In this prospective longitudinal study of children born VP or term, the authors examined language functioning at 13 years of age and language development from the age of 2 years.
Abstract
OBJECTIVES:
The objective of this study was to describe language functioning at 13 years of age and examine its developmental trajectory from 2 to 13 years of age in children born very preterm (VP) compared with term controls.
METHODS:
Two hundred and twenty-four children born VP (<30 weeks’ gestation) and 77 term controls had language skills assessed by using performance-based and/or parent-report measures at 2, 5, 7, and 13 years of age. Regression models were used to compare verbal memory, grammar, semantics, and pragmatic skills between the VP and term groups at 13 years of age. Linear mixed effects regression models were used to assess language trajectories from 2 to 13 years of age.
RESULTS:
Compared with term controls, children born VP had poorer functioning across all components of language (mean group differences ranged from −0.5 SD to −1 SD; all P < .05) at 13 years of age. At each follow-up age, the VP group displayed poorer language functioning than the term controls, with the groups exhibiting similar developmental trajectories (slope difference = −0.01 SD per year; P = .55).
CONCLUSIONS:
Children born VP continue to display language difficulties compared with term controls at 13 years of age, with no evidence of developmental “catch-up.” Given the functional implications associated with language deficits, early language-based interventions should be considered for children born VP.
What’s Known on This Subject:
Children born very preterm (VP) have poorer language functioning compared with term controls in early to middle childhood, but few researchers have conducted studies extending into late childhood or examined developmental trajectories.
What This Study Adds:
Children born VP experience generalized language deficits at 13 years of age compared with term controls. The developmental trajectory from 2 to 13 years of age reveals that language functioning is consistently reduced in children born VP.
Language is a multifaceted and complex neurodevelopmental domain that has important implications for functional outcomes such as academic achievement and social interactions.1,2 In studies of children born very preterm (VP) (<30 weeks’ gestation), language functioning can be dichotomized as expressive and receptive language and is most commonly conceptualized as comprising separate yet interdependent components of phonology, grammar, semantics, and pragmatics.3 Children born VP display poorer skills across a range of language components, including receptive and expressive language, in early and middle childhood compared with their peers born term (>37 weeks’ gestation).4–8 We recently reported that, at 7 years of age, children born VP had worse performance across several language components compared with term controls, including phonological awareness, semantics, grammar, discourse, and pragmatics.9 The authors of a few studies have examined language functioning in children born VP beyond middle childhood.10–12 In a well-characterized US cohort of children born VP who were 12 to 16 years old, the VP group had poorer performance than the term controls on syntax and semantics, but not on receptive vocabulary and phonological awareness.10,11 Although language difficulties present in the early years might persist into later childhood, more research is needed to understand language functioning in children born VP beyond middle childhood.
Longitudinal studies are needed to understand the trajectory of language development in children born VP compared with their peers born term, in part to determine if there is a need for intervention.13 The authors of the 2 existing longitudinal studies provide conflicting results. Recently, Putnick et al14 reported that overall language ability was stable between 4 and 8 years of age in children born VP, indicating that they were unlikely to “catch up” to the level of children born term. In contrast, Luu et al11 reported that the gap in receptive vocabulary skills of children born VP compared with term controls narrowed from 8 to 16 years of age. It remains unclear whether early poor language skills can be considered a delay and whether catch-up is expected, or if it is a persistent deficit.
To address gaps in the current literature, the first aim of this study was to examine language functioning in a cohort of children born VP compared with term controls in late childhood. It was expected that children born VP would show reduced performance across a range of language measures compared with term controls. The second aim was to go beyond cross-sectional comparisons13 and examine the developmental trajectory of language functioning in children born VP compared with term controls from the age of 2 to 13 years.
Methods
Participants
Participants were part of the Victorian Infant Brain Study, which included a prospective longitudinal cohort of children born VP and term controls. The VP cohort comprised 227 infants born VP (<30 weeks’ gestation) and/or with very low birth weight (<1250 g) who were recruited at birth between July 2001 and December 2003 from The Royal Women’s Hospital in Melbourne, Australia. Infants with genetic or congenital abnormalities associated with adverse neurologic outcomes and non-English–speaking parents were ineligible. Two infants subsequently died, and 1 child was later diagnosed with a congenital disorder, resulting in a living cohort of 224 infants born VP.
