Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2018 Apr 5;7:e34395. doi: 10.7554/eLife.34395

Shared rhythmic subcortical GABAergic input to the entorhinal cortex and presubiculum

Tim James Viney 1,, Minas Salib 1, Abhilasha Joshi 1, Gunes Unal 1,, Naomi Berry 1,, Peter Somogyi 1,
Editor: Laura Colgin2
PMCID: PMC5908441  PMID: 29620525

Abstract

Rhythmic theta frequency (~5–12 Hz) oscillations coordinate neuronal synchrony and higher frequency oscillations across the cortex. Spatial navigation and context-dependent episodic memories are represented in several interconnected regions including the hippocampal and entorhinal cortices, but the cellular mechanisms for their dynamic coupling remain to be defined. Using monosynaptically-restricted retrograde viral tracing in mice, we identified a subcortical GABAergic input from the medial septum that terminated in the entorhinal cortex, with collaterals innervating the dorsal presubiculum. Extracellularly recording and labeling GABAergic entorhinal-projecting neurons in awake behaving mice show that these subcortical neurons, named orchid cells, fire in long rhythmic bursts during immobility and locomotion. Orchid cells discharge near the peak of hippocampal and entorhinal theta oscillations, couple to entorhinal gamma oscillations, and target subpopulations of extra-hippocampal GABAergic interneurons. Thus, orchid cells are a specialized source of rhythmic subcortical GABAergic modulation of ‘upstream’ and ‘downstream’ cortico-cortical circuits involved in mnemonic functions.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

When an animal explores an environment, hippocampal ‘place cells’ represent different locations forming a spatial map and discharging rhythmic bursts of action potentials in temporal sequences (O'Keefe and Recce, 1993). Other spatially-modulated cells contribute to navigation, including ‘head direction cells’ in the presubiculum (PrS) (Brandon et al., 2013; Taube et al., 1990), and ‘grid cells’ and ‘border cells’ in the entorhinal cortex (EC) (Hafting et al., 2005; Solstad et al., 2008). Coordination of neuronal assemblies, such as place cell sequences, is controlled by local GABAergic neurons setting temporal windows of differential excitability and synchronizing neuronal activity over various time scales reflected by underlying network oscillations (Cobb et al., 1995; Ylinen et al., 1995). Such temporal coordination includes theta oscillations (~5–12 Hz) most prominent during movement and REM sleep (Kramis et al., 1975), gamma oscillations (~30–120 Hz), which are phase-amplitude coupled to theta (Colgin et al., 2009; Lasztóczi and Klausberger, 2016; Schomburg et al., 2014; Soltesz and Deschenes, 1993) and hippocampal sharp-wave associated ripple oscillations (SWRs, 130–230 Hz) mainly occurring during slow wave sleep, awake immobility and consummatory behavior (Buzsáki, 1986). Different GABAergic cell types target distinct subcellular domains of principal cells, each cell type preferentially firing during specific phases of network oscillations, which together contribute to a temporal redistribution of inhibition from the axon initial segment, to the soma, and all the way to the distal dendrites of principal cells (Somogyi et al., 2014; Varga et al., 2012). Given the diversity of rhythmic firing patterns these cortical GABAergic cell types, which neurons and circuits support their temporal coordination?

In the basal forebrain, the medial septum and the vertical and horizontal diagonal band nuclei (MSDB) contain cholinergic, glutamatergic and GABAergic neurons that project to different areas of the temporal cortex (Kondo and Zaborszky, 2016; Manns et al., 2001; Unal et al., 2015). The GABAergic projections preferentially target cortical GABAergic neurons in the hippocampus and retrosplenial cortex (Freund and Antal, 1988; Freund and Gulyás, 1991; Unal et al., 2015). One group of rhythmically-firing GABAergic MSDB neurons in the mouse named ‘Teevra neurons’ show selective innervation of the CA3 region of the hippocampus where they preferentially target axo-axonic cells and cholecystokinin-expressing (CCK) interneurons (Joshi et al., 2017). The functional roles of MSDB neurons have been investigated in vivo at the population level, with network effects mostly being studied in the dorsal CA1 (CA1d) region of the hippocampus, although most MSDB afferents to the hippocampus target CA3 and the dentate gyrus (Freund and Antal, 1988). Optogenetic activation of cholinergic MSDB neurons has been shown to promote theta oscillations in CA1d and dentate gyrus (Mamad et al., 2015; Vandecasteele et al., 2014). Glutamatergic MSDB neurons contribute to speed signals in both CA1d and medial EC (Fuhrmann et al., 2015; Justus et al., 2017), and subpopulations of GABAergic MSDB terminals in CA1d become activated during locomotion and salient stimuli (Kaifosh et al., 2013). In vitro, MSDB neurons have been shown to modulate the firing of hippocampal pyramidal cells and interneurons (Huh et al., 2010; Leão et al., 2012; Tóth et al., 1997). Recently, target neurons in the EC have been physiologically characterized based on their inputs from the MSDB, including those receiving parvalbumin (PV)-expressing input (Fuchs et al., 2016; Gonzalez-Sulser et al., 2014) and cholinergic input (Desikan et al., 2018). Other innervated cortical regions have received little attention (but see [Unal et al., 2015]). Does the area-selectivity of individual cholinergic MSDB neurons (Wu et al., 2014) and the combined area and synaptic target-neuron selectivity of single recorded and labeled GABAergic Teevra cells (Joshi et al., 2017) apply to other areas of the cortex innervated by the MSDB?

Neurons in the MSDB exhibit oscillatory firing at theta frequency, with rhythmically-firing neurons showing preferential coupling to different phases of theta cycles (Dragoi et al., 1999; Joshi et al., 2017; King et al., 1998; Petsche et al., 1962). Lesions or pharmacological inactivation of the MSDB result in impairments in spatial learning, a disruption of theta rhythmicity and grid cell firing dynamics in the EC, and a marked reduction in hippocampal theta power (Brandon et al., 2013; Brito and Thomas, 1981; Hinman et al., 2016; Jeffery et al., 1995; Koenig et al., 2011; McNaughton et al., 2006; Yoder and Pang, 2005). Subpopulations of GABAergic MSDB neurons, which include PV+ Teevra neurons (Joshi et al., 2017) and other PV+ neurons (Borhegyi et al., 2004; Simon et al., 2006; Varga et al., 2008), likely represent some of the strong rhythmic bursting neurons that have been recorded in freely moving rats (King et al., 1998). It is currently unknown how different types of rhythmic bursting neurons contribute to cortical circuits outside the CA3 area (Joshi et al., 2017). This is due to the wide range of preferred theta phase coupling by individual cells, the general lack of information on the topography of MSDB projections covering both the hippocampal formation and extra-hippocampal cortical regions, and until recently, a lack of information on single long-range projection axons. We have investigated the behavior-dependent firing patterns, axonal projections and cortical targets of GABAergic MSDB neurons that projected to the EC in awake mice.

Results

Medial septal neurons projecting to the entorhinal cortex

We investigated MSDB projections to the EC by injecting a monosynaptically-restricted retrograde viral tracer, PRV-hSyn-Cre (Oyibo et al., 2014), into the caudo-dorsal EC (Figure 1—figure supplement 1a,b) and a Cre-dependent adeno-associated virus (AAV) encoding EYFP into the MSDB. The PRV-hSyn-Cre virus, a mutant pseudorabies virus of the alpha-herpesvirus subfamily, is non-cytotoxic and highly tropic, resulting in Cre expression only in neurons that directly project to the injection site. After ≥2 weeks incubation, strong Cre-dependent EYFP expression in neurons was mainly in the rostral part of the dorsal medial septum (MS; Figure 1a, Figure 1—figure supplement 1c) representing 60.5% of retrogradely-labeled neurons (n = 129 cells from six mice; mean ± s.d. 21.5 ± 11.7 total EYFP+ neurons/mouse). The remainder were distributed in the vertical DB (24.8%), horizontal DB (12.4%) and lateral septum (2.3%).

Figure 1. Medial septal GABAergic neurons terminating in the entorhinal cortex also innervate the dorsal presubiculum and retrosplenial cortex.

(a) Coronal section showing EC-projecting GFP-immunoreactive neurons restricted to the dorsal MS, following injection of PRV-hSyn-Cre into the caudo-dorsal EC and AAVDIO-EYFP into the MSDB (animal MS60). Top right, enlarged view of the boxed region. Bottom right, horizontal section (animal MS77) showing GFP-immunoreactive neurons (cyan) restricted to the rostral part of the dorsal MS, delineated by PV immunoreactivity (magenta). (b) Virally-labeled axon collaterals of EC-projecting MSDB neurons densely innervating extra-hippocampal regions (left, 1) and sparsely innervating the DG and CA1 (right, 2) from a single coronal section (animal MS60). Inset, locations of Figures 1 and 2 (boxes). (c) A subset of axon terminals from EC-projecting MSDB neurons in the RSg (GFP, cyan) are immunoreactive for VGAT (yellow, arrows) (animal MS66). (d) Coronal section of the PrSd (animal MS60) with axon collaterals and terminals from EC-projecting MSDB neurons. Inset, enlarged view of boxed region (arrows, axon terminals). (e) An EC-projecting medial septal neuron soma (asterisk, from animal MS66) immunoreactive for GFP (cyan, plasma membrane) was weakly immunoreactive for PV (magenta), SATB1 (yellow, nucleus) and mGluR1a (green, membrane). Note GFP-negative neurons with similar (arrows) or different (arrowheads) molecular profiles. (f) Quantification of PV (P), SATB1 (S) and mGluR1a (G) immunoreactivity for virally-labeled EC-projecting neurons located in the dorsal MS (data from six animals). Scale bars (μm): (a) 200 (left image), 100 (right images); (b) 100 (inset 500); (c) 5; (d) 100, inset 10; (e) 10. Image type: (a–b) Widefield epifluorescence, reverse contrast, 70 μm thick sections. (c) confocal image, single optical section, 0.31 μm thick; (d) confocal image, reverse contrast, maximum intensity z-projection, 35 sections, 30.96 μm thick; (e) confocal image, single optical section, 0.38 μm thick. LS, lateral septum; cc, corpus callosum; ac, anterior commissure.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Medial septal neurons terminating in the entorhinal cortex also innervate the dorsal presubiculum.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(a) Distribution of EYFP +axons (reverse contrast fluorescence) from the MSDB in the caudal EC across three 70 µm-thick coronal sections from the same animal (MS60). Left, three sections caudal of the injection site; middle, two sections caudal of the injection site. Boxed regions are enlarged below. Right, three sections rostral of the injection site, with the location of parasubiculum (PaS) and superficial layers of the lateral EC (lEC) marked by Wfs1 immunoreactivity (top). Blood vessel, b.v. Axons are distributed across all EC layers. (b) Left, the injection site of animal MS60 is marked by an asterisk. Right, location of EYFP+ axons from the MSDB in the dorsal EC (horizontal section, animal MS78). (c) Expanded view of a horizontal section (see Figure 1a) showing GFP-immunoreactive EC-projecting medial septal neurons (cyan) restricted to the rostro-dorsal MS (animal MS77). PV, magenta. (d) Coronal section of the temporal cortex (animal MS60, see Figure 1b) with virally-labeled axon collaterals of EC-projecting MSDB neurons innervating extra-hippocampal regions (PrSd, RSg). Top right, enlarged view of boxed region. V1, primary visual cortex; mEC, medial EC; SUB, subiculum. Image type: (a–d) Widefield epifluorescence, 70 μm thick sections. Images in (a), (b) (right) and (c) are montages. Scale bars (µm): (a) left: top 500, bottom 200; middle: top 500, bottom 100; right: 200; (b) 200; (c) 100; (d) 250, inset 100.

Virally labeled medial septal axons with extensive terminals were observed in all layers of the EC (n = 4 mice, from 18 coronal or horizontal sections), with collaterals traveling both radially and horizontally (Figure 1—figure supplement 1a,b). These EC-projecting axons also gave dense collaterals in other extra-hippocampal regions where they formed terminals (n = 6 mice; Figure 1b–d, Figure 1—figure supplement 1d). We quantified the distribution of axonal collaterals in hippocampal and extra-hippocampal cortical regions (n = 3 mice; three sections per animal; total 274 axons). The proportion of axonal branches in the dorsal presubiculum (PrSd) and granular retrosplenial cortex (RSg) (median: 37%; interquartile range (IQR): 34.3–47.2%) was substantially greater than in CA1, the dentate gyrus and the dorsal subiculum (SUBd) (median: 5%; IQR: 2.4–9%), where only rare axonal collaterals were observed (p=1.6 × 10−7, Kruskal-Wallis test). Within the PrSd and RSg, 71.0 ± 25.9% of EYFP+ axonal terminals (mean ±s.d, n = 1046/1416 counted terminals within 12 sampled areas from three mice) were immunoreactive for vesicular GABA transporter (VGAT, Figure 1c). Together these results demonstrate that a large subpopulation of GABAergic EC-projecting MSDB neurons innervate other extra-hippocampal areas, primarily the PrSd and RSg.

Neuronal subpopulations in the MSDB can be defined by the expression of different molecules (Wei et al., 2012), and combinational expression profiles help define distinct cell types (Viney et al., 2013). We observed that metabotropic glutamate receptor 1a (mGluR1a), along with the transcription factor SATB1 (Huang et al., 2011), show differential immunoreactivity with parvalbumin (PV). As in the cortex, PV neurons in the MSDB represent a subpopulation of GABAergic neurons, but PV is expressed by many different kinds of neurons (Simon et al., 2006; Varga et al., 2008; Viney et al., 2013). GABAergic Teevra neurons are immunopositive (+) for PV and SATB1 but lack detectable immunoreactivity (–) for mGluR1a (Joshi et al., 2017). Within the MS, 21.0% of EC-projecting (EYFP+) neurons were triple immunopositive for PV, SATB1, and mGluR1a, while 37.1% showed no detectable signal for these three molecules (n = 62 tested neurons, six mice; Figure 1e,f). Neurons found nearby as PV+/SATB1+/mGluR1a– did not project to the EC (Figure 1f), consistent with the profile of CA3-projecting Teevra cells (Joshi et al., 2017). Within the DB, 44.8% of neurons were only SATB1+ with the remainder of cells being triple immunonegative (n = 29 tested neurons, six mice). A subset of EYFP+ neurons were tested for choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) (Kondo and Zaborszky, 2016; Unal et al., 2015), PV and mGluR1a. Only 2/13 tested were ChAT+/PV–/mGluR1a–, 1/13 was only mGluR1a+, and 10/13 lacked detectable immunoreactivity (ChAT–/PV–/mGluR1a–). The PV– and ChAT– MSDB neurons probably comprise both GABAergic and glutamatergic EC-projecting neurons (Fuchs et al., 2016; Gonzalez-Sulser et al., 2014; Justus et al., 2017).

Firing patterns of septo-entorhinal neurons

To determine the temporal patterns of signals sent from the basal forebrain to the EC, we lowered glass electrodes into the MSDB in drug-free awake head-restrained mice to record neurons extracellularly, then visualized their axon collaterals and terminals in the cortex by juxtacellular labeling (Figure 2). Eight labeled neurons had projection axons heading towards the EC via the dorsal fornix, with 5/8 forming thin collaterals and terminals within deep layers of the caudo-dorsal EC (Figure 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 1, Tables 1 and 2). The main axons of the other three neurons faded before collateralization in the EC due to insufficient labeling (Table 1). Nevertheless, all eight neurons showed strong rhythmic burst firing occurring during both locomotion and immobility (Figures 2a,b,d,e and 3f,g), with longer bursts displaying accommodation (Figures 2a,b and 3f,g). Approximately 10% of bursts were >200 ms, and were often associated with high gamma power in hippocampal LFPs during immobility (Figures 2a,b and 3g) (Carr et al., 2012).

Figure 2. Firing patterns of medial septal neurons projecting to the EC.

(a) Top, rhythmic burst firing of EC-projecting medial septal neuron TV50a during locomotion. CA1 LFP, local field potential in SP. Red, examples of bursts of spikes associated with peaks of CA1 theta oscillations. Bottom, rhythmic burst firing during immobility with some spikes during some SWRs (magenta; 130–230 Hz filtered CA1 LFP). Examples of long accommodating bursts are shown below the asterisks. (b) Same as in (a) for another EC-projecting neuron, MS14i, which fires less rhythmic bursts at high rate during locomotion (top) and rhythmic bursts during immobility (bottom). Red bars, examples of bursts spanning two theta cycles. (c) Reconstruction of the soma, dendrites, and axon (ax; arrows: towards dorsal fornix) of MS14i in the tip of dorsal MS. Dashed line, midline. Local axon collaterals, which give rise to terminals, are marked by arrowheads. Cut ends not included in the reconstruction are marked by small bars. Scale bar, 50 μm. (d,e) Autocorrelograms of spikes during locomotion and immobility for TV50a (d) and MS14i (e). (f) Firing rate versus CA1 theta phase plots show preferential firing near the peak of theta during movement (locomotion and small movements) for eight labeled neurons (color-coded) projecting to extra-hippocampal regions. Black, sinusoid representation of two idealized cycles. Data are duplicated to represent rhythmicity. (g) Left, part of the time series from (a) during locomotion displaying several theta cycles in the unfiltered LFP (thick arrows mark theta troughs), and 55–80 Hz filtered LFP mid-gamma cycles (thin arrows mark high amplitude gamma troughs). Note that gamma troughs occur mostly at the peak of theta cycles. Right, event correlation of detected mid-gamma troughs relative to theta troughs from the MS14i recorded CA1d LFP. Note peak around 100 ms corresponding to the peak of theta cycles. (h) Normalized spike count versus CA1 mid-gamma phase for all significantly coupled EC-projecting neurons (see also Table 3).

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Rhythmicity of identified orchid cells.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Autocorrelograms of spikes for identified orchid cells during different behavioral states. Note that cells fire rhythmically during all behavioral states.

Figure 3. Firing patterns and cortical target regions of a GABAergic medial septal orchid cell.

(a) Reconstruction of part of the axon of neuron TV58g axon (cortical regions color-coded). Black dashed line, rostral (left) and caudal (right) innervation zones. Pie chart, number (parentheses) and proportion (%) of axonal varicosities. Projection axon enters the cortex via the dorsal fornix (green dashed line). (b) Presubicular terminals of TV58g (cyan) were VGAT+ (yellow, arrows). (c) Micrograph (left, asterisk) and reconstruction (right) of the soma and dendrites (blue) and axon (cyan, ax) of TV58g in the tip of dorsal MS. Black dashed line, midline. A local axon collateral is marked by an arrowhead. A cut dendrite is marked by a small bar. (d) The soma of TV58g (cyan, neurobiotin, arrow) was SATB1+ (green, nuclei) and PV+ (yellow). Note similar profiles of adjacent neurons. (e) Dendrites of TV58g (cyan) were mGluR1a+ (magenta, arrows). (f) Rhythmic burst firing during locomotion. CA1 LFP, local field potential in SP. Red, examples of detected peaks of CA1 theta oscillations and associated MS neuron spikes. Note preferential coupling of bursts to theta peak. (g) Rhythmic burst firing during immobility with some spikes during some SWRs (magenta; ripples, 130–230 Hz filtered CA1 LFP). Examples of accommodating bursts are shown below asterisks in (f) and (g). Nb, neurobiotin. FC, fasciola cinereum, AB, angular bundle, LS, lateral septum, vDB, vertical diagonal band. Images (z-thickness in µm and z-projection type): (d) 2.3, maximum, (b) 2.61, maximum, (e) 0.37, single optical section. Scale bars (µm): a, 500, b,e, 5, c, 50, inset, 500, d, 10.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Location of the axons of orchid cells in the caudo-dorsal entorhinal cortex.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(a) Consecutive series of ‘reference’ coronal sections through the caudal temporal cortex in three columns (top to bottom;~70 µm thickness; one brain) immunoreacted for CB and PV and VGluT2 (three channels per section; widefield epifluorescence montages, reverse contrast). (b) Actual coronal sections of brains TV58, TV50, MS14, MS19 and AJ50 containing the axons of recorded and labeled orchid cells (not visible) aligned relative to reference sections in (a). Asterisks (cyan) mark exact positions of orchid cell axons and/or terminals in the EC. Two TV58 sections and one of TV50 have missing ventral and dorsal regions, respectively. Images are from diaminobenzidine (DAB)-based horseradish peroxidase (HRP) reacted sections except the second row (TV58g section) that shows widefield fluorescence of a streptavidin-conjugated fluorophore along with immunoreactivity for PV and double-immunoreacted for ChAT and SATB1, delineating layers. Sections from MS14 and MS19 have been flipped horizontally to appear in the same orientation as the others. PaS, parasubiculum. Scale bar, 1 mm.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Location of labeled orchid cell axon in the dorsal presubiculum (I).

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

Reference images of in situ hybridization expression from the Allen Brain Atlas at different rostro-caudal levels (top to bottom) of coronal mouse brain sections for Slc17a6 (a), Satb2 (b), Etv1 (c) and Nos1 (d) genes. Note strong Nos1 mRNA expression in PrS, but absence in the adjacent areas. Asterisks, filled circles and open circles (all cyan): examples of approximate locations of orchid cells TV58g, AJ50j and AJ50h axon terminals and/or thin collaterals. Granular retrosplenial cortex, RSg; dorsal subiculum, SUBd.
Figure 3—figure supplement 3. Location of labeled orchid cell axon in the dorsal presubiculum (II).

Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

Reference images of in situ hybridization expression from the Allen Brain Atlas at different rostro-caudal levels (top to bottom) of coronal mouse brain sections for Slc17a6 (a) Satb2 (b) and Etv1 (c) genes for the delineation of the dorsal presubiculum. (d) Two representative light microscopic coronal sections of calbindin immunoreactivity (HRP-reaction) show delineation of layers 1–6. Layer two is a compact layer, and layer four is an interplexiform layer. Arrow, part of layer two enriched with CB-immunopositive neurons. Scale bars, 100 µm. (e) Representative sagittal sections of the three genes from (a–c). Asterisks, filled circles and open circles (all cyan): examples of approximate locations of orchid cells TV58g, AJ50j and AJ50h axon terminals and/or thin collaterals; the most caudal filled circle in (b) corresponds to the most caudal location of AJ50j projection axon. Entorhinal cortex, EC.
Figure 3—figure supplement 4. Delineation of synaptic target regions of orchid cells.

Figure 3—figure supplement 4.

(a) Reference images of in situ hybridization expression for Amigo2 highlights CA2 and the FC at different levels of the septal pole of the hippocampus (coronal sections). Top to bottom, rostral to caudal examples. Note that the FC is caudal of medial CA2 (CA2m). Bottom image, enlarged view of innervated FC region. (b,c,) In situ hybridization data for Tmem178 delineates CA3, CA2 and FC (b) and layer 6 of EC (c) in coronal sections. (d), In situ hybridization data for Tmem178 delineates the PaS in caudal coronal sections. Magnification in a,b) are the same; others are different. Asterisks, filled circles and open circles (all cyan): examples of approximate locations of orchid cells TV58g, AJ50j and AJ50h axon terminals and/or thin collaterals; filled circle in c) corresponds to the most caudal location of AJ50j projection axon. Corpus callosum, CC; rhinal sulcus, S. All images obtained from the Allen Brain Atlas.
Figure 3—figure supplement 5. Resemblance of a medial septal axon to a moth orchid.

Figure 3—figure supplement 5.

Top, Phalaenopsis orchid. Bottom, orchid cell cortical axon (TV58g).
Figure 3—video 1. Reconstruction of orchid cell TV58g.
Download video file (4.4MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34395.014
Green arrow, dorsal; red arrow, lateral; blue arrow, caudal.

Table 1. Axon terminal distribution and firing patterns of identified orchid cells.

All projection axons traveled in the dorsal fornix. *Varicosities were sampled from different numbers of brain sections per labeled neuron. **Main axon observed dorsal of SUBd/PrSd, too weak to observe collaterals. ***Main axon faded away just rostral of caudo-dorsal EC. Abbreviations: c, fine axon collaterals observed; u, unknown/unavailable; n.o., not observed. Spike burst defined as >3 spikes with interspike intervals (ISIs) < 40 ms. Firing rates and burst incidence (Hz) are expressed as mean of 1 s bins ± s.d.; intraburst frequency (Hz) as mean ±s.d.; burst duration and interburst interval (ms) are expressed as median and interquartile range. Abbreviations: LM, locomotion; SM, small movements including limbs, tail, and shifts in posture; WS, high-frequency whisking and/or sniffing in the absence of other movements; IM, immobility. KS, Kolmogorov-Smirnov; SP, stratum pyramidale; SO, stratum oriens.

Cell name TV58g TV50a MS14i MS19b AJ50j AJ50h AJ44j MS74b
Target hemisphere Right Right Left Left Left Right Right Right
Varicosities per region (n sampled)* Medial septum 61 u 146 n.o. n.o. 51 n.o. u
Dentate gyrus n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. u
CA3 n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. u u
CA2 n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. u u
CA1 n.o. n.o. (c) n.o. n.o. n.o. u u
SUBd 33 n.o. 3 104 n.o. c u u
FC 22 (c) n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. u u
RSg 85 (L6) n.o. n.o. n.o. 51 n.o. u u
Angular Bundle 55 11 n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. u u**
PrSd L1-2 n.o. 3 8 15 c n.o. u u
PrSd L3 n.o. 10 31 149 n.o. c u u
PrSd L5-6 635 66 45 34 27 29 u u
PaS n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. n.o. u u
EC 695 c 32 63 (c)*** 6 u u
Labeling strength +++ ++ ++ ++ ++ ++ + +
Behavioral states Max speed (cm/s) u u 6.3 18.8 17.2 16.5 12.0 u
Firing rate LM 48.4 ± 6.0 51.4 ± 10.6 67.8 ± 12.9 43.4 ± 9.3 65.3 ± 11.7 33.6 ± 9.6 76.7 ± 11.0 55.0 ± 11.0
Firing rate SM 41.6 ± 5.3 47.6 ± 8.8 51.0 ± 12.7 33.3 ± 8.6 42.5 ± 9.7 27.7 ± 3.8 43.8 ± 9.9 u
Firing rate WS 47.1 ± 9.3 u 47.9 ± 9.0 33.2 ± 8.0 34.7 ± 6.1 24.3 ± 4.9 37.7 ± 2.5 34.2 ± 14.9
Firing rate IM 44.2 ± 12.0 46.3 ± 10.5 48.2 ± 10.0 31.9 ± 5.2 46.6 ± 9.9 25.4 ± 7.6 38.1 ± 6.8 37.6 ± 16.0
Burst incidence LM 5.0 ± 1.0 6.3 ± 0.9 3.8 ± 1.7 4.4 ± 0.9 6.7 ± 1.0 4.1 ± 1.7 6.2 ± 1.9 5.2 ± 1.3
Burst incidence SM 4.4 ± 0.5 5.7 ± 1.0 4.7 ± 1.1 3.6 ± 1.1 5.5 ± 1.3 3.3 ± 0.8 5.6 ± 1.2 u
Burst incidence WS 4.4 ± 0.9 u 4.6 ± 1.1 4.4 ± 1.3 4.6 ± 1.0 2.8 ± 0.8 6.0 ± 1.0 2.9 ± 1.4
Burst incidence IM 4.2 ± 1.2 5.0 ± 0.9 4.9 ± 0.9 4.5 ± 1.3 5.4 ± 1.0 3.1 ± 1.3 5.3 ± 1.1 3.5 ± 0.9
CA1d theta (5 – 12 Hz) LFP measurement SO/SP SP upper SP SP SP SP SP SP
Preferred theta phase 159° 176° 152° 167° 177° 264° 130° 233°
Mean vector length (r) 0.37 0.50 0.23 0.27 0.51 0.35 0.44 0.34
Rayleigh P-value <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001
Total n spikes 1081 2679 6659 5143 1743 1413 860 578
Spikes per cycle (mean ± s.d.) 6.8 ± 3.1 6.7 ± 3.0 8.7 ± 4.2 7.2 ± 3.4 7.2 ± 3.1 4.6 ± 1.9 7.7 ± 3.3 8.1 ± 3.2

Table 2. Details of juxtacellular labeling.

Number of juxtacellular labeling attempts on the final recording day for each animal (neurobiotin is metabolized within 24 hr). Letters refer to individually recorded neurons. E.g. in animal TV58, recorded neurons ‘e’ and ‘g’ were selected for juxtacellular labeling; in animal TV50 only neuron ‘a’ was recorded and labeled. A ‘labeling attempt’ refers to the application of current pulses to a recorded neuron. Modulation refers to successful entrainment of action potentials to the duration of positive current pulses (Pinault, 1996). Strength and duration of modulation are also estimated in order to predict the labeling strength (Table 1).

Animal name Labeling attempts from single neurons Modulated neurons Recovered neurons
TV58 2 (e, g) 1 (g, very strong, >5 min; rostral of e) 1 (g)
TV50 1 (a) 1 (a, strong, 77 s) 1 (a, damaged)
MS14 1 (i) 1 (i, strong, 1 min) One strong (i); one weak, dorsal of i, dendrites cross with i, potentially gap-junction coupled
MS19 1 (b) 1 (b, strong, 30 s) 1 (b)
AJ50 4 (g-j) 3 (g, strong, 6 s, lost; h strong 30 s, dorsal of g; j, strong, 2 min, rostral of h) Two strong (h, j); one very weak; dorsal of h, potentially gap-junction coupled
AJ44 1 (j) 1 (j, strong, 12 s) 1 (j)
MS74 1 (b) 1 (b, strong, 2 min) 1 (b, damaged)
MS17 1 (k) 1 (k, strong, 30 s, lost cell after) 1 (k)

We simultaneously recorded the local field potential (LFP) in strata pyramidale/oriens of the dorsal CA1 (CA1d) region of the hippocampus, a reliable and stable reference location for comparing different neurons. All neurons were significantly coupled to theta oscillations, with 6/8 preferentially firing around the peak, and 2/8 on the descending phase (p<0.0001 for all cells, Rayleigh test for uniformity; Figures 2a,b,f and 3f, Table 1). Cortical principal and GABAergic neurons couple to gamma oscillations nested within theta cycles, with ~55–80 Hz ‘mid-gamma’ oscillations arriving in CA1 directly from the EC around the peak of pyramidale/oriens theta cycles (Colgin et al., 2009; Harris et al., 2003; Lasztóczi and Klausberger, 2016; Schomburg et al., 2014; Soltesz and Deschenes, 1993; Somogyi et al., 2014). As expected, mid-gamma oscillations were coupled to theta cycles preferentially at the theta peak (detected from CA1d LFPs of n = 7/8 animals with recorded septo-entorhinal neurons, Figure 2g). Mid-gamma oscillations were also detected during ‘large amplitude irregular activity’ (LIA) of the LFP (between periods of high power theta oscillations, also referred to as ‘non-theta’; Figures 2a,b and 3g). A subset of neurons preferentially coupled to the peak of mid-gamma oscillations (p<0.001 for n = 3/7 tested neurons, Rayleigh test; Figure 2h, Table 3), suggesting that theta-coupled septo-entorhinal neurons participate in entorhinal cortical gamma activity (Chrobak et al., 2000).

Table 3. Firing patterns of identified orchid cells.

Abbreviations: u, unknown/unavailable. Spike burst defined as > 3 spikes with ISIs < 40 ms. Intraburst frequency (Hz) is expressed as mean ± s.d.; burst duration and interburst interval (ms) as median and interquartile range. LM, locomotion; SM, small movements including limbs, tail, and shifts in posture; WS, high-frequency whisking and/or sniffing in the absence of other movements; IM, immobility. KS, Kolmogorov-Smirnov.

Cell name TV58g TV50a MS14i MS19b AJ50j AJ50h AJ44j MS74b
Behavioral states Burst duration LM 87.0, 79.8 66.8, 30.5 121.0, 193.0 96.2, 57.5 63.2, 22.6 70.1, 36.3 68.2, 48.9 80.1, 40.1
Burst duration SM 86.7, 47.5 71., 42.0 84.2, 56.7 85.7, 53.4 54.0, 23.8 65.2, 56.3 70.1, 38.3 u
Burst duration WS 91.8, 77.1 u 91.7, 61.5 92.0, 56.8 59.2, 30.2 68.3, 35.5 52.4, 30.3 91.5, 91.9
Burst duration IM 94.6, 65.5 70.2, 46.0 81.1, 48.9 100.0, 57.3 57.8, 33.3 73.9, 33.7 68.6, 42.7 97.0, 75.0
Two-sample KS test P-value
burst duration LM-IM
0.5928 0.0021 <0.0001 0.6573 0.0003 0.3998 0.1947 0.0293
Interburst interval LM 187.7, 80.9 146.0, 35.2 179.6, 162.5 193.5, 91.8 135.8, 41.3 160.0, 148.9 129.0, 52.1 155.3, 86.4
Interburst interval SM 214.2, 81.2 159.9, 49.3 178.7, 99.1 230.1, 166.5 159.9, 66.1 257.6, 209.6 158.2, 67.6 u
Interburst interval WS 205.1, 100.3 u 189.9, 105.1 255.4, 180.0 178.1, 137.8 291.7, 315.5 157.3, 73.9 254.5, 229.1
Interburst interval IM 203.3, 94.5 177.3, 92.3 180.3, 87.5 238.3, 161.8 163.3, 66.2 255.8, 201.0 161.5, 61.7 249.0, 135.2
Two sample KS test P-value
interburst interval LM-IM
0.0725 <0.0001 0.0039 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001
Intraburst frequency LM 95.9 ± 22.1 111.4 ± 24.8 95.8 ± 24.4 83.7 ± 24.6 136.1, 27.2 75.9 ± 23.6 131.4 ± 30.9 95.2 ± 20.2
Intraburst frequency SM 84.9 ± 19.5 105.7 ± 24.5 99.7 ± 30.0 82.8 ± 27.5 121.0, 27.2 79.2 ± 17.7 96.0 ± 24.5 u
Intraburst frequency WS 96.6 ± 24.3 u 93.1 ± 24.3 80.7 ± 22.7 110.6, 27.4 71.8 ± 10.2 96.4 ± 13.6 75.6 ± 16.1
Intraburst frequency IM 94.7 ± 22.8 113.1 ± 34.0 100.6 ± 28.0 77.6 ± 19.7 124.0, 32.6 78.1 ± 16.8 89.3 ± 18.2 79.3 ± 22.9
Two sample KS test P-value intraburst freq LM-IM 0.3102 0.0693 0.0357 0.0180 <0.0001 0.0602 <0.0001 <0.0001
CA1d mid-gamma (55 – 80 Hz) Preferred gamma phase u u 164° 158° u 191° u u
Mean vector length (r) u u 0.04 0.25 u 0.19 u u
Rayleigh P-value u 0.0860 0.0008 <0.0001 0.6225 <0.0001 0.0799 0.4700
Total n spikes u 1240 3553 2052 1445 398 579 437
CA1d SWRs
(130 – 230 Hz)
n SWRs 34 40 57 58 0 0 0 16
Mean rate inside (Hz) 57.08 31.37 57.71 4.88 u u u u
Lambda rate outside (Hz) 44.82 49.14 48.96 33.16 u u u u
Poisson P-value 0.0181 0.0015 0.0238 <0.0001 u u u u

GABAergic orchid cells project to multiple extra-hippocampal regions

As predicted by the viral tracing, single recorded and labeled EC-projecting neurons innervated multiple extra-hippocampal regions, with the majority of their terminals outside the EC located in the PrSd (Figures 3 and 4, Figure 3—figure supplement 2, Figure 3—figure supplement 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 4, Table 1, Figure 3—video 1). Extracellularly recorded EC-projecting neurons were immunopositive for PV (n = 8/8 tested cells), SATB1 (n = 6/6), and mGluR1a (n = 6/6), and their terminals were immunoreactive for VGAT (n = 2/2 tested cells; Figure 3b,d,e, Table 4). Based on the shared cortical target regions (Table 1), similarity in rhythmic burst firing patterns (Figure 2a,b, Figure 3f,g; see below), strong theta coupling (Figure 2f), overlapping molecular profiles (Table 4), and the resemblance of the axon to Phalaenopsis orchids (Figure 3—figure supplement 5), we name these GABAergic PV+ neurons ‘orchid cells’.

Figure 4. Postsynaptic GABAergic target neurons of a GABAergic orchid cell, TV58g.

Figure 4.

(a) Selected collaterals and varicosities (see Figure 2a). Asterisk, location of target neuron in b. (b) Boutons (cyan, neurobiotin, arrows) in the PrSd (asterisk in a) are apposed to a neuron immunopositive for mGluR1a (i, top, magenta, somato-dendritic membrane), SATB1 (ii, middle, green, nucleus), and PV (iii, bottom, green) but lacking detectable immunoreactivity for both nNOS (ii, yellow) and CB (iii, yellow). Arrowheads in iii (bottom right) highlight a CB-immunoreactive process. Two neighboring PV-immunonegative neurons (1, 2) were not targeted. (c) Electron micrographs of synapses (arrows) made by boutons (b) of TV58g on interneurons in the PrSd with the proximal dendrite of a PV+ cell (i, b11 in b), (ii) the dendrite of an unknown interneuron and (iii) soma of another interneuron. (d) Main axon and collateral (arrow, branch point) along with terminals surrounding a neuron in layer 6 of RSg (reverse contrast, confocal microscopic image). The target neuron contains a high level of endogenous biotin (high background), revealed by the streptavidin-conjugated fluorophore signal. (e) The target neuron soma (asterisk) innervated by multiple terminals (cyan) from (d) showed nuclear immunoreactivity for SATB1 (yellow). (f) The dendrite of the target showed positive immunoreactivity for PV (green); arrows highlight two terminals in close apposition. Nb, neurobiotin. (g) Schematic summary of the data presented in Figures 3 and 4 and Tables 1 and 6. Green circles represent the relative proportions of cortical axon terminals from all orchid cells (see Table 1). Immunoreactivity of synaptic target neurons is color-coded based on innervated cortical region. Innervated cortical interneurons may target principal cells (dark gray) on the dendrites, soma or axon initial segment. Bottom right, two idealized theta cycles with representative orchid cell spike bursts at the peak of CA1d theta and EC theta (see Figure 6). Images (z-thickness in µm and z-projection type): (b) 3.63/4.3/4.76, (i/ii/iii), maximum, (d) 22.09 standard deviation, (e) 2.8 maximum, (f) 2.88 maximum. Scale bars: (a) 100 µm, (b,e,f) 10 µm, (c) 0.5 µm (applies to i/ii/iii), (d) 20 µm.

Table 4. Immunohistochemical profiles of orchid neurons.

Positive (+) or negative (-) immunoreactivity observed within subcellular domain: s, soma; n, nucleus; d, proximal dendrite; a, axon; t, axon terminals; u, unknown (unavailable or inconclusive). *Soma was not recovered; dendrites were beaded indicative of a cell damaged during labeling. **No axon was recovered.

Cell name TV58g TV50a* MS14i MS19b AJ50h AJ50j AJ44j MS74b* MS17k**
Immunoreactivity PV ad+ a+ ds+ a+ a+ a+ a+ a+ ds+
SATB1 n+ u n+ n+ n+ n+ n+ u n+
mGluR1a d+ u d+ s+ ds+ ds+ ds+ u ds+
VGAT t+ u u u t+ u u u u
NK1R d- u u s- u s- s- u ds+
PCP4 d+ a+ ds+ u u u u u u
SMI32 d+ u u u u u u u u
Calbindin d- ad- d- ad- u u u a- d-
Calretinin d- ad- d- ad- u u u u d-
ChAT sd- a- u u u u u u d-
nNOS sd- u u u u u u u u

Within the dorsal tip of the MS, orchid cell dendritic trees had prominent apical and basal dendrites (Figures 2c and 3c). Dendrites were often twisted, exhibited filopodia, with the basal dendrites ending in specialized filopodia-like distal tips (Figures 3c and 2c). Orchid cells had 6.0 ± 1.8 primary dendrites (mean ±s.d., n = 4 cells) extending from all axes, which were similar to the distributions of CA3-projecting Teevra cell dendrites (Joshi et al., 2017) (χ2 = 0.02, p=0.8817, n = 4 orchid cells versus n = 4 Teevra cells). The hooked (e.g. MS14i, AJ50h, AJ50j) or straight (e.g. TV58g) projection axons could originate from the soma, the basal (MS14i, Figure 2c) or apical (TV58g, Figure 3c) dendrite, with local collaterals forming terminals within the MS. Basket-like groups of axon terminals were observed around other somata. We conclude that PV+/SATB1+/mGluR1a + orchid cells provide rhythmic bursts of GABAergic input to specific extra-hippocampal regions of the temporal cortex.

Preferential synaptic targets of orchid cells

To gain insight into the role of rhythmic GABAergic input from orchid cells to cortical circuits, we followed orchid-like collaterals to synaptic targets (Figures 3a and 4). Terminals of strongly-labeled orchid cell TV58g were observed in the fasciola cinereum (FC; marked by Amigo2 mRNA expression as in CA2) (Figure 3a, Figure 3—figure supplement 4a,b, Table 5; see also Materials and Methods - Additional details of orchid cell projections) (Laeremans et al., 2013), followed more temporally by terminals in RSg layer 6 (Figure 3a, Figure 3—figure supplement 2b); this region was also innervated by another cell (AJ50j, Table 1). Apart from the FC, no terminals of any orchid cell were observed in the hippocampus or dentate gyrus (n = 0/6 cells, Table 1). We observed that the PrSd could be defined by Slc17a6, Satb2, Etv1 and Nos1 expression patterns (Figure 3—figure supplement 2, Figure 3—figure supplement 3). This region was extensively innervated by TV58g via three main collaterals, a smaller branch in the angular bundle and a minor sub-branch extending from the PrSd into the SUBd (Figures 3a and 4a). Terminals were exclusively in PrSd layers 5–6, defined by differential calbindin immunoreactivity (41.6% of cortical varicosities, Table 1, Figure 3—figure supplement 3d); other cells additionally innervated more superficial layers (n = 5 cells, Table 1). Interestingly, axon terminals were found at high density in PrSd ‘hot spots’, such as in the extreme medial zone (Figure 4a). The SUBd was also innervated by three other cells (Table 1). The axon of TV58g subsequently extensively innervated caudo-dorsal EC (45.6% of cortical varicosities, Figure 3a, Figure 3—figure supplement 1, Figure 3—figure supplement 3, Table 1) preferentially in layer 6 (expression of the Tmem178 gene; Figure 3—figure supplement 4c, Table 5); we observed terminals from three other orchid cells at a similar location (Figure 3—figure supplement 1, Table 1). No terminals were observed in the Wfs1-expressing PaS region (n = 0/6 cells; Table 1; Figure 3—figure supplement 4d).

