing is considered the gold standard [58,62], even in very recent works [61]. We chose this highly labor-intensive method to ensure the validity of the lesion data and have gone through several procedures to ensure the reliability (inter-rater reliability values between our two investigators and an experienced radiologist were: mean percentage volume difference, 9% ± 8% and 4% ± 3%; mean percentage of discrepant voxels, 7% ± 4% and 6% ± 2%).
For WM connection, we adopted a previously reported template of the whole-brain WM network [27] to have a common reference template for the WM lesion patterns in the individual patients. Building neural RDMs in the current approach can only be done in the common template space where voxels are lined up, so that lesion patterns for different patients can be compared (i.e., for a same voxel, whether patients have lesion or not) and to be used as features for machine-learning model computation. The template we adopted was constructed using deterministic fiber tracking based on diffusion imaging data of 48 healthy participants ([27]; S1A Fig). This template contains 688 WM connections across 90 GM nodes (parcellated by the AAL atlas [63]). Briefly, the WM reconstruction was first applied in each healthy subject using determinative tracking among every two AAL regions. The resulting tracking maps in the subjects’ native space were transformed to a binary map in the MNI space. The binary maps of the MNI space for all subjects were then overlaid to generate a count map. Finally, a group-level threshold was set at voxel value (>25% of subjects; cluster size > 300 voxels) to determine whether a pair of brain regions was anatomically connected. The details of template construction can be found in [27]. Deterministic tracking was used because it has determinate termination conditions (FA values and fiber angles). It tends to suffer more false negatives but offers a clear border of WM connection to avoid invading to GM. While probabilistic tracking is generally considered more sensitive than deterministic tracking and thus revealing of more WM structures [64–66], it also increases the probability of false connections, and the biological meanings of the probabilistic values are uncertain, while it is relatively clear for the measurements used in deterministic tracking [64]. Note that the DTI imaging acquisition was suboptimal, according to standards nowadays, because of pragmatic issues in collecting patient and healthy group data using the same scanner. Nonetheless, the existence and shape of the connections showed generally good correspondence with WM networks constructed from other datasets (e.g., [67]; see also below for validation analyses).
Then, the patients’ lesion mask was converted to MNI space and overlapped with the WM connection template or the GM region masks (see [27] for details). In each WM connection/GM region, intact voxels (i.e., without lesion) were labeled 0 and lesioned voxels were labeled 1. This resulted in a binary V × N matrix in which V denoted the total number of voxels in the WM connection/GM region and N the number of patients, constituting a feature set for each machine-learning model. To ensure that the WM connections/GM regions had enough subjects with lesion coverage, we only tested the WM connections and GM regions with at least five subjects having damage and with more than 20 voxels lesioned per patient (see lesion distribution at S1E Fig). A total of 680 out of 688 connections and 80 out of 90 AAL regions were included in the following analyses. Because the input feature data only contained binary values and the range was consistent with the behavioral data, no normalization was applied in the feature set.
Bootstrapping
An item’s naming accuracy across all patients was not always 50%. Unbalanced training labels (e.g., the numbers of 0’s and 1’s in the training data were not equal) would ruin the classification ability because the training model always classifies the test sample into the group whose labels are predominant in number. A bootstrapping method was used to address this issue. Before classification, the subjects were reallocated into two groups: one group with correct responses and the other group with incorrect responses. We selected all subjects with the less common response of the two groups (e.g., if the accuracy of one item was 60%, all subjects with incorrect responses were selected) along with the same number of subjects randomly chosen from the other group. Thus, a new dataset for each item was constructed, with an accuracy across patients of 50%. The sample sizes of the training data for the 100 items ranged from 12 to 78 subjects (mean = 41.8 ± 8.5). This procedure was repeated 100 times for each item in each WM connection/GM region.
SVM training and testing
For each WM connection or GM region, a linear SVM with default parameters [68] was used. For each item (e.g., scissors), an SVM classifier was trained based on the balanced naming labels and voxel-wise lesion patterns. The resultant classifier was used to predict the naming score (1 or 0) of all patients who were not included in the training set using their lesion patterns; for patients who were included in the training set, a leave-two-out cross-validation scheme was used. This combined procedure ended up with a predicted score for each patient (each patient was a testing case once across testing iterations). The correspondence (simple matching coefficient) between this predicted score (based on training model of one item) and the actual naming score of each of the other items (e.g., axe) was calculated and was considered the neural similarity between the training item and this other item (i.e., scissors–axe similarity) on the particular WM connection being tested. All cross-item and within-item similarity could be obtained this way, resulting in a 100 × 100 similarity matrix. We averaged the symmetrical cells in the matrix according to the principal diagonal to obtain a symmetric matrix. Each cell in the matrix was then averaged across all 100 bootstrapped samples to produce the final 100 × 100 (1-similarity) neural RDM.