The term control group comprised 77 infants born at >37 to ≤41 weeks’ gestation: 46 recruited at birth from Melbourne’s The Royal Women’s Hospital and 31 recruited at 2 years of age from the community to match the sociodemographics of the VP cohort. The study was approved by the Human Research and Ethics Committees of The Royal Women’s Hospital and The Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne. Parents gave written informed consent for their children to participate.
Procedure
Children had neurodevelopmental assessments at 2, 5, 7, and 13 years of age, which were corrected for prematurity (Supplemental Fig 2). Corrected age is commonly used in research studies of children born VP to account for known biases on cognitive tests, even in late childhood.15,16 Language functioning was assessed as part of a larger neuropsychological test battery at each follow-up. Assessors were blinded to a child’s group membership (VP or term) and previous assessment scores.
Two-Year Follow-up
Language functioning was estimated by using the parent-reported Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Developmental Profile, a standardized tool for evaluating early functional communication skills in children aged 6 to 24 months.17 Social and Speech composite scores (mean = 10, SD = 3) were combined to provide a summary score of language ability.
Social risk was assessed by using a demographic questionnaire, which was used to collect information on 6 social factors associated with child development (family structure, primary caregiver education, primary income earner employment and occupation, language spoken at home, and maternal age at birth).18–20 Each factor was scored from 0 to 2 and totaled to give an overall score from 0 to 12. Scores ≥2 were considered to indicate higher social risk.
Five-Year Follow-up
Language functioning was estimated by using an overall sum of the Receptive and Expressive Language composite scores (mean = 100, SD = 15) from the Kaufman Survey of Early Academic and Language Skills, an individually administered measure of children’s school readiness and language skills that is normed for children aged 3 to 6 years.21
Seven-Year Follow-up
Language functioning was assessed by using the Core Language Index (mean = 100, SD = 15) of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Fourth Edition (CELF-4) designed for children aged 5 to 21 years.22
Thirteen-Year Follow-up
The Core Language Index from the CELF-4 was also used to assess overall language functioning at 13 years of age, generated from the 4 core subtests that are used to assess verbal memory, grammar, semantics, and expressive vocabulary.
Pragmatic skills were estimated by using the parent-reported Children’s Communication Checklist, Second Edition (CCC-2), which is normed for children aged 4 to 16 years and consists of 70 items divided into 10 subscales: speech, syntax, semantics, coherence, inappropriate initiation, stereotyped language, use of context, nonverbal communication, social relations, and interests.23 The scaled scores from the first 8 subscales (mean = 10, SD = 3) are summed to derive the General Communication Composite (GCC), which is used to distinguish children with communication difficulties from typically developing children. In addition, a Pragmatics Composite (PC)24–26 can be calculated to estimate pragmatic aspects of language that are not readily assessed by conventional language assessment tools, by summing the scaled scores of 5 subscales: coherence, inappropriate initiation, stereotyped language, use of context, and nonverbal communication.
General cognitive function (IQ) was estimated by using the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition (mean = 100, SD = 15).27
Statistical Analysis
Linear regression was used to compare mean differences in language functioning between VP children and term controls at 13 years of age (Aim 1). Regression models were fitted by using generalized estimating equations with an exchangeable correlation structure to allow for the clustering of multiple births in the study, and results were reported with robust (sandwich) estimators of SE. Social risk was entered as a covariate in adjusted analyses. A sensitivity analysis was conducted excluding children who had hearing loss (VP n = 8; term n = 1), spoke languages other than English at home (VP n = 18; term n = 7), and/or experienced significant developmental delay at 13 years of age (defined as a composite IQ <70 and/or a diagnosis of severe cerebral palsy; VP n = 12). Some children were excluded for multiple reasons. The proportion of children who were considered language impaired, defined as scoring below −1.25 SD of the CELF-4 test mean,28 less than or equal to the fifth percentile for the individual CCC-2 subscales,23 ≤10th percentile (standard score of 45 according to Australian norms) for the GCC,23 and in the impaired range on at least 2 subscales of the PC,29 were compared between the VP and term groups by using logistic regression.
