Abstract
Objective
The benefits of exercise on brain health is well known in aging and psychiatric populations. However, the relationship between habitual exercise in young and healthy adults remains unclear. This study explored the effects an eight-week exercise prescription on cognitive function, brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and cathepsin B (CTHB) in young and healthy adults.
Methods
A total of 22 low-active, young and healthy adults were recruited from a local university. A total of 12 participants performed an eight-week exercise prescription and 12 participants served as controls. Cognitive assessments, cardiorespiratory fitness and plasma BDNF and CTHB concentrations were measured at baseline and eight weeks.
Results
Results showed exercise improved cardiorespiratory fitness (p = 0.044, d = 1.48) with no improvements in cognitive function or no changes in plasma BDNF and CTHB concentrations.
Conclusion
We provide evidence that a short-term course of moderate exercise does not improve cognitive function or change plasma biochemical markers concentrations in young and healthy adults, despite mild improvements in cardiorespiratory fitness. These results suggest that cognitive health may peak during early adulthood leaving little room for improvement throughout this period of the lifespan.
Keywords: Exercise, BDNF, Cathepsin B, Cognitive function
Introduction
Research has consistently demonstrated the health benefits of habitual exercise. Not only has exercise been shown to prevent disease, but exercise is considered an effective treatment for several medical conditions (Naci & Ioannidis, 2013; Pedersen & Saltin, 2006). More recently, considerable attention has focused on the positive effects of exercise on brain structure and function. In elderly populations, exercise has been shown to increase brain volume in selective areas such as the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (Colcombe et al., 2006; Erickson et al., 2009, 2011). Additionally, exercise improves memory (Erickson et al., 2011; Voss et al., 2013), executive function (Voss et al., 2010), attention (Salthouse & Davis, 2006) and decreases cognitive processing speed (Salthouse & Davis, 2006). Exercise has also shown to be effective in treating mental health disorders such as anxiety (Herring, O’Connor & Dishman, 2010), depression (Blumenthal et al., 1999; Stathopoulou et al., 2006) and schizophrenia (Stathopoulou et al., 2006). Complementing human findings, the rodent literature has shown exercise to upregulate adult neurogenesis (van Praag, Kempermann & Gage, 1999), increase neuronal survival (Kobilo et al., 2011), enhance dendritic growth (Leggio et al., 2005), increase dendritic spine density (Eadie, Redila & Christie, 2005), enhance synaptic plasticity (Farmer et al., 2004), induce angiogenesis (Swain et al., 2003), enhance learning (van Praag et al., 2005) and improve memory (Marlatt et al., 2012).
Exercise activates cascades of molecular and cellular signaling mechanisms within the central nervous system. Although the precise mechanisms underlying the neurogenic effects of exercise remain unclear, a growing body of literature suggests that exercise activates neurotrophic mechanisms known to promote neuroplasticity. Most notably, brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is emerging as a key molecule underlying the benefits of exercise on brain function (Cotman, Berchtold & Christie, 2007). BDNF is an activity-dependent secreted protein essential for neural growth, neural survival (Barde, 1990) and synaptoplastic processes critical for learning and memory (Pang & Lu, 2004; Yamada, Mizuno & Nabeshima, 2002). The brain contributes to approximately 70–80% of peripheral BDNF where it is stored and released from circulating platelets upon activation (Fujimura et al., 2002; Rasmussen et al., 2009; Yamamoto & Gurney, 1990). BDNF is also produced in various peripheral tissues including skeletal muscle and adipose tissue and is able to cross the blood-brain barrier (Matthews et al., 2009; Pan et al., 1998; Sornelli et al., 2009). In rodents, exercise rapidly increases the BDNF gene expression in brain regions involved with learning and memory formation, particularly in the hippocampus (Berchtold et al., 2005; Cotman & Berchtold, 2002; Cotman, Berchtold & Christie, 2007; Neeper et al., 1995). Similar to the increases of central BDNF expression observed in rodents, research has consistently shown that acute exercise increases peripheral BDNF concentrations in humans (Ferris, Williams & Shen, 2007; Knaepen et al., 2010; Rojas Vega et al., 2006). However, the literature supporting elevations in resting peripheral BDNF concentrations following a long-term exercise intervention have been mixed. In older adults, a one year moderate intensity aerobic intervention significantly increased resting plasma and serum BDNF concentrations (Erickson et al., 2011) that was positively associated with age (Leckie et al., 2014). In patients with major depressive disorder, an eight week moderate aerobic and resistance intervention significantly increased plasma BDNF concentrations (Gourgouvelis et al., 2018) while no change in serum BDNF concentrations were observed following a three month aerobic exercise intervention (Krogh et al., 2014). In young and healthy adults, a five week moderate aerobic intervention significantly increased resting plasma BDNF concentrations (Zoladz et al., 2008) while no increase in plasma BDNF concentrations were observed following 12 weeks of strength or 12 weeks of moderate endurance training (Schiffer et al., 2009) and no change in serum BDNF concentrations following three weeks of moderate aerobic activity (Griffin et al., 2011). The lack of consistent findings examining the effects of long-term exercise on BDNF might be attributed to the mode of exercise, varying intensities and exercise durations between studies.