Table 5. Marker genes used to define sub-regions of the temporal cortex.

Marker gene Gene product Expression profile Reference
Amigo2 Adhesion molecule with Ig like domain 2 CA2, FC, temporal CA3 http://mouse.brain-map.org/experiment/show/71250310
Etv1 Ets variant 1 (ER81) SUBd, RSg http://mouse.brain-map.org/experiment/show/72119595
Nos1 Neuronal nitric oxide synthase 1 PrS http://mouse.brain-map.org/experiment/show/75147762
Satb2 Special AT-rich sequence binding protein 2 CA1, RSg, PrS http://mouse.brain-map.org/experiment/show/73992708
Slc17a6 solute carrier family 17 member 6 (VGLUT2) SUBd, RSg, PrS http://mouse.brain-map.org/experiment/show/73818754
Tmem178 Transmembrane protein 178 CA2, FC, CA3, EC layer 6 http://mouse.brain-map.org/experiment/show/73992709
Wfs1 Wolfram syndrome one homolog (Wolframin) CA1d, PaS http://mouse.brain-map.org/experiment/show/74881161

We determined the molecular profiles of 13 targeted cortical neurons from the most strongly-labeled orchid cell (n = 1 neuron; Table 6) and used electron microscopy to test the reliability of predicting synaptic connections from light microscopy. In the PrSd, 5/5 tested targets were PV+ (Figure 4a,b) and 3/7 tested were nNOS + interneurons, including 1 PV+ cell (Table 6). The PrSd expresses a high level of Nos1 mRNA (Figure 3—figure supplement 2d), and we found ~70% overlap of PV and nNOS immunoreactivity (data not shown). In the electron microscopic study (from the same orchid cell), all 11 tested boutons formed synapses with interneurons; 6 (55%) of them dendrites (Figure 4ci,ii), and five somata (Figure 4ciii), including a PV+/SATB1+/mGluR1a + interneuron (Figure 4b) receiving four synapses (Figure 4ci). These data are consistent with postsynaptic interneuron targets of GABAergic MS terminals in rodent CA3 and RSg (Freund and Antal, 1988; Freund and Gulyás, 1991; Joshi et al., 2017; Unal et al., 2015; Viney et al., 2013). Overall, somata and proximal dendrites of GABAergic interneurons received extensive innervation by orchid cell terminals (Figure 4b–f). Targets in three regions (PrSd, RSg and FC) were SATB1+ (n = 13/13; Figure 4b,e), but CCK– (n = 0/10) (Table 6). The axon showed target selectively, as nearby interneurons with different molecular profiles were not targeted (Figure 4b). The identity of SATB1+ neurons in these regions remain to be determined by visualizing the processes of the cells, and are likely to include both PV+ and somatostatin+ interneurons (Nassar et al., 2015), given that SATB1 is highly expressed within these subpopulations in the cortex (Close et al., 2012; Denaxa et al., 2012). Full characterization of GABAergic interneurons in the PrS, RSg, and EC awaits further studies possibly using transcriptomic data. We observed for four orchid cells that their axon terminals were apposed to some cortical neurons with high levels of endogenous biotin (e.g. Figure 4d; n = 8/13 targets, Table 6), suggesting that these are interneurons with high metabolic activity, such as ‘fast spiking’ neurons. These data, along with the preferential targets of PV+ MSDB axons in EC (Unal et al., 2015), suggest that PV+ orchid cells influence extra-hippocampal regions, via rhythmic modulation of select subpopulations of local GABAergic interneurons innervating them (Figure 4g).

Table 6. Cortical target neurons of orchid cell TV58g.

Molecular profiles of presumed postsynaptic neurons, based on close apposition of medial septal terminals; +, detectable positive immunoreactivity or signal; –, undetectable immunoreactivity or signal in vicinity of immunopositive signals; u, unknown (unavailable or inconclusive). Parentheses indicate weak immunoreactivity or signal. Endo-biotin, endogenous biotin (detected with streptavidin-conjugated fluorophore) may indicate high metabolic activity due to a high density of mitochondria, such as in ‘fast spiking’ neurons.

Target ID Location PV SATB1 mGluR1a Endo-biotin CCK CB nNOS Figure
S45A FC + +
S45B u + u u
S45D + + +
+
S45G + + + +
S44A RSg L6 + + 4d-f
S44B u + u u
S38A PrSd L5-6 + + + + 4b
S38C + + u +
S38I u + + (+) u +
S38J u + u + +
S36A + + u + u u u
S34A + + u + u u
S34C + + (+) u u +
Total + 7 13 4 8 0 1 5

Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells

Dynamic changes in network oscillations reflect ongoing behavior, with marked changes in power often indicating a transition to a different behavioral state. We categorized four behavioral states in the head-restrained mouse: locomotion (LM); small movements (SM), which typically included postural shifts, limb and tail motion; spontaneous whisking and/or sniffing (WS) without other movements; and immobility (IM) (Table 7). During LM and SM, theta oscillations dominated the CA1d LFP (Figures 2a,b, 3f and 5a). During WS and IM, LIA emerged in the hippocampus and more regular ‘delta’ 2–4 Hz oscillations were present in the MS (Figure 5—figure supplement 1a) (Fujisawa and Buzsáki, 2011; Nerad and McNaughton, 2006; Wolansky et al., 2006), often at higher power when the mouse was breathing more deeply. Orchid cell rhythmicity was evident during all four behavioral states, with autocorrelograms exhibiting robust peaks and troughs (Figure 2d,e, Figure 2—figure supplement 1). The peak in the autocorrelograms was at 143.8 ± 19.8 ms during locomotion (mean ±s.d., n = 8 cells) but occurred later during the other states (SM: 169.6 ± 38.8 ms, n = 7; WS: 167.5 ± 18.6 ms, n = 6; IM: 174.4 ± 26.4 ms, n = 8; Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Rhythmic firing was thus within the lower frequency range of 5–12 Hz theta oscillations (~80–200 ms) during all behavioral states, with cells exhibiting faster rhythmic firing patterns during locomotion when theta oscillations were at their highest frequency and power (Figure 5a, Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Theta oscillations were observed intermittently during SM, WS and IM, which may account for orchid cell rhythmic firing at the lower theta frequency range. But when we tested for rhythmic burst firing during CA1d LIA (<5 Hz) ‘non-theta’ periods (Figure 5—figure supplement 1a), we found that 5/8 orchid cells increased firing close to the peak of the variable duration LIA cycles, with bursts beginning on the rising phase (neurons AJ44j, TV50a, TV58g, AJ50j, and MS19b; Figure 5b). These firing patterns are consistent with the preferential firing around the peak of each theta cycle (Figure 2f). These data suggest that orchid cells can couple to irregular low frequency (<5 Hz) cortical activity in addition to 5–12 Hz theta oscillations.

Table 7. Details of mouse behavior.

LM, locomotion; SM, small postural movements; WS, theta-frequency whisking and/or sniffing without small movements; IM, immobility. *LM periods for these recordings were classified based on high amplitude long-duration (>1 s) signals from the accelerometer with high theta power, and short-duration low amplitude accelerometer signals as SM; those periods outside movement were classed as IM, which may include some whisking/sniffing. Training: limited (limited exposure to head-fixation prior to recording the cell, unfamiliar environment); moderate (typical exposure, familiar with environment); fully trained (fully habituated, familiar with environment and head-fixation). Mouse activity is ordered from the most to least frequent.

Animal name Cell ID Recording conditions Training Monitoring Mouse activity
TV58 TV58g Circular treadmill Moderate Video, EMG, accelerometer IM, WS, SM, LM
TV50 TV50a Circular treadmill Moderate Accelerometer, no video* IM/(WS), LM, SM
MS14 MS14i Circular treadmill Moderate Video, wheel movement IM, SM, WS, LM
MS19 MS19b Circular treadmill Limited Video, wheel movement SM, LM, WS, IM
AJ50 AJ50j Circular treadmill Fully trained Video, wheel movement IM, WS, SM, LM
AJ50h Circular treadmill Fully trained Video, wheel movement LM, IM, SM, WS
AJ44 AJ44j Circular treadmill Fully trained Video, wheel movement IM, SM, LM, WS
MS74 MS74b Frisbee (with netting) Fully trained Video, accelerometer IM, LM, WS
TV77 TV77q Circular treadmill Moderate Video, wheel movement IM, WS, SM, LM
TV78 TV78d Circular treadmill Moderate Video, wheel movement IM/(WS), LM, SM. Whiskers not in focus in video.
TV78k Circular treadmill Moderate Video, wheel movement IM, SM, LM, WS
TV78l Circular treadmill Moderate Video, wheel movement IM, SM, (LM), (WS)
Whiskers not in focus in video. Slow LM.
MS17 MS17k Circular treadmill Moderate Video, wheel movement IM, LM, SM, WH
MS58 MS58i Running disc Moderate Accelerometer, no video* IM/(WS), SM, LM
MS24 MS24a Circular treadmill Moderate Video, wheel movement IM, LM, WS, SM
MS84 MS84f Frisbee (with paper) Fully trained IM, LM, SM, WS
TV72 TV72n Circular treadmill Limited Video, wheel movement IM, SM, WS, LM. 50% of LM was involuntary.
TV85 TV85e Running disc Limited Accelerometer, video IM, WS, SM, LM. 66% of LM was involuntary.
TV86 TV86b Running disc Moderate Accelerometer, video IM, WS, LM, SM

Figure 5. Orchid cell firing reflects behavioral states.

(a) Firing of orchid cells AJ50h (top) and AJ50j (bottom), recorded consecutively in the same session. Algorithm detected bursts are shown below spikes (sequential bursts marked alternately green and brown). Note periods of theta oscillations and non-theta periods during different behavioral states. (b) Left, rhythmic burst firing of orchid cell AJ44j during CA1 LIA (‘non-theta’ periods), with red bars highlighting bursts aligned mostly to the ascending/peak phase of LIA oscillations. Right, raster of spikes within (red) and outside (gray) zero-crossings of LIA cycles, ordered by cycle length (minimum interval 200 ms). Time 0 marks the falling transition of the LFP (zero crossing). Asterisk, position of the peak of the LFP during slower LIA cycles, corresponding to greater spike counts. (c), Mean firing rate index for different states (IM versus LM) for identified orchid cells. All neurons increase their firing from IM to LM. (d) Burst incidence during IM versus LM for identified orchid cells (color-coded, cell IDs on right side), putative orchid cells (gray) and septo-hippocampal cells (black). (e) Probability density functions (PDF) of burst durations of identified orchid cells (color-coded as in c) during LM (top) and IM (bottom), showing that peak burst durations are greater than 50 ms (gray line). (f) Preferred CA1 theta phase (degrees) of identified orchid cells (color-coded as in d), putative orchid cells (gray), and identified Teevra cells (black, cells from Joshi et al., 2017); vector length as radial axis (r). (g) Median interburst interval for identified and putative orchid cells during different behavioral states (points; some neurons lack particular states). LM, locomotion; SM, small movements; WS, whisking/sniffing without movement; IM, immobility.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Burst firing characteristics during different behavioral states.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(a) Time-frequency plots and LFP time series during immobility for orchid cell TV50a. Top, CA1d LFP, bottom, MS LFP, same time period. Note LIA in CA1d LFP consisting of various slow frequencies (red/yellow), and ‘delta’ oscillation in MS LFP. ‘Slow1’ and ‘Slow2’ are other detected slower oscillations. LIA and delta low-pass filtered traces are shown below their respective wideband LFPs. The MS LFP is located very close to the soma of TV50a, hence high amplitude action potentials. Asterisks, location of ripples. (b) Probability density functions (PDF) of burst durations of putative orchid cells during LM (top) and IM (bottom), showing that peak burst durations are greater than 50 ms (light gray line). (c, d) Interburst intervals (c) and intraburst frequencies (d) during different behavioral states for identified orchid cells AJ50h (top) and AJ50j (bottom) recorded sequentially in the same animal. White bars, median. Boxes, quartiles. LM, locomotion; SM, small movements; WS, whisking/sniffing without movement; IM, immobility.

In addition to behavioral state-dependent differences in rhythmic firing, the mean firing rates of orchid cells during locomotion were different than rates during immobility (mean ±s.d. 55.2 ± 14.1 Hz LM versus 39.8 ± 8.1 Hz IM; n = 8, t7 = 3.93, p=0.0057, paired t-test; Figure 5a,c, Table 1). We hypothesized that this increase in mean firing rate during locomotion was due to changes in the spike burst properties. We encountered a diversity of rhythmic bursting and non-bursting neurons in the MS. The common features that we recognized for orchid cells were: (1) a high burst incidence during both locomotion and immobility (>3.0 Hz); (2) long duration bursts during locomotion and immobility (median >50 ms); (3) an increase in firing during locomotion (Figure 5c); (4) strong coupling mostly around the peak of CA1d theta oscillations (mean vector lengths > 0.2; Figures 5d–f and 2f, Tables 1 and 3). We occasionally observed orchid cell bursts spanning two consecutive theta cycles (Figure 2b). We did not observe any non-EC projecting MS neurons (e.g. septo-hippocampal neurons) that exhibited all 4 features of the reported orchid cells (data not shown).

We encountered other neurons (n = 7 unlabeled and n = 1 labeled PV+/SATB1+ mGluR1a + neuron) that matched the criteria and were classified as putative orchid cells (Figure 5d,f,g, Figure 5—figure supplement 1b, Tables 4, 8 and 9). These cells also had different mean firing rates during locomotion and immobility (61.7 ± 18.0 Hz LM versus 43.4 ± 8.9 Hz IM; n = 8, t7 = 3.80, p=0.0067, paired t-test; Table 8). As with the labeled neurons, most putative orchid cells showed a phase preference close to the peak of CA1d theta oscillations (group mean phase (circular mean ± circular s.d.): identified, 178.6° ± 39.1°, n = 8; putative, 183.3° ± 38.5°, n = 8; difference = 4.7°; p=0.8143, permutation test; Figure 5f, Tables 1 and 9). Putative orchid cells were also coupled to mid-gamma oscillations, and like orchid cells maintained a peak phase preference (n = 3/8 tested neurons, Table 9). To test the reliability of our prediction of orchid cell identity by these signature firing patterns, we recorded then labeled two medial septal neurons that had similar long bursts (TV77q, median (and IQR), 69.0 (48.1) ms; TV78l, 86.3 (60.5) ms; Table 10) but a low burst incidence (TV77q, mean ± s.d., 1.5 ± 1.2 Hz; TV78l, 1.8 ± 1.2 Hz; Figure 5d). These two additional cells, which were SATB1 +but PV‒, projected via the fimbria and innervated the dentate gyrus and CA3 (data not shown). Another group of neurons, the Teevra cells, which target CA3 but not the dentate gyrus, are the most rhythmic neurons of the MSDB (Joshi et al., 2017), and thus had a high mean burst incidence (range LM 2.0–7.2 Hz and IM 1.1–5.8 Hz, n = 13 identified cells). However, in contrast to orchid cells, Teevra cells mostly exhibited short duration bursts during locomotion (Joshi et al., 2017) and preferentially fired at the trough of theta oscillations (group mean phase: identified orchid cells, 178.6°± 39.1°, n = 8; identified Teevra cells, 37.5°± 53.5°, n = 13; difference = 141.1°; p=0.0388; Figure 5f). Overall, no terminals of any of these septo-hippocampal neurons (n = 15 neurons) were observed in extra-hippocampal regions, indicating the reliability of our prediction of orchid cell firing characteristics.

Table 8. Behavior-dependent firing patterns of putative orchid cells.

*Labeled soma observed in the dorsal MS; only a faint projection axon was recovered. **Destroyed soma observed in the dorsal MS; no axon was recovered. Abbreviations: u, unknown/unavailable. Spike burst defined as > 3 spikes with ISIs < 40 ms. Firing rates and burst incidence (Hz) are expressed as mean of 1 s bins ± s.d.; intraburst frequency (Hz) as mean ± s.d.; burst duration and interburst interval (ms) as median and interquartile range. LM, locomotion; SM, small movements including limbs, tail, and shifts in posture; WS, high-frequency whisking and/or sniffing in the absence of other movements; IM, immobility. KS, Kolmogorov-Smirnov.

Cell name MS17k* MS58i* MS24a MS84f TV78d TV78k TV85e TV86b
Max speed (cm/s) 9.9 u 16.8 u 14.3 4.5 u u
Firing rate LM 51.6 ± 25.5 80.9 ± 20.2 48.7 ± 10.7 65.2 ± 7.5 31.5 ± 11.9 60.6 ± 12.5 87.6 ± 14.9 67.9 ± 9.1
Firing rate SM 51.8 ± 13.8 42.3 ± 9.1 50.1 ± 11.7 56.0 ± 10.9 41.6 ± 18.3 34.4 ± 12.1 47.3 ± 14.3 49.5 ± 6.4
Firing rate WS 46.7 ± 13.0 52.1 ± 14.7 44.2 ± 11.5 48.9 ± 11.6 u 30.3 ± 15.5 50.1 ± 10.1 55.1 ± 9.3
Firing rate IM 44.6 ± 13.8 52.1 ± 14.7 33.7 ± 8.8 48.4 ± 9.2 39.5 ± 9.8 33.6 ± 9.6 58.0 ± 13.0 37.0 ± 9.7
Burst incidence LM 3.3 ± 2.2 6.6 ± 1.0 5.3 ± 1.4 5.1 ± 1.5 3.6 ± 1.3 3.6 ± 1.0 7.0 ± 1.4 5.3 ± 1.2
Burst incidence SM 4.7 ± 1.0 4.9 ± 1.4 4.0 ± 0.9 4.6 ± 1.4 4.0 ± 1.1 3.3 ± 0.9 5.2 ± 1.5 5.0 ± 0.0
Burst incidence WS 4.7 ± 1.3 u 3.4 ± 1.0 4.8 ± 1.3 u 2.8 ± 1.0 5.4 ± 1.1
Burst incidence IM 4.3 ± 1.2 4.9 ± 1.1 3.3 ± 1.2 4.8 ± 1.1 3.8 ± 1.0 3.3 ± 1.0 5.3 ± 1.1
Burst duration LM 86.7, 115.8 70.2, 22.0 57.4, 36.8 86.4, 111.8 76.9, 39.4 156.3, 101.6 64.1, 34.0 89.6, 62.7
Burst duration SM 90.8, 84.8 67.3, 47.2 107.0, 84.0 99.2, 77.6 90.0, 57.6 112.9, 70.2 62.5, 25.9 101.2, 77.0
Burst duration WS 82.9, 74.0 76.7, 57.4 97.3, 65.1 96.7, 93.0 u 118.3, 50.0 62.1, 43.4 96.5, 70.3
Burst duration IM 84.4, 67.7 76.7, 57.4 94.1, 73.0 94.3, 69.9 95.3, 72.1 112.6, 72.3 65.9, 45.1 75.8, 43.4
Two sample KS test P-value
burst duration LM-IM
0.3902 0.0024 <0.0001 0.0294 <0.0001 <0.0001 0.5547 0.0045
Interburst interval LM 166.6, 155.2 133.1, 31.1 155.6, 96.5 147.4, 115.9 197.8, 163.9 233.8, 155.6 133.0, 32.1 163.1, 91.8
Interburst interval SM 190.7, 135.2 169.8, 117.3 236.9, 138.7 178.1, 86.8 206.6, 136.8 267.4, 163.5 163.1, 73.2 181.8, 84.8
Interburst interval WS 197.9, 95.7 u 259.9, 194.5 189.1, 92.9 u 311.2, 299.0 159.7, 79.0 185.4, 95.3
Interburst interval IM 201.8, 117.9 170.7, 100.1 260.7, 186.0 187.2, 88.1 238.1, 136.9 267.6, 137.6 159.9, 91.8 194.4, 113.2
Two sample KS test P-value
inter-burst interval LM-IM
0.0484 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 0.0130 <0.0001 <0.0001
Intraburst frequency LM 103.4 ± 35.6 141.9, 24.7 144.3, 46.8 105.1, 23.8 83.3, 22.9 81.8, 17.9 150.2, 32.8 119.4, 32.4
Intraburst frequency SM 94.2 ± 28.5 101.9, 26.1 104.4, 35.8 89.3, 27.0 87.4, 22.8 68.0, 14.6 117.9, 28.2 96.3, 24.6
Intraburst frequency WS 95.6 ± 38.8 u 97.8, 25.9 88.9, 24.2 u 70.8, 17.8 118.2, 26.9 105.0, 26.8
Intraburst frequency IM 95.0 ± 27.3 101.9, 30.3 82.5, 27.4 86.9, 22.7 89.4, 27.2 72.4, 19.8 135.5, 39.0 96.3, 25.4
Two sample KS test P-value
intraburst frequency LM-IM
0.0914 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 0.0449 <0.0001 0.0003 <0.0001

Table 9. Network state-dependent firing patterns of putative orchid cells.

*Labeled soma observed in the dorsal MS; only a faint projection axon was recovered. **Destroyed soma observed in the dorsal MS; no axon was recovered. Abbreviations: u, unknown/unavailable.