Linear mixed effects regression was used to examine the trajectory of language development in the VP group compared with term controls across the ages of 2, 5, 7, and 13 years, and age was included as a continuous variable (Aim 2). Children who could not complete the CELF-4 language assessment because of disability, reduced comprehension, or behavioral difficulties were given a standard score of 0 at the ages of 7 (VP n = 8)9 and 13 years (VP n = 4). A z score was calculated for overall language functioning at each follow-up and used as the outcome, with the group (VP or term) included as a predictor, and a random intercept and age effect to allow for clustering of observations within a child. An interaction term was included in the mixed model to determine if the effect of age on overall language functioning differed according to group. Analyses were conducted by using Stata 13.0 (StataCorp, College Station, TX).
Results
At the 13-year follow-up, 179 children born VP (80%) and 61 term controls (80%) were assessed. The sample characteristics of children who participated in the 13-year follow-up are reported in Table 1. Compared with 11 term controls, 55 children born VP had a history of receiving allied health services, with 27 children born VP receiving allied health support at the time of the 13-year follow-up. Speech pathology services were accessed by 34 children (VP n = 29; term n = 5), with 13 children (VP n = 11; term n = 2) receiving speech therapy at the 13-year follow-up (Supplemental Table 4).
TABLE 1.
Characteristic | VP | Term |
---|---|---|
(n = 179) | (n = 61) | |
Neonatal | ||
Gestational age, wk, mean (SD) | 27.4 (1.9) | 39.1 (1.3) |
Birth wt, g, mean (SD) | 962 (225) | 3305 (524) |
Male sex, n (%) | 92 (51) | 25 (41) |
Multiple birth, n (%) | 80 (45) | 4 (7) |
SGAa, n (%) | 15 (8) | 1 (3) |
BPDa, n (%) | 63 (35) | 0 |
Sepsisa, n (%) | 60 (34) | 0 |
Postnatal corticosteroidsa, n (%) | 17 (10) | 0 |
Grade III or IV IVHa, n (%) | 8 (4) | 0 |
Cystic PVLa, n (%) | 7 (4) | 0 |
13-y follow-up | ||
Age (y) at assessment, mean (SD) | 13.3 (0.4) | 13.2 (0.5) |
Receiving speech therapy, n (%) | 11 (6) | 2 (3) |
Receiving allied health servicesb, n (%) | 27 (15) | 0 |
KBIT IQ < 70, n (%) | 12 (7) | 0 |
ADHDc, n (%) | 18 (10) | 2 (3) |
ASDc, n (%) | 8 (5) | 0 |
Severe cerebral palsyc, n (%) | 2 (1) | 0 |
Sociodemographic | ||
Social risk, high, at 2 ya, n (%) | 95 (58) | 19 (32) |
ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; BPD, bronchopulmonary dysplasia; IVH, intraventricular hemorrhage; KBIT, Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; PVL, periventricular leukomalacia; SGA, small for gestational age.
Some group sizes differ because of missing data.
Allied health service is defined as currently receiving psychological services, occupational therapy, and/or physiotherapy.
Cerebral palsy and psychiatric diagnoses are drawn from the 7-y follow-up.
Language Outcome at the Age of 13 Years
As expected, children born VP performed more poorly than term controls on all language components assessed, with group differences ranging from 0.5 to 1.0 SD (Table 2). After controlling for social risk, adjusted mean differences were slightly reduced but remained lower in the VP group compared with term children, as was the case for the sensitivity analysis.
TABLE 2.
