Abstract
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been known for over a decade to have the potential to greatly enhance the process of developing novel therapeutic drugs for prevalent health conditions. However, the use of fMRI in drug development continues to be relatively limited because of a variety of technical, biological, and strategic barriers that continue to limit progress. Here, we briefly review the roles that fMRI can have in the drug development process and the requirements it must meet to be useful in this setting. We then provide an update on our current understanding of the strengths and limitations of fMRI as a tool for drug developers and recommend activities to enhance its utility.
Introduction and scope
Here, we provide an update on the state of the art in the use of fMRI in the drug development process, including the requirements it must meet, its current capabilities, challenges that limit its use, and a set of activities that are proposed to meet the challenges. Although our review covers both task-based and resting-state fMRI, it echoes some of the themes of a recent review that was limited in scope to only resting-state fMRI, including the requirements for use of fMRI as a biomarker, the need for collaborative research efforts and validation, and the challenge of biological confounds [1]. Here, we also provide an update on several of the issues raised by a review on this topic published over 10 years ago, especially in relation to homologies between animal and human fMRI data, limitations to the interpretability of fMRI data, and quantitative fMRI techniques [2]. Finally, we also update information about best practices for fMRI in clinical trials, a topic that has been presented previously [3,4].
We begin by discussing the broader context surrounding fMRI in drug development.
Definitions: fMRI
This review is concerned with fMRI of the brain, with data predominantly provided either by blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) [5,6] or arterial spin-labeling (ASL) perfusion MRI [7] sequences. Furthermore, we consider three experimental settings within which fMRI data are collected. First, task-based fMRI uses sensory or cognitive stimuli to provoke responses from brain regions or circuits involved in responding to the stimuli. These provoked responses include changes in fMRI signal amplitudes (i. e., activations or deactivations) as well as changes in functional connectivity (low-frequency temporal correlations in fMRI signals between brain regions). Second, resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) is used to examine functional connectivity during ostensible rest times [8]. Third, pharmacological MRI (phMRI) records fMRI signals following the administration of pharmacological agents [9]. Other dynamic MRI techniques (such as dynamic contrast-enhanced imaging) or dynamic neuroimaging techniques outside of MRI (such as positron emission tomography, PET) fall outside of the scope of this review.
Definitions: drug development
The drug development process starts with identification of a biological target hypothesized to be implicated in a disease process. Thousands of molecules might then be tested for their chemical properties and ability to bind to the target molecule in vitro [10,11]. Of those, tens of molecules are tested in preclinical animal models of the disease. In addition to toxicity, molecules are tested for their pharmacokinetics (PK), bioavailability at the target organ, in vivo target engagement, biological or chemical response that can be directly linked to the molecular action in the organism (pharmacodynamics, PD), and efficacy in the animal model [12,13]. This process builds confidence that the handful of molecules with the best in vitro and in vivo profiles will also be safe, engage the intended target, and potentially treat the disease in humans. Activities then shift to human clinical trials, where the process can include four different phases. Phase 0 studies are used to test scientific hypotheses or novel imaging methods in the absence of therapy, or to evaluate novel therapeutic strategies at presumed subclinical (‘micro’) doses [14–16]. In Phase 1, tens of individuals are enrolled to demonstrate that the drug is tolerable and safe at multiple doses, including those anticipated to evoke an efficacious clinical response [17–20]. PK and PD responses are increasingly assessed in Phase 1 to provide better-informed dose selection or design of subsequent Phase 2 trials. In Phase 2, on the order of hundreds of subjects are typically tested at a single or few doses to compare therapeutic responses against those of a similar cohort treated with placebo or control therapy. Safety assessments are made to assess the less-common adverse effects of the drug. In Phase 3, usually hundreds to thousands of subjects are tested at multiple sites, typically at a single dose, to confirm the safety and efficacy profiles suggested in earlier Phase trials. Throughout this process, there is the potential for imaging assays, such as fMRI, to impact the mechanistic evaluation of drugs and differentiation between treatment responders and nonresponders.
Possible roles for fMRI in drug development
The challenges presented by drug development for central nervous system (CNS) indications have motivated the search for PD methodologies that readily translate from preclinical models to patients and predict clinical efficacy [21]. Preclincal studies use transgenic or inducible rodent models of disease and in vitro studies that serve to characterize the pharmacologic properties and predicted clinical effects of a novel molecule [22–25]. However, many of these approaches lack predictive validity to complex human neuropsychiatric disorders. In addition, comparing a novel first-in-class molecule to a ‘gold standard’ reference molecule can be difficult: there is no useful reference for many of the psychiatric conditions with large unmet needs, and existing preclinical models might have relatively low sensitivity for novel mechanisms of action [26,27]. These difficulties motivate the search for alternative measurements in animal models that, alongside existing in vitro and animal model techniques, help inform human studies via homologies between animal and human measurements. Although not yet commonly used for this purpose, fMRI has the potential to help meet this need.
In early-Phase clinical studies, fMRI methods can provide a means to detect a functional CNS effect of pharmacological treatment in brain regions appropriate to the mechanism and/or target population of the compound [9,28–30]. Although it is not technically a marker of target engagement (i.e., of pharmacological agent binding to a target site), an fMRI signal can provide indirect evidence of target engagement if a biologically plausible link can be established between the fMRI response and the molecular target [31–33]. Dose–response and exposure–response relationships established using fMRI are of particular value to guide dose selection for later Phases [34–36].
Later phases (Phase 2 and 3) involve large patient studies at multiple clinical sites designed to identify or confirm clinical efficacy [37]. The emphasis for fMRI in these types of study is more likely to be on demonstrating normalization of a disease-related fMRI signal, at one or very few dose levels. Most such studies aspire for submission to regulators as part of a new drug approval. Consequently, these studies might include fMRI to provide a more-objective demonstration of disease modification, thereby increasing the evidence base for a regulatory submission.
How fMRI is viewed by regulatory agencies
The US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) and the European Medicines Agency (EMA) are regulatory agencies that approve the commercialization and human use of new drugs [38,39]. Approval is based on the review of evidence provided by drug sponsors on the safety and efficacy of the new drug in treating specific disease indications [40,41]. Both agencies have acknowledged that, for the process of drug development to continue to thrive, novel technologies that facilitate the drug development process have to be continuously developed [42–44]. This is why both agencies have proposed a formal process for the qualification of technologies, such as fMRI, for specific fit-for-purpose uses in drug development. Medical imaging tools have typically been considered as biomarkers in this context, that is, ‘a characteristic that is objectively measured and evaluated as an indicator of normal biological processes, pathogenic processes, or pharmacologic responses to a therapeutic intervention’. [45]. For the regulatory agencies, biomarkers could be used in drug development to: (i) more accurately define a disease (i.e., a diagnostic biomarker); (ii) stratify patients by disease severity (i.e., a prognostic biomarker); (iii) identify patients most likely to benefit from therapy (i.e., a predictive biomarkers); and (iv) monitor response to therapy (i.e., a PD biomarker) [46]. Interested parties can submit a request for qualification of a biomarker if they believe that there is a need that can be met by the biomarker in a specific context of use and enough data to support its use in that context. The agencies will review the application and issue either an opinion on whether they agree with the strength of the argument for the biomarker use and context, or advice about what might be additionally required to issue a qualification opinion. Once qualified for use in a pre-defined context, the agencies will accept biomarker data as evidence, within the context of use, for new drug safety and efficacy. For example, total kidney volume measured by imaging techniques has been proposed by the Polycystic Kidney Disease Outcomes Consortium as a prognostic biomarker for enrichment of clinical trials in autosomal dominant polycystic kidney disease and qualified by the FDA and EMA. However, change in kidney volume has not yet been qualified as a PD biomarker [47]. Molecular imaging biomarkers of the dopamine transporter are also being considered for qualification as enrichments biomarkers for clinical trials [48].
The burden of proof for qualification of a biomarker, such as fMRI, is high, and has not yet been fully standardized by the FDA and EMA. Typically, the agencies require data from more than a few small trials in which the biomarker has demonstrated value in the specified context of use. Also required are characterizations of the precision and reproducibility of the biomarker. Despite scientific interest in fMRI, there remain few industry-sponsored trials with sufficiently rigorous fMRI data for regulatory agencies to consider when reviewing an application for a new therapeutic. No requests have been made to qualify fMRI as a drug development tool. However, a consortium of academic institutes and industry managing ‘The European Autism Interventions – A Multicentre Study for Developing New Medications’ (EU-AIMS), a project funded and run under the Innovative Medicines Initiative (IMI), has requested advice and support for the qualification of several imaging biomarkers to be used to stratify populations of patients with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Among those biomarkers is fMRI using animated shapes theory of mind task and social and nonsocial reward anticipation task paradigms. The EMA is considering these fMRI biomarkers and has issued a letter of support to explore these biomarkers further [49].
To explain why biomarker qualifications of fMRI have been limited to date, below we review what is required for fMRI to demonstrate value and the current challenges that limit its ability to be useful in this setting.
Current state of fMRI as a tool for drug developers
The past 20 years of research on fMRI in drug development have clarified what technical and logistical requirements the technology must meet to be useful in a clinical trial setting, as well as what useful capabilities it is known to have. Here, we summarize these requirements and capabilities.
Requirements
fMRI readouts should be both reproducible and modifiable by the pharmacological agent
Ideally, fMRI readouts would be standardized and broadly accepted; this would facilitate comparison between studies conducted at different sites, by different sponsors, and with different molecules. As with any assay designed to assess an intervention, the reproducibility of an fMRI paradigm forms part of its initial characterization and validation as fit for purpose. In addition, the readouts should respond to pharmacological manipulation: a Phase 1 fMRI study, for example, would be expected to establish dose–response and exposure–response relationships between the selected readouts and the administered compound to inform dose selection for subsequent patient trials [32,35,50–52]. Both reproducibility and responsiveness are important: a paradigm that is highly reproducible but impervious to pharmacological manipulation will not be useful, for example. Ideally, evidence of pharmacological modulation should be presented with suitable comparator compounds [53,54].
Measurement characteristics of fMRI equipment need to be carefully established
To ensure the reliability, sensitivity, specificity, and accuracy of collected fMRI data, a quantitative, industry-standard method for assessing the fMRI measurement process is required. For structural MRI, the National Institute of Standards and Technology/International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine (NIST/ISMRM) system phantom provides such a method, including standardized MRI readouts to which scanners can be calibrated, as well as international standards for those readouts [55]. These readouts include contrast, resolution, and accuracy of distance and volume measurements in the image space. The NIST/RSNA Quantitative Imaging Biomarkers Alliance (QIBA) apparent diffusion coefficient (ADC) phantom similarly provides standardized readouts and international standards for diffusion MRI sequences [56]. Combining this approach with dynamic assessments, such as temporal signal-to-noise ratios (SNR), as recommended by the Functional Biomedical Informatics Research Network (fBIRN) [57], would be required to allow scanners worldwide to be quantitatively evaluated for their fMRI performance. The most widely used and validated techniques could not only be standardized, but also tied to a quantitative gold standard recognized by global regulatory bodies. This level of industry-standard measurement quantification is required for fMRI to be a viable technique in late-stage clinical trials, especially multisite ones.
fMRI acquisition and analysis should be prespecified before the study
The extent to which the acquisition, processing, and derivation of fMRI endpoints can be prespecified will impact its utility as a tool for clinical trials. For much of its history, fMRI research has emphasized mapping the linkage between brain activation and behavior, thus representing a signal detection problem [58]. Unfortunately, attempts to distinguish signal from noise are fraught with confounds because of the high dimensionality of fMRI data, leading to a high probability of type I errors. The likelihood of false-positives becomes even more dangerous when users do not understand the statistics underlying their empirical claims (Henson’s ‘imager’s fallacy’ [59]) or engage in circular selection (‘voodoo correlation’) [60] and non-independent analysis (‘double dipping’) [61]. In relation to this, an important concern of under-powered fMRI studies was first pointed out in the commentary to the ‘voodoo correlation’ paper [62] in the context of fMRI for human brain mapping. This was then later elaborated on in depth [63,64]. When a study is underpowered, the power (probability) to detect a true effect is low. This results in consequences, such as overestimation of the true effect (because only large observed effects pass the P value threshold). This is also referred to also as the ‘winner’s curse’. Subsequently, a low reproducibility of follow-up studies ensues, because they find evidence of smaller or no effects, thus failing to reproduce findings of the initial study. As the field has evolved, more emphasis has been placed on a clear reporting of methodology, including experimental design, correction for multiple comparisons, return on investment (ROI) definition, and the statistical tests performed, ideally with sufficient detail to allow replication of the analysis [65,66]. Yet, even the most clearly reported post-hoc analysis is insufficient to inform a clinical trial where experimental power and reliability must be estimated in advance, with implications for study design and sample size [67]. In the context of drug development, fMRI methodology should be held to the same standard as other clinical endpoints, namely, methods must be prespecified and fixed for the duration of the study. This prespecification should include a thorough description of task design and implementation, image acquisition and quality control, data preprocessing, ROI definition, model estimation, and endpoint calculation [3,4]. With such prescribed methodology in hand, an fMRI experiment can be reduced to a binary outcome more suitable to inform drug development decisions. In drug development, primary endpoints and hypotheses are typically based on prespecified ROIs [68,69] and power calculations are performed accordingly to avoid underpowering.