It was recently demonstrated that cathepsin B (CTHB), a cysteine proteinases produced by contracting skeletal muscle, is capable of penetrating the blood-brain barrier and upregulating both BDNF expression and hippocampal neurogenesis in wild-type mice (Moon et al., 2016). Following long-term running, researchers also observed an increase in plasma CTHB concentrations that was associated with improved memory performance in mice, Rhesus monkeys and humans (Moon et al., 2016).
Deficits in cognitive function and BDNF expression have been mainly observed in several age associated neurodegenerative diseases and psychiatric disorders (Bocchio-Chiavetto et al., 2010; Diniz & Teixeira, 2011; Erickson & Barnes, 2003). As such, research investigating the exercise-cognition relationship has focused on these populations, with few studies examining this relationship in young and healthy adults. The objectives of this study were to investigate the effects of a well characterized eight-week exercise intervention on cognitive function in low-active, young and healthy adults. We targeted low-active individuals as they may show a greater effect of a relatively short duration intervention. We also investigated whether changes in cognitive function were linked to changes in plasma BDNF and CTHB concentrations.
Methods
Participants
A total of 22 university students (mean age = 21.10, SD = 1.27; 12 females) were recruited from a local university in Oshawa, ON, Canada. All participants completed the physical activity readiness questionnaire to screen for contraindications to exercise. Inclusion criteria included: male or female age 18–30, no history of mental health illness, low-active (exercise less than 20 min, three times weekly) and low cardiorespiratory fitness based on the Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology guidelines (Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology, 1998). Participants were then randomly assigned to an exercise intervention group or a control group to provide baseline and post assessment comparisons. All measures were performed at baseline and then again following the eight-week intervention (see Fig. 1 for the experiment timeline). Participants were instructed not to engage in physical activity the day of testing. This study was approved by the Ontario Institute of Technology Research Ethics Board #11979 - (10-104). All participants provided written consent.
Neuropsychological measures
Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery
Cognitive performance was evaluated using the Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB) software (Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge, UK; http://www.cambridgecognition.com/cantab/cognitive-tests/). CANTAB is currently the most widely published automated neuropsychological test battery (Wild et al., 2008) possessing high levels of concurrent validity and test–retest reliability (Fowler et al., 1995). CANTAB is an accurate, faster and more efficient method to assess cognitive functioning than traditional pen and paper tools (Fray & Robbins, 1996). All tests use non-verbalisable patterns and are presented on a computer touch screen in a game-like format that provides immediate feedback to reduce boredom (Levaux et al., 2007). The CANTAB tests included in our assessment battery assess executive function, learning and memory which have previously shown to improve following an exercise intervention (Colcombe et al., 2004; Erickson et al., 2011; Ruscheweyh et al., 2011; Voss et al., 2010) and to be sensitive to changes in the hippocampus and frontal lobes (de Rover et al., 2011; Owen et al., 1991; Winocur et al., 2006). A brief description of each test included in this study is provided below.
Delayed matching to sample
This test assesses recognition memory for patterns. The subject is shown a complex visual pattern and then must choose one of four similar patterns that matches the original pattern. In some trials the sample and the choice patterns are shown simultaneously, while in others there is a delay of 0, 4 or 12 s before the choices appear. Outcome measures included accuracy and correct response latency.
The paired associates learning
This test assesses visual memory and new learning and is sensitive to changes in the temporal and frontal lobes. In this test, boxes are displayed on the screen and are opened in a randomized order with one or more containing a pattern. Each pattern is then displayed one at a time in the middle of the screen and the participant must identify the box where the pattern was located. The participant proceeds to the next stage when all the correct locations are identified. The test has an increasing level of difficulty that ranges from two to eight patterns to be remembered. The total number of errors was used as the outcome measure for this test.
The spatial recognition memory
This test is a measure of visual spatial recognition that uses a forced-choice discrimination paradigm in which participants must choose between previously learned and novel stimuli. The percentage of total responses correct was used as the outcome measure.
The intra–extra dimensional set shift
This test is a measure of rule acquisition and reversal. This test assesses visual discrimination and attentional set formation, as well as maintenance, shifting and flexibility of attention. The number of stages completed and total number of errors were used as the outcome measures.
Plasma collection
Blood samples were collected from each participant at baseline and eight weeks by venipuncture into ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid tubes and centrifuged within 30 min. Fibrinogen containing plasma supernatant was aliquotted and stored at −85 °C until analysis. Plasma BDNF and total CTHB were quantified using enzyme-linked immunosorbant assays (ELISA) following manufacturer’s protocols (R&D Systems, Minneapolis, MN, USA; BioLegend, San Diego, CA, USA). ELISA plates were read at a wavelength of 450 nm using a Synergy HTTR microplate reader (BioTek Instruments Inc., Winooski, VT, USA).