Language Measure | VP | Term | Unadjusted | Adjusteda | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | MD (95% CI) | P | MD (95% CI) | P | |
Whole sample | ||||||
CELF-4 | n = 179 | n = 61 | ||||
Verbal memory | 8.8 (3.3) | 10.2 (2.7) | −1.4 (−2.3 to −0.6) | .001 | −1.2 (−2.1 to −0.3) | .01 |
Grammar | 8.9 (3.4) | 10.4 (2.0) | −1.5 (−2.3 to −0.8) | <.001 | −1.3 (−2.1 to −0.4) | .002 |
Semantics, receptive | 9.3 (3.4) | 11.5 (2.2) | −2.2 (−3.0 to −1.4) | <.001 | −2.0 (−2.8 to −1.2) | <.001 |
Semantics, expressive | 9.9 (3.6) | 11.9 (2.4) | −1.9 (−2.7 to −1.1) | <.001 | −1.7 (−2.6 to −0.8) | <.001 |
Semantics, total | 9.6 (3.6) | 11.8 (2.3) | −2.2 (−3.0 to −1.4) | <.001 | −2.0 (−2.9 to −1.1) | <.001 |
Expressive vocabulary | 10.9 (3.7) | 12.8 (2.3) | −1.8 (−2.7 to −1.0) | <.001 | −1.6 (−2.4 to −0.7) | <.001 |
Core language index | 97.9 (19.8) | 108.7 (11.6) | −10.9 (−15.3 to −6.4) | <.001 | −9.3 (−14.1 to −4.5) | <.001 |
CCC-2 | n = 164b | n = 58b | ||||
Coherence | 8.1 (3.6) | 9.5 (3.0) | −1.7 (−2.7 to −0.7) | .001 | −1.7 (−2.7 to −0.6) | .002 |
Inappropriate initiation | 8.7 (3.8) | 9.8 (3.1) | −1.6 (−2.6 to −0.5) | .003 | −1.1 (−2.2 to 0.0) | .04 |
Stereotyped language | 8.6 (3.5) | 10.1 (2.8) | −1.9 (−2.9 to −1.0) | <.001 | −1.7 (−2.6 to −0.7) | .001 |
Use of context | 7.4 (3.9) | 9.6 (3.3) | −2.5 (−3.6 to −1.4) | <.001 | −2.3 (−3.4 to −1.2) | <.001 |
Nonverbal communication | 8.1 (3.6) | 9.4 (3.0) | −1.5 (−2.5 to −0.5) | .003 | −1.4 (−2.3 to −0.4) | .006 |
PC | 40.9 (16.2) | 48.4 (13.0) | −9.5 (−13.9 to −5.0) | <.001 | −8.4 (−12.8 to −3.9) | <.001 |
GCC | 65.5 (24.9) | 77.1 (19.8) | −14.8 (−21.5 to −8.1) | <.001 | −13.4 (−20.1 to −6.8) | <.001 |
Sensitivity analysis (excludes children with significant developmental impairment, deafness, or English as second language) | ||||||
CELF-4 | n = 143b | n = 54b | ||||
Verbal memory | 9.1 (3.0) | 10.2 (2.8) | −1.1 (−1.9 to −0.1) | .02 | −0.9 (−1.8 to 0.1) | .08 |
Grammar | 9.3 (3.0) | 10.3 (2.1) | −1.0 (−1.8 to −0.3) | .009 | −0.8 (−1.7 to 0.0) | .06 |
Semantics, receptive | 9.6 (3.1) | 11.4 (2.3) | −1.9 (−2.7 to −1.0) | <.001 | −1.7 (−2.6 to −0.8) | <.001 |
Semantics, expressive | 10.3 (3.3) | 11.8 (2.5) | −1.5 (−2.3 to −0.6) | .001 | −1.3 (−2.3 to −0.4) | .004 |
Semantics, total | 9.9 (3.3) | 11.7 (2.4) | −1.8 (−2.7 to −0.9) | <.001 | −1.7 (−2.6 to −0.8) | <.001 |
Expressive vocabulary | 11.3 (3.4) | 12.7 (2.3) | −1.4 (−2.3 to −0.6) | .001 | −1.2 (−2.1 to −0.3) | .007 |
Core language index | 100.3 (17.2) | 108.3 (11.9) | −8.0 (−12.6 to −3.5) | .001 | −6.7 (−11.5 to −1.8) | .007 |
CCC-2 | n = 110b | n = 51b | ||||
Coherence | 8.3 (3.6) | 9.6 (2.9) | −1.5 (−2.6 to −0.4) | .006 | −1.5 (−2.5 to −0.4) | .008 |
Inappropriate initiation | 8.9 (4.0) | 9.8 (3.0) | −1.4 (−2.5 to −0.2) | .02 | −0.9 (−2.0 to −0.3) | .14 |
Stereotyped language | 8.8 (3.4) | 10.1 (2.8) | −1.7 (−2.7 to −0.7) | .001 | −1.5 (−2.5 to −0.5) | .003 |
Use of context | 7.7 (3.9) | 9.6 (3.2) | −2.2 (−3.4 to −1.0) | <.001 | −2.0 (−3.2 to −0.9) | .001 |
Nonverbal communication | 8.2 (3.7) | 9.3 (3.0) | −1.3 (−2.4 to −0.2) | .02 | −1.2 (−2.2 to −0.1) | .03 |
PC | 42.0 (16.3) | 48.3 (12.7) | −8.4 (−13.2 to −3.6) | .001 | −7.4 (−12.0 to −2.7) | .002 |
GCC | 67.6 (24.3) | 76.7 (19.7) | −12.5 (−19.8 to −5.3) | .001 | −11.3 (−18.2 to −4.4) | .001 |
MD, mean difference.