For later-phase human trials, heterogeneous multiple-site implementation must be feasible
Significant logistical requirements must be met when deploying fMRI to large numbers of heterogeneous, nonacademic imaging facilities, as is required for typical late-stage clinical trials. The Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) [70], the IMAGEN study [71], the Human Connectome Project [72,73], and the Function Biomedical Informatics Research Network (fBIRN) [74] have advanced the state of the art in techniques for meeting these requirements. For multicenter fMRI at the level of dozens to hundreds of imaging facilities, methods must be ‘turnkey’ and able to accommodate all major manufacturers, models, and even field strengths. High-performance MRI scanners are not always located near subject recruitment centers, necessitating pragmatic decisions in the planning phase of a multicenter fMRI study. The most basic decision is whether fMRI acquisition can involve 1.5 T or 3 T systems. If acquisition must occur at multiple field strengths to facilitate recruitment, analytic endpoints emphasizing within-subject designs are most amenable, although ways to account for variance across magnets exist [75,76]. The decision to include 1.5 T systems also has implications for acquisition parameters and how these are standardized across sites. Clinical imaging facilities are so diverse in their equipment (vendor, model, software release, coil, and gradient configuration) and technologist experience level that complete standardization is not feasible. Instead, parameters most likely to impact endpoint derivation should be identified and fixed across sites, while other less-critical settings can be allowed to ‘float’. Thus, protocol optimization involves achieving certain fixed parameters and adjusting others to maximize performance at that site. At a minimum, factors that should be consistent across sites are the type of pulse sequence, in-plane voxel size, slice thickness and spacing, temporal resolution (TR) and number of observations, flip angle, coverage, and behavioral conditions. Suggestions for this parameter set, and the impact of different choices, have been proposed elsewhere [77]. A field map (including reconstruction of both magnitude and phase information) should be acquired to allow for distortion correction of the echo planar imaging (EPI) time course [78]. Multichannel coils are preferred for the increased signal: noise ratio they provide [79]. Fat suppression and parallel imaging should be used if available to increase tissue contrast and reduce acquisition times [80,81].
In our experience, conditions allowing the assessment of whole-brain functional connectivity are achievable at most scanners encountered at clinical imaging facilities worldwide. Indeed, published data suggest that carefully controlled, prespecified, auditable task-free fMRI studies can yield a rich connectome amenable to informing drug development, once logistical hurdles are overcome [82]. However, because of broad diversity across sites, leading-edge acquisition techniques, such as multiband fMRI [83], are not presently realistic in this setting, and neither are hardware peripherals needed for task-based fMRI typically available.
Technologists must be proficient in fMRI acquisition
A standardized imaging protocol will only perform well in the hands of a well-trained technologist. Although the technologist is the person most directly involved in acquiring the data, surprisingly little attention is given to the conduct and verification of their training for clinical trial protocols. Although most clinical-imaging personnel have never encountered fMRI before, they can become proficient in its proper execution through a combination of detailed reference materials, training via the phone or web, and test scans on a phantom or healthy volunteer. The technologist must be consistent in setting up the room, positioning and instructing the subject, and placing the field of view. Perhaps most important, they must be equipped to intervene when data quality is substandard, for example, detecting excessive head motion and reimmobilizing the subject for a repeat scan. Finally, special procedures need to be in place for the handling and transfer of an fMRI time series, which can run to several thousand digital imaging and communications in medicine (DICOM) files on some systems [3].
Rigorous quality-control procedures must be in place
A rigorous quality-control (QC) procedure must be in place to ensure that the site acquires analyzable data for the duration of the trial. Errors left undetected quickly become systematic and could render worthless subjects from an entire site. This QC need not only detect errors at the site, but also must be tracked and fully auditable, especially in the context of regulatory approval [3]. Again, suggestions for what such a QC process should entail have been made [84], but at minimum should include DICOM header checks of protocol compliance, tests of dynamic range and temporal SNR, artifact inspection, adequacy of field of view (FOV) placement, and head motion (now commonly reduced to a single vector, framewise displacement [85]).
Current capabilities
Drugs can induce acute and longer-term changes in fMRI signals
A large number of published studies have demonstrated that a range of fMRI methods and paradigms are sensitive to changes following both acute (i.e., after a single dose) or chronic (i.e., multiple dose) pharmacological treatment. Most of these have been academic studies using marketed drugs whose efficacy and effective doses have already been established. Several studies have shown that the fMRI response of the amygdala to photographs of faces with negative affect is increased in patients with depression, and that antidepressant drugs can normalize this response at clinically effective doses [86,87]. Intravenous challenge with the NMDA receptor antagonist ketamine, used as a model of glutamatergic dysfunction in psychiatric disorders, elicits a widespread fMRI response [88–94]. The ketamine response can be blocked by antiglutamatergic compounds [88,89] and can reverse the phMRI signal evoked by some (but not all) antipsychotic agents and compounds designed to attenuate glutamate release [95,96]. Additional drug classes that induce phMRI signals include analgesics [97–101], antipsychotics [102], cognitive enhancers [103], drugs of abuse [104,105], calcium channel blockers [106,107], cyclooxygenase-2 (COX-2) inhibitors [108], muscarinic acetylcholine receptor modulators [109–111], and therapies traditionally thought to impact solely immune system activity [112]. In addition to traditional phMRI studies, pharmacological modulation of functional connectivity in animal models and humans has also been reported [113–118].
In many of these cases, phMRI signals can be hypothesized based on a priori knowledge of CNS target expression derived from other data sources, such as PET [119–122]. The identified drug-associated phMRI signals likely include both direct (‘on-target’) and indirect (‘downstream’) effects. In this context, ‘on-target’ refers to changes in brain activity directly related to and colocalised with the site of the pharmacological action of the compound, whereas ‘downstream’ effects include activation of connected brain circuits and structures that might not have a high density of the pharmacological target but whose activity is altered by administration of the compound. Both of these contributions to the signal can have implications for efficacy and safety [123].
For example, pain research exemplifies how fMRI techniques can be utilized to not only phenotype patients, but also to determine whether a PD effect is present within specific disease pathways. MRI has been pivotal to identifying the network of brain regions modulated during acute, nociceptive processing of evoked sensory stimuli (e.g., thermal or mechanical stimuli) and how parameters that define the stimulus (e.g., magnitude, duration, frequency, or mode) are represented in the brain [124]. For example, it is well known that as the intensity [125] or frequency [126] of thermal stimuli increases, the level of potentiation increases in spinal cord, brainstem, subcortical, and cortical structures. It is also well known that stimulus-dependent potentiation, as well as attenuation, occurs in CNS structures and circuits mediating not only sensory aspects of pain, but also affective and motivational ones [127,128]. It can be argued that the nonsensory components of pain are just as crucial as the sensory aspects of pain in driving the overall experience of pain and lowering quality of life [129]. In fact, in imaging studies of patients with chronic pain, the function of regions such as the nucleus accumbens [130], insula [131,132], and cingulate [133] have been shown to correlate with the level of reported clinical pain or disease-specific pain (e.g., knee movement in patients with knee osteoarthritis).
An additional example is fMRI of opioid antagonists combined with receptor occupancy (RO)-PET. The complementary value of fMRI when combined with RO-PET, providing central PD in addition to target engagement data, was illustrated in a combined RO-PET and fMRI study of novel (GSK1521498) and comparator (naltrexone) M-opioid antagonists [134], compounds that had been studied as potential therapeutics in disorders of compulsive consumption [135–137]. The fMRI paradigm was designed to test the hypothesis that these compounds attenuate the functional brain response to a palatable gustatory stimulus. Following M-opioid RO using [11C]-carfentanil PET, revealing dose-dependent receptor occupancy in the ventral striatum, GSK1521498 attenuated the BOLD responses in the amygdala and nucleus accumbens more strongly than did naltrexone. Given that both modalities were acquired from the same patients, the fMRI analysis could explicitly take account of RO within the same patient.
fMRI can identify converging mechanisms across drugs
Pain provides a clear case in which fMRI not only elucidates dysfunction among common and disparate CNS structures across diverse clinical conditions, but also overlapping PD effects upon aberrant fMRI signals [138]. In particular, in fibromyalgia [139], osteoarthritis pain [140], complex regional pain syndrome [141], and models of neuropathic pain [142], abnormal insula and cingulate cortex activity has been reported as being suppressed through a variety of pharmacological strategies. As importantly demonstrated by Harris et al., functional neuroimaging endpoints derived from the insula and inferior parietal lobe predicted pregabalin treatment response towards experimentally evoked or clinical pain states [143]. In patients with hand osteoarthritis, the change in visual analog scale (VAS) pain levels quantified between placebo and naproxen treatments and reported during evoked hand pain correlated with the change in thalamic and somatosensory cortex BOLD responses. Nonetheless, the causal or correlational relationship between fMRI endpoints and subjective accounts of pain is not ubiquitous within the pain neuroimaging literature. For instance, Flodin et al. did not find that functional connectivity changes observed in patients with rheumatoid arthritis (RA) correlated with clinical endpoints [i.e., global VAS, disease activity score (DAS)-28, or RA duration], an observation that might have resulted as a consequence of enhanced head movement within the patient cohort or inadequate powering of the study [144].
Functional MRI findings in humans [97] and animals [145] clearly indicate that multivariate fMRI patterns are shared across compounds that are effective for the same indication (e.g., acute pain) but nonetheless differ wildly in mechanism (e.g., opioids, nonsteroidal antiflammatories, and even tetrahydrocannabinol). However, a mechanism without direct access to the CNS might not be necessary to impact brain function and yield analgesia; yet, detecting a PD effect in the CNS could represent an early and important biomarker suggestive of analgesic efficacy. For example, in arthritic and rheumatic conditions, peripherally acting therapies result in clinically meaningful analgesia but also induce functional changes in CNS structures mediating sensory, affective, and motivational aspect of pain [108,146,147]. A recent review exemplifying the utility and challenges of fMRI in measuring CNS function in diseases harboring clinical pain in conjunction with analagous evaluation in corresponding preclinical pain models is provided elsewhere [148].
fMRI can support translation between clinical and preclinical studies
In drug development, the clinical biomarker plan for a candidate therapeutic is formulated while the compound is still being optimized in preclinical testing (the discovery phase). It is advantageous to be able to demonstrate an effect of the compound in preclinical species (typically rodents) on the same or similar biomarker as that being considered for use in the clinical phases. Recent studies have addressed the degree to which fMRI responses and their pharmacological modulation are consistent across species [149]. For example, acute intravenous injection of the NMDAR antagonist phencyclidine (PCP) elicits a strong phMRI response pattern in the rat brain involving the prefrontal and cingulate cortices and the thalamus and, to a lesser extent, the hippocampus [96]. This aligns well with the pattern observed in rat 2-deoxy-glucose (2DG) studies in response to ketamine [150]. A similar activation pattern was observed in healthy human volunteers given ketamine [88,89,151]. In a second example, the phMRI response to intravenous buprenorphine was concordant in many regions in rats and healthy humans [120], although deactivation of some regions was noted in rats but not in humans.