Fitness assessment
In order to assess baseline fitness levels to determine eligibility and starting intensity for the exercise intervention baseline cardiovascular fitness was assessed with the YMCA cycle ergometer protocol recommended by the American College of Sports Medicine (Beekley et al., 2004; Golding, Myers & Sinning, 1989; Pescatello & American College of Sports Medicine, 2014). This protocol is a submaximal exercise that estimates maximal oxygen consumption (VO2max) from heart rate (HR) measurements and perceived exertion. The protocol consisted of two or more consecutive 3-min stages at a given workload. The objective was to elevate the participant’s HR to a target zone between 110 beats per minute and approximately 85% of the age-predicted maximum HR for two consecutive stages. The initial workload consisted of a 25 W workload at a cadence of 50 revolutions per minute. The workload of the subsequent stages increased by the amount specified by the YMCA protocol based on the average HR during the last 2 min of each stage. When the target HR was achieved for two consecutive stages, the test was considered complete. Participants were also assessed at eight weeks to determine changes in cardiorespiratory fitness.
Exercise prescription
The exercise prescription was based on international guidelines of a minimum of 150 min per week of moderate to vigorous intensity aerobic exercise in combination with resistance activities two times per week, for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory, musculoskeletal and neuromotor fitness in healthy adults (Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology, 1998; Garber et al., 2011). All exercise sessions were supervised by a qualified exercise professional. The exercise group performed one aerobic only and two resistance sessions per week on non-consecutive days for a duration of eight weeks. The exercise intensity for aerobic and resistance sessions were individualized based on each participant’s target HR that ranged between 60% and 80% of their age-predicted maximum HR. Radial pulse was frequently measured throughout each aerobic and resistance exercise session to confirm that participants maintained their target HR range.
Aerobic session
Participants were given the opportunity to choose their aerobic activity on either the treadmill, stationary bike or elliptical machine. Aerobic workloads were based on HR response and were increased by 5-min increments, over the eight weeks, reaching a maximum of 60 min per session.
Resistance sessions
Resistance sessions combined a whole-body exercise prescription engaging the larger muscle groups. For each session, participants performed eight resistance exercises using free weights and resistance training machines. Exercises were performed consecutively in two or three supersets (with an 8–12 repetition range) to minimize rest times and to maintain target HR range. Initial workloads were individualized for each participant based on approximately 95% of their 10 repetition maximum. Subsequent workloads were increased approximately 5% once participants were able to complete three sets of 12 repetitions. Exercises were changed every four weeks to avoid adaptation; although still targeting the same muscle groups. Each session incorporated a 5 min aerobic warmup and concluded with a 15 min aerobic activity.
Statistical analysis
All data were analyzed using Prism GraphPad software, version 6.0. Continuous data are presented as means and standard deviation (SD) and categorical data are presented as frequencies. Independent samples t-tests were used to compare baseline variables across groups for continuous variables and Fisher’s exact tests were used to compare categorical variables. Paired t-tests were used to determine within-group changes from pre-post testing. A two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures (Group by Time) was used to determine pre-post changes and between‐group differences. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05 (two-tailed) and all p-values were Bonferroni corrected. A modified version of Cohen’s D (dppc2) specifically designed for pre-post experiments to account for any differences at baseline was used to calculate effects sizes (Morris, 2007).
Results
Baseline characteristics between groups
Baseline group analyses revealed no significant differences for sex, age, BMI, BDNF and CTHB (see Table 1). One (n = 1) participant from the control group discontinued VO2max testing due to exhaustion and was excluded from the VO2max analysis. The control group showed a significantly higher mean VO2max than the exercise group, t(19) = 3.29, p = 0.004, however all participants met the Poor Health Benefit Rating Zone for cardiorespiratory fitness criteria based on the Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology guidelines (Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology, 1998).
Table 1. Baseline characteristics of participants.
Variables | Exercise group (n = 12) | Controls (n = 10) | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Sex: (male/female) | 6/6 | 4/6 | 1 | 0.639a |
Age (years) | 21.08 (1.24) | 21.16 (1.30) | 20 | 0.976b |
BMI (kg/m2) | 24.67 (3.68) | 22.90 (4.42) | 20 | 0.319b |
VO2max | 17.06 (6.12) | 26.17 (6.53)c | 20 | 0.004b |
BDNF (pg/ml) | 9,079 (3,480) | 8,752 (2,198) | 20 | 0.799b |
CTHB (pg/ml) | 36,011 (10,778) | 42,225 (19,021) | 20 | 0.354b |
Notes:
Data are expressed as the mean with the standard deviation in parentheses. Significant p-values (< 0.05) are in bold.
BMI, body mass index; VO2max, maximum oxygen consumption.
Pearson’s chi-square.
Student’s t-test.
One missing value.
Pre-post measures
A two-way ANOVA with repeated measures revealed a group by time interaction for VO2max, f (1, 19) = 7.90, p = 0.044; d = 1.48, indicating that the exercise intervention was able to improve cardiorespiratory fitness. Biochemical marker analysis revealed no significant group-by-time effects for BDNF, f (1, 20) = 1.29, p = 0.296; d = 0.39, or CTHB, f (1, 19) = 0.812, p = 0.379; d = 0.253, see Fig. 2. Although the cognitive analyses from the CANTAB battery revealed no significant group-by-time effects for the delayed matching to sample, paired associates learning (PAL), spatial recognition memory or intra–extra dimensional set shift (see Fig. 3), a paired t-test revealed that the exercise group performed significantly better on the PAL task post intervention t(11) = 3.30, p = 0.042; d = 1.35. All pre-post results are shown in Table 2.
Table 2. Results of pre-post changes for body mass index, fitness, BDNF, CTHB and CANTAB measures.