Adjusted for social risk.
Sample numbers differ because of missing data.
Across most language domains, a higher proportion of children born VP had an impairment compared with term controls, and odds ratios ranged from 2.1 to 8.1 (Table 3). Impairment rates in the VP group were particularly elevated for expressive language measures. Similarly, a higher proportion of children born VP (45%) had an impairment on at least 1 language measure compared with term controls (18%; odds ratio = 4.0; 95% confidence interval [CI] = 1.9 to 8.5; P < .001).
TABLE 3.
Language Component | VP | Term | Odds Ratio (95% CI) | P |
---|---|---|---|---|
CELF-4 | n = 179 | n = 61 | ||
Verbal memory, n (%) | 24 (13) | 3 (5) | 2.9 (0.8 to 10.1) | .07 |
Grammar, n (%) | 19 (11) | 2 (3) | 3.4 (0.8 to 15.4) | .09 |
Semantics, receptive, n (%) | 23 (13) | 0 | N/Aa | .003 |
Semantics, expressive, n (%) | 32 (18) | 2 (3) | 6.4 (1.5 to 27.9) | .005 |
Semantics, total, n (%) | 24 (13) | 0 | N/Aa | .002 |
Expressive vocabulary, n (%) | 23 (13) | 1 (2) | 8.1 (1.1 to 62.2) | .01 |
Core language index, n (%) | 18 (10) | 2 (3) | 3.1 (0.7 to 14.2) | .10 |
CCC-2 | n = 164b | n = 58b | ||
Coherence | 24 (18) | 3 (5) | 4.6 (1.3 to 16.2) | .02 |
Inappropriate initiation | 33 (24) | 6 (10) | 3.5 (1.3 to 9.5) | .03 |
Stereotyped language | 18 (13) | 4 (7) | 2.1 (0.7 to 6.3) | .20 |
Use of context | 45 (33) | 7 (12) | 4.6 (1.8 to 11.7) | .002 |
Nonverbal communication | 23 (17) | 3 (5) | 3.9 (1.1 to 13.4) | .03 |
PC, n (%) | 36 (28) | 8 (14) | 3.0 (1.2 to 7.2) | .04 |
GCC, n (%) | 34 (25) | 6 (10) | 4.2 (1.5 to 11.6) | .02 |
Language impairment is defined as scoring below −1.25 SD of the CELF-4 test mean, less than or equal to the fifth percentile for CCC-2 subscales, ≤10th percentile (standard score = 45) for the GCC, and in the impaired range on ≥2 subscales of the PC. N/A, not applicable.
Odds ratio not calculated because of a value of 0 cases in 1 of the groups.
Group numbers differ across variables because of missing data.