Cell name MS17k* MS58i** MS24a MS84f TV78d TV78k TV85e TV86b
CA1d theta (5 – 12 Hz) LFP measurement SP SO/SP SO/SP Alveus SP SP SP SP
Pref. theta phase 176° 133° 178° 177° 261° 163° 160° 243°
Mean vector length (r) 0.29 0.42 0.43 0.31 0.38 0.26 0.41 0.30
Rayleigh P-value <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001
Total n spikes 727 2869 3287 4624 2154 1291 2792 1292
Spikes per cycle (mean ± s.d.) 9.0 ± 3.7 7.6 ± 3.9 6.8 ± 3.9 8.8 ± 2.7 5.3 ± 3.4 7.5 ± 4.1 7.3 ± 4.4 9.6 ± 3.6
CA1d mid-gamma
(55 – 80 Hz)
Pref. gamma phase u u 155° u 144° u 207° u
Mean vector length (r) u u 0.3210 u 0.12 u 0.04 u
Rayleigh P-value 0.1919 0.6089 <0.0001 0.0870 <0.0001 0.3506 0.0417 0.4627
Total n spikes 1003 1032 284 1227 871 1457 2000 1274
CA1d SWRs (130 – 230 Hz) n SWRs 24 6 55 0 24 46 19 42
Mean rate inside (Hz) 69.35 u 27.47 u 3.75 19.67 u 4.48
Lambda rate outside (Hz) 46.33 u 36.92 u 40.71 31.92 u 42.31
Poisson P-value 0.0004 u 0.0176 u <0.0001 0.0068 u <0.0001

Table 10. Firing patterns of identified septo-hippocampal neurons.

(Related to Figure 5) Abbreviations: u, unknown/unavailable. Spike burst defined as > 3 spikes with ISIs < 40 ms. Firing rates and burst incidence are expressed as mean of 1 s bins ± s.d.; intraburst frequency as mean ± s.d.; burst duration and interburst interval as median and interquartile range. LM, locomotion; SM, small movements including limbs, tail, and shifts in posture; WS, high-frequency whisking and/or sniffing in the absence of other movements; IM, immobility.

Cell name TV77q TV78l
Behavioral states Firing rate LM (Hz) 38.4 ± 12.2 25.3 ± 1.7
Firing rate SM (Hz) 22.5 ± 7.6 16.7 ± 5.8
Firing rate WS (Hz) 16.0 ± 7.5 23.3 ± 8.6
Firing rate IM (Hz) 15.6 ± 8.1 21.6 ± 6.2
Burst incidence LM (Hz) 3.7 ± 1.4 2.5 ± 0.6
Burst incidence SM (Hz) 2.6 ± 0.9 1.5 ± 1.2
Burst incidence WS (Hz) 1.5 ± 1.1 1.3 ± 1.2
Burst incidence IM (Hz) 1.5 ± 1.2 1.8 ± 1.2
Burst duration LM (ms) 69.7, 73.6 85.9, 96.2
Burst duration SM (ms) 75.9, 40.5 64.9, 19.6
Burst duration WS (ms) 74.3, 38.6 109.3, 102.4
Burst duration IM (ms) 69.0, 48.1 86.3, 60.5
Interburst interval LM (ms) 196.6, 206.7 537.7, 412.8
Interburst interval SM (ms) 332.0, 250.9 457.3, 418.8
Interburst interval WS (ms) 427.4, 588.3 427.3, 214.8
Interburst interval IM (ms) 445.5, 615.9 393.4, 384.6
Intraburst frequency LM (Hz) 85.3 ± 27.1 69.8 ± 35.9
Intraburst frequency SM (Hz) 78.1 ± 28.3 65.6 ± 15.9
Intraburst frequency WS (Hz) 82.9 ± 27.2 69.3 ± 13.3
Intraburst frequency IM (Hz) 85.7 ± 31.1 64.1 ± 15.6
CA1d theta (5 – 12 Hz) LFP measurement SP SP
Preferred theta phase 230° u
Mean vector length (r) 0.27 u
Rayleigh P-value <0.0001 0.2819
Total n spikes 1206 505
Spikes per cycle (mean ± s.d.) 3.1 ± 2.7 u
CA1d SWRs(130 – 230 Hz) n SWRs 50 61
Mean rate inside (Hz) 6.20 0.65
Lambda rate outside (Hz) 16.05 21.73
Poisson P-value 0.0025 <0.0001

During immobility, with LIA in the hippocampus, orchids cells fired with rhythmic bouts of both long and short bursts, with intermittent non-bursting periods composed of spikes with inter-spike intervals >40 ms (Figures 5a,e, 2a,b and 3g, Figure 5—figure supplement 1a,b). In contrast, once locomotion was initiated, the interval between the bursts changed significantly, revealing the striking shorter rhythmic bursting pattern (group median inter-burst interval (and IQR) during LM = 157.8 (42.7) ms, during IM = 198.1 (69.7) ms, n = 16 neurons, p<0.0001, Sign test; Figures 5a,g, 2a,b and 3f, Figure 5—figure supplement 1c, Tables 3 and 8). Median inter-burst intervals were also different between small movements and immobility (SM = 181.8 (61.3) ms, IM = 194.4 (65.9) ms, n = 15 neurons, p=0.0352, Sign test) but not between whisking/sniffing and immobility (WS = 197.9 (73.0) ms, during IM = 201.8 (74.7) ms, n = 13 neurons, p=0.5811, Sign test).

In line with the inter-burst interval, the mean intraburst frequency also changed between the behavioral states of locomotion and immobility (109.7 ± 24.6 Hz LM versus 94.8 ± 17.4 Hz IM; n = 16, t15 = 3.14, p=0.0067, paired t-test). This was reflected by different intraburst frequency distributions during the two behavioral states (n = 12/16 neurons with p<0.05, two-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov (KS) tests LM versus IM; cell AJ50j but not AJ50h was different during LM versus IM; Figure 5—figure supplement 1d, Tables 3 and 8), and different distributions of burst durations (n = 10/16 cells with p<0.05, two-sample KS tests LM versus IM; Figure 5e, Figure 5—figure supplement 1b, Tables 3 and 8). However, the overall median burst durations did not change between these states (group median burst duration (IQR), LM = 78.5 (20.8) ms, IM = 82.8 (22.9) ms, n = 16 neurons, p=0.2101, Sign test; Tables 3 and 8). In summary, the transition from immobility to locomotion results in a shorter inter-burst interval and a higher intraburst frequency, in line with an increase in 5–12 Hz theta power and thus theta-frequency rhythmic bursts.

Relationship between dorsal hippocampal and caudal entorhinal rhythmic activity

We used a common reference LFP in CA1d for the comparison of spike timing and phase coupling of different orchid cells and other MSDB neurons (Borhegyi et al., 2004; Joshi et al., 2017). However, orchid cells project to extra-hippocampal areas, which may differ in the temporal dynamics of population activity. Orchid cell terminals were located in the caudo-dorsal EC (Figure 6a, Figure 3—figure supplement 1), but multi-unit and LFP recordings from rodent EC are typically made in regions of the EC that facilitate their delineation into ‘medial’ and ‘lateral’ divisions (Chrobak and Buzsáki, 1998; Igarashi et al., 2014; Mizuseki et al., 2009). In order to compare the relationships of LFPs directly, we targeted the EC and CA1d of mice with glass electrodes and recorded LFPs simultaneously (Figure 6b–d). The most accurate way to identify LFP recording sites is by visualizing neurons, hence, we juxtacellularly labeled individual principal cells and/or interneurons at specific depths. Post hoc recovery of the labeled neurons revealed that recording sites were in the deep layers of the caudo-dorsal EC in 4 of 8 mice (Figure 6a–d). The other four mice had labeled neurons outside the EC; these animals were excluded from analysis. During locomotion, theta oscillations dominated both CA1d and caudo-dorsal EC LFPs, with theta cycles in phase as in the rat (Mizuseki et al., 2009) (Figure 6b,c,e). Accordingly, EC theta cycle troughs were significantly coupled to CA1d theta cycle troughs (p<0.001 for n = 4 mice (two sites per animal), Rayleigh test; circular mean ±circular s.d., 10.5 ± 15.8°, n = 4 mice (mean of 2 sites per animal); Figure 6e). During immobility, irregular LFP dynamics in the two structures showed similar activity, but the EC LFP lacked high-power ripples during hippocampal sharp waves (Figure 6c) (Chrobak and Buzsáki, 1996). Medial septal neuron firing rate distributions during SWRs were different from a Poisson distribution (n = 4/4 orchid cells from four mice and n = 5 putative orchid cells from four mice; all p<0.05; Figure 6—figure supplement 1, Tables 3 and 9), with 4/9 neurons reducing their firing rate during SWRs (Figure 6—figure supplement 1d,g–i) (Borhegyi et al., 2004; Dragoi et al., 1999; Varga et al., 2008). Moreover, a transient firing rate increase was observed after ripples, which accounted for some of the bursts that were >200 ms in duration (Figures 2a,b and 3f, Figure 6—figure supplement 1a,b,d,g). These data suggest that phase coupling to network oscillations by orchid cells, relative to CA1d, is maintained in the most caudal target region of the cells, the caudo-dorsal EC.

Figure 6. Phase relationship of LFPs in dorsal CA1 and entorhinal cortex.

(a) The position of axon terminals from orchid cell MS14i in the caudo-dorsal EC is marked by an asterisk (DAB-based HRP reacted, 70 µm-thick coronal section flipped horizontally). (b–d) Coronal sections from different animals (TV116, TV92, TV112) showing the EC at similar rostro-caudal levels to (a) with position of glass electrode tracts (cyan, dashed lines). Cross symbols mark sites of labeled neurons used to assess relative depth of LFP recording sites. (b) Left, reverse contrast fluorescence image (neurobiotin signal). Asterisks mark two LFP recording sites. Right, overlaid entorhinal (magenta) and CA1d (black) LFPs recorded at sites 1 and 2. Arrows, sharp waves. (c) Immunoreactivity for CB (magenta) and visualization of neurobiotin (Nb, cyan; widefield epifluorescence). Asterisks mark two LFP recording sites. Right, LFP recordings from site three during locomotion and immobility. Ripples, 130–230 Hz filtered CA1d LFP (also marked by arrows in the LFP). (e) Theta phase histogram for EC LFP theta (5–12 Hz) trough counts (normalized) relative to CA1d theta phase (n = 4 mice, two sites per mouse). Scale bars for all images: 500 µm.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Ripple-related activity of orchid cells and putative orchid cells.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(a-d) Firing probability density (top), spike raster (middle) and cumulative distribution functions (CDFs, bottom) of firing patterns during (red) and outside (gray) SWRs for four identified orchid cells. In the raster plot, continuous staggered lines delineate start and end of aligned ripples, horizontal lines delineate ripples within 0.5 s of an aligned ripple. The CDFs show the firing rate distributions during actual ripples (red curves) and for 1000 shuffled distributions (gray) across three behavioral states (immobility, whisking/sniffing, small movements); black curves, median shuffled distributions; dashed curves, 95% confidence intervals. When the red curve falls between the confidence intervals the cell fires during SWRs by chance. MS19b is inhibited during most SWRs, whereas TV58g is activated during SWRs with high firing rates. (e–i) Same as (a–d) but for putative orchid cells. MS17k is activated during SWRs, with SWRs mostly occurring on the second half of individual rhythmic bursts (e, top).

MSDB neurons immunopositive for PV and/or SATB1 and/or mGluR1a

Finally, we estimated the number of MSDB neurons immunoreactive for combinations of PV, SATB1 and mGluR1a by a stereological method (Figure 7), to provide a baseline for all studies in mouse. We found that SATB1 could be used to delineate the borders of the entire MSDB (Figure 7—figure supplement 1a–c). We estimated the neuronal population containing at least one of the three molecules as 50,680 ± 2422 neurons (mean ± SEM, from 422 ± 35 counted neurons (mean ± s.d.), n = 3 mice, Figure 7—figure supplement 1d). The largest group comprised PV–/SATB1+/mGluR1a– neurons (37.9 ± 2.5%, Figure 7b, Figure 7—figure supplement 1d), which were also the most numerous cells projecting to the EC (Figure 1f). Triple immunopositive neurons, which include the orchid cells, represented 7.2 ± 0.8% of the population. The proportion of PV+/SATB1+/mGluR1a + neurons was similar to PV+/SATB1+/mGluR1a‒ neurons (7.7 ± 0.5%), but no cell of the latter group was observed to project to the EC (Figure 7b, Figure 7—figure supplement 1d, Figure 1f). Apart from PV+/SATB1+/mGluR1‒ CA3-projecting Teevra cells (Joshi et al., 2017) and PV+/SATB1+/mGluR1a+ EC projecting orchid cells, the identities of neurons within the other groups remain to be determined. We conclude that orchid cells, comprising <7% of the entire MSDB population of neurons immunoreactive for at least one of the three molecules, form a subpopulation of GABAergic neurons in the dorsal MS. They provide rhythmic GABAergic input to target interneurons during both locomotion and immobility in distinct extra-hippocampal regions, primarily the PrSd and EC (Figure 4g).

Figure 7. Proportions of MSDB neurons immunoreactive for PV, SATB1 and mGluR1a.

(a) Confocal microscopic z-projection (3 µm thick) of SATB1 (green), PV (magenta) and mGluR1a (cyan) immunoreactivity in the MS. Arrows, two PV+/SATB1+/mGluR1a+ neurons. Arrowhead, PV+/SATB1–/mGluR1a– neuron. Double arrowhead, PV–/SATB1+/mGluR1+ neuron. DAPI, yellow. Scale bar, 50 µm. (b) Stereological estimation of neuronal subpopulations in the MSDB immunoreactive for combinations of PV (P), SATB1 (S) and mGluR1a (G). Total estimated counts are shown above each bar. Error bars, standard error.

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Stereological estimation of the number of neurons immunoreactive for PV and/or SATB1 and/or mGluR1a across the entire MSDB.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(a-c) Widefield epifluorescence montages of coronal sections of the MSDB at three different rostro-caudal levels. Nuclear SATB1 immunoreactivity (green) was used to define the borders of the MSDB. Asterisk marks a labeling artefact. PV, magenta; mGluR1a, cyan. (d) Pie charts of estimated proportions (%) of neurons across the entire MSDB tested for immunoreactivity for the three molecules (based on n = 444, 441 and 382 neurons counted from three mice). Total number of estimated neurons are shown below each pie chart. Scale bar (a–c) 500 µm. MS, medial septum; vDB, vertical Diagonal Band; ac, anterior commissure; hDB, horizontal Diagonal Band; SFi, septofimbrial nucleus.

Discussion

A wealth of knowledge on the relationship between theta-rhythmic activity in hippocampus and the MSDB points to a critical role in the temporal coordination of mnemonic processes (Alonso et al., 1987; Borhegyi et al., 2004; Dragoi et al., 1999; Fuhrmann et al., 2015; Gaztelu and Buño, 1982; Huh et al., 2010; Joshi et al., 2017; Kaifosh et al., 2013; Leão et al., 2012; Mamad et al., 2015; McNaughton et al., 2006; Mizumori et al., 1989; Petsche et al., 1962; Vandecasteele et al., 2014; Viney et al., 2013; Yoder and Pang, 2005). A large proportion of MSDB projections target extra-hippocampal areas, including the EC (Fuchs et al., 2016; Gonzalez-Sulser et al., 2014; Jeffery et al., 1995; Justus et al., 2017; Unal et al., 2015). GABAergic MSDB neurons projecting to the medial EC preferentially innervate layers II and V (Gonzalez-Sulser et al., 2014), and the small PV+ subpopulation of GABAergic MSDB neurons provides a preferential input to ‘fast spiking’ interneurons in layer II (Fuchs et al., 2016). The cellular organization of these circuits may be more diverse than previously anticipated, as we reveal by extracellular recording and selective targeting of neurons with particular firing patterns for labeling. Our observation of exclusively extra-hippocampal projecting GABAergic medial septal orchid cells innervating the caudo-dorsal EC reveals a specialized pathway that directly modulates extra-hippocampal circuits while bypassing the hippocampus.

We found the population of PV+/SATB1+/mGluR1a+ neurons within the MSDB to be relatively small, but the viral tracing revealed that a proportion of these innervated the EC, including those within the rostro-dorsal MS. It remains to be determined what other kinds of MSDB neurons apart from GABAergic orchid cells express these three markers. Orchid cells may correspond to the ‘constitutively bursting’ PV+ neurons with long burst durations and high intraburst frequencies identified in urethane-anesthetized rats (Borhegyi et al., 2004; Simon et al., 2006) (see also Type 1A cells in [Alonso et al., 1987; Gaztelu and Buño, 1982; King et al., 1998]). Like orchid cells, neurons reported by (Borhegyi et al., 2004) had local axon collaterals, which were proposed to inhibit theta-trough firing MSDB neurons, but both the theta-trough and theta-peak firing MSDB neurons were hypothesized to project the hippocampus, in contrast to orchid cells. In awake mice, four major clusters of highly rhythmic medial septal neurons have recently been defined (Joshi et al., 2017). The Teevra cell cluster had short burst durations during locomotion and did not significantly change their mean firing rate between locomotion and rest periods, whereas the Komal cell cluster had long burst durations and increased their firing during locomotion compared to rest periods. Juxtacellular labeling of Teevra cells revealed that they targeted CA3 but no extra-hippocampal areas. These CA3-projecting neurons preferentially fired at the trough of CA1d theta oscillations. Komal cells were not labeled but most preferentially fired around the peak of CA1d theta oscillations. In contrast to Teevra cells, rhythmically bursting orchid cells, reported here, increase firing during locomotion, have long duration bursts, fire preferentially at the peak of CA1d theta oscillations, and innervate the PrSd and EC. Thus, orchid cells represent a distinct subpopulation of Komal cells defined by their theta phase firing preferences and synaptic target regions. The other subpopulation of Komal cells that preferentially fire at the theta trough (Joshi et al., 2017) remain to be defined using juxtacellular labeling in awake animals, as we have focussed on theta peak firing cells for the very difficult labeling experiments in the present study.

The sites of cortical axon terminations of orchid cells reflect well the major projections arising specifically from the PrSd that terminate in both the RSg and caudo-dorsal EC (van Groen and Wyss, 1990; Vann et al., 2009). To the best of our knowledge, most in vivo recordings within the EC have been localized to the well-characterized regions of medial and/or lateral EC (Burgalossi et al., 2014; Chrobak and Buzsáki, 1998; Deshmukh and Knierim, 2011; Deshmukh et al., 2010; Hafting et al., 2005; Igarashi et al., 2014; Ray et al., 2017), thus the caudo-dorsal EC remains to be defined in terms of its firing pattern repertoire. However, it is expected that any grid cells in this region would have tighter grid cell spacing compared to more ventral regions (Hafting et al., 2005). Parts of the lateral EC show weaker theta power in the LFP compared to medial EC in rats, along with lateral EC neurons displaying weaker theta modulation (Deshmukh et al., 2010). Based on the medial septal input revealed here, we predict that the caudo-dorsal EC contains interneurons and principal cells with strong theta and gamma modulation (Beed et al., 2013). The shared cortical target regions of single orchid cells demonstrate that orchid cells provide strong GABAergic input to presubicular, retrosplenial, and entorhinal interneurons at the peak of CA1d/EC theta cycles during locomotion, and at the peak of slower (<5 Hz) cortical activity during immobility. Such multi-area innervation is likely to support the firing dynamics of spatially-modulated neurons, including grid cells, which lose their spatial periodicity upon MS inactivation (Brandon et al., 2013). Encoding of information takes place preferentially at the theta peak (Manns et al., 2007), coincident with the preferential firing of layer 3 EC principal neurons that project to CA1d (Mizuseki et al., 2009). Therefore, disinhibition of EC glutamatergic neurons by orchid cells around the theta peak may facilitate the transfer of temporal sequences relating to navigation to the hippocampus (Koenig et al., 2011; Schlesiger et al., 2015).

Principal cell assemblies are synchronized over a 25 ms time scale (Harris et al., 2003), which matches the membrane time constant of hippocampal pyramidal cells (Spruston and Johnston, 1992) and is within the range of gamma oscillations. Orchid cells were able to couple their firing to CA1d mid-gamma frequency oscillations, which originate in the EC (Colgin et al., 2009; Lasztóczi and Klausberger, 2016; Schomburg et al., 2014) where orchid cell terminals are located. One potential source of gamma and ripple modulation in the MS is directly from GABAergic hippocampo-septal cells (Jinno et al., 2007; TothTóth et al., 1993). The degree of coupling by individual orchid cells to mid-gamma oscillations (from lack of coupling to strong coupling) may depend on the level of habituation/training of the animal. During each theta cycle, orchid cells may coordinate the firing of principal cell assemblies in ‘upstream’ (PrSd) and ‘downstream’ (RSg, EC) regions via the phasic modulation of innervated GABAergic interneurons. We present two main hypotheses. (1) During the peak of the CA1d theta cycle, theta-peak firing orchid cells could inhibit target interneurons that preferentially fire during the trough of theta, leading to disinhibition of principal cells around the theta peak, including EC principal neurons projecting to CA1. This is counter-phased by the theta-trough firing Teevra cells and their theta-peak firing CA3 axo-axonic cell target interneurons (Joshi et al., 2017; Viney et al., 2013), leading to disinhibition of CA3 pyramidal neurons on the descending phase (Mizuseki et al., 2009), which project to CA1. (2) Alternatively or in addition, orchid cells could target theta-peak firing cortical interneurons that are modulated at mid-gamma frequency, leading to mid-gamma modulation of PrSd and EC principal cells around the peak of theta cycles (Manns et al., 2007). This extra-hippocampal pathway may contribute to the synchronization of inter-areal cortico-cortical loops via theta-nested gamma frequency rhythmic inhibition of cortical GABAergic circuits (Jeffery et al., 1995; Kim et al., 2015). Such a population rhythm could then be relayed to the hippocampus from the EC (Chrobak et al., 2000; Igarashi et al., 2014; Mizuseki et al., 2009). A similar temporal coordination with a wider temporal window may exist during quiescent periods, when theta power is lower and the intraburst frequency of orchid cells decreases.