Only recently has rsfMRI been convincingly back-translated to rats and mice. Several groups identified functional networks in the rodent brain that align with known anatomical connectivity and are similar to analogous networks in the human brain [152–154]. These include a hippocampal–prefrontal system considered to be the rodent analog of the default mode network (DMN) [155–158], a frontolateral network (anatomically similar to the human ‘task-positive’ network and anticorrelated to the DMN [159]), and bilateral sensory and subcortical systems [160]. Emerging data have begun to demonstrate consistent pharmacological modulation of resting-state networks across species [116].
Challenges
In addition to the requirements of the drug development process and the capabilities of fMRI in this setting, there is a set of challenges that must be addressed to increase the utility of fMRI in clinical trials. The questions include technical ones about how fMRI studies should be performed, as well as biological ones about the inferences that should be made about the functioning of the brain based on fMRI data.
Technical challenges
Optimal methods for acquisition and post processing of fMRI data are not established
Each fMRI experiment includes a large space of design parameters, both at the acquisition and postprocessing stages. For acquisition, Inglis [161] provides a list of dozens of parameters that must be set, broken down into those that should be required to be reported in publications (Table 1 in Ref. [161]). Postprocessing steps generally include realignment, slice-timing correction, co-registration, normalization, segmentation, and smoothing [162]. Additional steps include regressing out motion and physiological artifacts. Each postprocessing step is implemented differently in different software packages, each with their own sets of operating parameters. Guidance on how exactly to set these operating parameters to optimally detect fMRI signals of interest or to enhance intra- or intersubject reliability is sparse and incomplete, with some noteworthy exceptions. For example, Poldrack et al. provide guidelines for task-related fMRI experimental design, preprocessing, and statistical modeling, including how many experimental sessions and volumes per session should be acquired [66]. Van Dijk et al. [77] evaluated a variety of acquisition and postprocessing parameters for rsfMRI to determine whether settings of these parameters made any appreciable difference to the detection of DMNs and attention networks, against a reference network. Carp [163] evaluated 6912 unique analysis pipelines for a single event-related fMRI experiment, reporting substantial variability across pipelines in terms of BOLD signal strength, localization, and spatial extent. These reviews and studies necessarily only provide partial guidance about the large fMRI design space and suggest that greater knowledge of optimal methods for acquisition and processing of fMRI remains a major research need.
A larger problem than how to set specific operating parameters within existing processing paradigms is whether the paradigms themselves are optimal. The general linear model for modeling fMRI data, for example, is at the heart of most major data-processing software but has limitations, and alternatives have been proposed [164]. In addition, the spatially invariant hemodynamic response function is predominant, despite longstanding evidence that hemodynamic response characteristics vary by location [165]. Given that alternatives to these dominant processing paradigms are not implemented in broadly deployed software, it is not clear in what situations they provide superior results to more-conventional paradigms.
Optimal summary measures from postprocessed fMRI data are not established
Although image-based representations of group-level analyses are commonly seen as the primary fMRI output of interest in a research setting, predefined numeric summary values from each scan are needed to use fMRI as a biomarker in drug development studies. There are several motivations for summary values: (i) the drastically lower dimensionality allows results of the fMRI experiment to be imported into standard databases, combined with other data, such as PK, and analyzed by accredited statisticians. These databases are auditable and operate under strict revision and access control, safeguards that assure a high level of data integrity; (ii) defining summary readouts as primary endpoints before conducting the study requires the practitioner to prescribe specific and simple a priori hypotheses, an exercise that can highlight uncertainty in the anticipated outcome and prevent false positive rate inflation [166]; (iii) summary values can reduce the multiple comparison burden and, thus, avoid ill-defined choices among correction schemes; and (iv) endpoints extracted from voxelwise analyses can be subject to bias because of circularity [61], and determining the neuroanatomical localization of an effect in a set of voxels can be cumbersome or ill-defined.
However, although there is a clear need for low-dimensional fMRI summary measures, there is no widespread agreement on optimal approaches. Graph-theoretical summaries [167] and factor analytic methods, such as independent components analysis (ICA) [168], are used extensively in exploratory research studies, but these methods have many variants and operating parameters. The more-traditional approach of selecting a ROI and reducing all signals in a region to a single summary score [169] requires a choice of summary score (e.g., the mean, median, or mode). The optimal summary measure for any given clinical trial is unclear and can depend on the hypothesized action of the drug. If the treatment is hypothesized to strongly affect a single brain region, an ROI-based analysis might be effective, but if effective correlations among regions are hypothesized to be modified, a graph theoretical approach might be preferred. If the hypothesized effects cover a broad network of regions, a seed-based or ICA approach might capture the hypothesized effect. One reason for the lack of broad agreement on standardized summary measures for fMRI is that the range of task paradigms, pharmacological mechanisms, and imaging sites involved is large, and few standardization studies have been published.
Repeatability of fMRI paradigms is under-reported
One key reason for a lack of agreement on optimal fMRI methods is that rigorous validation studies, such as test–retest experiments, are rarely published. Intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) is by far the most-frequently used metric for quantifying test–retest reliability in the few fMRI studies that have been published [67,170–173]. The ranges of the ICC for fMRI signals evoked by robust visual stimulus in primary visual cortex vary from fair to high (0.4–0.8) [172]. Fair–high reliability was also found for strong painful stimuli (ICC = 0.5–0.875) [125]. A cognitive emotive test battery [67] comprising faces, reward, and n-Back tasks showed fair–good reliability for the reward task (ICC = 0.56–0.62) and, depending on the ROI, also fair–good reliability for the n-back task (ICC = 0.44–0.57). For the faces task, the reliability was low (0.0–0.16). For phMRI using ketamine [151], ICCs were high for ROIs that were expected to respond to ketamine and ranged from fair to high across the brain (ICC = 0.30–0.73). Similar findings were also reported for the reliability of rsfMRI (ICC = 0.5–0.67) [171,173]. Similarly to stimulus-driven fMRI, there is a consistent association between greater strength of resting-state functional connectivity and greater reliability. However, group-level reproducibility is usually good [174]. Paradigms with low within-subject reproducibility are unsuitable for within-subject (crossover) study designs but might be useful in parallel-group designs. For example, the same faces task that showed poor within-subject reproducibility [67] has been widely used in parallel-group designs, reproducibly showing exaggerated amygdala responses to negative emotional stimuli in conditions such as depression, and pharmacological attenuation of this response following drug treatment [86]. These studies have investigated the repeatability of a very small segment of the large fMRI design space, and there is much room for additional validation studies. Repeatability and/or reliability estimation is an integral part of proof-of-concept (POC) fMRI studies in drug development. Given that the main driver of these studies is the investigation of the modulation of the fMRI signal by an administered drug or placebo, the effect size of this intervention is of most interest. The repeatability and/or reliability indices, such as ICC, inform within-subject variability for within-subject designs (e.g. crossover). There are usually no predefined criteria for minimum ICC or other measures of repeatability, because, even in the case of high within-subject variability, the effect might be strong (or overwhelming) and fMRI biomarkers might be informative. Conversely, high reliability does not warrant necessarily sensitivity to the intervention, which would render the biomarker useless for its particular purpose. However, when the estimate of ICC is very low (e.g., <0.3), additional investigation is warranted to determine the causes of the low reliability of the experiment.
When repeatability is reported, it is incompletely characterized
ICC belongs to a class of scaled indices of reliability, defined as a ratio of variance components derived from a mixed effect model (in the simplest case, a random effects analysis of variance, ANOVA). Thus, ICC simultaneously reflects within- and between-subject variability. Therefore, when ICC is used for comparisons concerning agreement, there is an implicit assumption of equal between-subject variability. Carrasco et al. [175] pointed out that, for the evaluation of reliability, within-subject variability is essential. Given that ICC incorporates both within- and between-subject variability, it reflects not only agreement, but also distinguishability between subjects. As a consequence, although ICC is by far the predominantly reported measure of repeatability, it can provide different answers about concordance than can indices based solely on within-subject variability. Therefore, authors are recommended [175] to report, in addition to ICC, agreement indices that reflect only within-subject variability, such as within-subject standard deviation, total deviation index, repeatability coefficient, or within-subject coefficient of variation. Importantly, reporting within-subject standard deviation (with corresponding 95% confidence limits) obtained from a test–retest study for a particular ROI can be used to inform future studies based on within-subject designs [76]. When reporting the ICC, it is desirable to refer to a particular underlying statistical model, such as ANOVA, and the type of ICC which results from it. For example, one-way ANOVA results in the ICC referred to as ICC 1.1 in Ref. [170], whereas two-way ANOVA results in ICC 2.1 [170].
It is unclear how to optimally utilize simultaneous multislice acquisitions
New developments in MRI pulse sequences and gradient hardware now enable higher-resolution fMRI, most notably through simultaneous multislice (SMS) or ‘multiband’ and multiplexed excitations [176]. There is limited information on the precise benefits conveyed by these techniques in vivo. For instance, when SMS is used to achieve greater TR (e.g., [177]), it will add degrees of freedom to model-based analysis and could thereby improve statistical power (e.g., [177]). Higher TR can also enable better characterization of cardiac and respiratory effects in the data [178,179]. The Ernst angles at the shorter TRs mean that smaller flip angles are used. Although this reduces SAR, it also reduces the signal-to-noise ratio of the individual images. When SMS is used to achieve greater spatial resolution (e.g., [180]), it can improve sensitivity by reducing partial volume or susceptibility effects. Alternatively, SMS can be used in combination with multiecho sequences, for example to maintain a 3-mm isotropic/2 s TR spatiotemporal resolution with three or more echo times [177,181]. The peer-reviewed literature thus far lacks a clear empirical evaluation of most of these tradeoffs in vivo.
It is unclear what role low-level sensorimotor paradigms should have in conditions that are not primary sensorimotor deficits
Low-level sensorimotor tasks can have an important role in clinical trials, even for clinical conditions that are not characterized by primary sensory or motor deficits. For example, in diseases of cerebrovascular dysfunction, analysis of the characteristics of the hemodynamic response to a simple sensorimotor task can provide valuable information. This approach takes advantage of the relatively high reproducibility of the BOLD signals generated by sensorimotor tasks [182], as well as the relative ease with which subjects are able to comply with the task. Such paradigms have been used to explore effects of an analgesic on brain activity during visual stimulation [183]. In addition, a disease that is believed to be associated with regional modulation of cerebrovascular function can be matched with a sensorimotor task that reliably activates the same region, enabling localized, non-invasive assessment of vascular function. One example of this application of sensorimotor fMRI was reported in subjects with cerebral amyloid angiopathy (CAA) [184]. CAA is characterized by deposition of amyloid in the walls of leptomeningeal and cerebrocortical vessels [185] and has a predilection to the occipital lobe [186]. A visual stimulus and transcranial Doppler ultrasound had been used previously in patients with CAA to demonstrate a reduced reactivity in the posterior cerebral artery [187]. Dumas et al. used a similar flashing checkerboard stimulus to induce a robust BOLD response in the occipital lobe. They demonstrated that patients with CAA, compared with healthy age-matched controls, exhibited both a reduced BOLD amplitude and increased time to peak BOLD response in the visual cortex, consistent with impaired cerebrovascular reactivity measures shown in both patients with [187], and animal models of, CAA [188]. Taken together, the evidence supporting this functional imaging biomarker as an indication of underlying cerebrovascular pathology of this sort is strong. However, the full breadth of application of low-level sensorimotor paradigms to neurological diseases has not yet been fully explored.