Exercise (n = 12) | Control (n = 10) | Group differences | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variables | Pre mean (SD) | Post mean (SD) | t | p | d | Pre mean (SD) | Post mean (SD) | t | p | d | F | p | dppc2 |
Biological | |||||||||||||
BMI (kg/m2) | 24.66 (3.68) | 24.98 (3.19) | 0.18 | 0.86 | 0.075 | 22.90 (4.42) | 22.64 (4.35) | 0.11 | 0.92 | 0.049 | 0.040 | 0.84 | 0.14 |
VO2max (ml/kg/min) | 17.06 (6.12) | 25.79 (11.29) | 3.41 | 0.024 | 1.39 | 26.17 (6.53) | 25.42 (3.24) | 0.41 | 0.70a | 0.19 | 7.90 | 0.044a | 1.48 |
BDNF (pg/ml) | 9,079 (3,479) | 8,680 (3,781) | 0.51 | 0.62 | 0.21 | 8,751 (2,198) | 9,528 (2,612) | 1.25 | 0.24 | 0.56 | 1.29 | 0.27 | 0.39 |
CTHB (pg/ml) | 36,011 (10,778) | 36,332 (15,178) | 0.079 | 0.939 | 0.024 | 40,972 (18,366) | 37,526 (10,826)a | 1.38 | 0.204 | 0.195 | 0.812 | 0.379 | 0.253 |
CANTAB | |||||||||||||
DMS (% correct) | 92.22 (8.91) | 91.67 (9.04) | 0.14 | 0.89 | 0.057 | 90.67 (10.52) | 89.33 (10.04) | 0.41 | 0.69 | 0.18 | 0.022 | 0.89 | 0.081 |
DMS response latency (ms) | 3,364 (1,057) | 3,428 (863.7) | 0.143 | 0.889 | 0.058 | 3,660 (730.3) | 3,239 (494.6) | 1.91 | 0.089 | 0.85 | 0.838 | 0.37 | 0.52 |
PAL (total errors) | 7.42 (4.80) | 3.42 (3.32) | 3.30 | 0.042 | 1.35 | 7.80 (5.453) | 4.20 (3.99) | 1.94 | 0.085 | 0.87 | 0.035 | 0.85 | 0.077 |
SRM (% correct) | 73.75 (9.56) | 77.08 (9.41) | 1.69 | 0.12 | 0.69 | 76.50 (12.48) | 73.00 (16.36) | 0.78 | 0.45 | 0.35 | 2.20 | 0.15 | 0.61 |
SRM response latency (ms) | 2,588 (833.3) | 2,089 (728.3) | 1.63 | 0.13 | 0.67 | 2,805 (1,160) | 2,234 (1,397) | 1.29 | 0.23 | 0.58 | 0.019 | 0.89 | 0.071 |
IED (errors) | 23.58 (19.50)a | 12.92 (3.92)a | 1.75 | 0.108 | 0.66 | 19.70 (15.24) | 21.20 (21.35) | 0.335 | 0.745 | 0.15 | 2.41 | 0.136 | 0.68 |
Notes:
Data are expressed as mean with SD in parentheses. Significant p-values (< 0.05) are in bold.
All p-values are Bonferroni corrected.
SD, standard deviation; BMI, body mass index; VO2max, maximal oxygen consumption; BDNF, brain-derived neurotrophic factor; CTHB, cathepsin B; CANTAB, Cambridge automated test automated battery; DMS, delayed matching to sample; PAL, paired associate learning; SRM, spatial recognition memory; IED, intra–extra dimensional set shift.
One missing value.
Discussion
This present study demonstrated that an eight-week exercise intervention, based on the minimum recommended guidelines, was able to significantly improve cardiorespiratory fitness in low-active, young and healthy adults. We provide evidence that exercise did not significantly improve cognitive function or change resting plasma BDNF or CTHB concentrations, despite improvements in fitness. Our findings suggest that if biomarker levels are within normal ranges at baseline, then exercise does not change these ranges in young and healthy adults. Cognitive function peaks during this time and thus to show effects of exercise on brain function may require more sensitive tests than provided by standardize cognitive testing batteries.
While the present study did not observe any significant group-by-time effects for cognitive performance, the exercise group showed improved accuracy for the PAL task. Although our findings agree with previous research (Etnier et al., 2006; Felez-Nobrega et al., 2017; Kamijo et al., 2010; Themanson, Pontifex & Hillman, 2008), there has been evidence that regular exercise may have cognitive benefits for young adults (Aberg et al., 2009; Guiney & Machado, 2013; Themanson, Pontifex & Hillman, 2008). The inconsistent behavioral findings in the exercise-cognition literature in young adults may be attributed to using cognitive performance measures not sensitive enough to detect small changes in cognitively high-functioning young adults. Whereas the benefits of exercise on cognitive performance observed in elderly adults may be attributed to the potentially greater gains where an age-related decline of cognitive functioning has occurred.