Trajectory of Language Development
Across the ages of 2, 5, 7, and 13 years, the VP group showed lower language functioning compared with term controls (overall mean difference = −0.7 SD; 95% CI = −0.9 to −0.5 SD; P < .001), with little evidence that the average level of language function varied with age (slope = −0.06; 95% CI = −0.2 to 0.01 SD per year; P = .29). There was little evidence that the trajectory of language development varied by group (slope difference = −0.01 SD per year; 95% CI = −0.04 to 0.02; interaction P = .55; Fig 1), and this finding did not change when the analysis was adjusted for social risk (interaction P = .49) or in the sensitivity analysis that excluded children with hearing loss or English as a second language and/or who experienced significant developmental delay (interaction P = .48).
Discussion
This study revealed that language deficits are an ongoing concern for children born VP, with marked and generalized deficits observed at the age of 13 years. Consistent with this finding, our longitudinal trajectory analyses revealed no evidence of developmental catch-up for the VP group, with children born VP displaying persistent language difficulties compared with term controls between the ages of 2 and 13 years. Our findings were robust, remaining largely unaltered after adjusting for social risk and excluding children with hearing loss, English as a second language, intellectual impairment, or severe cerebral palsy.
Our VP cohort had elevated rates of language impairment compared with term controls, with rates similar to those reported in previous studies of younger VP samples.2,4,8,9 Impairments were observed across all language components assessed at 13 years of age in children born VP, reflective of a generalized language impairment rather than selective deficits (eg, pragmatic impairment). This is consistent with the findings of our earlier study of language in the same cohort, in which children born VP displayed impairments in phonology, grammar, semantics, discourse, and pragmatics at 7 years of age,9 as well as with a meta-analysis whose authors reported poorer receptive and expressive semantics in school-aged children.30 In contrast, Luu et al10 reported selective difficulties in syntactic and semantic language skills in a cohort of 12-year-old children born with very low birth weight, who showed comparable phonological processing skills compared with term controls. Although difficulties across the language components suggested a generalized language deficit, it should be noted that there was an increased risk for impairments for expressive language skills in the VP group.
Language develops as a series of hierarchically organized abilities, with later higher-order functions incorporating and building on earlier skills.31 Therefore, long-term longitudinal studies whose authors examine language extending into the adolescent years are needed to determine the long-term trajectory of language development in children born VP. In our study, we provide important insight into the development of language in children born VP, with poorer language functioning and almost identical developmental slopes compared with term controls from 2 to 13 years of age. Our findings are supported by a recent longitudinal study of children born VP from infancy to early school age that revealed consistently lower language functioning.14 However, the authors of a meta-analysis reported an increase in difficulties on core language tests in children born preterm from 3 to 12 years of age,30 whereas Luu et al11,12 reported an improvement in receptive vocabulary skills in children born VP from 3 to 12 and 8 to 16 years of age. These mixed findings may be explained by methodological differences, such as study design, use of term control groups or normative comparisons, and language measures used. It is important to note that significant interindividual variability is observed in language outcomes of children born VP, and further research is needed to examine this heterogeneity.
Persisting language difficulties in our VP cohort across childhood highlights the importance of long-term follow-up, including language function. Language functioning has an important role in the development of social competencies, such as social interaction and peer relations32 and healthy internalizing and externalizing behaviors.33 Academic achievement, including reading, writing, and mathematics, has also been associated with language skills in VP samples.34,35 As such, research focused on intervention strategies for children born VP aimed at improving early language development is needed. The majority of targeted language intervention strategies focused on promoting parental awareness and interaction and improving specific aspects of children’s language and communication skills have resulted in encouraging outcomes, including improvements in expressive vocabulary and syntactic and phonological development.36,37 However, evidence from controlled trials remains limited,38 and the efficacy of early language interventions for children born VP remains a topic for future research. Early identification of children at risk for ongoing language problems and in most need of early intervention is important, yet at this stage, the factors contributing to language impairment in children born VP are poorly understood.
The strengths of this study were that we assessed multiple components of language in a large cohort of children born VP, used a control group, and explored the developmental trajectory of general language functioning from 2 to 13 years of age. As is inherent in developmental longitudinal studies, different language assessment tools were administered at the different follow-up waves. However, all tools are valid measures of language functioning and performance was based on our term control group’s distribution rather than on different test norms. We acknowledge that our developmental trajectory results are limited to overall language functioning, and we were unable to explore the development of specific language domains. Data regarding the influence of familial dispositions for childhood language deficits were not collected. As such, this will be an important avenue for future research because it may provide a richer understanding of genetically based factors useful for identifying children who would benefit most from early support.