The value added by 7T is unclear
Since the inception of fMRI, there has been a steady push to acquire human data at ever-higher field strengths because of the significant theoretical increases in sensitivity that higher field strengths afford [189–191]. These benefits can facilitate higher spatial and/or TR than by using lower strength magnets [191–193]. Although a large bulk of early human studies were performed at 1.5T, over the past 10 years, 3T studies have been the norm, and large, multisite clinical trials performed entirely at 3T sites are now feasible. Scanners with 7T field strength are now available at dozens of early-adopting research sites, and there are aspirations to roll out 9.4T scanners as a next step [194–196]. However, the practical real-world advantages of 7T over 3T, in a realistic clinical trial setting, have not yet been determined. In particular, increased magnetic field strength creates increases in artifact levels, scaling of physiologic noise with field strength, and a dependence of effects on voxel size [197]; it is not clear whether these downsides (in addition to the high cost of 7T devices [192]) overwhelm the empirical increases in sensitivity achieved by the higher field strength. To date, one study compared 3T with 7T in task-related fMRI, revealing robust activations in the bilateral medial temporal lobe during associative memory encoding at both field strengths but significantly stronger memory-related hippocampal activation at 7T, suggesting higher empirical sensitivity at 7T [198]. However, comparative studies that would inform decisions about eventual movement toward clinical trials at 7T are in their infancy.
fMRI time-series are rarely shown in clinical studies
In preclinical experiments, the fMRI time-series from pertinent ROIs are routinely displayed [199]. This is also desirable for clinical fMRI studies because it enhances confidence in the measured signal. Recent examples of clinical studies where the time-series have been displayed include [151,200]. In the study by de Simoni et al. [151], strong repeatable phMRI signals induced by ketamine were clearly demonstrated. In applications of fMRI of pain, exploratory analysis of temporal shapes of the noxious versus innocuous stimuli provided insights into improved modeling of the fMRI signal [73,200]. However, while showing time-series used to be common (e.g., [201]), clinical fMRI time-series are now rarely shown in publications, possibly because the lower field strengths used in clinical studies give rise to noisier time-series than their higher-field preclinical counterparts. Nonetheless, showing such time-series would strengthen confidence in clinical fMRI study results.
Open biological questions
What is the set of molecular drivers of fMRI signal changes?
PhMRI studies often ascribe treatment-related BOLD signal changes to changes in the functioning of neurons. However, such inferences must necessarily be physiologically agnostic because local changes in the BOLD signal are determined by at least four physiological effects, namely changes in the cerebral blood volume (CBV), cerebral blood flow (CBF), the cerebral metabolic rate of oxygen metabolism (CMRO2), as well as glucose metabolism [202]. Past research shows an undeniable association between cerebral hemodynamics and activity of neuronal populations, although exact relationships are still a matter of debate [203–207]. There is a consensus that graded increases in neuronal metabolism result in graded increases in the BOLD response, whereas corresponding decreases in neuronal metabolism result in negative BOLD responses [208–211]. However, a precise understanding of the relative contributions of metabolic, vascular, and neural processes to the BOLD signal is still under development. For some pharmacological treatments, such a lack of physiological specificity could be a major limitation to the ability to make development decisions based on phMRI results. For example, investigators might have a priori hypotheses about which of the physiological changes are plausible for the treatment to elicit, based on earlier preclinical experiments; separate measurement of separate physiological effects could provide an important indicator of agreement with prior experiments. In addition, effects on specific physiological parameters might be seen as advantageous by developers. Emerging approaches could help to identify distinct physiological changes induced by the treatment. So-called ‘calibrated’ BOLD, a T2*-based method, collects BOLD data during a controlled CO2 challenge to ‘calibrate’ the BOLD signal; together with simultaneous BOLD-ASL imaging sequences, this allows simultaneous estimation of CBF and CMRO2 [212–215]. Another method, a T2* and T2-based method known as ‘quantitative BOLD’ or ‘qBOLD’ [216], provides similar information but from a single scan that requires no hypercapnia challenge. T2-based methods, such as TRUST [217] and QUIXOTIC [218], are also promising [219]. The collection of BOLD and fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) PET data simultaneously, with FDG PET providing the measurement of tissue metabolic properties, is also emerging [220]. Although these methods are promising, no systematic study has evaluated which ones add significant value to specific clinical trial designs or classes of compound.
In addition to fundamental questions about the biological drivers of the BOLD signal, the more-practical question of how to best remove effects of systemic physiological processes (including respiration and cardiac function) from the BOLD signal, and even whether to do so at all when assessing drug effects, remains understudied [221,222]. In addition, any extrinsic action that might change neuronal activity, blood volume, or perfusion is a potential confound of drug effects on fMRI. These include use of concomitant psychoactive drugs or other drugs that affect perfusion, as well as any activities that might affect arousal, cognitive state, and blood flow, such as caffeine intake, sleep, and exercise [223]. However, it is unclear how aggressively each of these factors should be controlled to minimize confounding effects on fMRI data from clinical trials.
What is the full range of homologies between rodent and human fMRI findings?
As described above, similarities in pharmacological modulation of fMRI readouts have been observed between rodents and humans in select clinical domains and treatments. Unfortunately, the full set of such similarities is not known, and is difficult to predict because of the many structural differences between the rodent and human brain [224,225]. Cortical areas are substantially more differentiated and occupy a greater fraction of the total brain volume in humans, and structural homologies for some human brain regions are only beginning to be identified in rats [226,227]. Moreover, it is known that certain human therapeutic targets (e.g., NK1, muscarinic acetylcholine receptor M4) have different characteristics in rodent species [228,229]. A thorough and systematic characterization of similarities and differences in pharmacological effects in rodents and humans across a range of treatments would be critical to delineate the extent to which responses generalize across species and which fMRI readouts are most sensitive.
How can compounds with slow PK be assessed?
The phMRI technique centers on the detection of a direct, pharmacologically elicited hemodynamic response in the brain. Most often, a test agent is administered partway through fMRI scanning, allowing the temporal response to the agent to be quantified (e.g., [50,53,230–233]). This approach works well for compounds whose PK and route of administration (e.g., intravenous) leads to a rapid, easily detectable signal change. However, neuroscience compounds entering clinical drug development are most frequently formulated for oral administration and/or would have extended PK profiles. For such compounds, the length of the required phMRI scan would be impractical.
Using perfusion MRI to measure CBF has been proposed as a solution to this problem [234]. Unlike traditional phMRI, each perfusion scan yields quantified maps of CBF in absolute physiological units, thus allowing CBF measures to be compared between scans taken on different hours or days [235]. Some studies have demonstrated that this approach can be used to detect direct effects of orally administered pharmaceuticals on the brain [236–243] as well as yield overlapping, yet slightly different spatial patterns compared with BOLD fMRI [244,245]. However, the sensitivity of this contrast mechanism might be lower than that of BOLD [223]. Moreover, there is some evidence that a same-day baseline scan could be required to increase sensitivity to drug effects, compared with simply comparing CBF maps from the same subject on different days (under different treatment conditions) [241]. The relative ease of acquisition of a resting perfusion scan is a strong practical advantage. However, currently, perfusion scans are not widely collected in a clinical trial setting, and requisite high-density head coils might not be available at all clinical trial sites. Whether the potential advantages of perfusion MRI over traditional fMRI outweigh its costs and disadvantages in a clinical trial setting remains an open question.
Another approach to dealing with slow PK is to assess differences in inter-regional functional connectivity before and after pharmacological challenge (e.g., [236–238]). Similar to perfusion MRI, this approach does not require a continuous time-series of imaging data to be acquired that includes both pre- and post-challenge data. However, the readouts from functional connectivity analysis are more complex than in traditional phMRI, making interpretation of results more challenging.
Agonist–antagonist fMRI designs are yet another approach. In these designs, a ‘probe’ compound (e.g., ketamine) is administered intravenously and elicits a strong phMRI response [53,54]. The compounds of interest are administered beforehand, such that the phMRI scan coincides with the time of high exposure to the test article, allowing any modulatory effect on the probe signal (e.g., reversal) to be examined.
How can fMRI be used to inform dosing?
fMRI has relatively underexplored potential in early-phase drug discovery for informing dosing regimens. In principle, a consistent relationship between dose and the BOLD response could enable predictions about the expected brain response at higher or lower doses. However, there are few examples of the use of this kind of pharmacometric fMRI modeling in the literature. For example, it is not clear whether it is more effective to model fMRI signals as a nonlinear function of plasma concentration of a compound (a pharmacokinetic approach; e.g., [246], or as a mediator of the association between plasma concentration and PD effects [246–248]. Future work should evaluate whether important dose-related fMRI effects are present not only in the magnitude of the fMRI response, but also in its temporal derivates and their dependency on spatial location, as suggested by early work on biphasic responses to dopamine and GABA manipulations [249–251]. Addressing such complexity with computationally tractable models is a continuing challenge [252].
How to bridge the fMRI ‘inference gap’ is unclear
Even if a plausible relationship between treatment dose and BOLD response can be obtained, a continuing challenge is the ‘inference gap’: the uncertain relationship between the magnitude of a pharmacological effect seen in fMRI and probable clinical efficacy. In comparison, RO PET imaging has matured to such a degree that goal occupancy ranges for some targets and molecular modes of action are established or can be estimated from animal studies. For example, striatal D2 dopamine RO of at least ~60% is associated with effective antipsychotic activity but occupancy greater than ~80% elicits adverse effects [32]. This goal range would allow a company to confidently test a clinical hypothesis about the benefit of a treatment. The classic example of this is the neurokinin 1 (NK1) antagonist class of compounds, which turned out to be clinically ineffective in affective and pain indications, despite using doses that were known, based on RO-PET studies, to yield near 100% occupancy [123]. Without the occupancy data, it would not be possible to be sure whether the doses chosen were high enough to fully engage the target. The ability to plausibly bridge this inference gap would enhance the value of fMRI for early-phase drug development. The published literature contains numerous reports of significant pharmacological effects in fMRI studies for a range of marketed compounds, at a single clinically effective dose, and studies with more than one dose are becoming more common [33,253]. Combining RO PET with fMRI can help link target engagement (RO-PET) and PD effect (fMRI) explicitly [254]. However, the ability to interpret the dose–response curve and relate it to probable clinical efficacy, especially for novel therapeutic targets, remains a challenge.
What role should fMRI have in subject selection, stratification, and enrichment?
Most uses of fMRI in Phase 3 clinical trials have centered on characterizing treatment effects. However, fMRI has the potential to identify subjects that should be enrolled in one arm of a trial or another. An example of this type of approach comes from obesity research, in which fMRI can identify obese individuals whose brains are hyper-responsive versus weakly responsive to images of food cues; the hyper-responsive individuals go on to respond more poorly to weight-loss treatment, assumedly because of a poorer ability to maintain low food intake [255]. In clinical trials with weight loss as a primary outcome, the hyperactivators might be at higher risk of nonresponse and, thus, are stratified to a higher dose regimen. Alternatively, clinical trials with fMRI change as a primary outcome might enrich the sample for such hyperactivators, because these are the individuals most able to show a response to treatment: their fMRI activation is the farthest from normal and has the most room to normalize. Whether fMRI should have such a subject-selection role in clinical trials is currently understudied. To determine whether fMRI should have this role in any disease setting, important questions need to be addressed, including how to reduce fMRI signals down to a single number used to make subject selection decisions, and how well the fMRI characteristics predict response to treatment.
Proposed activities
Previous sections have made it clear that major obstacles must be overcome to enable the broader use of fMRI in clinical trials. For several of these obstacles, specific research and development projects that could help to overcome them are obvious. However, in addition, community-wide activities could further enhance the utility of fMRI in the drug development process. Each of these activities are centered on the ultimate goal of providing fMRI tools that are sensitive to drug-induced change, relative to repeatability; valid with respect to established clinical endpoints; and standardized across measurement platforms.