In an attempt to link a physiological mechanism underlying the effects of exercise on cognitive function, we measured plasma BDNF and CTHB concentrations. We did not observe any change in plasma BDNF concentrations following the exercise intervention agreeing with previous research conducted in young and healthy adults (Griffin et al., 2011; Schiffer et al., 2009). Though our study did not replicate Moon et al. (2016) who observed an increase in plasma CTHB following four months of aerobic exercise, our intervention was much shorter in duration. It is worth noting that the animal literature has reported reduced CTHB gene expression in the cardiac muscle of mice following five days of treadmill running (Smuder et al., 2013) and no change in CTHB activity in skeletal muscle following 8 h of exhaustive exercise (Salminen, 1984). However, little is known about the role that CTHB plays in cognitive function in humans.
Limitations
This study was limited by the small sample size. Second, while our eight-week exercise intervention was able to significantly increase in cardiorespiratory fitness in young and healthy adults who were low-active, all participants in the exercise group remained in the poor fitness category following the intervention. As such, significant improvements in cognitive function may have been observed with an intervention of longer duration featuring additional training sessions. Future research should investigate the dose-response effects of exercise on cardiorespiratory fitness, cognitive function and biomarkers by comparing various durations of exercise. Furthermore, the CANTAB battery used for this study might not have been sensitive enough to detect changes in this high-functioning group of young and healthy adults. It is possible that the effects of exercise on cognitive behavior may only emerge when the task is extremely difficult (Voss et al., 2011).
Conclusion
This study provides evidence that eight weeks of the minimum recommended dose of exercise does not change cognitive function, BDNF or CTHB concentrations in young and healthy, low-active adults. Our sample is representative of the a 95% of the adult population in the United States and Canada who do not meet the recommended physical activity guidelines for health benefits (Colley et al., 2011; Troiano et al., 2008). In order to understand the magnitude of the effects of exercise on cognitive function, and the relationship between cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive function across the life span, it is critical that we understand the impact of exercise on cognitive function in young and healthy populations that are physically active and inactive.
Supplemental Information
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Joanne Free and Julia Green-Johnson from the University of Ontario of Technology, for their assistance, technical expertise and guidance.
Funding Statement
Joanne Gourgouvelis received the Ontario Graduate Scholarship. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Additional Information and Declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Author Contributions
Joanne Gourgouvelis conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, prepared figures and/or tables, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, approved the final draft.
Paul Yielder conceived and designed the experiments, analyzed the data, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper.
Sandra T. Clarke performed the experiments, analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper.
Hushyar Behbahani performed the experiments, analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper.
Bernadette Murphy conceived and designed the experiments, analyzed the data, prepared figures and/or tables, authored or reviewed drafts of the paper, approved the final draft.
Human Ethics
The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):
This study was approved by the Ontario Institute of Technology Research Ethics Board #11979 - (10–104).
Data Availability
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
The raw data are provided in the Supplemental Dataset Files.
References
- Aberg et al. (2009).Aberg MAI, Pedersen NL, Torén K, Svartengren M, Bäckstrand B, Johnsson T, Cooper-Kuhn CM, Aberg ND, Nilsson M, Kuhn HG. Cardiovascular fitness is associated with cognition in young adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106(49):20906–20911. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905307106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barde (1990).Barde YA. The nerve growth factor family. Progress in Growth Factor Research. 1990;2(4):237–248. doi: 10.1016/0955-2235(90)90021-B. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beekley et al. (2004).Beekley MD, Brechue WF, Dehoyos DV, Garzarella L, Werber-Zion G, Pollock ML. Cross-validation of the YMCA submaximal cycle ergometer test to predict VO2max. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 2004;75(3):337–342. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2004.10609165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berchtold et al. (2005).Berchtold NC, Chinn G, Chou M, Kesslak JP, Cotman CW. Exercise primes a molecular memory for brain-derived neurotrophic factor protein induction in the rat hippocampus. Neuroscience. 2005;133(3):853–861. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.03.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumenthal et al. (1999).Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA, Moore KA, Craighead WE, Herman S, Khatri P, Waugh R, Napolitano MA, Forman LM, Appelbaum M, Doraiswamy PM, Krishnan KR. Effects of exercise training on older patients with major depression. Archives of Internal Medicine. 1999;159(19):2349–2356. doi: 10.1001/archinte.159.19.2349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bocchio-Chiavetto et al. (2010).Bocchio-Chiavetto L, Bagnardi V, Zanardini R, Molteni R, Gabriela Nielsen M, Placentino A, Giovannini C, Rillosi L, Ventriglia M, Riva MA, Gennarelli M. Serum and plasma BDNF levels in major depression: a replication study and meta-analyses. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry. 2010;11(6):763–773. doi: 10.3109/15622971003611319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology (1998).Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology . Public Health Agency of Canada; 1998. Canada’s physical activity guide to healthy active living. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colcombe et al. (2006).Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, McAuley E, Elavsky S, Marquez DX, Hu L, Kramer AF. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2006;61(11):1166–1170. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.11.1166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colcombe et al. (2004).Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen NJ, Webb A, Jerome GJ, Marquez DX, Elavsky S. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2004;101(9):3316–3321. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0400266101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colley et al. (2011).Colley RC, Garriguet D, Janssen I, Craig CL, Clarke J, Tremblay MS. Physical activity of Canadian adults: accelerometer results from the 2007 to 2009 Canadian Health Measures Survey. Health Reports. 2011;22(1):7–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cotman & Berchtold (2002).Cotman CW, Berchtold NC. Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends in Neurosciences. 2002;25(6):295–301. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(02)02143-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cotman, Berchtold & Christie (2007).Cotman CW, Berchtold NC, Christie L. Exercise builds brain health: key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation. Trends in Neurosciences. 2007;30(9):464–472. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Rover et al. (2011).de Rover M, Pironti VA, McCabe JA, Acosta-Cabronero J, Arana FS, Morein-Zamir S, Hodges JR, Robbins TW, Fletcher PC, Nestor PJ, Sahakian BJ. Hippocampal dysfunction in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a functional neuroimaging study of a visuospatial paired associates learning task. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(7):2060–2070. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diniz & Teixeira (2011).Diniz BS, Teixeira AL. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and Alzheimer’s disease: physiopathology and beyond. NeuroMolecular Medicine. 2011;13(4):217–222. doi: 10.1007/s12017-011-8154-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eadie, Redila & Christie (2005).Eadie BD, Redila VA, Christie BR. Voluntary exercise alters the cytoarchitecture of the adult dentate gyrus by increasing cellular proliferation, dendritic complexity, and spine density. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2005;486(1):39–47. doi: 10.1002/cne.20493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erickson & Barnes (2003).Erickson CA, Barnes CA. The neurobiology of memory changes in normal aging. Experimental Gerontology. 2003;38(1–2):61–69. doi: 10.1016/S0531-5565(02)00160-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erickson et al. (2009).Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Voss MW, Chaddock L, Hu L, Morris KS, White SM, Wójcicki TR, McAuley E, Kramer AF. Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus. 2009;19(10):1030–1039. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erickson et al. (2011).Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, Kim JS, Heo S, Alves H, White SM, Wojcicki TR, Mailey E, Vieira VJ, Martin SA, Pence BD, Woods JA, McAuley E, Kramer AF. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108(7):3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etnier et al. (2006).Etnier JL, Nowell PM, Landers DM, Sibley BA. A meta-regression to examine the relationship between aerobic fitness and cognitive performance. Brain Research Reviews. 2006;52(1):119–130. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farmer et al. (2004).Farmer J, Zhao X, van Praag H, Wodtke K, Gage F, Christie B. Effects of voluntary exercise on synaptic plasticity and gene expression in the dentate gyrus of adult male sprague–dawley rats in vivo. Neuroscience. 2004;124(1):71–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.09.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Felez-Nobrega et al. (2017).Felez-Nobrega M, Hillman CH, Cirera E, Puig-Ribera A. The association of context-specific sitting time and physical activity intensity to working memory capacity and academic achievement in young adults. European Journal of Public Health. 2017;27(4):741–746. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckx021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferris, Williams & Shen (2007).Ferris LT, Williams JS, Shen C-L. The effect of acute exercise on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive function. Psychobiology and Behavioral Strategies. 2007;39(4):728–734. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e31802f04c7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fowler et al. (1995).Fowler KS, Saling MM, Conway EL, Semple JM, Louis WJ. Computerized delayed matching to sample and paired associate performance in the early detection of dementia. Applied Neuropsychology. 1995;2(2):72–78. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an0202_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fray & Robbins (1996).Fray PJ, Robbins TW. CANTAB battery: proposed utility in neurotoxicology. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 1996;18(4):499–504. doi: 10.1016/0892-0362(96)00027-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujimura et al. (2002).Fujimura H, Altar CA, Chen R, Nakamura T, Nakahashi T, Kambayashi J, Sun B, Altar C, Tandon NN. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is stored in human platelets and released by agonist stimulation. Thrombosis and Haemostasis. 2002;87(4):728–734. doi: 10.1055/s-0037-1613072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garber et al. (2011).Garber CE, Blissmer B, Deschenes MR, Franklin BA, Lamonte MJ, Lee I-M, Nieman DC, Swain DP, American College of Sports Medicine American College of Sports Medicine position stand. Quantity and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory, musculoskeletal, and neuromotor fitness in apparently healthy adults: guidance for prescribing exercise. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2011;43(7):1334–1359. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e318213fefb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golding, Myers & Sinning (1989).Golding LA, Myers CR, Sinning WE. Y’s Way to Physical Fitness: The Complete Guide to Fitness Testing and Instruction. Third edition. Champaign: Human Kinetics Publishers; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Gourgouvelis et al. (2018).Gourgouvelis J, Yielder P, Clarke ST, Behbahani H, Murphy BA. Exercise leads to better clinical outcomes in those receiving medication plus cognitive behavioral therapy for major depressive disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2018;9:37. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffin et al. (2011).Griffin ÉW, Mullally S, Foley C, Warmington SA, O’Mara SM, Kelly ÁM. Aerobic exercise improves hippocampal function and increases BDNF in the serum of young adult males. Physiology & Behavior. 2011;104(5):934–941. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guiney & Machado (2013).Guiney H, Machado L. Benefits of regular aerobic exercise for executive functioning in healthy populations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2013;20(1):73–86. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0345-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herring, O’Connor & Dishman (2010).Herring MP, O’Connor PJ, Dishman RK. The effect of exercise training on anxiety symptoms among patients. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2010;170(4):321. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamijo et al. (2010).Kamijo K, O’leary KC, Pontifex MB, Themanson JR, Hillman CH. The relation of aerobic fitness to neuroelectric indices of cognitive and motor task preparation. Psychophysiology. 2010;47(5):814–821. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00992.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knaepen et al. (2010).Knaepen K, Goekint M, Heyman EM, Meeusen R. Neuroplasticity–exercise-induced response of peripheral brain-derived neurotrophic factor: a systematic review of experimental studies in human subjects. Sports Medicine. 2010;40(9):765–801. doi: 10.2165/11534530-000000000-00000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobilo et al. (2011).Kobilo T, Liu Q-R, Gandhi K, Mughal M, Shaham Y, van Praag H. Running is the neurogenic and neurotrophic stimulus in environmental enrichment. Learning & Memory. 2011;18(9):605–609. doi: 10.1101/lm.2283011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krogh et al. (2014).Krogh J, Rostrup E, Thomsen C, Elfving B, Videbech P, Nordentoft M. The effect of exercise on hippocampal volume and neurotrophines in patients with major depression–a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2014;165:24–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.04.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leckie et al. (2014).Leckie RL, Oberlin LE, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Szabo-Reed A, Chaddock-Heyman L, Phillips SM, Gothe NP, Mailey E, Vieira-Potter VJ, Martin SA, Pence BD, Lin M, Parasuraman R, Greenwood PM, Fryxell KJ, Woods JA, McAuley E, Kramer AF, Erickson KI. BDNF mediates improvements in executive function following a 1-year exercise intervention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;8:985. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leggio et al. (2005).Leggio MG, Mandolesi L, Federico F, Spirito F, Ricci B, Gelfo F, Petrosini L. Environmental enrichment promotes improved spatial abilities and enhanced dendritic growth in the rat. Behavioural Brain Research. 2005;163(1):78–90. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levaux et al. (2007).Levaux M-N, Potvin S, Sepehry AA, Sablier J, Mendrek A, Stip E. Computerized assessment of cognition in schizophrenia: promises and pitfalls of CANTAB. European Psychiatry. 2007;22(2):104–115. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2006.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marlatt et al. (2012).Marlatt MW, Potter MC, Lucassen PJ, van Praag H. Running throughout middle-age improves memory function, hippocampal neurogenesis, and BDNF levels in female C57BL/6J mice. Developmental Neurobiology. 2012;72(6):943–952. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews et al. (2009).Matthews VB, Åström M-B, Chan MHS, Bruce CR, Krabbe KS, Prelovsek O, Åkerström T, Yfanti C, Broholm C, Mortensen OH, Penkowa M, Hojman P, Zankari A, Watt MJ, Bruunsgaard H, Pedersen BK, Febbraio MA. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is produced by skeletal muscle cells in response to contraction and enhances fat oxidation via activation of AMP-activated protein kinase. Diabetologia. 2009;52(7):1409–1418. doi: 10.1007/s00125-009-1364-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moon et al. (2016).Moon HY, Becke A, Berron D, Becker B, Sah N, Benoni G, Janke E, Lubejko ST, Greig NH, Mattison JA, Duzel E, van Praag H. Running-induced systemic cathepsin B secretion is associated with memory function. Cell Metabolism. 2016;24(2):332–340. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2016.05.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris (2007).Morris SB. Estimating effect sizes from the pretest-posttest-control group designs. Organizational Research Methods. 2007;11(2):364–386. doi: 10.1177/1094428106291059. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Naci & Ioannidis (2013).Naci H, Ioannidis JPA. Comparative effectiveness of exercise and drug interventions on mortality outcomes: metaepidemiological study. British Medical Journal. 2013;347:f5577. doi: 10.1136/bmj.f5577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neeper et al. (1995).Neeper SA, Gómez-Pinilla F, Choi J, Cotman C. Exercise and brain neurotrophins. Nature. 1995;373(6510):109. doi: 10.1038/373109a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owen et al. (1991).Owen AM, Roberts AC, Polkey CE, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Extra-dimensional versus intra-dimensional set shifting performance following frontal lobe excisions, temporal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. Neuropsychologia. 1991;29(10):993–1006. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(91)90063-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pan et al. (1998).Pan W, Banks WA, Fasold MB, Bluth J, Kastin AJ. Transport of brain-derived neurotrophic factor across the blood–brain barrier. Neuropharmacology. 1998;37(12):1553–1561. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00141-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pang & Lu (2004).Pang PT, Lu B. Regulation of late-phase LTP and long-term memory in normal and aging hippocampus: role of secreted proteins tPA and BDNF. Ageing Research Reviews. 2004;3(4):407–430. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2004.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedersen & Saltin (2006).Pedersen BK, Saltin B. Evidence for prescribing exercise as therapy in chronic disease. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports. 2006;16(Suppl. 1):3–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2006.00520.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pescatello & American College of Sports Medicine (2014).Pescatello LS, American College of Sports Medicine . ACSM’s Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription. Ninth Edition. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Health; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Rasmussen et al. (2009).Rasmussen P, Brassard P, Adser H, Pedersen MV, Leick L, Hart E, Secher NH, Pedersen BK, Pilegaard H. Evidence for a release of brain-derived neurotrophic factor from the brain during exercise. Experimental Physiology. 