Conclusions
Difficulties in language functioning in this cohort of children born VP remained stable from 2 to 13 years of age. Although generalized language deficits were observed, the VP group had marked difficulties on expressive components of language. With our findings, we emphasize the need for effective and timely interventions for language skills in children born VP to close the gap with their term peers.
Acknowledgments
We thank the people in the Victorian Infant Brain Study team for all their valuable work and the study children and families for their continuous support.
Glossary
- CCC-2
Children’s Communication Checklist, Second Edition
- CELF-4
Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Fourth Edition
- CI
confidence interval
- GCC
General Communication Composite
- PC
Pragmatics Composite
- VP
very preterm
Footnotes
Ms Nguyen collected data, conducted initial analyses, drafted the initial manuscript, and revised the manuscript; Profs Anderson, Doyle, and Inder, Associate Profs Cheong and Thompson, and Dr Spencer-Smith conceptualized and designed the study and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript; Drs Ellis and Scratch coordinated and supervised data collection and reviewed the manuscript; Ms Zannino planned and assisted in statistical analyses and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Burnett revised statistical analyses and critically reviewed the manuscript; Ms Pascoe collected data and reviewed and revised the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
FUNDING: Supported by the Australian National Health and Medical Council (Centre for Research Excellence 1060733; Project Grants 237117, 491209, and 1066555; Senior Research Fellowship [Prof Anderson] 1081288; Career Development Fellowship [Associate Prof Thompson] 1085754), the Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, The Royal Children’s Hospital Foundation, and the Victorian Government’s Operational Infrastructure Support Program.
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.
References
- 1.Pritchard VE, Clark CA, Liberty K, Champion PR, Wilson K, Woodward LJ. Early school-based learning difficulties in children born very preterm. Early Hum Dev. 2009;85(4):215–224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Woodward LJ, Moor S, Hood KM, et al. . Very preterm children show impairments across multiple neurodevelopmental domains by age 4 years. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2009;94(5):F339–F344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Vohr B. Speech and language outcomes of very preterm infants. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2014;19(2):78–83 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Barre N, Morgan A, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ. Language abilities in children who were very preterm and/or very low birth weight: a meta-analysis. J Pediatr. 2011;158(5):766–774.e1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Foster-Cohen S, Edgin JO, Champion PR, Woodward LJ. Early delayed language development in very preterm infants: evidence from the MacArthur-Bates CDI. J Child Lang. 2007;34(3):655–675 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Guarini A, Marini A, Savini S, Alessandroni R, Faldella G, Sansavini A. Linguistic features in children born very preterm at preschool age. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2016;58(9):949–956 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wolke D, Meyer R. Cognitive status, language attainment, and prereading skills of 6-year-old very preterm children and their peers: the Bavarian Longitudinal Study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1999;41(2):94–109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Foster-Cohen SH, Friesen MD, Champion PR, Woodward LJ. High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very preterm. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2010;31(8):658–667 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Reidy N, Morgan A, Thompson DK, Inder TE, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ. Impaired language abilities and white matter abnormalities in children born very preterm and/or very low birth weight. J Pediatr. 2013;162(4):719–724 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Luu TM, Ment LR, Schneider KC, Katz KH, Allan WC, Vohr BR. Lasting effects of preterm birth and neonatal brain hemorrhage at 12 years of age. Pediatrics. 2009;123(3):1037–1044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Luu TM, Vohr BR, Allan W, Schneider KC, Ment LR. Evidence for catch-up in cognition and receptive vocabulary among adolescents born very preterm. Pediatrics. 2011;128(2):313–322 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Luu TM, Vohr BR, Schneider KC, et al. . Trajectories of receptive language development from 3 to 12 years of age for very preterm children. Pediatrics. 2009;124(1):333–341 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Thomas MSC, Annaz D, Ansari D, Scerif G, Jarrold C, Karmiloff-Smith A. Using developmental trajectories to understand developmental disorders. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2009;52(2):336–358 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Putnick DL, Bornstein MH, Eryigit-Madzwamuse S, Wolke D. Long-term stability of language performance in very preterm, moderate-late preterm, and term children. J Pediatr. 2017;181:74–79.e3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Wilson-Ching M, Pascoe L, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ. Effects of correcting for prematurity on cognitive test scores in childhood. J Paediatr Child Health. 2014;50(3):182–188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Doyle LW, Anderson PJ. Do we need to correct age for prematurity when assessing children? J Pediatr. 2016;173:11–12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Wetherby AM, Prizant BM. Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales: Developmental Profile. Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes Publishing; 2002 [Google Scholar]