Significance testing (permutation and FDR)
The nonparametric permutation test (10,000 times) was used to estimate the significance of the classification model for each individual edge. For each permutation, the patient labels were randomly exchanged to shuffle the relationship between behavioral data and lesion data. The averaged accuracy of the principal diagonal cells (i.e., within-item prediction accuracies) was then computed. The p-value was calculated as the fraction of accuracies from all permutations that were greater than the actual accuracy using correct labels. For each WM connection, an independent classification model was built. To control for false positives caused by comparisons across multiple edges, we applied FDR as a multi-comparison correction method. The neural RDMs of WM connections/GM regions with significant within-item prediction accuracies at the threshold of FDR q < 0.05 were considered meaningful and were used for further analyses.
Representational similarity analyses: Correlating neural RDMs with behavioral RDMs
The neural RDMs were correlated with behavioral RDMs using Spearman correlation. Specifically, for each WM connection, its neural RDM (a 100 [-item] × 100 [-item] matrix) and the semantic RDM (a 100 [-item] × 100 [-item] matrix) were both converted to a 1 × 4,950 vector. Correlation was computed on these two vectors (4,950 pairs of values). The r values were used to determine the extent of specific information encoded in the WM connections/GM regions. The FDR (q < 0.05) was used for multiple comparison correction. To investigate the higher-order semantic effects beyond modality-specific attributes, partial correlation analyses were performed between the semantic RDM and neural RDMs, with the modality-specific attribute RDMs (and the peripheral and categorical matrices) as nuisance variables. As explained in the “Behavioral RDM Construction” session, we adopted two ways of treating missing values in the modality-specific attributes (e.g., animal items were not rated on “manipulation” property)—setting it to be 1 (most dissimilar with other items on this modality) or to “NaN” (missing value). The RSA mapping procedure was implemented using a custom MATLAB function.
Validation analyses
Quality of the WM fiber tracking
We used the HCP database to check the WM template that we used in the main analyses, because HCP contains high-quality diffusion MRI data with advanced acquisition and processing methods [69]. Diffusion scans were acquired in a Siemens 3T Skyra scanner using a 2D spin-echo single-shot multiband EPI sequence with a multiband factor of three and a monopolar gradient pulse. The spatial resolution was 1.25 mm isotropic, TR = 5,500 ms, TE = 89 ms. A multishell diffusion scheme was used. The b-values were 1,000, 2,000, and 3,000 s/mm2. The total number of diffusion sampling directions was 270. We used the dataset “unrelated 40” on the ConnectomeDB website (https://db.humanconnectome.org/) for fiber reconstruction. After excluding two subjects with technical problems in acquisition, the remaining 38 subjects were included in the analyses. The preprocessing, reconstruction, and fiber tracking were performed with DSI-studio software (dsi-studio.labsolver.org). To reduce the fiber-crossing problem, we reconstructed the diffusion data using the generalized q-sampling imaging (GQI) method [70].
Controlling for the effects of patient disease type and lesion hemisphere
We computed the neural RDMs in WM connections using data from the 67 stroke patients or from the 30 patients with unilateral left hemispheric lesions.
Supporting information
Abbreviations
- AAL
automated anatomical labeling
- ATL
anterior temporal lobe
- DTI
diffusion tensor imaging
- FA
fractional anisotropy
- FDR
false discovery rate
- fMRI
functional magnetic resonance imaging
- FOV
field of view
- GM
gray matter
- GQI
generalized q-sampling imaging
- HARDI
high angular resolution diffusion imaging
- HCP
Human Connectome Project
- IFOF
inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus
- ILF
inferior longitudinal fasciculus
- IPL
inferior parietal lobule
- MTG
middle temporal gyrus
- NaN
not a number
- RDM
representational dissimilarity matrix
- RSA
representational similarity analysis
- STG
superior temporal gyrus
- SVM
support vector machine
- TBI
traumatic brain injury
- TE
echo time
- TR
repetition time
- UF
uncinate fasciculus
- WM
white matter
Data Availability
All relevant data are available from Open Science Framework database (URL: https://osf.io/h7upk/?view_only=52b8f86cffa14ed4844e4a1b9cd429cb).