Orchid cells may also coordinate SWRs. Ripples also occur downstream of CA1 within deep layers of the PrS and EC (Chrobak and Buzsáki, 1996), the target regions of orchid cells. Since ipsilateral entorhinal ripples occur 5–30 ms after CA1d ripples (Chrobak and Buzsáki, 1996), it is possible that the increase in firing of orchid cells following CA1d SWRs coordinates the offline replay/recall of spatial memory traces within extra-hippocampal areas; most SWRs were probably ‘awake’ SWRs under our conditions (Roumis and Frank, 2015). Different levels of arousal may account for differences in firing rates during SWRs.

Cortical target interneurons of orchid cells included PV+ and nNOS+ neurons. Most cortical PV+ neurons are considered to be ‘fast spiking’ basket and axo-axonic cells, and display rhythmic modulation at theta frequency (Preston-Ferrer et al., 2016). In the PrSd, some PV+ neurons are immunopositive for nNOS, which is also expressed by other interneuronal types. The in vivo firing patterns of interneurons in FC, PrSd, and RSg are unknown (Henriksen et al., 2010) except for ‘theta’ cells recorded in the vicinity of PrSd head direction cells (Preston-Ferrer et al., 2016; Taube et al., 1990). ‘Fast spiking’ neurons have also been recorded in more caudal regions of PrS but do not show strong theta modulation (Tukker et al., 2015). Visualization of interneuron axon terminals is required to define their target cortical layers as well as their target subcellular domains on postsynaptic principal cells. Based on the high density of GABAergic MSDB terminals around somata and dendrites of PV+ cells in PrSd, we hypothesize that postsynaptic targets of orchid cells will be strongly theta modulated, supporting the rhythmic coordination of pyramidal cell assemblies. The occasional bursts spanning two theta cycles may be responsible for the ‘theta cycle skipping’ observed in some EC interneurons (Brandon et al., 2013).

In summary, orchid cells provide a specialized source of subcortical GABAergic input to specific extra-hippocampal regions that are functionally-related and converge to the caudo-dorsal EC. Their movement-related theta-rhythmic burst firing, and similarly strong rhythmic bursting during immobility, likely serve as a mechanism - via target interneurons - for the temporal coordination of cortico-cortical neurons. These cortico-cortical principal neurons project from ‘upstream’ areas (PrSd) to ‘downstream’ areas (RSg, EC) forming transient, gamma-timescale assemblies involved in mnemonic processes representing sequential information, such as routes of navigation.

Materials and methods

Surgical procedures

All procedures involving experimental animals were approved by the Department of Pharmacology Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body under approved personal and project licenses in accordance with the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act, 1986 (UK) and associated regulations. Adult male C57Bl7/J mice (originating from Charles River Laboratories; n = 120 mice for head-plate implantation, 23–42.5 g; n = 10 mice for viral tracing, 22–28 g) were maintained on a 12/12 hr light-dark cycle (lights on during the day), and prior to surgery housed in groups of up to four within individually ventilated cages. Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane (IsoFlo, Abbott) followed by a sub-cutaneous injection of opioid analgesic buprenorphine (Vetergesic, 0.1 mg/kg) and maintained with 1–3% (vol/vol) isoflurane. The scalp was clipped and mice were fixed to a stereotaxic frame (Kopf Instruments) using ear bars and a jaw bar. Ocular lubricant was applied, and small volumes of the non-steroidal anti-inflammatory analgesic meloxicam (Metacam, Boehringer Ingelheim) were injected into the scalp. Under aseptic conditions, an incision was made along the scalp at the midline and the skull was exposed.

Head-plate implantation

Two small M1 screws were placed in holes drilled above the cerebellum, one of which was used as the electrical reference and ground. Another screw was fixed at 1.50 mm anterior and 1.70 mm lateral of Bregma for a frontal cortical EEG; occasionally a second EEG screw was placed over the retrosplenial cortex or V1. A machined glass reinforced plastic head-plate (either a 0.7 g or 1.1 g version, custom made at the Department of Physics, Oxford University) was positioned over the screws, and bone cement was used to fix the head-plate and screws to the skull. Craniotomies were made above the MSDB (0.85 mm anterior and 0 mm lateral of Bregma) and right CA1d (2.50 mm posterior and 1.70 mm lateral of Bregma). In 8/120 mice, a craniotomy was made above the right EC (4.85 mm posterior and 3.10 mm lateral of Bregma for seven mice; 4.30 mm posterior and 3.10 mm lateral for one mouse (TV92)). Craniotomy sites were covered in silicon and mice were left to recover (typically 1 d). A standardized distress scoring system was used to monitor the recovery. For some experiments, craniotomies were instead carried out during a second surgery, using the same anesthesia regime as above.

Viral tracing

Small craniotomies were made above the MS (0.85 mm anterior and 0.60 mm lateral of Bregma, 10° latero-medial angle; 3.65 mm ventral to dura mater) to inject the anterograde Cre-dependent adeno-associated virus (pAAV2-EF1a-DIO-hChR2(H134R)-EYFP; Vector BioLabs; n = 10/10 mice), and above the caudal EC (4.85 mm posterior and 3.20 mm lateral of Bregma; 1.20–1.90 mm ventral to dura mater) to inject mutant pseudorabies virus PRV-hSyn-Cre (PRV IE180-null-hSyn-Cre; gift from Prof Lynn Enquist, Princeton University; CNNV grant no. P40RR018604; (Oyibo et al., 2014)) either unilaterally (n = 5/10 mice; animals MS60, MS61, TV89, TV96, TV102) or bilaterally (n = 5/10 mice; MS66, MS76, MS77, MS78, TV95). Membrane-bound expression of ChR2-EYFP from the AAV is dependent on the expression of Cre from PRV-hSyn-Cre within the same neurons. Both viruses were pressure-injected using glass pipettes (tip diameter: 12–20 µm) attached to 1 µl syringes at a rate of ~50 nl/min. Accordingly, we injected 400 nl of AAV into the MSDB (Unal et al., 2015), and 200–400 nl of PRV-hSyn-Cre into the caudo-dorsal EC (200 nl/site in MS60, TV89; 250 nl/site in MS61, TV95; 300 nl/site in TV96, TV102; 400 nl/site in MS66, MS76, MS77, MS78). Mice were perfuse-fixed 14–30 days after injections in order to ensure axonal transport of both viruses. Following sectioning, weak EYFP expression was observed within the cytoplasm of a minority of somata at the injection site in two animals (data not shown); strong thick and thin axons could be followed rostrally towards the MSDB and did not originate from the weakly-labeled local EC neurons. Four injected mice were excluded due to lack of EYFP expression. Injection sites were localized based on the lesion caused at the tip of the injection pipette (due to the pressure of the injection). These small lesions, which were often surrounded by a few small patches of accumulated fluorophores, were visible under high magnification and represented the centre of the injection site (Figure 1—figure supplement 1b).

In vivo extracellular recordings and juxtacellular labeling

Experiments were carried out in a dedicated recording room during the light phase, typically 1–2 d after the craniotomies. Mice were habituated to a circular treadmill (see speed measurements below), a running disc (Fast Trac, LBS Ltd, Surrey, UK), or a Frisbee (radius 15 cm) (Table 7), below a stereotaxic frame and attached to a head-restraint device (custom made at the Department of Physics, Oxford University) for short durations (few minutes) then for longer durations. Animals AJ44 and AJ50 were trained to run on the circular treadmill for 7–10 d before the craniotomy. Two separate glass electrodes filled with 2.5–3.0% neurobiotin (wt/vol) in 0.5 M NaCl (10–18 MΩ) were advanced into the brain with micromanipulators (IVM-1000, Scientifica Ltd, Maidenhead, UK), targeting CA1d at a 10° postero-anterior angle (sometimes filled only with 0.5 M NaCl), and the midline dorsal MS (0° angle, near or directly through the sagittal sinus). For EC recordings, a 0° angle was used in 7 of 8 mice. In one mouse (TV92), a 10° anterior to posterior angle was used; this mouse also had a craniotomy over the MS. All signals were amplified x1,000 (ELC-01MX, BF-48DGX and DPA-2FS modules, NPI Electronic). Both wideband (0.3 Hz to 10 kHz) and band-pass filtered (action potentials, 0.8–5 kHz; LFPs, 0.3–500 Hz) signals were acquired in parallel and digitized at 20 kHz (Power1401, Cambridge Electronic Design). HumBugs (Digitimer) were used to remove 50 Hz noise. A video camera was used to monitor behavior, and wheel movement and speed were recorded using an Arduino (see below and Table 7). In some experiments, an accelerometer was placed on the wheel to detect wheel movement, and in one experiment (animal TV58) an electromyogram (EMG) was used to help detect movement of the animal (from the neck muscle). Data were recorded in Spike2 software (CED). Extracellularly recorded cells in the MSDB were juxtacellularly labeled using 200 ms current pulses (Pinault, 1996) followed by a 4–8 hr recovery period (Table 2). Unlabeled single neurons were classified as from the MSDB if they were recorded near juxtacellularly labeled cells, or their firing patterns closely matched neurons that were already established as being from the MSDB. A total of 16 animals were used for analysis (Table 7). Two other animals containing MS neurons with axon projecting to the EC were excluded because the axons could not be matched to a recorded neuron. Selection of MSDB neurons for recording was initially random. After recovery of the first three neurons with EC-projecting axons (TV50a, TV58g, MS14i), subsequent experiments were biased towards targeting neurons in the MS that had similar rhythmic bursting firing patterns to these three neurons; most other neurons were bypassed. Therefore, other kinds of EC-projecting GABAergic neurons may not have been recorded. Animals with recorded neurons projecting to other parts of the temporal cortex and with firing patterns that differ from orchid cells will be reported in detail in planned future studies. The CA3-projecting Teevra neurons (Figure 5f) are taken from a separate study (Joshi et al., 2017). In experiments with juxtacellular labeling in the EC (n = 8 mice; animals TV92, TV108, TV111-116), the recovery period was ~1 hr.

Tissue processing

Mice were deeply anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (50 mg/kg, i.p.) and transcardially perfused with 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde 15% v/v saturated picric acid, 0.05% glutaraldehyde in 0.1 M PB at pH 7.4. Some brains were postfixed overnight in glutaraldehyde-free fixative. After washing in 0.1 M PB, 70–100 µm coronal sections were cut using a Leica VT 1000S vibratome (Leica Microsystems) and stored in 0.1 M PB with 0.05% sodium azide at 4°C. Streptavidin-conjugated fluorophores were used to visualize neurobiotin-labeled neuronal processes within tissue sections previously permeabilized by Tris-buffered saline (TBS) with 0.3% Triton X-100 (TBS-Tx) or through rapid 2x freeze-thaw (FT) over liquid nitrogen (cryoprotected in 20% sucrose in 0.1 M PB). For light microscopic visualization, analysis, and 3D neuronal reconstruction, TBS-Tx- or FT-processed sections were processed using horseradish peroxidase (HRP)-based diaminobenzidine (DAB) reactions as previously described (Viney et al., 2013).

Immunohistochemistry

For the molecular identification of labeled neurons and their postsynaptic target neurons, immunohistochemistry was carried out as previously described (Unal et al., 2015; Viney et al., 2013). Specificity information for primary antibodies is in Table 11. To test the immunoreactivity of multiple markers on the same neurons (e.g. on postsynaptic targets of septo-cortical neurons), we employed an iterative strategy based on area-dependent marker frequency, subcellular localization, colocalization probability, antibody species, and fluorophore. Typically, we tested rare markers first, along with non-cytoplasmic markers (e.g. nucleus or cell membrane) to maximize the available subcellular domains for testing. Multiple fluorescence channel immunoreactivity was documented with epifluorescence or confocal microscopy (see below) followed by demounting the sections, washing in TBS or TBS-Tx, and repeating the procedure with different antibodies. Cytoplasmic markers, such as calcium binding proteins, were often tested last. In each round of immunohistochemistry, ‘negative’ controls were included that lacked the primary antibodies, along with ‘positive’ controls from a different brain that included the primary antibodies. In the next round of immunohistochemistry, the previous positive controls became new ‘negative’ controls by lacking the new primary antibodies. Thus, the number of control sections increased with each round of processing, and each control was compared to the test section, along with a comparison of images acquired in the same location before and after each round of immunohistochemistry. Undetectable immunoreactivity within the neuron for a given fluorescence channel was excluded for that particular round if no new signals were present in the vicinity of the neuron of interest. A rare marker recognized by a particular primary antibody was tested first if the antibody was raised in the same species as a primary antibody for a common marker to avoid saturation by binding to the existing secondary antibody.

Table 11. Specificity information for primary antibodies.

Rb, rabbit; Gt, goat; Ms, mouse; Gp, guinea pig; Ck, chicken.

Molecule Host Dilution Source Specificity information RRID
Calbindin (CB) Rb 1:5000 Swant, CB-38 (lot 5.5) Supplementary Table 2 in (Viney et al., 2013) RRID:AB_2721225
Calretinin (CR) Rb 1:500-1:1000 Swant, 7699/3 hr (lot 18299) Supplementary Table 2 in (Viney et al., 2013) RRID:AB_10000321
Calretinin (CR) Gt 1:1000 Swant, CG1 Table 1 in (Unal et al., 2015) RRID:AB_10000342
Choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) Gt 1:500 Chemicon, AB144P Table 1 in (Unal et al., 2015) RRID:AB_2079751
GFP Ck 1:500 Aves Labs, GFP-1020 Table 1 in (Unal et al., 2015) RRID:AB_10000240
Metabotropic glutamate receptor 1a (mGluR1a) Gp 1:500-1:1000 Frontier Institute, mGluR1a-GP-Af660 Characterized by Tanaka et al. 2000 Eur. J. Neurosci. 12, 781–792 RRID:AB_2531897
Neuronal nitrogen oxide synthase (nNOS) Rb 1:1000 EMD Millipore, AB5380 Supplementary Table 2 in (Viney et al., 2013) RRID:AB_91824
Parvalbumin (PV) Rb 1:1000 Swant, PV-28 Supplementary Table 2 in (Viney et al., 2013) RRID:AB_2315235
Parvalbumin (PV) Gt 1:1000 Swant, PVG-214, lot 3.6 Supplementary Table 2 in (Viney et al., 2013) RRID:AB_2313848
Parvalbumin (PV) Gp 1:5000 Synaptic Systems, 195 004, lot 5 Supplementary Table 2 in (Viney et al., 2013) RRID:AB_2156476
Purkinje cell protein 4 (PCP4) Rb 1:1000 Santa Cruz, sc-74816, lot G0814 Characterized by San Antonio et al. 2014 J. Comp. Neurol. 522, 1333–1354 RRID:AB_2236566
Pro-cholecystokinin (pro-CCK) Rb 1:500 April 2005 gift (similar to Frontier Institute, CCK-pro-Rb-Af350) Supplementary Table 2 in (Viney et al., 2013)
SATB1 (N-14) Rb 1:200 Abcam, ab70004 Supplementary Table 2 in (Viney et al., 2013) RRID:AB_1270545
SATB1 (N-14) Gt 1:200-1:250 Santa Cruz, sc-5989 Supplementary Table 2 in (Viney et al., 2013) RRID:AB_2184337
SMI32 (neurofilament H non-phosphorylated) Ms 1:1000 Covance, SMI-32R lot 14835102 Similar to immunoreactivity characterized in primates by Campbell and Morrison 1989 J. Comp. Neurol. 282, 191–205 and in rats by Ouda et al. 2012 Brain Struc. Func. 217, 19–36 RRID:AB_509997
Substance P receptor (NK1R) Rb 1:500 Millipore, AB5060, lot LV1525037 Characterized by Shigemoto et al. 1993 Neurosci. Lett. 153, 157–160 RRID:AB_2200636
Vesicular GABA transporter (VGAT) Gp 1:500 Synaptic Systems, 131 004 Supplementary Table 2 in (Viney et al., 2013) RRID:AB_887873
Vesicular glutamate transporter 2 (VGLUT2) Gp 1:2000 Synaptic Systems, 135 404 lot 135404/16 Similar to immunoreactivity characterized in mouse hippocampus by Herzog et al. 2006 J. Neurochem. 99, 1011–1018 RRID:AB_887884
Wfs1 Rb 1:500 Proteintech, 11558–1-AP Table 1 in (Unal et al., 2015) RRID:AB_2216046

Microscopy

Confocal microscopy (Zeiss LSM 710 with ZEN software) was used to document identified neurons and their targets, as previously described (Unal et al., 2015; Viney et al., 2013). Overviews of multi-channel multi-round sections tested with immunohistochemistry were acquired with widefield epifluorescence either on the same microscope as used for confocal imaging (with Axiovision software), or on a Leitz DMRB microscope (Leica). Electron microscopy was carried out as previously described (Unal et al., 2015; Viney et al., 2013).

Electrophysiological data analysis

Data were analyzed in Mathematica (Wolfram Research), MATLAB (MathWorks) and Spike2 (CED). Movement periods were detected by the combination of video, wheel activity and in some cases EMG or accelerometer. Whisking, postural shifts and respiratory rate were qualitatively observed from the video. For labeled neurons, only data acquired before juxtacellular labeling were used for analysis. Teevra cell firing patterns were taken from 13 identified (recorded then juxtacellularly labeled) CA3-projecting cells reported in Table 1 of (Joshi et al., 2017), with their ‘RUN’ defined here as locomotion (LM) and ‘REST’ defined as immobility (IM). Note that most Teevra cells were recorded from fully trained head-restrained mice under goal-directed movement conditions (e.g. running for a sucrose reward).

Local field potentials

The position of the CA1d LFP recording was estimated based on the polarity of sharp waves (Buzsáki, 1986) and the presence of ripples (Buzsáki et al., 2003). Both strata oriens and pyramidale contained positive sharp waves, and stratum radiatum contained negative sharp waves. The upper part of superficial stratum pyramidale consisted of both positive and negative sharp waves. In stratum pyramidale, pyramidal cells were often recorded. In one case (animal TV58), a hippocampal neuron weakly labeled at the recording site was used as confirmation. In addition to polarity of the SWRs, in three cases (AJ44j, AJ50h, AJ50j), mid gamma (55–80 Hz; detection threshold:>1 s.d. above the mean cycle amplitude) coupling to the peak of theta oscillatory activity was also used to estimate the location of LFP recording in stratum pyramidale. Mid-gamma (55–80 Hz) is coupled to the peak of CA1 pyramidale theta oscillatory activity (Colgin et al., 2009; Lasztóczi and Klausberger, 2014; Schomburg et al., 2014).

Theta and gamma

Theta periods were detected by filtering the CA1d LFP for theta (5–12 Hz) and delta (2–4 Hz) and computing a power ratio. For cell MS74b, theta (and gamma, below) was detected from an electroencephalogram (EEG) at 2.10 mm posterior and −2.50 mm lateral of Bregma, which was coherent with CA1d strata oriens/pyramidale. Theta phase was calculated by linear interpolation between troughs of the band-pass filtered theta oscillations, with 0° and 360° set as the troughs. The Rayleigh test was used to test for uniformity of circular phase distributions. Mean phase and mean vector length were used as measures of the preferred phase and coupling strength, respectively, both for the spike-theta and gamma troughs-theta coupling. Mid-gamma (55–80 Hz) troughs were detected from the CA1d LFP for the entire recording period (detection threshold:>1 s.d. above the mean cycle amplitude), with most gamma troughs detected during movement-related theta oscillations. If cross-frequency coupling was observed (gamma troughs coupled to theta peak), spike coupling was measured for gamma cycles as described for theta above. The troughs of EC theta cycles (detected from the 5–12 Hz filtered EC LFP) were also tested for their coupling to CA1d theta oscillations, as described above. Phase histograms were smoothed by convolving with a Gaussian.

Large amplitude irregular activity

To detect zero-crossings of large amplitude irregular activity (LIA) in the CA1d LFP (oscillations < 5 Hz; ‘non-theta’), the wideband LFP was low-pass filtered in Spike2 by smoothing (0.08 s window), followed by DC removal (0.2 s window). The falling level zero-crossings of the cycles were detected with a minimum interval of 0.2 s.

Sharp-wave associated ripple oscillations

The power of the 130–230 Hz band-pass filtered CA1d LFP was used to detect SWRs, with a threshold of at least 4 s.d. above the mean power. Firing rate changes during SWRs were compared to 1000 shuffled firing rate distributions of periods outside SWRs (excluding periods with detected theta oscillations), as previously described (Katona et al., 2014) (their Method 1). Neurons with less than 20 detected SWRs were excluded.

Firing patterns

Mean firing rates were calculated in 1 s windows within each behavioral state. The LM-IM index (Figure 5) was calculated by normalizing mean firing rates [(LM - IM)/(LM + IM)], with a positive value indicating an increase in mean firing rate during LM and a negative value indicating a decrease in mean firing rate during LM. Bursts were defined as a train of >3 spikes with interspike intervals (ISIs) < 40 ms. Burst incidence was defined by the total number of bursts in 1 s windows. Interburst intervals were calculated by measuring the time elapsed between the first spikes of consecutive bursts.