- 18.Treyvaud K, Ure A, Doyle LW, et al. . Psychiatric outcomes at age seven for very preterm children: rates and predictors. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54(7):772–779 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Roberts G, Howard K, Spittle AJ, Brown NC, Anderson PJ, Doyle LW. Rates of early intervention services in very preterm children with developmental disabilities at age 2 years. J Paediatr Child Health. 2008;44(5):276–280 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Treyvaud K, Doyle LW, Lee KJ, et al. . Social-emotional difficulties in very preterm and term 2 year olds predict specific social-emotional problems at the age of 5 years. J Pediatr Psychol. 2012;37(7):779–785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Kaufman Survey of Early Academic and Language Skills (K-SEALS). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc; 1993 [Google Scholar]
- 22.Semel E, Wiig EH, Secord WA. Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals 4 (CELF-4) Technical Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2003 [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bishop DVM. The Children’s Communication Checklist. 2nd ed London, United Kingdom: Psychological Corporation; 2003 [Google Scholar]
- 24.Helland WA, Helland T, Heimann M. Language profiles and mental health problems in children with specific language impairment and children with ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2014;18(3):226–235 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Helland WA. Differentiating children with specific language impairment and children with Asperger syndrome using parental reports. Ann Psychiatry Ment Health. 2014;2(3):1013 [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bignell S, Cain K. Pragmatic aspects of communication and language comprehension in groups of children differentiated by teacher ratings of inattention and hyperactivity. Br J Dev Psychol. 2007;25(4):499–512 [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test Second Edition (KBIT-2) Manual. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson; 1997 [Google Scholar]
- 28.Tomblin JB, Records NL, Zhang X. A system for the diagnosis of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. J Speech Hear Res. 1996;39(6):1284–1294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Chiat S, Roy P. Early phonological and sociocognitive skills as predictors of later language and social communication outcomes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(6):635–645 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.van Noort-van der Spek IL, Franken MC, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Language functions in preterm-born children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2012;129(4):745–754 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bornstein MH, Hahn CS, Putnick DL, Suwalsky JTD. Stability of core language skill from early childhood to adolescence: a latent variable approach. Child Dev. 2014;85(4):1346–1356 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.St Clair MC, Pickles A, Durkin K, Conti-Ramsden G. A longitudinal study of behavioral, emotional and social difficulties in individuals with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). J Commun Disord. 2011;44(2):186–199 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bornstein MH, Hahn CS, Suwalsky JTD. Language and internalizing and externalizing behavioral adjustment: developmental pathways from childhood to adolescence. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25(3):857–878 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Guarini A, Sansavini A, Fabbri C, et al. . Long-term effects of preterm birth on language and literacy at eight years. J Child Lang. 2010;37(4):865–885 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Wocadlo C, Rieger I. Phonology, rapid naming and academic achievement in very preterm children at eight years of age. Early Hum Dev. 2007;83(6):367–377 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Thomas-Stonell N, Oddson B, Robertson B, Rosenbaum P. Predicted and observed outcomes in preschool children following speech and language treatment: parent and clinician perspectives. J Commun Disord. 2009;42(1):29–42 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kaiser AP, Roberts MY. Advances in early communication and language intervention. J Early Interv. 2011;33(4):298–309 [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wake M, Levickis P, Tobin S, et al. . Improving outcomes of preschool language delay in the community: protocol for the Language for Learning randomised controlled trial. BMC Pediatr. 2012;12(1):96–106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]