Funding Statement
National Key Basic Research Program of China www.most.gov.cn (grant number 2013CB837300, 2014CB846100). Received by YB. The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Beijing Brain Project www.bjkw.gov.cn (grant number Z16110100020000, Z161100000216125). Received by YB. The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. National Natural Science Foundation of China www.nsfc.gov.cn (grant number 4131671128, 31521063, 81671772, 31700944). 4131671128, 31521063 received by YB; 81671772 received by GG; 31700944 received by YF. The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. National Program for Special Support of Top-notch Young Professionals. The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Martin A, Haxby J V, Lalonde FM, Wiggs CL, Ungerleider LG. Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of color and knowledge of action. Science. 1995;270: 102–105. doi: 10.1126/science.270.5233.102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Fernandino L, Binder JR, Desai RH, Pendl SL, Humphries CJ, Gross WL, et al. Concept Representation Reflects Multimodal Abstraction: A Framework for Embodied Semantics. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26: 2018–2034. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Martin A. GRAPES-Grounding representations in action, perception, and emotion systems: How object properties and categories are represented in the human brain. Psychon Bull Rev. 2015; 979–990. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0842-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Binder JR, Conant LL, Humphries CJ, Fernandino L, Simons SB, Aguilar M, et al. Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2016;3294: 1–45. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1147426 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Caramazza A, Hillis AE, Rapp BC, Romani C. The multiple semantics hypothesis: multiple confusions? Cogn Neuropsychol. Taylor & Francis; 1990;7: 161–189. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lambon Ralph MA, Jefferies E, Patterson K, Rogers TT. The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. Nature Publishing Group; 2017;18: 42–55. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.150 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Patterson KE, Nestor PJ, Rogers TT. Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8: 976–987. doi: 10.1038/nrn2277 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Clarke A, Tyler LK. Object-Specific Semantic Coding in Human Perirhinal Cortex. J Neurosci. 2014;34: 4766–4775. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2828-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Quiroga R. Concept cells: the building blocks of declarative memory functions. Nat Rev Neurosci. Nature Publishing Group; 2012;13: 587–97. doi: 10.1038/nrn3251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Quiroga R, Kreiman G, Koch C, Fried I. Sparse but not “grandmother-cell”coding in the medial temporal lobe. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008; 87–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Damasio AR. The brain binds entities and events by multiregional activation from convergence zones. Neural Comput. MIT Press; 1989;1: 123–132. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Han Z, Ma Y, Gong G, He Y, Caramazza A, Bi Y. White matter structural connectivity underlying semantic processing: Evidence from brain damaged patients. Brain. 2013;136: 2952–2965. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Duffau H. The anatomo-functional connectivity of language revisited New insights provided by electrostimulation and tractography. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46: 927–934. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Duffau H, Capelle L, Sichez N, Denvil D, Lopes M, Sichez JP, et al. Intraoperative mapping of the subcortical language pathways using direct stimulations An anatomo-functional study. Brain. 2002;125: 199–214. doi: 10.1093/Brain/Awf016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kümmerer D, Hartwigsen G, Kellmeyer P, Glauche V, Mader I, Kloppel S, et al. Damage to ventral and dorsal language pathways in acute aphasia. Brain. 2013;136: 619–629. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws354 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Agosta F, Henry RG, Migliaccio R, Neuhaus J, Miller BL, Dronkers NF, et al. Language networks in semantic dementia. Brain. 2010;133: 286–299. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp233 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lambon Ralph MA, Cipolotti L, Manes F, Patterson K, Ralph. Taking both sides: do unilateral anterior temporal lobe lesions disrupt semantic memory? Brain. 2010;133: 3243–55. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq264 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wandell BA. Clarifying Human White Matter. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2016;39: 103–128. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-070815-013815 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kalat J. Biological psychology. Nelson Education; 2015.