Time-frequency plots (Figure 5—figure supplement 1) used the ContinuousWaveletTransform in Mathematica (Morlet wavelet, 1 kHz sampling rate, 12 octaves, 16 voices).

Speed measurements

The circular treadmill (Table 7) consisted of a polyurethane foam cylinder of 146 mm diameter and 125 mm length, rotating on an axle supported on one side. On the axle was a disc for an optical quadrature encoder, providing 2000 edges per revolution (sum of rising and fall edges of channels A and B). The optical encoder was connected to an Arduino Uno microcontroller running our own code, and outputting through a 12-bit 2-channel digital to an analogue converter (DAC). The Arduino program generated a counter for the wheel movement, the counter increasing every time the wheel made 1/2000th of a rotation in the forward-running direction, and decreasing for the backward direction. The output consisted of two signals, speed and movement period, 0 to +4 V, that were re-digitized by the Power1401 and recorded concurrently with electrophysiological signals in Spike2. The speed signal was calculated at 10 ms intervals, using the distance traveled by the wheel as recorded by the counter, divided by time elapsed since the last calculation. The values sent to the DAC were calibrated so that 1 V output signal represented a speed of 0.6912 m/s at the cylinder surface. The movement period signal was raised to 4 V when the counter increased by two compared to a value 10 ms earlier, and dropped to 0 V if the counter increased by less than 11 compared to a value taken 50 ms earlier. Speed was not measured for all mice.

Neuronal reconstructions and delineation of cortical areas

Allen Brain Atlas images of selected marker genes expressed in coronal mouse brain sections were used as reference sections to define the positions of labeled orchid cell axons within processed fluorescence and DAB-reacted sections (Table 5, Figure 3—figure supplement 2, Figure 3—figure supplement 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 4). Reconstruction of neuronal processes within each series of DAB-reacted sections were carried out using Neurolucida (MBF Bioscience) in 3D, as previously described (Viney et al., 2013). For the partial cortical axon reconstruction of TV58g (Figure 3, Figure 3–video 1), regional, sub-regional and laminar boundaries were assigned based on key Allen Brain Atlas gene expression profiles (Table 5) and a series of DAB-reacted sections immunoreacted for calbindin (Figure 3—figure supplement 3d). The reconstructed axon was scaled up to the original unprocessed (freshly sectioned) z-thickness. Reconstructions of somata, dendrites, and local axon in the medial septum were carried out on a drawing tube in 2D (Leitz Dialux 22 microscope). Reconstructions are available at http://neuromorpho.org/.

Quantification of virally-labeled axons

The axonal collateralization of MSDB neurons projecting to the EC was compared between extra-hippocampal areas and the hippocampus. Sections containing the PrSd, RSg, CA1, dentate gyrus and SUB that could be delineated within the same section were sampled from animals with anterogradely labeled EYFP+ MSDB axons (n = 3 animals, three sections each; 70–100 µm thickness per section). Representative regions of interest were defined (ROIs; width 19.5 µm, height spanning all the layers in each cortical area; n = 3 samples per region per section) and all EYFP+ axons that crossed both lateral edges of the ROI were counted using ImageJ (n = 120 ROIs; n = 274 axons). The proportion of axons per brain region of the total in each section was calculated and compared using the Kruskal-Wallis test.

Virally-labeled EYFP+ axon terminals from three mice (MS60, MS66, MS76) were tested for their immunoreactivity to VGAT in the PrSd and RSg. A sample of 2–3 70 µm-thick sections from each brain were used for quantification. For each section, an LSM710 confocal microscope (with Axio Imager.Z1, Carl Zeiss) with ZEN 2008 software v5.0 (Zeiss) was used to acquire two-channel fluorescence z-stacks at 40x magnification. Using the ImageJ Cell Counter plugin, all EYFP+ axon terminals within the stacks were counted, and classified as VGAT immunopositive or lacking detectable VGAT immunoreactivity. The latter classification was assigned if other VGAT-immunopositive puncta were imaged nearby within the same optical section (100% of cases).

Stereological counting

Three adult male C57BL6/J mice (28–30 g) were anesthetized with a terminal dose of pentobarbitone and perfuse-fixed (4% PFA and 0.05% glutaraldehyde) followed by post-fixation overnight (0.4% PFA). Brains were sectioned coronally at 50 µm and collected in series from a randomly selected point before the MSDB structure.

A random starting section was chosen for each brain, and every third section thereafter was selected, to cover the entire MSDB, resulting in 8–9 sections per animal. Sections were permeabilized in TBS-Tx and blocked for 1 hr in 20% normal horse serum (NHS) in TBS-Tx at room temperature, then incubated in a primary antibody solution containing TBS-Tx and 1% NHS for 3 nights at 2–8°C. Sections were washed then incubated in a secondary antibody solution containing TBS-Tx and 1% NHS for 4 hr at room temperature. They were then washed, incubated in a DAPI solution (1:1000 in 0.1M PB) for 60 s, washed and mounted on slides in Vectashield. Antibodies used (Table 11): guinea pig anti-mGluR1a, goat anti-SATB1, rabbit anti-PV, donkey anti-guinea pig Alexa Fluor 488 1:1000, donkey anti-goat Cy3 1:400, donkey anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 647 1:250.

The optical fractionator method was used to carry out systematic random sampling. Stereo Investigator (MBF Bioscience) was used for obtaining images and stereological counting. Each section was scanned using a fully motorized Axio Imager M.2 fluorescence microscope, with a 63 × 1.4 NA oil objective. The probe depth was 5 µm, with a 2 µm guard zone above, and an interval of 1 µm. The grid size was 240 × 160 µm, and the counting frame was 120 × 80 µm. Segment contours were drawn using a 10x or 5x objective, using the SATB1 marker for a guide to delineate the MSDB region. The mounted section thickness was set to 50 µm for all sections.

For counting, markers were used for the seven categories of PV only, mGluR1a only, SATB1 only, PV and SATB1, PV and mGluR1a, mGluR1a and SATB1, and PV, mGluR1a and SATB1. The point at which the nucleolus first appeared from above was the point at which the neuron was counted. DAPI was used to identify this for mGluR1a-positive cells. Only neurons immunopositive for at least one marker were counted. Nucleoli which touched the green inclusion line of the counting frame were counted, but not counted if they touched the red exclusion line. In cases where the contour intersected a neuron, it was counted if more than half of the nucleolus fell within the contour boundary.

For statistics, we used the Gundersen coefficient of error (m = 1). For all markers within each brain, the errors were 0.05, 0.06 and 0.05 (n = 3 mice). Potential sources of error include antibody penetration for PV and mGluR1a, such that counts of these two markers may be slightly under-represented.

Statistics and sample size

All statistical tests were run in Mathematica or MATLAB and are stated in the text. In all cases we used an α level of 0.05. The permutation test used 10,000 permutations. The circular statistics toolbox in MATLAB was also used (P. Berens, CircStat: A Matlab Toolbox for Circular Statistics, Journal of Statistical Software, Volume 31, Issue 10, 2009, http://www.jstatsoft.org/v31/i10). Sample sizes for each experiment are stated in the text. Initially, neurons were recorded randomly by slowly advancing the glass electrode through the medial septum, and a subset were juxtacellularly labeled in order to determine their cortical target region(s) post hoc. In later experiments, neurons with specific firing pattern signatures (see Results, ‘Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells’) were targeted while all other neurons were ignored. We aimed to have a standard deviation of <10 Hz for the mean firing rates during immobility for orchid cells. Neurons with firing patterns and projections that differed from orchid cells and Teevra cells will be reported in detail elsewhere (including the neurons represented in Table 10).

Additional details of orchid cell projections

The first collateral of orchid cell TV58g specifically innervated a sub-region of FC medial to SUBd and ventral to the caudal end of the corpus callosum (Figure 3—figure supplement 4a,b). A faint thin axon, originating from the MSDB near the labeling site of cell TV50a projected to the medial CA2/FC region (identified by Amigo2 gene expression, Figure 3—figure supplement 4) but could not be proven to originate from the main projection axon. Cells MS19b and AJ50j projected along the dorsal fornix near the midline similar to TV58g (Figure 3 ), but TV50a, MS14i, AJ50h, AJ44j and MS74b orchid cell axons traveled along the lateral dorsal fornix, passing CA1d, before heading to SUBd/PrSd. A single branch from the main axon of MS14i in the fornix crossed into CA1 stratum lacunosum, but no varicosities were observed until the branch traveled temporally and innervated the SUBd. AJ50j innervated two separate regions of RSg, one was more septal (layer 6), other was temporal (deep and superficial layers) (Figure 3—figure supplement 2Figure 3—figure supplement 3). The axons of AJ44j and MS74b were weakly labeled: the main axons faded away within the angular bundle adjacent to the PrSd.

Acknowledgements

We thank Linda Katona for help with physiological analysis; Kristina Wagner for help with tissue processing and electron microscopy; Michael Howarth, Katja Hartwich, Amar Sharma, Kathryn Newton, Szabolcs Biro, Eszter Kormann and Laszlo Marton for technical assistance; Ben Micklem for advice on reconstructions, stereology, and for building the speed monitor; Thomas Forro for advice on recording in head-restrained mice. We thank Balint Lasztóczi and A Tugrul Ozdemir for comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. We thank Lynn Enquist at Princeton University for stocks of PRV-hSyn-Cre, and David Dupret and Pavel Perestenko for aliquots of AAV. We also thank M Watanabe for the pro-CCK antibody.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Tim James Viney, Email: tim.viney@pharm.ox.ac.uk.

Peter Somogyi, Email: peter.somogyi@pharm.ox.ac.uk.

Laura Colgin, The University of Texas at Austin, Center for Learning and Memory, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Medical Research Council MC_UU_12024/4 to Tim James Viney, Minas Salib, Abhilasha Joshi, Gunes Unal, Naomi Berry, Peter Somogyi.

  • Wellcome 108726/Z/15/Z to Tim James Viney, Minas Salib, Abhilasha Joshi, Gunes Unal, Naomi Berry, Peter Somogyi.

  • Felix Doctoral Scholarship to Abhilasha Joshi.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All procedures involving experimental animals were approved by the Department of Pharmacology Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body under approved personal and project licenses (project licence number: 30/3240) in accordance with the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act, 1986 (UK) and associated regulations. All surgery was performed under isoflurane anesthesia with a peri-operative dose of buprenorphine, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34395.031

References

  1. Alonso A, Gaztelu JM, Buño W, García-Austt E. Cross-correlation analysis of septohippocampal neurons during theta-rhythm. Brain Research. 1987;413:135–146. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90162-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Beed P, Gundlfinger A, Schneiderbauer S, Song J, Böhm C, Burgalossi A, Brecht M, Vida I, Schmitz D. Inhibitory gradient along the dorsoventral axis in the medial entorhinal cortex. Neuron. 2013;79:1197–1207. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Borhegyi Z, Varga V, Szilágyi N, Fabo D, Freund TF. Phase segregation of medial septal GABAergic neurons during hippocampal theta activity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:8470–8479. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1413-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brandon MP, Bogaard AR, Schultheiss NW, Hasselmo ME. Segregation of cortical head direction cell assemblies on alternating theta cycles. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:739–748. doi: 10.1038/nn.3383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brito GNO, Thomas GJ. T-maze alternation, response patterning, and septo-hippocampal circuitry in rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 1981;3:319–340. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(81)90003-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Burgalossi A, von Heimendahl M, Brecht M. Deep layer neurons in the rat medial entorhinal cortex fire sparsely irrespective of spatial novelty. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2014;8:74. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2014.00074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buzsáki G, Buhl DL, Harris KD, Csicsvari J, Czéh B, Morozov A. Hippocampal network patterns of activity in the mouse. Neuroscience. 2003;116:201–211. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00669-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buzsáki G. Hippocampal sharp waves: Their origin and significance. Brain Research. 1986;398:242–252. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91483-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Carr MF, Karlsson MP, Frank LM. Transient slow gamma synchrony underlies hippocampal memory replay. Neuron. 2012;75:700–713. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chrobak JJ, Buzsáki G. High-frequency oscillations in the output networks of the hippocampal-entorhinal axis of the freely behaving rat. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;16:3056–3066. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-09-03056.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chrobak JJ, Buzsáki G. Gamma oscillations in the entorhinal cortex of the freely behaving rat. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18:388–398. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-01-00388.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chrobak JJ, Lörincz A, Buzsáki G. Physiological patterns in the hippocampo-entorhinal cortex system. Hippocampus. 2000;10:457–465. doi: 10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:4&#x0003c;457::AID-HIPO12&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Close J, Xu H, De Marco García N, Batista-Brito R, Rossignol E, Rudy B, Fishell G. Satb1 is an activity-modulated transcription factor required for the terminal differentiation and connectivity of medial ganglionic eminence-derived cortical interneurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:17690–17705. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3583-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cobb SR, Buhl EH, Halasy K, Paulsen O, Somogyi P. Synchronization of neuronal activity in hippocampus by individual GABAergic interneurons. Nature. 1995;378:75–78. doi: 10.1038/378075a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Colgin LL, Denninger T, Fyhn M, Hafting T, Bonnevie T, Jensen O, Moser M-B, Moser EI. Frequency of gamma oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus. Nature. 2009;462:353–357. doi: 10.1038/nature08573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Denaxa M, Kalaitzidou M, Garefalaki A, Achimastou A, Lasrado R, Maes T, Pachnis V. Maturation-promoting activity of SATB1 in MGE-derived cortical interneurons. Cell Reports. 2012;2:1351–1362. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2012.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Deshmukh SS, Knierim JJ. Representation of Non-Spatial and Spatial Information in the Lateral Entorhinal Cortex. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2011;5:69. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Deshmukh SS, Yoganarasimha D, Voicu H, Knierim JJ. Theta modulation in the medial and the lateral entorhinal cortices. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010;104:994–1006. doi: 10.1152/jn.01141.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Desikan S, Koser DE, Neitz A, Monyer H. Target selectivity of septal cholinergic neurons in the medial and lateral entorhinal cortex. PNAS. 2018;115:E2644–E2652. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1716531115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dragoi G, Carpi D, Recce M, Csicsvari J, Buzsáki G. Interactions between hippocampus and medial septum during sharp waves and theta oscillation in the behaving rat. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1999;19:6191–6199. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-14-06191.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Freund TF, Antal M. GABA-containing neurons in the septum control inhibitory interneurons in the hippocampus. Nature. 1988;336:170–173. doi: 10.1038/336170a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Freund TF, Gulyás AI. GABAergic interneurons containing calbindin D28 K or somatostatin are major targets of GABAergic basal forebrain afferents in the rat neocortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1991;314:187–199. doi: 10.1002/cne.903140117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fuchs EC, Neitz A, Pinna R, Melzer S, Caputi A, Monyer H. Local and distant input controlling excitation in layer ii of the medial entorhinal cortex. Neuron. 2016;89:194–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fuhrmann F, Justus D, Sosulina L, Kaneko H, Beutel T, Friedrichs D, Schoch S, Schwarz MK, Fuhrmann M, Remy S. Locomotion, theta oscillations, and the speed-correlated firing of hippocampal neurons are controlled by a medial septal glutamatergic circuit. Neuron. 2015;86:1253–1264. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fujisawa S, Buzsáki G. A 4 Hz oscillation adaptively synchronizes prefrontal, VTA, and hippocampal activities. Neuron. 2011;72:153–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gaztelu JM, Buño W. Septo-hippocampal relationship during EEG theta rhythm. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1982;54:375–387. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(82)90201-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gonzalez-Sulser A, Parthier D, Candela A, McClure C, Pastoll H, Garden D, Sürmeli G, Nolan MF. GABAergic projections from the medial septum selectively inhibit interneurons in the medial entorhinal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:16739–16743. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1612-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, Moser EI, Moser EI. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2005;436:801–806. doi: 10.1038/nature03721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Harris KD, Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Dragoi G, Buzsáki G. Organization of cell assemblies in the hippocampus. Nature. 2003;424:552–556. doi: 10.1038/nature01834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Henriksen EJ, Colgin LL, Barnes CA, Witter MP, Moser MB, Moser EI. Spatial representation along the proximodistal axis of CA1. Neuron. 2010;68:127–137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hinman JR, Brandon MP, Climer JR, Chapman GW, Hasselmo ME. Multiple running speed signals in medial entorhinal cortex. Neuron. 2016;91:666–679. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.06.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Huang Y, Zhang L, Ding Y-Q, Hu Z-L, Chen J-Y, Ding Y-Q. Distribution of Satb1 in the central nervous system of adult mice. Neuroscience Research. 2011;71:12–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2011.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Huh CY, Goutagny R, Williams S. Glutamatergic neurons of the mouse medial septum and diagonal band of broca synaptically drive hippocampal pyramidal cells: Relevance for hippocampal theta rhythm. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2010;30:15951–15961. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3663-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Igarashi KM, Lu L, Colgin LL, Moser EI, Moser EI. Coordination of entorhinal–hippocampal ensemble activity during associative learning. Nature. 2014;510:143–147. doi: 10.1038/nature13162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Jeffery KJ, Donnett JG, O′Keefe J. Medial septal control of theta-correlated unit firing in the entorhinal cortex of awake rats. NeuroReport. 1995;6:2166–2170. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199511000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jinno S, Klausberger T, Marton LF, Dalezios Y, Roberts JDB, Fuentealba P, Bushong EA, Henze D, Buzsaki G, Somogyi P. Neuronal Diversity in GABAergic Long-Range Projections from the Hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:8790–8804. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1847-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Joshi A, Salib M, Viney TJ, Dupret D, Somogyi P. Behavior-Dependent Activity and Synaptic Organization of Septo-hippocampal GABAergic Neurons Selectively Targeting the Hippocampal CA3 Area. Neuron. 2017;96:1342–1357. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.10.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Justus D, Dalügge D, Bothe S, Fuhrmann F, Hannes C, Kaneko H, Friedrichs D, Sosulina L, Schwarz I, Elliott DA, Schoch S, Bradke F, Schwarz MK, Remy S. Glutamatergic synaptic integration of locomotion speed via septoentorhinal projections. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:16–19. doi: 10.1038/nn.4447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kaifosh P, Lovett-Barron M, Turi GF, Reardon TR, Losonczy A. Septo-hippocampal GABAergic signaling across multiple modalities in awake mice. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1182–1184. doi: 10.1038/nn.3482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Katona L, Lapray D, Viney TJ, Oulhaj A, Borhegyi Z, Micklem BR, Klausberger T, Somogyi P. Sleep and movement differentiates actions of two types of somatostatin-expressing GABAergic interneuron in rat hippocampus. Neuron. 2014;82:872–886. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kim T, Thankachan S, McKenna JT, McNally JM, Yang C, Choi JH, Chen L, Kocsis B, Deisseroth K, Strecker RE, Basheer R, Brown RE, McCarley RW. Cortically projecting basal forebrain parvalbumin neurons regulate cortical gamma band oscillations. PNAS. 2015;112:3535–3540. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1413625112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. King C, Recce M, O'Keefe J. The rhythmicity of cells of the medial septum/diagonal band of Broca in the awake freely moving rat: relationships with behaviour and hippocampal theta. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;10:464–477. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00026.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Koenig J, Linder AN, Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S. The spatial periodicity of grid cells is not sustained during reduced theta oscillations. Science. 2011;332:592–595. doi: 10.1126/science.1201685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kondo H, Zaborszky L. Topographic organization of the basal forebrain projections to the perirhinal, postrhinal, and entorhinal cortex in rats. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2016;524:2503–2515. doi: 10.1002/cne.23967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kramis R, Vanderwolf CH, Bland BH. Two types of hippocampal rhythmical slow activity in both the rabbit and the rat: Relations to behavior and effects of atropine, diethyl ether, urethane, and pentobarbital. Experimental Neurology. 1975;49:58–85. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(75)90195-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Laeremans A, Nys J, Luyten W, D’Hooge R, Paulussen M, Arckens L. AMIGO2 mRNA expression in hippocampal CA2 and CA3a. Brain Structure and Function. 2013;218:123–130. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0387-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lasztóczi B, Klausberger T. Layer-Specific GABAergic Control of Distinct Gamma Oscillations in the CA1 Hippocampus. Neuron. 2014;81:1126–1139. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lasztóczi B, Klausberger T. Hippocampal place cells couple to three different gamma oscillations during place field traversal. Neuron. 2016;91:34–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.05.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Leão RN, Mikulovic S, Leão KE, Munguba H, Gezelius H, Enjin A, Patra K, Eriksson A, Loew LM, Tort ABL, Kullander K. OLM interneurons differentially modulate CA3 and entorhinal inputs to hippocampal CA1 neurons. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:1524–1530. doi: 10.1038/nn.3235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Mamad O, McNamara HM, Reilly RB, Tsanov M. Medial septum regulates the hippocampal spatial representation. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2015;9:166. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Manns ID, Mainville L, Jones BE. Evidence for glutamate, in addition to acetylcholine and GABA, neurotransmitter synthesis in basal forebrain neurons projecting to the entorhinal cortex. Neuroscience. 2001;107:249–263. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00302-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Manns JR, Zilli EA, Ong KC, Hasselmo ME, Eichenbaum H. Hippocampal CA1 spiking during encoding and retrieval: Relation to theta phase. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2007;87:9–20. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. McNaughton N, Ruan M, Woodnorth M-A. Restoring theta-like rhythmicity in rats restores initial learning in the Morris water maze. Hippocampus. 2006;16:1102–1110. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mizumori SJY, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. Reversible inactivation of the medial septum: selective effects on the spontaneous unit activity of different hippocampal cell types. Brain Research. 1989;500:99–106. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)90303-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Mizuseki K, Sirota A, Pastalkova E, Buzsáki G. Theta oscillations provide temporal windows for local circuit computation in the entorhinal-hippocampal loop. Neuron. 2009;64:267–280. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Nassar M, Simonnet J, Lofredi R, Cohen I, Savary E, Yanagawa Y, Miles R, Fricker D. Diversity and overlap of parvalbumin and somatostatin expressing interneurons in mouse presubiculum. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2015;9:20. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2015.00020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Nerad L, McNaughton N. The septal EEG suggests a distributed organization of the pacemaker of hippocampal theta in the rat. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;24:155–166. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04902.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. O'Keefe J, Recce ML. Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm. Hippocampus. 1993;3:317–330. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450030307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Oyibo HK, Znamenskiy P, Oviedo HV, Enquist LW, Zador AM. Long-term Cre-mediated retrograde tagging of neurons using a novel recombinant pseudorabies virus. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy. 2014;8:86. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2014.00086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Petsche H, Stumpf C, Gogolak G. The significance of the rabbit's septum as a relay station between the midbrain and the hippocampus I. The control of hippocampus arousal activity by the septum cells. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1962;14:202–211. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(62)90030-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pinault D. A novel single-cell staining procedure performed in vivo under electrophysiological control: morpho-functional features of juxtacellularly labeled thalamic cells and other central neurons with biocytin or Neurobiotin. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 1996;65:113–136. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(95)00144-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Preston-Ferrer P, Coletta S, Frey M, Burgalossi A. Anatomical organization of presubicular head-direction circuits. eLife. 2016;5:e14592. doi: 10.7554/eLife.14592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Ray S, Burgalossi A, Brecht M, Naumann RK. Complementary Modular Microcircuits of the Rat Medial Entorhinal Cortex. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2017;11:20. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2017.00020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Roumis DK, Frank LM. Hippocampal sharp-wave ripples in waking and sleeping states. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2015;35:6–12. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Schlesiger MI, Cannova CC, Boublil BL, Hales JB, Mankin EA, Brandon MP, Leutgeb JK, Leibold C, Leutgeb S. The medial entorhinal cortex is necessary for temporal organization of hippocampal neuronal activity. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:1123–1132. doi: 10.1038/nn.4056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Schomburg EW, Fernández-Ruiz A, Mizuseki K, Berényi A, Anastassiou CA, Koch C, Buzsáki G. Theta phase segregation of input-specific gamma patterns in entorhinal-hippocampal networks. Neuron. 2014;84:470–485. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Simon AP, Poindessous-Jazat F, Dutar P, Epelbaum J, Bassant MH. Firing properties of anatomically identified neurons in the medial septum of anesthetized and unanesthetized restrained rats. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:9038–9046. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1401-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Solstad T, Boccara CN, Kropff E, Moser MB, Moser EI. Representation of geometric borders in the entorhinal cortex. Science. 2008;322:1865–1868. doi: 10.1126/science.1166466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Soltesz I, Deschenes M. Low- and high-frequency membrane potential oscillations during theta activity in CA1 and CA3 pyramidal neurons of the rat hippocampus under ketamine-xylazine anesthesia. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1993;70:97–116. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.70.1.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Somogyi P, Katona L, Klausberger T, Lasztóczi B, Viney TJ. Temporal redistribution of inhibition over neuronal subcellular domains underlies state-dependent rhythmic change of excitability in the hippocampus. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences. 2014;369:20120518. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Spruston N, Johnston D. Perforated patch-clamp analysis of the passive membrane properties of three classes of hippocampal neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1992;67:508–529. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.3.508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Taube JS, Muller RU, Ranck JB. Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and Quantitative Analysis. Journal of Neuroscience. 1990;10:420–435. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-02-00420.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Tóth K, Borhegyi Z, Freund TF. Postsynaptic targets of GABAergic hippocampal neurons in the medial septum-diagonal band of broca complex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1993;13:3712–3724. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-09-03712.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Tóth K, Freund TF, Miles R. Disinhibition of rat hippocampal pyramidal cells by GABAergic afferents from the septum. The Journal of Physiology. 1997;500:463–474. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1997.sp022033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tukker JJ, Tang Q, Burgalossi A, Brecht M. Head-Directional Tuning and Theta Modulation of Anatomically Identified Neurons in the Presubiculum. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:15391–15395. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0685-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Unal G, Joshi A, Viney TJ, Kis V, Somogyi P. Synaptic Targets of Medial Septal Projections in the Hippocampus and Extrahippocampal Cortices of the Mouse. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:15812–15826. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2639-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. van Groen T, Wyss JM. The postsubicular cortex in the rat: characterization of the fourth region of the subicular cortex and its connections. Brain Research. 1990;529:165–177. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90824-U. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Vandecasteele M, Varga V, Berényi A, Papp E, Barthó P, Venance L, Freund TF, Buzsáki G. Optogenetic activation of septal cholinergic neurons suppresses sharp wave ripples and enhances theta oscillations in the hippocampus. PNAS. 2014;111:13535–13540. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1411233111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Vann SD, Aggleton JP, Maguire EA. What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:792–802. doi: 10.1038/nrn2733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Varga C, Golshani P, Soltesz I. Frequency-invariant temporal ordering of interneuronal discharges during hippocampal oscillations in awake mice. PNAS. 2012;109:E2726–E2734. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1210929109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Varga V, Hangya B, Kránitz K, Ludányi A, Zemankovics R, Katona I, Shigemoto R, Freund TF, Borhegyi Z. The presence of pacemaker HCN channels identifies theta rhythmic GABAergic neurons in the medial septum. The Journal of Physiology. 2008;586:3893–3915. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.155242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Viney TJ, Lasztoczi B, Katona L, Crump MG, Tukker JJ, Klausberger T, Somogyi P. Network state-dependent inhibition of identified hippocampal CA3 axo-axonic cells in vivo. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1802–1811. doi: 10.1038/nn.3550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Wei B, Huang Z, He S, Sun C, You Y, Liu F, Yang Z. The onion skin-like organization of the septum arises from multiple embryonic origins to form multiple adult neuronal fates. Neuroscience. 2012;222:110–123. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Wolansky T, Clement EA, Peters SR, Palczak MA, Dickson CT. Hippocampal slow oscillation: a novel EEG state and its coordination with ongoing neocortical activity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:6213–6229. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5594-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Wu H, Williams J, Nathans J. Complete morphologies of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in the mouse. eLife. 2014;3:e02444. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Ylinen A, Soltész I, Bragin A, Penttonen M, Sik A, Buzsáki G. Intracellular correlates of hippocampal theta rhythm in identified pyramidal cells, granule cells, and basket cells. Hippocampus. 1995;5:78–90. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450050110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Yoder RM, Pang KCH. Involvement of GABAergic and cholinergic medial septal neurons in hippocampal theta rhythm. Hippocampus. 2005;15:381–392. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Laura Colgin1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for sending your article entitled "Shared rhythmic subcortical GABAergic input to the entorhinal cortex and presubiculum" for peer review at eLife. Your article is being evaluated by three peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors and the evaluation is being overseen by Gary Westbrook as the Senior Editor.