2009;94(10):1062–1069. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2009.048512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rojas Vega et al. (2006).Rojas Vega S, Strüder HK, Vera Wahrmann B, Schmidt A, Bloch W, Hollmann W. Acute BDNF and cortisol response to low intensity exercise and following ramp incremental exercise to exhaustion in humans. Brain Research. 2006;1121(1):59–65. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruscheweyh et al. (2011).Ruscheweyh R, Willemer C, Krüger K, Duning T, Warnecke T, Sommer J, Völker K, Ho HV, Mooren F, Knecht S, Flöel A. Physical activity and memory functions: an interventional study. Neurobiology of Aging. 2011;32(7):1304–1319. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salminen (1984).Salminen A. Effects of the protease inhibitor leupeptin on proteolytic activities and regeneration of mouse skeletal muscles after exercise injuries. American Journal of Pathology. 1984;117(1):64–70. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salthouse & Davis (2006).Salthouse TA, Davis HP. Organization of cognitive abilities and neuropsychological variables across the lifespan. Developmental Review. 2006;26(1):31–54. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2005.09.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schiffer et al. (2009).Schiffer T, Schulte S, Hollmann W, Bloch W, Strüder HK. Effects of strength and endurance training on brain-derived neurotrophic factor and insulin-like growth factor 1 in humans. Hormone and Metabolic Research. 2009;41(3):250–254. doi: 10.1055/s-0028-1093322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smuder et al. (2013).Smuder AJ, Kavazis AN, Min K, Powers SK. Doxorubicin-induced markers of myocardial autophagic signaling in sedentary and exercise trained animals. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2013;115(2):176–185. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00924.2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sornelli et al. (2009).Sornelli F, Fiore M, Chaldakov GN, Aloe L. Adipose tissue-derived nerve growth factor and brain-derived neurotrophic factor: results from experimental stress and diabetes. General Physiology and Biophysics. 2009;28 Spec No:179–183. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stathopoulou et al. (2006).Stathopoulou G, Powers MB, Berry AC, Smits JAJ, Otto MW. Exercise interventions for mental health: a quantitative and qualitative review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2006;13(2):179–193. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2006.00021.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Swain et al. (2003).Swain RA, Harris AB, Wiener EC, Dutka MV, Morris HD, Theien BE, Konda S, Engberg K, Lauterbur PC, Greenough WT. Prolonged exercise induces angiogenesis and increases cerebral blood volume in primary motor cortex of the rat. Neuroscience. 2003;117(4):1037–1046. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00664-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Themanson, Pontifex & Hillman (2008).Themanson JR, Pontifex MB, Hillman CH. Fitness and action monitoring: evidence for improved cognitive flexibility in young adults. Neuroscience. 2008;157(2):319–328. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2008.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Troiano et al. (2008).Troiano RP, Berrigan D, Dodd KW, Mâsse LC, Tilert T, Mcdowell M. Physical activity in the United States measured by accelerometer. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2008;40(1):181–188. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e31815a51b3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Praag, Kempermann & Gage (1999).van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2(3):266–270. doi: 10.1038/6368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Praag et al. (2005).van Praag H, Shubert T, Zhao C, Gage FH. Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25(38):8680–8685. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1731-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voss et al. (2013).Voss MW, Heo S, Prakash RS, Erickson KI, Alves H, Chaddock L, Szabo AN, Mailey EL, Wójcicki TR, White SM, Gothe N, McAuley E, Sutton BP, Kramer AF. The influence of aerobic fitness on cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive function in older adults: results of a one-year exercise intervention. Human Brain Mapping. 2013;34(11):2972–2985. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voss et al. (2011).Voss MW, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T, Kramer AF. Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2011;111(5):1505–1513. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00210.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voss et al. (2010).Voss MW, Prakash RS, Erickson KI, Basak C, Chaddock L, Kim JS, Alves H, Heo S, Szabo AN, White SM, Wójcicki TR, Mailey EL, Gothe N, Olson EA, McAuley E, Kramer AF. Plasticity of brain networks in a randomized intervention trial of exercise training in older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2010;2:32. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wild et al. (2008).Wild K, Howieson D, Webbe F, Seelye A, Kaye J. Status of computerized cognitive testing in aging: a systematic review. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2008;4(6):428–437. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2008.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winocur et al. (2006).Winocur G, Wojtowicz JM, Sekeres M, Snyder JS, Wang S. Inhibition of neurogenesis interferes with hippocampus-dependent memory function. Hippocampus. 2006;16(3):296–304. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamada, Mizuno & Nabeshima (2002).Yamada K, Mizuno M, Nabeshima T. Role for brain-derived neurotrophic factor in learning and memory. Life Sciences. 2002;70(7):735–744. doi: 10.1016/s0024-3205(01)01461-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto & Gurney (1990).Yamamoto H, Gurney ME. Human platelets contain brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Journal of Neuroscience. 1990;10(11):3469–3478. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-11-03469.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zoladz et al. (2008).Zoladz JA, Pilc A, Majerczak J, Grandys M, Zapart-Bukowska J, Duda K. Endurance training increases plasma brain-derived neurotrophic factor concentration in young healthy men. Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology. 2008;59(Suppl 7):119–132. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
The raw data are provided in the Supplemental Dataset Files.