- 20.Mesulam M. Brain, mind, and the evolution of connectivity. Brain Cogn. Elsevier; 2000;42: 4–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Friederici AD, Gierhan SME. The language network. Curr Opin Neurobiol. Elsevier Ltd; 2013;23: 250–254. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.10.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Rogers TT, McClelland JL. Semantic cognition: A parallel distributed processing approach. MIT press; 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kriegeskorte N, Mur M, Bandettini P a. Representational similarity analysis—connecting the branches of systems neuroscience. Front Syst Neurosci. 2008;2: 4 doi: 10.3389/neuro.06.004.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Haushofer J, Livingstone MS, Kanwisher N. Multivariate patterns in object-selective cortex dissociate perceptual and physical shape similarity. PLoS Biol. 2008;6: 1459–1467. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060187 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kriegeskorte N, Mur M, Ruff D a, Kiani R, Bodurka J, Esteky H, et al. Matching categorical object representations in inferior temporal cortex of man and monkey. Neuron. Elsevier Ltd; 2008;60: 1126–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Peelen M V, He C, Han Z, Caramazza A, Bi Y. Nonvisual and Visual Object Shape Representations in Occipitotemporal Cortex: Evidence from Congenitally Blind and Sighted Adults. J Neurosci. 2014;34: 163–170. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1114-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fang Y, Han Z, Zhong S, Gong G, Song L, Liu F, et al. The semantic anatomical network: Evidence from healthy and brain-damaged patient populations. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36: 3499–3515. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22858 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Xia M, Wang J, He Y. BrainNet Viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS ONE. 2013;8: e68910 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068910 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kriegeskorte N, Mur M. Inverse MDS: inferring dissimilarity structure from multiple item arrangements. Front Psychol. Frontiers; 2012;3: 245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Schwartz MF, Kimberg DY, Walker GM, Brecher A, Faseyitan OK, Dell GS, et al. Neuroanatomical dissociation for taxonomic and thematic knowledge in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108: 8520–4. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014935108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Acosta-Cabronero J, Patterson K, Fryer TD, Hodges JR, Pengas G, Williams GB, et al. Atrophy, hypometabolism and white matter abnormalities in semantic dementia tell a coherent story. Brain. 2011;134: 2025–35. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Mahoney CJ, Malone IB, Ridgway GR, Buckley AH, Downey LE, Golden HL, et al. Neurobiology of Aging White matter tract signatures of the progressive aphasias. Neurobiol Aging. Elsevier Ltd; 2013;34: 1687–1699. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.12.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Galantucci S, Tartaglia MC, Wilson SM, Henry ML, Filippi M, Agosta F, et al. White matter damage in primary progressive aphasias: A diffusion tensor tractography study. Brain. 2011;134: 3011–3029. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Duffau H, Gatignol P, Mandonnet E, Peruzzi P, Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Capelle L. New insights into the anatomo-functional connectivity of the semantic system: a study using cortico-subcortical electrostimulations. Brain. 2005;128: 797–810. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh423 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Duffau H, Leroy M, Gatignol P. Cortico-subcortical organization of language networks in the right hemisphere: an electrostimulation study in left-handers. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46: 3197–209. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Rapp B, Cararnazza A. On the Distinction Between Deficits of Access and Deficits of Storage: A Question of Theory. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1993; 113–141. doi: 10.1080/02643299308253458 [Google Scholar]
- 37.Snowden JS, Goulding PJ, Neary D. Semantic dementia: a form of circumscribed cerebral atrophy. Behav Neurol. IOS Press; 1989; [Google Scholar]
- 38.Gorno-Tempini ML, Dronkers NF, Rankin KP, Ogar JM, Phengrasamy L, Rosen HJ, et al. Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol. Wiley Online Library; 2004;55: 335–46. doi: 10.1002/ana.10825 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Coutanche MN, Thompson-Schill SL. Creating Concepts from Converging Features in Human Cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2014; doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Peelen M V, Caramazza A. Conceptual object representations in human anterior temporal cortex. J Neurosci. 2012;32: 15728–15736. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1953-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Amedi A, Jacobson G, Hendler T, Malach R, Zohary E. Convergence of visual and tactile shape processing in the human lateral occipital complex. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12: 1202–1212. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.11.1202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hsu NS, Kraemer DJ, Oliver RT, Schlichting ML, Thompson-Schill SL. Color, context, and cognitive style: variations in color knowledge retrieval as a function of task and subject variables. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23: 2544–2557. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2011.21619 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Simmons WK, Ramjee V, Beauchamp MS, McRae K, Martin A, Barsalou LW. A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color. Neuropsychologia. Elsevier; 2007;45: 2802–2810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wang X, Han Z, He Y, Caramazza A, Song L, Bi Y. Where color rests: Spontaneous brain activity of bilateral fusiform and lingual regions predicts object color knowledge performance. Neuroimage. Elsevier Inc.; 2013;76: 252–263. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Fields RD. White matter in learning, cognition and psychiatric disorders. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31: 361–370. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.04.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Purger D, Gibson EM, Monje M. Myelin plasticity in the central nervous system. Neuropharmacology. Elsevier Ltd; 2015;110: 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2015.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Demerens C, Stankoff B, Logak M, Anglade P, Allinquant B, Couraud F, et al. Induction of myelination in the central nervous system by electrical activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93: 9887–92. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.18.9887 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Sánchez I, Hassinger L, Paskevich PA, Shine HD, Nixon RA. Oligodendroglia regulate the regional expansion of axon caliber and local accumulation of neurofilaments during development independently of myelin formation. J Neurosci. 1996;16: 5095–105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Egeland B, Sroufe LA, Erickson M. The developmental consequence of different patterns of maltreatment. Child Abuse Negl. Elsevier; 1983;7: 459–469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Zhao Y, Shi X, Qiu X, Lu W, Yang S, Li C, et al. Enriched environment increases the myelinated nerve fibers of aged rat corpus callosum. Anat Rec. Wiley Online Library; 2012;295: 999–1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Keller TA, Just MA. Altering cortical connectivity: remediation-induced changes in the white matter of poor readers. Neuron. Elsevier; 2009;64: 624–631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Bengtsson SL, Nagy Z, Skare S, Forsman L, Forssberg H, Ullén F. Extensive piano practicing has regionally specific effects on white matter development. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8: 1148–50. doi: 10.1038/nn1516 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ullén F. Is activity regulation of late myelination a plastic mechanism in the human nervous system? Neuron Glia Biol. Cambridge Univ Press; 2009;5: 29–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Scholz J, Klein MC, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H. Training induces changes in white-matter architecture. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12: 1370–1371. doi: 10.1038/nn.2412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Taubert M, Draganski B, Anwander A, Müller K, Horstmann A, Villringer A, et al. Dynamic properties of human brain structure: learning-related changes in cortical areas and associated fiber connections. J Neurosci. Soc Neuroscience; 2010;30: 11670–11677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Fischler I. Semantic facilitation without association in a lexical decision task. Mem Cognit. 1977;5: 335–339. doi: 10.3758/BF03197580 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Han Z, Ma Y, Gong G, Huang R, Song L, Bi Y. White matter pathway supporting phonological encoding in speech production: a multi-modal imaging study of brain damage patients. Brain Struct Funct. Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2014; 577–589. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0926-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Ashton EA, Takahashi C, Berg MJ, Goodman A, Totterman S, Ekholm S. Accuracy and reproducibility of manual and semiautomated quantification of MS lesions by MRI. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2003;17: 300–308. doi: 10.1002/jmri.10258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Gillebert CR, Humphreys GW, Mantini D. Automated delineation of stroke lesions using brain CT images. NeuroImage Clin. 2014;4: 540–548. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.03.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Guo D, Fridriksson J, Fillmore P, Rorden C, Yu H, Zheng K, et al. Automated lesion detection on MRI scans using combined unsupervised and supervised methods. BMC Med Imaging. BMC Medical Imaging; 2015;15: 50 doi: 10.1186/s12880-015-0092-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Pustina D, Coslett HB, Turkeltaub PE, Tustison N, Schwartz MF, Avants B. Automated segmentation of chronic stroke lesions using LINDA: Lesion identification with neighborhood data analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37: 1405–1421. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Fiez JA, Damasio H, Grabowski TJ. Lesion segmentation and manual warping to a reference brain: Intra-and interobserver reliability. Hum Brain Mapp. Wiley Online Library; 2000;9: 192–211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15: 273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Jeurissen B, Leemans A, Jones DK, Tournier JD, Sijbers J. Probabilistic fiber tracking using the residual bootstrap with constrained spherical deconvolution. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32: 461–479. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Johansen-Berg H, Rushworth MFS. Using Diffusion Imaging to Study Human Connectional Anatomy. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32: 75–94. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135735 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Zolal A, Juratli TA, Podlesek D, Rieger B, Kitzler HH, Linn J, et al. Probabilistic Tractography of the Cranial Nerves in Vestibular Schwannoma. World Neurosurg. Elsevier Inc; 2017;107: 47–53. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2017.07.102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Gong G, He Y, Concha L, Lebel C, Gross DW, Evans AC, et al. Mapping anatomical connectivity patterns of human cerebral cortex using in vivo diffusion tensor imaging tractography. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19: 524–536. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Chang C, Lin C. LIBSVM: A Library for Support Vector Machines. ACM Trans Intell Syst Technol. 2011;2: 1–39. doi: 10.1145/1961189.1961199 [Google Scholar]
- 69.Sotiropoulos SN, Jbabdi S, Xu J, Andersson JL, Moeller S, Auerbach EJ, et al. Advances in diffusion MRI acquisition and processing in the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage. Elsevier; 2013;80: 125–143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Yeh F-C, Wedeen VJ, Tseng W-YI. Generalized q-Sampling Imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. IEEE; 2010;29: 1626–1635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are available from Open Science Framework database (URL: https://osf.io/h7upk/?view_only=52b8f86cffa14ed4844e4a1b9cd429cb).