Given the rather long list of major essential revisions, the editors and reviewers invite you to respond within the next two weeks with an action plan and timetable for the completion of the additional work. We plan to share your responses with the reviewers and then issue a binding recommendation.

In particular, it is most important that you provide clarification about the exact quantitative criteria that justified classification of the "Orchid" cell type. The reviewers agreed that it is not possible to fully evaluate the significance of these findings until the data are presented more transparently and analyzed more thoroughly. Please find the specific concerns and requests of the reviewers outlined below.

Essential revisions:

1) A large part of the evidence for specificity of the projection to the EC comes from the experiment described in Figure 1. However, this experiment lacks adequate quantification. In particular, to support the claim that only sparse axons collaterals target the hippocampus, while labeling is found in other extra-hippocampal areas, a more rigorous analysis is required. Similarly, the statement that "A subset were immunoreactive for vesicular GABA transporter (VGAT, Figure 1C)" is anecdotal and requires proper quantification. For this experiment, it's also important to show where in the EC the axons terminate. For example, Fuchs et al. suggest that septal PV axons preferentially target layer II, whereas Gonzalez-Sulser et al., indicate axons are enriched in layers II-V.

2) It is unclear how many neurons (and animals) were recorded in total in order to identify the 8 labeled neurons reported. This should be clearly stated. It's also unclear how the 8 labeled neurons were selected. This should also be clearly stated. Were these the only neurons with axons projecting towards the EC, or were additional selection criteria applied? If this population matches the "21% of the virally-labeled MS neurons" (subsection “GABAergic orchid cells project to multiple extra-hippocampal regions”), then it should also make up about 20% of the recorded neurons that project to the EC, and a much smaller percentage of the total number of recorded neurons. If the percentage is much greater, or less, then what is the reason for this? This could substantially affect how one interprets the data.

3) The authors need to better explain to readers how the neurons that they have identified relate to previously identified populations of MS neurons. For example, in the passage in subsection “Medial septal neurons projecting to the entorhinal cortex”, the authors should explain why they tested for PV, mGlur1a, and SATB1 (e.g., what does SATB1 immunoreactivity indicate in the medial septum with regard to cell classification?). Also, the authors should clarify which cell groups correspond to cells that have been classified previously (as they do for "Teevra cells") and which correspond to cells of unknown classification. Similarly, in subsection “Preferential synaptic targets of orchid cells”, the authors write, "Targets in three regions…. Were SATB1+… but CCK-…" They should explain what this means, if anything. Does this point toward a particular type of cell? Another example of this is in subsection “MSDB neurons immunopositive for PV and/or SATB1 and/or mGluR1a”, where the authors write, "The largest group comprised PV-/SATB1+/mGluRa- neurons". What type of cells are these? Do they correspond to cells that have been reported previously?

4) With regard to Figure 2, the authors state that 5/8 neurons formed thin collaterals/terminals in deep layers of caudo-dorsal EC (subsection “Firing patterns of septo-entorhinal neurons”). What about the other 3 neurons?

5) The description of the dendrites of labeled MS neurons is anecdotal (subsection “GABAergic orchid cells project to multiple extra-hippocampal regions”). It would be strengthened by quantitative analysis and comparison to other populations of MS neurons.

6) The authors write, "We determined the molecular profiles of 13 targeted cortical neurons and used electron microscopy to test the reliability of predicting synaptic connections from light microscopy" (subsection “Preferential synaptic targets of orchid cells”). Please state from how many mice. This already seems a small number of representative neurons from which to draw a strong conclusion, and maybe even less so if from few mice.

7) The authors write, "The axon showed target selectively, as nearby interneurons with different molecular profiles were not targeted" (subsection “Preferential synaptic targets of orchid cells”). Again, proper quantification is needed here.

8) The introduction of 8 additional neurons in subsection “Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells” raises additional questions that require clarification. What are the false positive and false negative rates for the preceding criteria? In other words, when these criteria are applied to all neurons with recovered axons (including those that have not been reported), how many meet the criteria and don't have EC projecting axons, and how many don't meet the criteria but do have EC projecting axons / other properties of 'orchid cells'?

9) In subsection “Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells”, it is unclear what the authors meant in the following passage: "The first peak was at 143.8[…] during locomotion[…] but occurred later during the other states". What exactly does "the first peak" refer to?

10) In subsection “Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells”, what is meant by "due to state-dependent features of the spike bursts"?

11) The results described in subsection “Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells” are not entirely clear. At one point, the authors refer to "the identified CA3-projecting Teevra cells". Which ones? Do the authors mean the 2 cells that are mentioned in this subsection?

12) In the Materials and methods section, the authors write, "γ power was also used to estimate the location of the LFP recording". How exactly?

13) In the Materials and methods section, the authors write, "If cross-frequency coupling was observed". How was this defined?

14) In the Materials and methods section, the authors write, "Inter-burst intervals were calculated by taking the timing of the first spike of each burst". This method explanation does not make sense.

15) The experiment described in Figure 6 (subsection “Relationship between entorhinal and hippocampal oscillations”) is quite confusing. It states that 1 pyramidal cell was recorded in EC to confirm the recording location – does this imply that the data reported is only from 1 mouse? When talking about MS firing rate distributions being non-Poisson, is this referring to 9/9 neurons from the same mouse? More generally, the point of these experiments is unclear. How do they go beyond similar experiments from the Buzsaki lab (e.g. Chrobak and Buzsaki, 1998; Mizuseki et al., 2009)?

16) The data in Figure 7 would be more useful if they could be more directly compared to the cell labeling data. For example, is the proportion of triple positive neurons similar to the proportion of 'orchid cells' in the total population of recorded MS neurons? And, can the data in Figure 1fFbe normalized to the total cell populations in Figure 7?

17) Overall the organization of the manuscript makes it harder to follow than it perhaps needs to be. For example, would it be clearer if when describing the labeled neurons if their anatomy and molecular markers were described first, and then the electrophysiology was built upon from this? At present, the manuscript jumps back and forth between anatomy and electrophysiology.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Shared rhythmic subcortical GABAergic input to the entorhinal cortex and presubiculum" for further consideration at eLife. Your revised article has been evaluated by Gary Westbrook (Senior editor) and a Reviewing editor.

The manuscript has been improved, but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed as outlined below. The primary concern with the previous submission, namely that insufficient details and quantification were provided regarding the characterized orchid cells, has been satisfactorily addressed. However, not surprisingly given that the paper was so substantially revised, there are some minor issues that remain to be remedied. When this manuscript is resubmitted, you should either consistently mark all changes that were made or submit a clean version of the manuscript.

1) In the Introduction, "such as a place cell sequences" should be "such as place cell sequences".

2) In subsection “Medial septal neurons projecting to the entorhinal cortex”, add "for PV, SATB1, and mGluR1" after "triple immunopositive" to avoid confusion.

3) Insubsection “GABAergic orchid cells project to multiple extra-hippocampal regions”, the authors write, "mid-γ oscillations were coupled to theta cycles preferentially at the theta peak (n = 7/8 neurons)". This is confusing because γ was not recorded intracellularly in this study. So, clarification is needed to explain why measurements are given with regard to numbers of neurons.

4) In subsection “Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells”, "different in rates" should be "different than rates".

5) In subsection “MSDB neurons immunopositive for PV and/or SATB1 and/or mGluR1a”, the authors use the word "inhibited", but it would perhaps be better to use a different word choice because "inhibited" implies that IPSPs or IPSCs were actually measured.

6) In the Discussion section, the authors write, "orchid cells represent a distinct subpopulation of Komal cells", but this seems incorrect because the authors stated that orchid cells fire at the theta peak and Komal cells fire at the theta trough.

7) In the Discussion section, the authors should add, "in the present study" at the end of the sentence for the purpose of clarity.

8) In the Materials and methods section, the authors should replace "elsewhere" with "in planned future studies".

9) In the Materials and methods section”, "remove" should be "removal".

10) In the Materials and methods section, providing more detail (i.e., the width of the wavelet) would be helpful.

11) In Table 2, it is unclear what the letters a-k mean. This should be explained in the caption.

eLife. 2018 Apr 5;7:e34395. doi: 10.7554/eLife.34395.034

Author response


Thank you for sending your article entitled "Shared rhythmic subcortical GABAergic input to the entorhinal cortex and presubiculum" for peer review at eLife. Your article is being evaluated by three peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors and the evaluation is being overseen by Gary Westbrook as the Senior Editor.

Given the rather long list of major essential revisions, the editors and reviewers invite you to respond within the next two weeks with an action plan and timetable for the completion of the additional work. We plan to share your responses with the reviewers and then issue a binding recommendation.

In particular, it is most important that you provide clarification about the exact quantitative criteria that justified classification of the "Orchid" cell type. The reviewers agreed that it is not possible to fully evaluate the significance of these findings until the data are presented more transparently and analyzed more thoroughly. Please find the specific concerns and requests of the reviewers outlined below.

To describe quantitative criteria for classifying these rhythmic medial septal neurons as ‘orchid cells’, we present our answers to Questions 2, 3 and 8. To summarise these points, from 113 implanted mice (with typically 2-3 recording days per mouse comprising hundreds of single-neuron extracellular recordings during movement and rest behaviour) we recovered 10 medial septal neurons with EC-projecting axons, with 8 defined here as ‘orchid cells’. We have found other kinds of medial septal projection neurons, but the strongly labelled ones in our database are too few in number to recognise patterns and it will still take months to years to analyse them.

The 8 labelled EC-projecting neurons in this manuscript probably represent the most comprehensively defined group of subcortical neurons reported to date, since they not only have firing patterns that we recognised as different from another group of medial septal cells we reported recently (CA3-projecting Teevra cells; Joshi et al., 2017), but they also project to specific extra-hippocampal regions (mainly the dorsal presubiculum and caudo-dorsal entorhinal cortex).

These extra-hippocampal regions have been carefully studied in terms of their local circuits and corticocortical projections in behaving rodents (e.g. interneurons and principal cells, Tukker et al., 2015; Preston-Ferrer et al., 2016; Burgalossi et al., 2014; Schmidt-Hieber and Häusser, 2013; Boccara et al., 2010), neuroanatomy/electrophysiology (e.g. Ray et al., 2017; Insausti et al., 1997; Nassar et al., 2015) and in terms of their modulation by pathway-selective inputs (e.g. thalamocortical, Simonnet et al., 2017; medial septal GABAergic inputs, Fuchs et al., 2016; Gonzalez-Sulser et al., 2014;medial septal glutamatergic inputs, Justus et al., 2017). Our discovery and combined neuroanatomical/physiological definition of orchid cells advances the field by providing data on a specific group of subcortical neurons and circuits involved in cortical neuronal coordination, particularly theta and mid-γ frequency rhythmic activity, required for spatial navigation and mnemonic processes that involve extra-hippocampal circuits (e.g. Koenig et al., 2011; Jeffery et al., 1995; Brandon et al., 2013).

Essential revisions:

1) A large part of the evidence for specificity of the projection to the EC comes from the experiment described in Figure 1. However, this experiment lacks adequate quantification. In particular, to support the claim that only sparse axons collaterals target the hippocampus, while labeling is found in other extra-hippocampal areas, a more rigorous analysis is required. Similarly, the statement that "A subset were immunoreactive for vesicular GABA transporter (VGAT, Figure 1C)" is anecdotal and requires proper quantification. For this experiment, it's also important to show where in the EC the axons terminate. For example, Fuchs et al. suggest that septal PV axons preferentially target layer II, whereas Gonzalez-Sulser et al. indicate axons are enriched in layers II-V.

We have quantified axon collaterals in the hippocampus/dentate gyrus and extra-hippocampal areas, which is now reported in the Results section and Materials and methods section:

“We quantified the distribution of axonal collaterals in hippocampal and extra-hippocampal cortical regions (n=3 mice; 3 sections per animal). The proportion of axonal branches in the dorsal presubiculum (PrSd) and granular retrosplenial cortex (RSg) (median: 37%; interquartile range (IQR): 34.3% – 47.2%) was substantially greater than in CA1, the dentate gyrus and the dorsal subiculum (SUBd) (median: 5%; IQR: 2.4% – 9%), where only rare axonal collaterals were observed (P=1.6 x 10-7, Kruskal-Wallis test)”.

We have also quantified immunoreactivity for VGAT in virally-labelled axons (details added to Materials and methods section) and now state in the Results section:

“Within the dorsal presubiculum (PrSd) and granular retrosplenial cortex (RSg), 71.0 ± 25.9% of EYFP+ axonal terminals (mean ± s.d, n = 1046/1416 counted terminals within 12 sampled areas from 3 mice) were immunoreactive for vesicular GABA transporter (VGAT, Figure 1C).”

We have now revised Figure 1—figure supplement 1 to show in both coronal and horizontal sections where the virally-labelled axons terminate in the EC (panels a and b). Gonzalez-Sulser et al., 2014 reported that ~42% of virally-labelled neurons from the mouse MS/(DB) projecting to the medial EC were GABAergic. We found a high proportion of VGAT-immunoreactive terminals in the PrSd and RSg from collaterals of axons projecting to the caudo-dorsal EC. Gonzalez-Sulser et al., found that PV was in a minority of the mEC-projecting neurons (<10%). Thus, the enrichment they observed in layers II and V of mEC may represent mostly non-PV medial septal axons. Fuchs et al., 2016 showed that the PV+ subset of GABAergic MSDB axons target primarily layer II of mEC. The results on orchid cells complement these medial EC-related studies with a different approach, since we used retrograde labelling from the EC (PRV-hSyn-Cre) and anterograde Cre-dependent labelling in the MSDB (EYFP+) to restrict the population of labelled neurons to the injected caudo-dorsal EC and to find their collaterals en route to the EC. We now show in Figure 1—figure supplement 1 that axons projecting to the caudal EC terminate in multiple layers, including deep layers. PV+ neurons were 24.2% of the virally-labelled MSDB neurons (Figure 1F).

We have added the following to the Results section: “Virally labeled medial septal axons with extensive terminals were observed in all layers of the EC (n=4 mice, from 18 coronal or horizontal sections), with collaterals traveling both radially and horizontally (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A,B)” and have revised Figure 1—figure supplement 1 to show greater detail of medial septal terminals in the EC.