Form public–private partnerships to fund enhancement of novel technologies and enable replication and validation
There is a general consensus that the following activities would enhance the utilization of fMRI in the drug development process: soliciting and reanalyzing data from trials with null findings; replicating findings from prior trials using differing treatments or imaging methods; enhancing the usability of research-grade fMRI processing software to make it easily adopted by the research community; and publishing well-designed and executed data sets to serve as publicly available gold standards for the validation of novel imaging methods. However, each of these activities is difficult to receive funding for, from both funding agencies and industry sponsors. For example, despite isolated funding programs to enhance the usability of already-developed neuroimaging software [e.g., the Neuroimaging Informatics Tools and Resources Clearinghouse (NITRC) program, www.nitrc.org], the US National Institutes of Health (NIH) overwhelmingly focuses on providing funds for the development of novel neuroimaging techniques rather than for the maintenance and further enhancement of successful ones. Meanwhile, individual drug companies are primarily focused on the development of their own treatments rather than of specific research tools (such as fMRI) that are used as part of the development pathway.
Given that no specific entity makes a systematic effort to fund these activities, promising new fMRI techniques ‘die on the vine,’ novel studies recreate the mistakes or null findings of unpublished prior studies, and the viability of treatments remain unclear because of a lack of validation. Funding for such activities likely requires new public–private partnerships, including entities that jointly recognize that such activities have the potential to enhance the utility of fMRI in all clinical trials, including those sponsored by industry and funding agencies. Software partnerships could follow the NITRC model, funding software developers to make emerging neuroimaging technologies available on additional computing platforms and able to interface with additional imaging data types and software systems. Such partnerships would need effective means to disseminate the resulting software and track the success of dissemination. Replication and validation partnerships could focus on identifying the most-promising fMRI methods and findings, and on funding their replication using complementary measurement techniques or model systems. The end result of these partnerships would be a broader set of validated software tools and greater knowledge of the generalizability of findings from one treatment to another.
Develop infrastructure for sharing clinical trial data without exposing sponsors or CROs to significant risks
The drug industry invests major resources in clinical trials that include fMRI, and there are numerous ways in which additional value could be extracted from that investment after primary analyses are completed. Subsequent analyses could be used to power novel studies, to understand the findings of similarly conducted studies, and to evaluate novel fMRI methods in general. However, any trial sponsor that makes such fMRI data public faces significant risks. A biased party could reanalyze the data to support a spurious claim about the trial. Dissemination of participant data also poses risks for confidentiality. Thus, there is a need to develop whatever infrastructure is needed to enable as much clinical trial data sharing as is possible. Such data sharing could begin with FDA and EMA policies that require that trial operators submit all collected fMRI data to these regulatory bodies or an intermediary as a precondition to registering the trial. The data could be deidentified centrally, and various data characteristics could be provided only in the most-general terms to avoid identification of the participant and trial. Regulatory bodies could, as a starting point, only release fMRI data from placebo arms to accelerate testing of methods for assessing longitudinal fMRI change. Importantly, informed consent forms would need to ensure that participants understand the implications of their consent to such data sharing.
Establish an ongoing, regular conference on fMRI in clinical trials
The previous sections should make it clear that there are significant questions about the role fMRI can and should have in clinical trials, and a major need for ongoing research. However, there is currently no conference venue designed for investigators to exchange information about the advancing state of the art in this area, and for public–private partnerships aimed at overcoming structural issues to develop. None of the current conferences in neuropharmacology, neuroimaging, and neuroscience have the critical mass of focus on this topic to incorporate sessions on it in a well-reasoned way. A 1-day add-on meeting on fMRI in clinical trials attached to one of the major ongoing conferences, or a stand-alone meeting, would be beneficial. We hope that the current exposition makes clear the interest that such a meeting would provoke from industry and academia, and its high potential for self-sustaining growth.
Legitimize and strengthen the publishing of negative results
Functional MRI studies are time-consuming, labor-intensive, and expensive. Negative results in these studies are problematic usually because the study is not published and the data then languish, sometimes because the study was poorly executed, but often because of a lack of power, an assumed statistical model that was incorrect, or some other problem [256]. However, as a result of this ‘file drawer’ problem, it is difficult, if not impossible, to know how many other investigators have attempted to test the same hypothesis, and found no result. Before funding an fMRI research study to test a hypothesis, it would be good to know whether unsuccessful studies have already been done on the subject or, more likely, whether imaging data exist that could be reanalyzed to answer the question or refocus the study design.
There are several solutions to the file drawer problem. First is the publicly or collaboratively available neuroimaging data repository. This was the topic of a special issue for Neuroimage in January 2016, with a follow-up issue [257] covering several dozen repositories from the USA and Europe. Several of these repositories are open to new studies and data sets, providing a place both to look for relevant data and to share a study that provided no publishable results. Within the USA, there are also the NIH-supported databases, with Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) being the primary repository for National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH)-supported research study data (https://data-archive.nimh.nih.gov/rdocdb/).
Although public repositories address access to relevant unpublished data, they do not address the risk of nonpublishability, relative to the high cost of doing the fMRI study. The neuroimaging community has realized that the data themselves are worth publishing if they were collected well. The Nature Publishing Group supports an open-access journal Scientific Data (www.nature.com/sdata/) for ‘descriptions of scientifically valuable data sets’. Thus, a clean, well-executed study whose data are being made available can also aim for a Nature publication just on the data, regardless of the study results. It will be vital that such data-set publications include enough detail on equipment, subject characteristics, and data acquisition parameters for other scientists to use the data to make decisions about the designs of their own studies.
Beyond disseminating and publishing the data, publishing negative results could be valuable for furthering the qualification of fMRI in specific contexts. However, a minimum of rigor should be placed in the presentation of negative results to enhance the confidence of the community that the report does not constitute a ‘false-negative’. To that aim, the following should be observed when presenting negative results: (i) to strengthen the case for publishing the negative results, the paper should present data and arguments that demonstrate that negative results are not the result of (a) poor study design; (b) lack of standardization and/or harmonization of data acquisition protocol; (c) poor protocol compliance; (d) poor data quality; or (e) unique or nonstandard analyses; (ii) ideally, negative results should be published with public access to the original data sets; (iii) a publication discussion section should highlight possible reasons for the negative findings; and (iv) the use of peer-reviewed preregistered reports (e.g., at Cortex, Drug and Alcohol Dependence; https://osf.io/8mpji/wiki/home/) simultaneously with registration at ClinicalTrials.gov is encouraged.
Once negative-result publishing venues become more mature, funding agencies should build on their recent record of demanding more data sharing from their grantees; all grantees should be required to attempt the publication of negative results in the event that they meet the quality standards outlined above.
Conclusion
Here, we have provided an overview of the current state of knowledge regarding fMRI as a tool for clinical trials. A growing body of work has crystallized a set of known performance criteria that the technique must meet to become useful in a clinical trial setting, as well as a set of known limitations of the technique and a set of useful capabilities it is thought to have. In addition, there is a substantial set of open questions regarding how fMRI experiments should be performed, what inferences should be drawn from fMRI data, and how fMRI should be deployed in different trial scenarios. As a result of the known limitations and the open questions, a set of activities are prescribed that have the potential to push the boundaries and make fMRI a more-effective tool in a wider range of trials.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by NIH grants P30DK072476, U54 GM104940, and T32 DK064584. It grew out of discussions that took place at the ‘Functional MRI in Clinical Trials: State of the Science and Future Directions’ symposium that was held December 1–3, 2015, at Pennington Biomedical Research Center. The symposium was supported by Sue Turner, Takeda Pharmaceutical Company Ltd, Merck, and the Pennington Biomedical Research Foundation.
Biography
Owen Carmichael, is an associate professor at Pennington Biomedical and directs its Biomedical Imaging Center. His research interest is in developing new biomedical imaging techniques and applying them to brain aging, Alzheimer’s disease, and metabolic disorders.
References
- 1.Khalili-Mahani N, et al. Biomarkers, designs, and interpretations of resting-state fMRI in translational pharmacological research: a review of state-of-the-Art, challenges, and opportunities for studying brain chemistry. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017;38:2276–2325. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Wise RG, Tracey I. The role of fMRI in drug discovery. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2006;23:862–876. doi: 10.1002/jmri.20584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Schwarz AJ, et al. A procedural framework for good imaging practice in pharmacological fMRI studies applied to drug development# 1: processes and requirements. Drug Discov Today. 2011;16:583–593. doi: 10.1016/j.drudis.2011.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schwarz AJ, et al. A procedural framework for good imaging practice in pharmacological fMRI studies applied to drug development# 2: protocol optimization and best practices. Drug Discov Today. 2011;16:671–682. doi: 10.1016/j.drudis.2011.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Logothetis NK, Pfeuffer J. On the nature of the BOLD fMRI contrast mechanism. Magn Reson Imaging. 2004;22:1517–1531. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2004.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Logothetis NK, Wandell BA. Interpreting the BOLD signal. Annu Rev Physiol. 2004;66:735–769. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.66.082602.092845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Petersen ET, et al. Non-invasive measurement of perfusion: a critical review of arterial spin labelling techniques. Br J Radiol. 2006;79:688–701. doi: 10.1259/bjr/67705974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.van den Heuvel MP, Hulshoff Pol HE. Exploring the brain network: a review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;20:519–534. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Wandschneider B, Koepp MJ. Pharmaco fMRI: determining the functional anatomy of the effects of medication. Neuroimage Clin. 2016;12:691–697. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mathew SJ, et al. Novel drugs and therapeutic targets for severe mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:2080–2092. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wong EH, et al. Challenges and opportunities for drug discovery in psychiatric disorders: the drug hunters’ perspective. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;13:1269–1284. doi: 10.1017/S1461145710000866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Perez-Caballero L, et al. Fluoxetine: a case history of its discovery and preclinical development. Expert Opin Drug Discov. 2014;9:567–578. doi: 10.1517/17460441.2014.907790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Goineau S, et al. Overview of safety pharmacology. Curr Protoc Pharmacol. 2001;63(Unit 10.1) doi: 10.1002/0471141755.ph1001s63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gunn RN, Rabiner EA. Imaging in central nervous system drug discovery. Sem Nucl Med. 2017;47:89–98. doi: 10.1053/j.semnuclmed.2016.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Combes R, et al. Early microdose drug studies in human volunteers can minimise animal testing: proceedings of a workshop organised by Volunteers in Research and Testing. Eur J Pharm Sci. 2003;19:1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0928-0987(03)00040-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.LoRusso PM. Phase 0 clinical trials: an answer to drug development stagnation? J Clin Oncol. 2009;27:2586–2588. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2008.21.5798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hansen AR, et al. Phase 1 trial design: is 3 + 3 the best? Cancer Control. 2014;21:200–208. doi: 10.1177/107327481402100304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Le Tourneau C, et al. Dose escalation methods in phase I cancer clinical trials. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2009;101:708–720. doi: 10.1093/jnci/djp079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Cook N, et al. Early phase clinical trials to identify optimal dosing and safety. Mol Oncol. 2015;9:997–1007. doi: 10.1016/j.molonc.2014.07.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lesko LJ, et al. Optimizing the science of drug development: opportunities for better candidate selection and accelerated evaluation in humans. J Clin Pharmacol. 2000;40:803–814. doi: 10.1177/00912700022009530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Dawson GR, et al. Validation of experimental medicine methods in psychiatry: the P1vital approach and experience. Biochem Pharmacol. 2011;81:1435–1441. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2011.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.McGonigle P. Animal models of CNS disorders. Biochem Pharmacol. 2014;87:140–149. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2013.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Enna SJ, Williams M. Challenges in the search for drugs to treat central nervous system disorders. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2009;329:404–411. doi: 10.1124/jpet.108.143420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Belzung C, Lemoine M. Criteria of validity for animal models of psychiatric disorders: focus on anxiety disorders and depression. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2011;1:9–9. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-1-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Markou A, et al. Removing obstacles in neuroscience drug discovery: the future path for animal models. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;34:74–89. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Conn PJ, Roth BL. Opportunities and challenges of psychiatric drug discovery: roles for scientists in academic, industry, and government settings. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:2048–2060. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Markou A, et al. Removing obstacles in neuroscience drug discovery: the future path for animal models. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:74–89. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.English BA, et al. Use of translational pharmacodynamic biomarkers in early-phase clinical studies for schizophrenia. Biomark Med. 2014;8:29–49. doi: 10.2217/bmm.13.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wilson FJ, et al. Can pharmaco-electroencephalography help improve survival of central nervous system drugs in early clinical development? Drug Discov Today. 2014;19:282–288. doi: 10.1016/j.drudis.2013.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Borsook D, et al. Use of functional imaging across clinical phases in CNS drug development. Transl Psychiatry. 2013;3:e282. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hargreaves RJ, et al. Optimizing central nervous system drug development using molecular imaging. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2015;98:47–60. doi: 10.1002/cpt.132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wong DF, et al. The role of imaging in proof of concept for CNS drug discovery and development. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:187–203. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Nathan PJ, et al. Increasing pharmacological knowledge about human neurological and psychiatric disorders through functional neuroimaging and its application in drug discovery. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2014;14:54–61. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2013.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Paulus MP, et al. Dose-dependent decrease of activation in bilateral amygdala and insula by lorazepam during emotion processing. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:282–288. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Upadhyay J, et al. Modulation of CNS pain circuitry by intravenous and sublingual doses of buprenorphine. Neuroimage. 2012;59:3762–3773. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Miller B, et al. Prolactin and fMRI response to SKF38393 in the baboon. Peer J. 2013;1:e195. doi: 10.7717/peerj.195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Orloff J, et al. The future of drug development: advancing clinical trial design. Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2009;8:949–957. doi: 10.1038/nrd3025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.EMA. About the EMA. EMA; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 39.FDA. About FDA. FDA; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 40.FDA. Development & Approval Process (Drugs) FDA; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 41.EMA. Scientific Guidelines. EMA; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 42.EMA. Qualification of Novel Methodologies for Medicine Development. EMA; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 43.FDA. Drug Development Tools (DDT) Qualification Programs. FDA; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 44.FDA. Biomarker Qualification Program. FDA; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Arthur J, et al. Biomarkers and surrogate endpoints: preferred definitions and conceptual framework. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2001;69:89–95. doi: 10.1067/mcp.2001.113989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.FDA. BEST (Biomarkers, EndpointS, and other Tools) Resource: Glossary. FDA; 2016. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.FDA. Qualification of Biomarker–Total Kidney Volume in Studies for Treatment of Autosomal Dominant Polycystic Kidney Disease; Guidance for Industry. FDA; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 48.EMA. Letter of Support for Molecular Imaging of the Dopamine Transporter Biomarker as an Enrichment Biomarker for Clinical Trials for Early Parkinson’s Disease. EMA; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 49.EMA. Letter of Support to Explore MRI Methodology to be Used to Stratify Populations of People with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) EMA; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Wise RG, et al. Using fMRI to quantify the time dependence of remifentanil analgesia in the human brain. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:626. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Schweinhardt P, et al. Pharmacological FMRI in the development of new analgesic compounds. NMR Biomed. 2006;19:702–711. doi: 10.1002/nbm.1076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Oertel B, et al. Differential opioid action on sensory and affective cerebral pain processing. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2008;83:577–588. doi: 10.1038/sj.clpt.6100441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.De Simoni S, et al. Test–retest reliability of the BOLD pharmacological MRI response to ketamine in healthy volunteers. Neuroimage. 2013;64:75–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Doyle O, et al. Quantifying the attenuation of the ketamine pharmacological magnetic resonance imaging response in humans: a validation using antipsychotic and glutamatergic agents. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2013;345:151–160. doi: 10.1124/jpet.112.201665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Clarke LP, et al. Imaging as a biomarker: standards for change measurements in therapy workshop summary. Acad Radiol. 2008;15:501–530. doi: 10.1016/j.acra.2007.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Kessler LG, et al. The emerging science of quantitative imaging biomarkers terminology and definitions for scientific studies and regulatory submissions. Stat Methods Med Res. 2015;24:9–26. doi: 10.1177/0962280214537333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Friedman L, Glover GH. Report on a multicenter fMRI quality assurance protocol. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2006;23:827–839. doi: 10.1002/jmri.20583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Petersson KM, et al. Statistical limitations in functional neuroimaging: II. Signal detection and statistical inference. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1999;354:1261–1281. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1999.0478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Henson R. What can functional neuroimaging tell the experimental psychologist? Q J Exp Psychol A. 2005;58:193–233. doi: 10.1080/02724980443000502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Vul E, et al. Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2009;4:274–290. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01125.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Kriegeskorte N, et al. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:535–540. doi: 10.1038/nn.2303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Yarkoni T. Big correlations in little studies: inflated fMRI correlations reflect low statistical power—Commentary on Vul et al. (2009) Perspect Psychol Sci. 2009;4:294–298. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01127.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.David SP, et al. Potential reporting bias in fMRI studies of the brain. PLoS One. 2013;8:e70104. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Button KS, et al. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14:365–376. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Nichols TE, et al. Best practices in data analysis and sharing in neuroimaging using MRI. bioRxiv. 2016:054262. doi: 10.1038/nn.4500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Poldrack RA, et al. Guidelines for reporting an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2008;40:409–414. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Plichta MM, et al. Test-retest reliability of evoked BOLD signals from a cognitive-emotive fMRI test battery. Neuroimage. 2012;60:1746–1758. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Mitsis GD, et al. Regions of interest analysis in pharmacological fMRI: how do the definition criteria influence the inferred result? Neuroimage. 2008;40:121–132. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Buck R, et al. Detecting change in BOLD signal between sessions for atlas-based anatomical ROIs. NeuroImage. 2008;40:1157–1165. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Jack CR, Jr, et al. The Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI): MRI methods. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2008;27:685–691. doi: 10.1002/jmri.21049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Van Essen DC, et al. The IMAGEN study: reinforcement-related behavior in normal brain function and pathology. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15:1128–1139. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Van Essen DC, et al. The Human Connectome Project: a data acquisition perspective. Neuroimage. 2012;62:2222–2231. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Chen JI, et al. Differentiating noxious- and innocuous-related activation of human somatosensory cortices using temporal analysis of fMRI. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88:464–474. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.1.464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Glover GH, et al. Function biomedical informatics research network recommendations for prospective multicenter functional MRI studies. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2012;36:39–54. doi: 10.1002/jmri.23572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Friedman L, et al. Reducing inter-scanner variability of activation in a multicenter fMRI study: role of smoothness equalization. Neuroimage. 2006;32:1656–1668. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Browne RH. On the use of a pilot sample for sample size determination. Stat Med. 1995;14:1933–1940. doi: 10.1002/sim.4780141709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Van Dijk KR, et al. Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103:297–321. doi: 10.1152/jn.00783.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Hutton C, et al. Image distortion correction in fMRI: a quantitative evaluation. Neuroimage. 2002;16:217–240. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.De Zwart JA, et al. Signal-to-noise ratio and parallel imaging performance of a 16-channel receive-only brain coil array at 3. 0 Tesla. Magn Reson Med. 2004;51:22–26. doi: 10.1002/mrm.10678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Pruessmann KP, et al. SENSE: sensitivity encoding for fast MRI. Magn Reson Med. 1999;42:952–962. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Nagy Z, Weiskopf N. Efficient fat suppression by slice-selection gradient reversal in twice-refocused diffusion encoding. Magn Reson Med. 2008;60:1256–1260. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Smith SM, et al. Functional connectomics from resting-state fMRI. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17:666–682. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Larkman DJ, et al. Use of multicoil arrays for separation of signal from multiple slices simultaneously excited. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2001;13:313–317. doi: 10.1002/1522-2586(200102)13:2<313::aid-jmri1045>3.0.co;2-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Stocker T, et al. Automated quality assurance routines for fMRI data applied to a multicenter study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25:237–246. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Power JD, et al. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2014;84:320–341. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Delaveau P, et al. Brain effects of antidepressants in major depression: a meta-analysis of emotional processing studies. J Affect Disord. 2011;130:66–74. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.van Wingen GA, et al. Short-term antidepressant administration reduces default mode and task-positive network connectivity in healthy individuals during rest. Neuroimage. 2013;88C:47–53. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Deakin JF, et al. Glutamate and the neural basis of the subjective effects of ketamine: a pharmaco-magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:154–164. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Doyle OM, et al. Quantifying the attenuation of the ketamine pharmacological magnetic resonance imaging response in humans: a validation using antipsychotic and glutamatergic agents. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2013;345:151–160. doi: 10.1124/jpet.112.201665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Joules R, et al. Ketamine induces a robust whole-brain connectivity pattern that can be differentially modulated by drugs of different mechanism and clinical profile. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2015;232:4205–4218. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-3951-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Yu H, et al. Mapping the central effects of chronic ketamine administration in an adolescent primate model by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) Neurotoxicology. 2012;33:70–77. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2011.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Corlett PR, et al. Glutamatergic model psychoses: prediction error, learning, and inference. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:294–315. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Krystal JH, et al. Preliminary evidence of attenuation of the disruptive effects of the NMDA glutamate receptor antagonist, ketamine, on working memory by pretreatment with the group II metabotropic glutamate receptor agonist, LY354740, in healthy human subjects. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;179:303–309. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1982-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Large CH. Do NMDA receptor antagonist models of schizophrenia predict the clinical efficacy of antipsychotic drugs? J Psychopharmacol. 2007;21:283–301. doi: 10.1177/0269881107077712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Chin CL, et al. Awake rat pharmacological magnetic resonance imaging as a translational pharmacodynamic biomarker: metabotropic glutamate 2/3 agonist modulation of ketamine-induced blood oxygenation level dependence signals. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2011;336:709–715. doi: 10.1124/jpet.110.173880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Gozzi A, et al. Differential effects of antipsychotic and glutamatergic agents on the phMRI response to phencyclidine. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:1690–1703. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Duff EP, et al. Learning to identify CNS drug action and efficacy using multistudy fMRI data. Sci Transl Med. 2015;7:274ra216. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3008438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Liu CH, et al. Remifentanil administration reveals biphasic phMRI temporal responses in rat consistent with dynamic receptor regulation. Neuroimage. 2007;34:1042–1053. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Wager TD, et al. An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. N Engl J Med. 2013;368:1388–1397. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Upadhyay J, et al. Imaging drugs with and without clinical analgesic efficacy. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:2659–2673. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Gear R, et al. Pain facilitation brain regions activated by nalbuphine are revealed by pharmacological fMRI. PLoS One. 2013;8:e50169. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Sarpal DK, et al. Antipsychotic treatment and functional connectivity of the striatum in first-episode schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:5–13. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Risacher SL, et al. Cholinergic enhancement of brain activation in mild cognitive impairment during episodic memory encoding. Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:105. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Breiter HC, et al. Acute effects of cocaine on human brain activity and emotion. Neuron. 1997;19:591–611. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80374-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Stein EA, et al. Nicotine-induced limbic cortical activation in the human brain: a functional MRI study. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:1009–1015. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.8.1009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Hooker BA, et al. Gabapentin-induced pharmacodynamic effects in the spinal nerve ligation model of neuropathic pain. Eur J Pain. 2014;18:223–237. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00364.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Governo RJ, et al. Gabapentin evoked changes in functional activity in nociceptive regions in the brain of the anaesthetized rat: an fMRI study. Br J Pharmacol. 2008;153:1558–1567. doi: 10.1038/bjp.2008.27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Upadhyay J, et al. Pharmacological modulation of brain activity in a preclinical model of osteoarthritis. Neuroimage. 2013;64:341–355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Kocsis P, et al. Vascular action as the primary mechanism of cognitive effects of cholinergic, CNS-acting drugs, a rat phMRI BOLD study. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2014;34:995–1000. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2014.47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Byun NE, et al. Antipsychotic drug-like effects of the selective M4 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor positive allosteric modulator VU0152100. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:1578–1593. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Baker S, et al. Xanomeline modulation of the blood oxygenation level-dependent signal in awake rats: development of pharmacological magnetic resonance imaging as a translatable pharmacodynamic biomarker for central activity and dose selection. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2012;341:263–273. doi: 10.1124/jpet.111.188797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Rech J, et al. Association of brain functional magnetic resonance activity with response to tumor necrosis factor inhibition in rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis Rheum. 2013;65:325–333. doi: 10.1002/art.37761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Cole DM, et al. Differential and distributed effects of dopamine neuromodulations on resting-state network connectivity. Neuroimage. 2013;78:59–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Gass N, et al. Haloperidol modulates midbrain-prefrontal functional connectivity in the rat brain. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2013;23:1310–1319. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Gass N, et al. Sub-anesthetic ketamine modulates intrinsic BOLD connectivity within the hippocampal-prefrontal circuit in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:895–906. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Grimm O, et al. Acute ketamine challenge increases resting state prefrontal-hippocampal connectivity in both humans and rats. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232:4231–4241. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4022-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Kelly C, et al. L-dopa modulates functional connectivity in striatal cognitive and motor networks: a double-blind placebo-controlled study. J Neurosci. 2009;29:7364–7378. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0810-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Khalili-Mahani N, et al. Effects of morphine and alcohol on functional brain connectivity during ‘resting state’: a placebo-controlled crossover study in healthy young men. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33:1003–1018. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Hirvonen J, et al. Measurement of central mu-opioid receptor binding in vivo with PET and [11C]carfentanil: a test-retest study in healthy subjects. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging. 2009;36:275–286. doi: 10.1007/s00259-008-0935-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Becerra L, et al. Parallel buprenorphine phMRI responses in conscious rodents and healthy human subjects. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2013;345:41–51. doi: 10.1124/jpet.112.201145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Leppa M, et al. Acute opioid effects on human brain as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage. 2006;31:661–669. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Becerra L, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging measures of the effects of morphine on central nervous system circuitry in opioid-naive healthy volunteers. Anesth Analg. 2006;103:208–216. doi: 10.1213/01.ane.0000221457.71536.e0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Borsook D, et al. Decision-making using fMRI in clinical drug development: revisiting NK-1 receptor antagonists for pain. Drug Discov Today. 2012;17:964–973. doi: 10.1016/j.drudis.2012.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Lee MC, Tracey I. Imaging pain: a potent means for investigating pain mechanisms in patients. Br J Anaesth. 2013;111:64–72. doi: 10.1093/bja/aet174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Upadhyay J, et al. Test-retest reliability of evoked heat stimulation BOLD fMRI. J Neurosci Methods. 2015;253:38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Bosma RL, et al. Neural correlates of temporal summation of second pain in the human brainstem and spinal cord. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:5038–5050. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Morton DL, et al. Brain imaging of pain: state of the art. J Pain Res. 2016;9:613–624. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S60433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Tracey I. Can neuroimaging studies identify pain endophenotypes in humans? Nat Rev Neurol. 2011;7:173–181. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2011.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Elman I, et al. The missing p in psychiatric training: why it is important to teach pain to psychiatrists. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68:12–20. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Baliki MN, et al. Corticostriatal functional connectivity predicts transition to chronic back pain. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:1117–1119. doi: 10.1038/nn.3153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Maleki N, et al. Her versus his migraine: multiple sex differences in brain function and structure. Brain. 2012;135:2546–2559. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Napadow V, et al. Decreased intrinsic brain connectivity is associated with reduced clinical pain in fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum. 2012;64:2398–2403. doi: 10.1002/art.34412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Kulkarni B, et al. Arthritic pain is processed in brain areas concerned with emotions and fear. Arthritis Rheum. 2007;56:1345–1354. doi: 10.1002/art.22460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Rabiner EA, et al. Pharmacological differentiation of opioid receptor antagonists by molecular and functional imaging of target occupancy and food reward-related brain activation in humans. Mol Psychiatry. 2011;16:826. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Nathan PJ, et al. Opioid receptor modulation of hedonic taste preference and food intake: a single-dose safety, pharmacokinetic, and pharmacodynamic investigation with GSK1521498, a novel m-opioid receptor inverse agonist. J Clin Pharmacol. 2012;52:464–474. doi: 10.1177/0091270011399577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Yeomans MR, Gray RW. Opioid peptides and the control of human ingestive behaviour. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002;26:713–728. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(02)00041-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Yeomans MR, Wright P. Lower pleasantness of palatable foods in nalmefene-treated human volunteers. Appetite. 1991;16:249–259. doi: 10.1016/0195-6663(91)90062-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Duff EP, et al. Learning to identify CNS drug action and efficacy using multistudy fMRI data. Sci Transl Med. 2015;7:274ra216. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3008438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Harris RE, et al. Pregabalin rectifies aberrant brain chemistry, connectivity, and functional response in chronic pain patients. Anesthesiology. 2013;119:1453–1464. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Sanders D, et al. Pharmacologic modulation of hand pain in osteoarthritis: a double-blind placebo-controlled functional magnetic resonance imaging study using naproxen. Arthritis Rheumatol. 2015;67:741–751. doi: 10.1002/art.38987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Becerra L, et al. CNS measures of pain responses pre- and post-anesthetic ketamine in a patient with complex regional pain syndrome. Pain Med. 2009 doi: 10.1111/pme.12939. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1526-4637.2009.00559.x. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 142.Wanigasekera V, et al. Disambiguating pharmacodynamic efficacy from behavior with neuroimaging: implications for analgesic drug development. Anesthesiology. 2016;124:159–168. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Harris RE, et al. Pregabalin rectifies aberrant brain chemistry, connectivity, and functional response in chronic pain patients. Anesthesiology. 2013;119:1453–1464. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Flodin P, et al. Intrinsic brain connectivity in chronic pain: a resting-state fMRI study in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016:10. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00107. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00107 Article 107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 145.Bruns A, et al. ‘Domain gauges’: a reference system for multivariate profiling of brain fMRI activation patterns induced by psychoactive drugs in rats. Neuroimage. 2015;112:70–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Hess A, et al. Blockade of TNF-alpha rapidly inhibits pain responses in the central nervous system. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:3731–3736. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011774108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Wu Q, et al. Tumor necrosis factor inhibitor therapy in ankylosing spondylitis: differential effects on pain and fatigue and brain correlates. Pain. 2015;156:297–304. doi: 10.1097/01.j.pain.0000460310.71572.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Upadhyay J, et al. A critical evaluation of validity and utility of translational imaging in pain and analgesia: Utilizing functional imaging to enhance the process. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.08.004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 149.Francois J, et al. Ketamine suppresses the ventral striatal response to reward anticipation: a cross-species translational neuroimaging study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:1386–1394. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Duncan GE, et al. Differential effects of clozapine and haloperidol on ketamine-induced brain metabolic activation. Brain Res. 1998;812:65–75. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(98)00926-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.de Simoni S, et al. Test-retest reliability of the BOLD pharmacological MRI response to ketamine in healthy volunteers. Neuroimage. 2013;64:75–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Sierakowiak A, et al. Default mode network, motor network, dorsal and ventral basal ganglia networks in the rat brain: comparison to human networks using resting state-fMRI. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0120345. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Gozzi A, Schwarz AJ. Large-scale functional connectivity networks in the rodent brain. Neuroimage. 2016;127:496–509. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.12.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Ma Z, et al. Functional atlas of the awake rat brain: a neuroimaging study of rat brain specialization and integration. Neuroimage. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 155.Lu H, et al. Rat brains also have a default mode network. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:3979–3984. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1200506109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Schwarz AJ, et al. The low-frequency blood oxygenation level-dependent functional connectivity signature of the hippocampal-prefrontal network in the rat brain. Neuroscience. 2013;228:243–258. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.10.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Hsu LM, et al. Constituents and functional implications of the rat default mode network. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113:E4541–E4547. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1601485113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Ash JA, et al. Functional connectivity with the retrosplenial cortex predicts cognitive aging in rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113:12286–12291. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1525309113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Schwarz AJ, et al. Anti-correlated cortical networks of intrinsic connectivity in the rat brain. Brain Connect. 2013;3:503–511. doi: 10.1089/brain.2013.0168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Becerra L, et al. Robust reproducible resting state networks in the awake rodent brain. PLoS One. 2011;6:e25701. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Inglis B. A Checklist for fMRI Acquisition Methods Reporting in the Literature. The Winnower; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 162.Glasser MF, et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage. 2013;80:105–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Carp J. On the plurality of (methodological) worlds: estimating the analytic flexibility of fMRI experiments. Front Neurosci. 2012;6:149. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Chen G, et al. Applications of multivariate modeling to neuroimaging group analysis: a comprehensive alternative to univariate general linear model. NeuroImage. 2014;99:571–588. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Lee AT, et al. Discrimination of large venous vessels in time-course spiral blood-oxygen-level-dependent magnetic-resonance functional neuroimaging. Magn Reson Med. 1995;33:745–754. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910330602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Gelman A, Loken E. even when There Is no ‘Fishing Expedition’ or ‘p-Hacking’ and the Research Hypothesis Was Posited Ahead of Time. Columbia University; 2013. The Garden of Forking Paths: Why Multiple Comparisons Can be a Problem. [Google Scholar]
- 167.Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Calhoun VD, et al. A review of group ICA for fMRI data and ICA for joint inference of imaging, genetic, and ERP data. Neuroimage. 2009;45:S163–S172. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Poldrack RA. Region of interest analysis for fMRI. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2:67. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Shrout PE, Fleiss JL. Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol Bull. 1979;86:420. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.86.2.420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Fiecas M, et al. Quantifying temporal correlations: a test-retest evaluation of functional connectivity in resting-state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2013;65:231–241. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Friedman L, et al. Test-retest and between-site reliability in a multicenter fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29:958–972. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Shehzad Z, et al. The resting brain: unconstrained yet reliable. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:2209–2229. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Bennett CM, Miller MB. How reliable are the results from functional magnetic resonance imaging? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1191:133–155. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05446.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Carrasco JL, et al. Distinguishability and agreement with continuous data. Stat Med. 2014;33:117–128. doi: 10.1002/sim.5896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.Feinberg DA, Setsompop K. Ultra-fast MRI of the human brain with simultaneous multi-slice imaging. J Magn Reson. 2013;229:90–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jmr.2013.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.Smith SM, et al. Resting-state fMRI in the human connectome project. Neuroimage. 2013;80:144–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 178.Tong Y, et al. Short repetition time multiband echo-planar imaging with simultaneous pulse recording allows dynamic imaging of the cardiac pulsation signal. Magn Reson Med. 2014;72:1268–1276. doi: 10.1002/mrm.25041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179.Smith SM, et al. Temporally-independent functional modes of spontaneous brain activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:3131–3136. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121329109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180.De Martino F, et al. Whole brain high-resolution functional imaging at ultra high magnetic fields: an application to the analysis of resting state networks. Neuroimage. 2011;57:1031–1044. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 181.Kundu P, et al. Differentiating BOLD and non-BOLD signals in fMRI time series using multi-echo EPI. Neuroimage. 2012;60:1759–1770. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 182.Miezin FM, et al. Characterizing the hemodynamic response: effects of presentation rate, sampling procedure, and the possibility of ordering brain activity based on relative timing. Neuroimage. 2000;11:735–759. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 183.Harte SE, et al. Pharmacologic attenuation of cross-modal sensory augmentation within the chronic pain insula. PAIN. 2016;157:1933–1945. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 184.Dumas A, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging detection of vascular reactivity in cerebral amyloid angiopathy. Ann Neurol. 2012;72:76–81. doi: 10.1002/ana.23566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 185.Vonsattel JP, et al. Cerebral amyloid angiopathy without and with cerebral hemorrhages: a comparative histological study. Ann Neurol. 1991;30:637–649. doi: 10.1002/ana.410300503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 186.Vinters H, Gilbert J. Cerebral amyloid angiopathy: incidence and complications in the aging brain: II. The distribution of amyloid vascular changes. Stroke. 1983;14:924–928. doi: 10.1161/01.str.14.6.924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 187.Smith E, et al. Impaired visual evoked flow velocity response in cerebral amyloid angiopathy. Neurology. 2008;71:1424–1430. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000327887.64299.a4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 188.Han BH, et al. Cerebrovascular dysfunction in amyloid precursor protein transgenic mice: contribution of soluble and insoluble amyloid-β peptide, partial restoration via γ-secretase inhibition. J Neurosci. 2008;28:13542–13550. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4686-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 189.van der Zwaag W, et al. fMRI at 1.5, 3 and 7T: characterising BOLD signal changes. Neuroimage. 2009;47:1425–1434. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 190.Triantafyllou C, et al. Comparison of physiological noise at 1.5 T, 3 T and 7 T and optimization of fMRI acquisition parameters. Neuroimage. 2005;26:243–250. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 191.Beisteiner R, et al. Clinical fMRI: Evidence for a 7 T benefit over 3 T. NeuroImage. 2011;57:1015–1021. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 192.Beisteiner R. Improving clinical fMRI: better paradigms or higher field strength? Am J Neuroradiol. 2013;34:1972–1973. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 193.Yacoub E, et al. Imaging brain function in humans at 7 Tesla. Magn Reson Med. 2001;45:588–594. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 194.Yang X, et al. A comparison of distributional considerations with statistical analysis of resting state fMRI at 3 T and 7 T. Proc SPIE Int Soc Opt Eng. 2012;2012:8314. doi: 10.1117/12.911307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 195.Vaughan JT, et al. 7 T vs 4 T: RF power, homogeneity, and signal-to-noise comparison in head images. Magn Reson Med. 2001;46:24–30. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 196.van der Kolk AG, et al. Clinical applications of 7T MRI in the brain. Eur J Radiol. 2013;82:708–718. doi: 10.1016/j.ejrad.2011.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 197.Beisteiner R, et al. Clinical fMRI: evidence for a 7T benefit over 3T. Neuroimage. 2011;57:1015–1021. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 198.Theysohn N, et al. Memory-related hippocampal activity can be measured robustly using fMRI at 7 Tesla. J Neuroimaging. 2013;23:445–451. doi: 10.1111/jon.12036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 199.Zhao F, et al. Qualification of fMRI as a biomarker for pain in anesthetized rats by comparison with behavioral response in conscious rats. Neuroimage. 2014;84:724–732. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 200.Upadhyay J, et al. Improved characterization of BOLD responses for evoked sensory stimuli. Neuroimage. 2010;49:2275–2286. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 201.Kwong KK, et al. Dynamic magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity during primary sensory stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1992;89:5675–5679. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.12.5675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 202.Sheth SA, et al. Linear and nonlinear relationships between neuronal activity, oxygen metabolism, and hemodynamic responses. Neuron. 2004;42:347–355. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00221-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 203.Zilles K, et al. Cytoarchitecture and maps of the human cerebral cortex. In: Toga AW, editor. Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference. Academic Press; 2015. pp. 115–135. [Google Scholar]
- 204.Amunts K, Zilles K. Advances in cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human cerebral cortex. Neuroimaging Clin N Am. 2001;11:151–169. vii. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 205.Wohlschläger AM, et al. Linking retinotopic fMRI mapping and anatomical probability maps of human occipital areas V1 and V2. Neuroimage. 2005;26:73–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 206.Toga AW, editor. Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference. Academic Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 207.Ogawa S, et al. Functional brain mapping by blood oxygenation level-dependent contrast magnetic resonance imaging. A comparison of signal characteristics with a biophysical model. Biophys J. 1993;64:803. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(93)81441-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 208.Mathiesen C, et al. Modification of activity-dependent increases of cerebral blood flow by excitatory synaptic activity and spikes in rat cerebellar cortex. J Physiol. 1998;512:555–566. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.555be.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 209.Shmuel A, Grinvald A. Functional organization for direction of motion and its relationship to orientation maps in cat area 18. J Neurosci. 1996;16:6945–6964. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-21-06945.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 210.Smith AJ, et al. Cerebral energetics and spiking frequency: the neurophysiological basis of fMRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:10765–10770. doi: 10.1073/pnas.132272199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 211.Shulman RG, et al. Energetic basis of brain activity: implications for neuroimaging. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27:489–495. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 212.Ances B, et al. Test–retest stability of calibrated BOLD-fMRI in HIV− and HIV+ subjects. Neuroimage. 2011;54:2156–2162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 213.Goodwin JA, et al. Quantitative fMRI using hyperoxia calibration: reproducibility during a cognitive Stroop task. NeuroImage. 2009;47:573–580. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 214.Leontiev O, Buxton RB. Reproducibility of BOLD, perfusion, and CMRO 2 measurements with calibrated-BOLD fMRI. Neuroimage. 2007;35:175–184. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 215.Wey HY, et al. Baseline CBF, and BOLD, CBF, and CMRO2 fMRI of visual and vibrotactile stimulations in baboons. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2011;31:715–724. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2010.154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 216.He X, Yablonskiy DA. Quantitative BOLD: mapping of human cerebral deoxygenated blood volume and oxygen extraction fraction: default state. Magn Reson Med. 2007;57:115–126. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 217.Lu H, Ge Y. Quantitative evaluation of oxygenation in venous vessels using T2-Relaxation-Under-Spin-Tagging MRI. Magn Reson Med. 2008;60:357–363. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 218.Bolar DS, et al. QUantitative Imaging of eXtraction of oxygen and TIssue consumption (QUIXOTIC) using venular-targeted velocity-selective spin labeling. Magn Reson Med. 2011;66:1550–1562. doi: 10.1002/mrm.22946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 219.Yablonskiy DA, et al. Blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD)-based techniques for the quantification of brain hemodynamic and metabolic properties: theoretical models and experimental approaches. NMR Biomed. 2013;26:963–986. doi: 10.1002/nbm.2839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 220.Wehrl HF, et al. Simultaneous PET-MRI reveals brain function in activated and resting state on metabolic, hemodynamic and multiple temporal scales. Nat Med. 2013;19:1184–1189. doi: 10.1038/nm.3290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 221.Mitsis GD, et al. The effect of remifentanil on respiratory variability, evaluated with dynamic modeling. J Appl Physiol. 2009;106:1038–1049. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.90769.2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 222.Khalili-Mahani N, et al. The impact of ‘physiological correction’ on functional connectivity analysis of pharmacological resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2013;65:499–510. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 223.Iannetti G, Wise RG. BOLD functional MRI in disease and pharmacological studies: room for improvement? Magnetic Reson Imaging. 2007;25:978–988. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2007.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 224.Semendeferi K, et al. Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: a comparative study of area 10. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2001;114:224–241. doi: 10.1002/1096-8644(200103)114:3<224::AID-AJPA1022>3.0.CO;2-I. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 225.Clancy B, et al. Extrapolating brain development from experimental species to humans. Neurotoxicology. 2007;28:931–937. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2007.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 226.Kesner RP, Churchwell JC. An analysis of rat prefrontal cortex in mediating executive function. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2011;96:417–431. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 227.Krubitzer L. The organization of neocortex in mammals: are species differences really so different? Trends Neurosci. 1995;18:408–417. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(95)93938-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 228.Gigout S, et al. Different pharmacology of N-desmethylclozapine at human and rat M2 and M4 mAChRs in neocortex. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol. 2015;388:487–496. doi: 10.1007/s00210-014-1080-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 229.Pradier L, et al. Molecular determinants of the species selectivity of neurokinin type 1 receptor antagonists. Mol Pharmacol. 1995;47:314–321. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 230.McKie S, et al. Mirtazapine antagonises the subjective, hormonal and neuronal effects of m-chlorophenylpiperazine (mCPP) infusion: a pharmacological-challenge fMRI (phMRI) study. NeuroImage. 2011;58:497–507. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 231.Deakin JW, et al. Glutamate and the neural basis of the subjective effects of ketamine: a pharmaco-magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:154–164. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 232.Völlm BA, et al. Methamphetamine activates reward circuitry in drug naive human subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1715. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 233.Leppä M, et al. Acute opioid effects on human brain as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage. 2006;31:661–669. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 234.Chen Y, et al. Quantification of cerebral blood flow as biomarker of drug effect: arterial spin labeling phMRI after a single dose of oral citalopram. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2011;89:251–258. doi: 10.1038/clpt.2010.296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 235.Bruns A, et al. Validation of cerebral blood perfusion imaging as a modality for quantitative pharmacological MRI in rats. Magn Reson Med. 2009;61:1451–1458. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 236.Khalili-Mahani N, et al. Ketamine interactions with biomarkers of stress: a randomized placebo-controlled repeated measures resting-state fMRI and PCASL pilot study in healthy men. Neuroimage. 2015;108:396–409. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 237.Carhart-Harris RL, et al. The effects of acutely administered 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine on spontaneous brain function in healthy volunteers measured with arterial spin labeling and blood oxygen level-dependent resting state functional connectivity. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78:554–562. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 238.Carhart-Harris RL, et al. Neural correlates of the LSD experience revealed by multimodal neuroimaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113:4853–4858. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1518377113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 239.Chen Y, et al. Quantification of cerebral blood flow as biomarker of drug effect: arterial spin labeling phMRI after a single dose of oral citalopram. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2011;89:251–258. doi: 10.1038/clpt.2010.296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 240.Marquand AF, et al. Dissociable effects of methylphenidate, atomoxetine and placebo on regional cerebral blood flow in healthy volunteers at rest: a multi-class pattern recognition approach. Neuroimage. 2012;60:1015–1024. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 241.Shcherbinin S, et al. Modulatory effects of ketamine, risperidone and lamotrigine on resting brain perfusion in healthy human subjects. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232:4191–4204. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4021-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 242.Schouw ML, et al. Mapping the hemodynamic response in human subjects to a dopaminergic challenge with dextroamphetamine using ASL-based pharmacological MRI. Neuroimage. 2013;72:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 243.Wang DJ, et al. Potentials and challenges for arterial spin labeling in pharmacological magnetic resonance imaging. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2011;337:359–366. doi: 10.1124/jpet.110.172577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 244.Khalili-Mahani N, et al. Spatial heterogeneity of the relation between resting-state connectivity and blood flow: an important consideration for pharmacological studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:929–942. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 245.Tjandra T, et al. Quantitative assessment of the reproducibility of functional activation measured with BOLD and MR perfusion imaging: implications for clinical trial design. Neuroimage. 2005;27:393–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 246.Walter SA, et al. Positive allosteric modulator of GABA lowers BOLD responses in the cingulate cortex. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0148737. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0148737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 247.Black KJ, et al. Rapid quantitative pharmacodynamic imaging by a novel method: theory, simulation testing and proof of principle. PeerJ. 2013;1:e117. doi: 10.7717/peerj.117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 248.Koller JM, et al. Rapid quantitative pharmacodynamic imaging with Bayesian estimation. Front Neurosci. 2016;10:XX. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 249.Mandeville JB, et al. A receptor-based model for dopamine-induced fMRI signal. Neuroimage. 2013;75:46–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 250.Liu CH, et al. Remifentanil administration reveals biphasic phMRI temporal responses in rat consistent with dynamic receptor regulation. Neuroimage. 2007;34:1042–1053. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 251.Marota JJ, et al. Cocaine activation discriminates dopaminergic projections by temporal response: an fMRI study in rat. Neuroimage. 2000;11:13–23. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 252.Sidén P, et al. Fast Bayesian whole-brain fMRI analysis with spatial 3D priors. NeuroImage. 2017;146:211–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 253.Wandschneider B, Koepp MJ. Pharmaco fMRI: determining the functional anatomy of the effects of medication. NeuroImage Clin. 2016;12:691–697. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 254.Rabiner EA, et al. Pharmacological differentiation of opioid receptor antagonists by molecular and functional imaging of target occupancy and food reward-related brain activation in humans. Mol Psychiatry. 2011;16:826–835. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 255.Murdaugh DL, et al. fMRI reactivity to high-calorie food pictures predicts short-and long-term outcome in a weight-loss program. Neuroimage. 2012;59:2709–2721. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 256.Spellman BA. Introduction to the special section data, data, everywhere … especially in my file drawer. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2012;7:58–59. doi: 10.1177/1745691611432124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 257.Eickhoff S, et al. Sharing the wealth: neuroimaging data repositories. Neuroimage. 2016;124:1065–1068. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]