We also add the following to the Discussion section: “GABAergic MSDB neurons projecting to the medial EC preferentially innervate layers II and V (Gonzalez-Sulser et al., 2014a), and the small PV+ subpopulation of GABAergic MSDB neurons provides a preferential input to ‘fast spiking’ interneurons in layer II (Fuchs et al., 2016).”

2) It is unclear how many neurons (and animals) were recorded in total in order to identify the 8 labeled neurons reported. This should be clearly stated. It's also unclear how the 8 labeled neurons were selected. This should also be clearly stated. Were these the only neurons with axons projecting towards the EC, or were additional selection criteria applied? If this population matches the "21% of the virally-labeled MS neurons" (subsection “GABAergic orchid cells project to multiple extra-hippocampal regions”), then it should also make up about 20% of the recorded neurons that project to the EC, and a much smaller percentage of the total number of recorded neurons. If the percentage is much greater, or less, then what is the reason for this? This could substantially affect how one interprets the data.

113 mice were implanted with head plates and, in each recording session, several MS neurons were extracellularly recorded with glass electrodes. This was immediately followed by the challenging juxtacellular labelling attempt while the mouse was moving or resting. The above number of mice excludes animals with labelled CA3-projecting ‘Teevra’ neurons (Joshi et al., 2017). For the revised manuscript we recorded from an additional 7 mice in order to target the EC, giving a total of 120 head-plate implanted mice.

The first experiments with recovery of axon in EC (TV50a, TV58g, MS14i) enabled us to search for neurons with similar firing pattern signatures in all subsequent experiments (e.g. burst incidence and preferred theta phase). We therefore selectively targeted the kinds of neurons that were selected for juxtacellular labelling. Our criteria for selection were thus the firing patterns of the first 3 EC-projecting neurons (Table 1).

We have now added the following to subsection “in vivo extracellular recordings and juxtacellular labeling”: “Selection of MSDB neurons for recording was initially random. After recovery of the first 3 neurons with EC-projecting axons (TV50a, TV58g, MS14i), subsequent experiments were biased towards targeting neurons in the MS that had similar rhythmic bursting firing patterns to these 3 neurons; most other neurons were bypassed. Therefore, other kinds of EC-projecting GABAergic neurons may not have been recorded.”

EC-projecting neurons were difficult to target. From the 113 mice, we found only 2 other labelled EC-projecting neurons in addition to the 8 reported in the manuscript. These 2 neurons were excluded because the axon could not be matched to a recording from the multiple attempts in the same animal. Neuron ‘TV46i’ had similar firing patterns to the 8 reported cells (based only on a short recording time), and a projection axon in this animal faded close the EC; 1 collateral was observed in the presubiculum. However, other MS cells were labelled in this animal, so the recording could not unequivocally be matched to the axon. An axon in animal ‘MS98’ projected via a different route (ventral fimbria) to the EC but due to other recorded and labelled neurons in this animal, the axon could not be matched to one recording. Neurons projecting elsewhere in the cortex require further analysis which is very demanding. For most of the unreported labelled neurons, it was not possible to follow the axons to their target regions due to weak labelling. We have updated subsection “in vivo extracellular recordings and juxtacellular labeling” accordingly.

We have removed the sentence about the ‘matching 21% of virally-labelled MS neurons’ from the Results section and revised the Discussion section accordingly: “We found the population of PV+/SATB1+/mGluR1a+ neurons within the MSDB to be relatively small, but the viral tracing revealed that a proportion of these innervated the EC, including those within the rostro-dorsal MS. It remains to be determined what other kinds of MSDB neurons apart from GABAergic orchid cells express these 3 markers”.

3) The authors need to better explain to readers how the neurons that they have identified relate to previously identified populations of MS neurons. For example, in the passage in subsection “Medial septal neurons projecting to the entorhinal cortex”, the authors should explain why they tested for PV, mGlur1a, and SATB1 (e.g., what does SATB1 immunoreactivity indicate in the medial septum with regard to cell classification?). Also, the authors should clarify which cell groups correspond to cells that have been classified previously (as they do for "Teevra cells") and which correspond to cells of unknown classification. Similarly, in subsection “Preferential synaptic targets of orchid cells”, the authors write, "Targets in three regions…. Were SATB1+… but CCK-…" They should explain what this means, if anything. Does this point toward a particular type of cell? Another example of this is in subsection “MSDB neurons immunopositive for PV and/or SATB1 and/or mGluR1a”, where the authors write, "The largest group comprised PV-/SATB1+/mGluRa- neurons". What type of cells are these? Do they correspond to cells that have been reported previously?

We thank the reviewer for this important point about neuronal identities and we have revised our statements for clarity. To our knowledge, there is no previous report on mGluR1a and SATB1 expression in different combinations with PV in the medial septum. Previous classification has been limited to PV+/PV- or GAD+/GAD- (e.g. Simon et al., 2006), and also HCN1 immunoreactivity along the somatodendritic membrane of PV+ and PV- medial septal neurons (Varga et al. 2008). But this latter classification did not lead to a definition of cell types since the axons were not recovered. We have reported SATB1 in some medial septal neurons in the rat (Viney et al., 2003) and provide other novel marker combinations of recorded and labelled neurons in Table 4. We have now revised the first part of the Results section:

“Neuronal subpopulations in the MSDB can be defined by the expression of different molecules (Wei et al., 2012), and combinational expression profiles help define distinct cell types (Viney et al., 2013). We observed that metabotropic glutamate receptor 1a (mGluR1a), along with the transcription factor SATB1 (Huang et al., 2011), show differential immunoreactivity with parvalbumin (PV). As in the cortex, PV neurons in the MSDB represent a subpopulation of GABAergic neurons, but PV is expressed by many different kinds of neurons (Simon et al., 2006; Varga et al., 2008; Viney et al., 2013). GABAergic Teevra neurons are immunopositive (+) for PV and SATB1 but lack detectable immunoreactivity (–) for mGluR1a (Joshi et al., 2017).”

To our knowledge, no cell types in the presubiculum have been previously defined using SATB1 as one of the markers. Based on data from developmental studies of cortical SATB1 expression, we have updated the Results (lines 191-196): “Targets in three regions (PSd, RSg and FC) were SATB1+ (n=13/13; Figure 4B,E), but CCK– (n=0/10) (Table 6). The identity of SATB1+ neurons in these regions remain to be determined by visualising the processes of the cells, and are likely to include both PV+ and somatostatin+ interneurons (Nassar et al., 2015), given that SATB1 is highly expressed within these subpopulations in the cortex (Close et al., 2012; Denaxa et al., 2012)”

PV-/SATB1+/mGluRa- neurons are also undefined in the MSDB, and future work will elucidate their identities. We have updated this part of the Results to take this into account (subsection “MSDB neurons immunopositive for PV and/or SATB1 and/or mGluR1a”): “Apart from PV+/SATB1+/mGluR1‒ CA3-projecting Teevra cells (Joshi et al., 2017) and PV+/SATB1+/mGluR1a+ EC-projecting orchid cells, the identities of neurons within the other groups remain to be determined.”

4) With regard to Figure 2, the authors state that 5/8 neurons formed thin collaterals/terminals in deep layers of caudo-dorsal EC (subsection “Firing patterns of septo-entorhinal neurons”). What about the other 3 neurons?

We have now added (in subsection “Firing patterns of septo-entorhinal neurons”): “The main axons of the other 3 neurons faded before collateralization in the EC due to insufficient labeling (Table 1)”. However, the firing patterns, molecular profiles and in one case the other collaterals (AJ50j, Table 1), were similar to the 5 neurons with confirmed terminations in EC, which are reported in the subsequent Results sections.

5) The description of the dendrites of labeled MS neurons is anecdotal (subsection “GABAergic orchid cells project to multiple extra-hippocampal regions”). It would be strengthened by quantitative analysis and comparison to other populations of MS neurons.

We have counted the number and direction of primary dendrites from 4 orchid cells and 4 Teevra cells and added the following to the Results section:

“Orchid cells had 6.0 ± 1.8 primary dendrites (mean ± s.d., n=4 cells) extending from all axes, which were similar to the distributions of CA3-projecting Teevra cell dendrites (Joshi et al., 2017) (χ2 = 0.02, P = 0.8817, n=4 orchid cells versus n = 4 Teevra cells).”

6) The authors write, "We determined the molecular profiles of 13 targeted cortical neurons and used electron microscopy to test the reliability of predicting synaptic connections from light microscopy" (subsection “Preferential synaptic targets of orchid cells”). Please state from how many mice. This already seems a small number of representative neurons from which to draw a strong conclusion, and maybe even less so if from few mice.

The 13 identified synaptic junctions are on one neuron from 1 mouse (neuron TV58g, Table 6). We have updated this sentence accordingly. Analysis of the molecular profiles of targets was only possible for the most strongly labelled MS cell, since terminals were visible with fluorescence microscopy where targets could be tested with immunohistochemistry. Our previous data in rat and mouse CA3 and mouse retrosplenial cortex where we established that close apposition of medial septal GABAergic terminals using fluorescence microscopy predict with high confidence type II synaptic junctions with cortical interneurons (Joshi et al., 2017, Viney et al., 2013, Unal et al., 2015), supports our current predictions.

7) The authors write, "The axon showed target selectively, as nearby interneurons with different molecular profiles were not targeted" (subsection “Preferential synaptic targets of orchid cells”). Again, proper quantification is needed here.

We succeeded in identifying 13 targeted interneuron somata, all of which were SATB1 immunopositive in their nuclei and 10/10 tested were CCK-immunonegative (Table 6 and Figure 4). Only a subpopulation of MGE-derived interneurons show SATB1 expression, thus these data suggest that ‘orchid cells’ selectively innervate this subpopulation of interneurons. Full characterisation of GABAergic interneurons in the presubiculum and entorhinal cortex awaits further studies possibly using transcriptomic data (now stated in the Results section).

8) The introduction of 8 additional neurons in subsection “Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells” raises additional questions that require clarification. What are the false positive and false negative rates for the preceding criteria? In other words, when these criteria are applied to all neurons with recovered axons (including those that have not been reported), how many meet the criteria and don't have EC projecting axons, and how many don't meet the criteria but do have EC projecting axons / other properties of 'orchid cells'?

We thank the reviewer for raising these important points. We have not yet recovered any neuron with an ‘orchid cell’ firing pattern that targets other areas of the temporal cortex. Likewise, neurons with different firing patterns target other areas e.g. the two septo-hippocampal/dentate cells included for comparison (subsection “Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells”). The CA3-projecting Teevra cells (Joshi et al., 2017) did not target the dentate gyrus or other areas outside the hippocampus and had quantitatively different firing patterns to orchid cells. We have also added this sentence to the Results section: “We did not observe any non-EC projecting MS neurons (e.g. septo-hippocampal neurons) that exhibited all 4 features of the reported orchid cells (data not shown).”

Indeed, further labelling experiments will likely reveal additional cell types. For example, since the submission of the paper, we recently recorded a strongly-labelled theta trough-firing neuron with long bursts, also PV+, that targeted the RSg and CA1 but not the EC (unpublished data). It will take many months to years to obtain more of these neurons with a selective labelling targeting strategy based on differential firing patterns, as they are rarely encountered.

We state in the Materials and methods section under “in vivo extracellular recordings and juxtacellular labeling” that “animals with recorded neurons projecting to other parts of the temporal cortex and with firing patterns that differ from orchid cells will be reported in detail elsewhere”.

9) In subsection “Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells”, it is unclear what the authors meant in the following passage: "The first peak was at 143.8[…] during locomotion[…] but occurred later during the other states". What exactly does "the first peak" refer to?

The peak refers to the peak in each autocorrelogram (highest counts); we have updated the text to state this.

10) In subsection “Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells”, what is meant by "due to state-dependent features of the spike bursts"?

This has now been changed to: “We hypothesized that this increase in mean firing rate during locomotion was due to changes in the spike burst properties”

11) The results described in subsection “Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells” are not entirely clear. At one point, the authors refer to "the identified CA3-projecting Teevra cells". Which ones? Do the authors mean the 2 cells that are mentioned in this subsection?

To assess the reliability of predicting axon terminations from firing patterns, we included 2 novel neurons that had long duration bursts like orchid cells but had a low burst incidence and projected selectively to the dentate gyrus and CA3. We also contrasted previously published Teevra cells, which project to CA3, but not the dentate gyrus (Joshi et al., 2017). We now state that the Teevra cells are ‘another group of neurons’ separate to the 2 additional (novel) neurons.

12) In the Materials and methods section, the authors write, "γ power was also used to estimate the location of the LFP recording". How exactly?

We now explain in the Materials and methods section as follows: “In addition to polarity of the SWRs, in three cases (AJ44j, AJ50h, AJ50j), mid γ (55-80 Hz; detection threshold: >1 s.d. above the mean cycle amplitude) coupling to the peak of theta oscillatory activity was also used to estimate the location of LFP recording in stratum pyramidale. Mid-γ (55 – 80 Hz) is coupled to the peak of CA1 pyramidale theta oscillatory activity (Colgin et al., 2009; Lasztóczi and Klausberger, 2014; Schomburg et al., 2014).”

13) In the Materials and methods section, the authors write, "If cross-frequency coupling was observed". How was this defined?

We used the Rayleigh test for uniformity of distribution between the troughs of γ and theta cycles to establish cross frequency coupling. Mean phase and mean vector length were used as measures of the preferred phase and coupling strength, respectively. We have now clarified the Materials and methods section as follows:

“Theta phase was calculated by linear interpolation between troughs of the band-pass filtered theta oscillations, with 0° and 360° set as the troughs. The Rayleigh test was used to test for uniformity of circular phase distributions. Mean phase and mean vector length were used as measures of the preferred phase and coupling strength, respectively, both for the spike-theta and γ troughs-theta coupling. Mid-γ (55-80 Hz) troughs were detected from the CA1d LFP for the entire recording period (detection threshold: >1 s.d. above the mean cycle amplitude), with most γ troughs detected during movement-related theta oscillations. If cross-frequency coupling was observed (γ troughs coupled to theta peak), spike coupling was measured for γ cycles as described for theta above. Phase histograms were smoothed by convolving with a Gaussian.”

14) In the Materials and methods section, the authors write, "Inter-burst intervals were calculated by taking the timing of the first spike of each burst". This method explanation does not make sense.

We now write: “Interburst intervals were calculated by measuring the time elapsed between the first spikes of consecutive bursts.”

15) The experiment described in Figure 6 (subsection “Relationship between entorhinal and hippocampal oscillations”) is quite confusing. It states that 1 pyramidal cell was recorded in EC to confirm the recording location – does this imply that the data reported is only from 1 mouse? When talking about MS firing rate distributions being non-Poisson, is this referring to 9/9 neurons from the same mouse? More generally, the point of these experiments is unclear. How do they go beyond similar experiments from the Buzsaki lab (e.g. Chrobak and Buzsaki, 1998; Mizuseki et al., 2009)?

Data from the Buzsaki lab are from rats, and it is difficult to directly compare published data of their recording locations within the EC to the site of medial septal orchid cell terminals within the mouse EC. We have now completed revised this part and provide an updated Figure 6 with additional data based on experiments with 7 more head-plate implanted mice. Our aim was to determine whether the rhythmic network activity that we measure in the hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cell layer (our ‘reference LFP’) is similar to that at the site of orchid cell terminals, since orchid cells do not project to CA1. For all recordings we consistently use the CA1 LFP to analyse spike timing, because in CA1 we have a consistent comparison reference point in the pyramidal cell layer, particularly in relation to theta cycles.

In the revised subsection “Relationship between dorsal hippocampal and caudal entorhinal rhythmic activity”, we report that the caudo-dorsal EC, in mouse, has similar LFP rhythmic activity to CA1, based on the strong coupling of EC theta troughs to the troughs of CA1d theta troughs. This was achieved by recording LFPs from at least 2 sites within the caudo-dorsal EC simultaneously with CA1d strata pyramidale/oriens LFPs (Figure 6). The locations were accurately determined by juxtacellularly labelling principal neurons or interneurons and measuring the relative depth. These difficult experiments were successful in 4 mice; 4 other mice had neurons labelled outside the EC and were excluded from analysis. We have updated the Results section and Materials and methods section accordingly.

We have revised the part on MS neuron firing distributions around sharp-wave ripples to state that it includes n=4 orchid cells from 4 mice and n=5 putative orchid cells from 4 mice.

16) The data in Figure 7 would be more useful if they could be more directly compared to the cell labeling data. For example, is the proportion of triple positive neurons similar to the proportion of 'orchid cells' in the total population of recorded MS neurons? And, can the data in Figure 1fFbe normalized to the total cell populations in Figure 7?

Our recording strategy was based on the location of the first three recovered ‘orchid’ cells in a limited to a part of the medial septum, whereas the stereological data characterises the entire MSDB in order to provide a baseline for all studies (now stated in Results section). At this stage, we do not yet know if the molecular profile of orchid cells, which is different from Teevra cells, may include other GABAergic neurons with different axonal target areas (stated in the Discussion section).

17) Overall the organization of the manuscript makes it harder to follow than it perhaps needs to be. For example, would it be clearer if when describing the labeled neurons if their anatomy and molecular markers were described first, and then the electrophysiology was built upon from this? At present, the manuscript jumps back and forth between anatomy and electrophysiology.

We consider it important to show results of anatomical and electrophysiological analysis together because the data are obtained from the same neurons and we would like to emphasize the biological unity of the cells. We departed from this in the first and last figures that provide supporting evidence of the EC-projecting subpopulation of MSDB neurons. This is consistent with our previous efforts in rats e.g. reviewed in Somogyi et al., (2014).

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

1) In the Introduction, "such as a place cell sequences" should be "such as place cell sequences".

This has now been revised.

2) In subsection “Medial septal neurons projecting to the entorhinal cortex”, add "for PV, SATB1, and mGluR1" after "triple immunopositive" to avoid confusion.

Revised.

3) In subsection “GABAergic orchid cells project to multiple extra-hippocampal regions”, the authors write, "mid-γ oscillations were coupled to theta cycles preferentially at the theta peak (n = 7/8 neurons)". This is confusing because γ was not recorded intracellularly in this study. So, clarification is needed to explain why measurements are given with regard to numbers of neurons.

Changed to: “As expected, mid-γ oscillations were coupled to theta cycles preferentially at the theta peak (detected from CA1d LFPs of n=7/8 animals with recorded septo-entorhinal neurons, Figure 2G).”

4) In subsection “Behavioral and network state-dependent firing of orchid cells”, "different in rates" should be "different than rates".

This sentence now reads: “In addition to behavioral state-dependent differences in rhythmic firing, the mean firing rates of orchid cells during locomotion were different than rates during immobility”

5) In subsection “MSDB neurons immunopositive for PV and/or SATB1 and/or mGluR1a”, the authors use the word "inhibited", but it would perhaps be better to use a different word choice because "inhibited" implies that IPSPs or IPSCs were actually measured.

Changed to: “with 4/9 neurons reducing their firing rate during SWRs”.

6) In the Discussion section, the authors write, "orchid cells represent a distinct subpopulation of Komal cells", but this seems incorrect because the authors stated that orchid cells fire at the theta peak and Komal cells fire at the theta trough.

We thank the Editors for this crucial point. Teevra and Komal cell groups were clustered independently of preferred theta phase. Most Komal cells fire at the peak of CA1d theta oscillations, but there is a subpopulation that fire at the theta trough (Figure 1D in Joshi et al., 2017). Komal cells were not labelled in the Joshi et al., 2017 study. We now write:

“Juxtacellular labeling of Teevra cells revealed that they targeted CA3 but no extra-hippocampal areas. These CA3-projecting neurons preferentially fired at the trough of CA1d theta oscillations. Komal cells were not labeled but most preferentially fired around the peak of CA1d theta oscillations. In contrast to Teevra cells, rhythmically bursting orchid cells, reported here, increase firing during locomotion, have long duration bursts, fire preferentially at the peak of CA1d theta oscillations, and innervate the PrSd and EC. Thus, orchid cells represent a distinct subpopulation of Komal cells defined by their theta phase firing preferences and synaptic target regions. The other subpopulation of Komal cells that preferentially fire at the theta trough (Joshi et al., 2017) remain to be defined using juxtacellular labeling in awake animals, as we have focused on theta peak firing cells for the very difficult labeling experiments in the present study.”

7. In the Discussion section, the authors should add, "in the present study" at the end of the sentence for the purpose of clarity.

We have now written: “as we have focused on theta peak firing cells for the very difficult labeling experiments in the present study.”

8) In the Materials and methods section, the authors should replace "elsewhere" with "in planned future studies".

Now revised as: “Animals with recorded neurons projecting to other parts of the temporal cortex and with firing patterns that differ from orchid cells will be reported in detail in planned future studies”

9) In the Materials and methods section”, "remove" should be "removal".

Revised.

10) In the Materials and methods section, providing more detail (i.e., the width of the wavelet) would be helpful.

We now provide further details: “Time-frequency plots (Figure 5—figure supplement 1) used the ContinuousWaveletTransform in Mathematica (Morlet wavelet, 1 kHz sampling rate, 12 octaves, 16 voices).”

11) In Table 2, it is unclear what the letters a-k mean. This should be explained in the caption.

We have now included in the legend: “Letters refer to individually recorded neurons. E.g. in animal TV58, recorded neurons ‘e’ and ‘g’ were selected for juxtacellular labeling; in animal TV50 only neuron ‘a’ was recorded and labeled.”

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34395.031

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES