Skip to main content
Human Brain Mapping logoLink to Human Brain Mapping
. 2017 Sep 6;38(12):5845–5858. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23763

On the integrity of functional brain networks in schizophrenia, Parkinson's disease, and advanced age: Evidence from connectivity‐based single‐subject classification

Rachel N Pläschke 1,2,3,4, Edna C Cieslik 1,2,3,4, Veronika I Müller 1,2,3,4, Felix Hoffstaedter 1,2,3,4, Anna Plachti 2,4, Deepthi P Varikuti 1,2,3,4, Mareike Goosses 4, Anne Latz 1,2,3,4, Svenja Caspers 4,5,6, Christiane Jockwitz 4,5,7, Susanne Moebus 8, Oliver Gruber 9, Claudia R Eickhoff 2,4,7, Kathrin Reetz 6,10,11, Julia Heller 6,10,11, Martin Südmeyer 3,12, Christian Mathys 13, Julian Caspers 4,13, Christian Grefkes 14,15, Tobias Kalenscher 16, Robert Langner 1,2,3,4, Simon B Eickhoff 1,2,3,4,
PMCID: PMC5931403  NIHMSID: NIHMS929039  PMID: 28876500

Abstract

Previous whole‐brain functional connectivity studies achieved successful classifications of patients and healthy controls but only offered limited specificity as to affected brain systems. Here, we examined whether the connectivity patterns of functional systems affected in schizophrenia (SCZ), Parkinson's disease (PD), or normal aging equally translate into high classification accuracies for these conditions. We compared classification performance between pre‐defined networks for each group and, for any given network, between groups. Separate support vector machine classifications of 86 SCZ patients, 80 PD patients, and 95 older adults relative to their matched healthy/young controls, respectively, were performed on functional connectivity in 12 task‐based, meta‐analytically defined networks using 25 replications of a nested 10‐fold cross‐validation scheme. Classification performance of the various networks clearly differed between conditions, as those networks that best classified one disease were usually non‐informative for the other. For SCZ, but not PD, emotion‐processing, empathy, and cognitive action control networks distinguished patients most accurately from controls. For PD, but not SCZ, networks subserving autobiographical or semantic memory, motor execution, and theory‐of‐mind cognition yielded the best classifications. In contrast, young–old classification was excellent based on all networks and outperformed both clinical classifications. Our pattern‐classification approach captured associations between clinical and developmental conditions and functional network integrity with a higher level of specificity than did previous whole‐brain analyses. Taken together, our results support resting‐state connectivity as a marker of functional dysregulation in specific networks known to be affected by SCZ and PD, while suggesting that aging affects network integrity in a more global way. Hum Brain Mapp 38:5845–5858, 2017. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Keywords: schizophrenia, Parkinson's disease, normal aging, support vector machine, resting‐state fMRI, functional connectivity, brain networks, machine learning

INTRODUCTION

Schizophrenia (SCZ) and Parkinson's disease (PD) are two of the most prevalent and socio‐economically relevant brain diseases [Andlin‐Sobocki et al., 2005]. Although SCZ onset typically emerges during adolescence and early adulthood [Häfner et al., 2013], PD is characterized by an onset during late adulthood [Hughes et al., 1992; Poewe et al., 2017] and has been associated with premature aging, that is, earlier and more rapid neurodegeneration as compared with the course of normal aging (NA) [Rodriguez et al., 2015]. Both SCZ and PD are characterized by disease‐specific pathophysiological changes of the dopaminergic system [Jankovic, 2008; Toda and Abi‐Dargham, 2007], contrasting with a more global dopamine decline in NA [Bäckman et al., 2006]. However, it has been proposed that dopaminergic dysfunction in SCZ arises as a secondary effect due to alterations of the glutaminergic system [Laruelle et al., 2003]. In contrast, in PD dopaminergic deficiency represents the primary cause leading to pathophysiological upstream dysregulations of different neural systems [Obeso et al., 2008]. These neurobiological features of SCZ, PD and NA [Bäckman et al., 2006; Jankovic, 2008; Laruelle et al., 2003; Obeso et al., 2008; Rodriguez et al., 2015; Toda and Abi‐Dargham, 2007] may manifest themselves in functional connectivity alterations at the level of large‐scale brain networks [Cole et al., 2013; Kelly et al., 2009; Narr and Leaver, 2015; Prodoehl et al., 2014; Sala‐Llonch et al., 2015]. However, some putative commonalities (neurodegeneration, dopaminergic dysregulations, and altered connectivity) need to be juxtaposed with the prominent phenotypical differences between SCZ, PD, and NA [Bäckman et al., 2006; Jankovic, 2008; Narr and Leaver, 2015; Prodoehl et al., 2014; Sala‐Llonch et al., 2015; Toda and Abi‐Dargham, 2007] and the fact that the clinical presentations of SCZ and PD are very different [Eaton et al., 1995; Jankovic, 2008; Kalia and Lang, 2015; van Os and Kapur, 2009], raising the question whether various functional systems are differentially affected in the three conditions. Rather than assessing altered activations in different functional systems by conducting task‐based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, we examined altered functional connectivity within various functional networks robustly defined by meta‐analyses of task‐based neuroimaging studies in a comparative fashion [cf. New et al., 2015; Schilbach et al., 2016]. This has the practicable advantage of using easily accessible, short and standardized resting‐state (RS) data while at the same time incorporating the consolidated knowledge based on task‐based imaging into the analysis. We argue that such an approach is particularly relevant given that in contrast to RS imaging, task‐based assessments will rarely be feasible in a routine clinical setting.

Alterations in functional network integrity patterns in SCZ, PD or older adults (compared with respective healthy/young controls) can be captured by using machine learning‐based classification. For extracting a diagnostically relevant marker that allows the classification of individual subjects based on the connectivity in functional brain networks, multivariate decoding algorithms like support vector machine (SVM) should provide the most appropriate approach for this endeavor. Rather than testing each connection independently for group differences, SVMs are trained on part of the data by weighting all connections in order to separate the known clinical status from healthy controls (HCs). Classification accuracy can then be determined by assessing the ability to predict group membership of previously unseen subjects. Applied to (whole‐brain) connectivity data, this approach has previously been found to distinguish SCZ patients [cf. Arbabshirani et al., 2016; Kambeitz et al., 2015; Wolfers et al., 2015] or PD patients [cf. Chen et al., 2015; Long et al., 2012] from HCs, as well as aged from young subjects (NA) [cf. Meier et al., 2012; Vergun et al., 2013].

Previous pattern‐classification studies aimed at providing the best possible classification performance on whole‐brain connectivity. In contrast, the aim of this work was to assess whether specific functionally defined networks are altered in SCZ, PD, and NA. Although previous studies mainly used Independent Component Analysis (ICA) based data‐driven methods to extract major RS networks [Damoiseaux et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2009], our work is based on a priori meta‐analytically defined networks associated with specific sets of behavioral functions such as working memory [Rottschy et al., 2012] or emotional processing [Sabatinelli et al., 2011]. In contrast to well‐established RS networks, these networks represent the consolidated information from hundreds of task‐based fMRI studies and hence those locations in the brain that are reliably activated when subjects perform tasks pertaining to a particular mental function. We thus argue that these nodes define robust functional networks in the brain related to specific mental domains. In turn, the functions associated with RS networks are usually derived from a reverse inference approach, as these lack any direct relationship to mental functions [Poldrack, 2011]. We suggest that this more direct relationship between the network‐nodes and actual task‐demands is an important advantage of our approach. Moreover, the employed strategy results in an a priori, unbiased definition of the respective networks, whereas ICA‐based networks are usually defined from the current data [Cole et al., 2010]. Our meta‐analytically derived network model approach thus offers the potential to investigate functional connectivity within robust a priori brain networks that are implicated in processing a specific mental process.

Therefore, this study aimed to examine whether the known impairment of different functions in SCZ, PD, or aging, respectively, would equally translate into a high classification accuracy for a given network in the respective group, based on the connectivity pattern within this network. As a “proof‐of‐principle” approach we therefore intended to investigate whether various a priori networks based on task‐activation findings carry differential disease‐related information assessable by RS imaging. To this end, we examined two diseases which are clinically very disparate but well studied in the previous neuroimaging literature. The findings were then juxtaposed to findings on age‐related effects in the same networks. Thereby, we could evaluate whether the respective networks carry differential information related to the different conditions or, conversely, whether the different networks carry differential information related to a particular condition. Given some putative commonalities and especially phenotypical differences, the aim was to examine the possibility for differential classification of SCZ, PD, und age, rather than to primarily study the specific diseases and their clinical separation from each other or aging per se. In our investigation, these three groups thereby serve as examples to evaluate this approach. For example, we assume that connectivity in the reward (Rew) network will be potent in differentiating SCZ patiens from matched HCs, as several studies have shown impairments related to reward learning in SCZ, and the neurobiology of this network has been linked to psychosis [Deserno et al., 2013; Heinz and Schlagenhauf, 2010; Radua et al., 2015]. Likewise, we would expect a good classification accuracy for PD patients based on FC in the motor network, given that motor impairments represent the core feature of this disease [Jankovic, 2008], and motor circuits in the brains of PD patients are altered during motor tasks and at rest [Herz et al., 2014; Prodoehl et al., 2014; Tessitore et al., 2014]. Finally, NA is accompanied by cognitive decline in various domains [Glisky, 2007], such as deterioration in working memory function [Braver and West, 2008]. For the latter, age‐related neural changes have repeatedly been shown at task [Dennis and Cabeza, 2008; Rajah and D'Esposito, 2005] and rest [Keller et al., 2015]. Accordingly, we assume that the working memory (WM) network allows a clear distinction between old and young adults.

In an explorative manner, we furthermore assessed a broad set of networks associated with different behavioural domains (cognitive, social‐affective, motivational, and motor‐related) since all three conditions (PD, SCZ, and NA) show alterations in various functional domains on the behavioral and neural level [Barch, 2005; Duncan et al., 2013; Seidler et al., 2010]. Importantly, in our approach, we reasoned that classification performance may be interpreted as an indication for the amount of information contained in a given network regarding a particular disease or age status, and thus of the degree of change observed in the integrity of particular networks under these conditions.

We assume that classification performance will be best for connectivity in those networks that subserve mental functions known to be affected in SCZ and PD. SCZ is characterized by prominent social‐affective/motivational alterations [Brunet‐Gouet and Decety, 2006; Deserno et al., 2013; Heinz and Schlagenhauf, 2010; Kring and Elis, 2013; Radua et al., 2015], whereas in PD motor impairments are most affected [Herz et al., 2014; Rowe and Siebner, 2012; Tessitore et al., 2014]. We, therefore, hypothesized that social‐affective/motivational and motor‐related networks provide a superior classification of SCZ and PD patients, respectively. As both diseases are accompanied by cognitive impairments as well, we assumed that cognitive networks may also be predictive to some degree [Barch, 2005; Duncan et al., 2013; Elgh et al., 2009; Nieoullon, 2002]. As NA is associated with a broad spectrum of decline affecting various functional systems (albeit to a varying degree) [Hedden, 2007; Mather, 2016; Seidler et al., 2010], we expected that most networks allowed for an accurate discrimination of old from young adults.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Samples

Schizophrenia

RS fMRI data and phenotypical information of 86 SCZ patients and 84 HCs obtained from the COBRE sample (http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/retro/cobre.html) and the University Hospital of Göttingen, Germany, were included in the analysis. SCZ diagnosis was assigned as assessed by the DSM‐IV‐TR based on the structured clinical interview (SCID‐P) and the International Classification of Diseases (ICD‐10), respectively. SCZ symptom severity was assed using the Positive and Negative Symptom Scale (PANSS) [Kay et al., 1987] evaluating the severity of positive and negative symptoms as well as the general psychopathology. Patients received their regular medication therapy with considerable variability in the exact compounds used and a high prevalence of combination drug therapy (medicated patients but exact medication and dose unknown for Olanzapine equivalent dose [Gardner et al., 2010]: COBRE: 50.9%; Göttingen: 25.8%; medication status unknown: COBRE: 1 SCZ patient; Göttingen: 2 SCZ patients).

Parkinson's disease

RS fMRI data of 80 PD patients and 84 HCs obtained from the RWTH Aachen University Hospital and the University Hospital Düsseldorf, Germany, were included in the analysis. Diagnosis of PD was assigned by consultant neurologists with longstanding expertise in movement disorders based on clinical examination and review of the medical history. Included PD patients fulfilled the standard UK Brain Bank criteria for PD and had on average a mild cognitive impairment as confirmed by the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) but no major depression symptoms [Hoops et al., 2009; Hughes et al., 1992; Nasreddine et al., 2005].

To assess PD symptom severity and evaluate motor impairments the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale Part III [Movement Disorder Society Task Force on Rating Scales for Parkinson's Disease, 2003] (UPDRS) and Hoehn and Yahr Scale (H & Y Scale) [Hoehn and Yahr, 1967] were applied. All patients were medicated with their regular individual PD‐related treatment (medication and dose unknown for Levodopa equivalent daily dose [Tomlinson et al., 2010]: Aachen: 28.1%; Düsseldorf: 12.5%).

Healthy controls

RS fMRI data of HC (HCSCZ and HCPD) were obtained from the four different sites as respective clinical subjects (SCZ and PD), and were without any record of neurological or psychiatric disorders as confirmed via structured clinical screening.

Normal aging

RS fMRI data of 95 old (age range: 55–70 years) and 93 young (age range: 20–35 years) participants with an age range of 15 years in each group were obtained from the population‐based 1000BRAINS study [Caspers et al., 2014] and another separate study at the Research Centre Jülich, Germany. This relative small age‐range aims to enhance the subsample homogeneity. “NA” in old participants refers to the absence of neurodegenerative diseases. Older adults showed cognitive performance adequate for their age (DemTect > 13) as assessed by the Mild Cognitive Impairment and Early Dementia Detection (DemTect) assessment [Kalbe et al., 2004] and all participants did not exhibit clinically relevant symptoms for depression (BDI‐II < 13) as evaluated via the Beck Depression Inventory‐II [Beck et al., 1996].

Importantly, target and control groups (i.e., patients vs. HCs, old vs. young adults) of all three samples (PD, SCZ, NA) represent subsamples from larger samples that were post‐hoc matched for gender, within‐scanner movement and (only for the clinical samples) age (cf. Table 1 for sample and group matching characteristics). Written informed consent from all subjects and approval by the local ethics committees was obtained from all sites. Joint reanalysis of the anonymized data was approved by the ethics committee of the Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf.

Table 1.

Sample and group matching characteristics

Sample n (males) Age (years) Head movement (DVARS) Age at onset (years) Illness duration (years) Antipsychotic/dopaminergic medication Neuropsychology and psychopathology
SCZ sample OZP‐equivalent PANSS: Total/PS/NS/GEN
COBRE
SCZ patients 55 (46) 38 ± 14 1.66 ± 0.55* 20 ± 8 17 ± 14 13 ± 8 58 ± 14/14 ± 5/14 ± 5/29 ± 8
HCscz 55 (42) 38 ± 12 1.44 ± 0.41
Göttingen
SCZ patients 31 (25) 32 ± 10 1.47 ± 0.30* 25 ± 8 7 ± 8 14 ± 9 52 ± 11/12 ± 3/13 ± 4/28 ± 6
HCscz 29 (22) 32 ± 9 1.31 ± 0.23
Total
SCZ patients 86 (71) 36 ± 13 1.59 ± 0.48*
HCscz 84 (64) 36 ± 11 1.39 ± 0.36
PD sample LEDD H & Y Scale UPDRS‐III MoCA
Aachen
PD patients 32 (21) 64 ± 9 0.51 ± 0.16 59 ± 8 6 ± 5 449 ± 238 2 ± 1 23 ± 12 27 ± 2
HCPD 33 (20) 63 ± 6 0.62 ± 0.29
Düsseldorf
PD patients 48 (30) 59 ± 9 0.69 ± 0.26 51 ± 9 8 ± 6 1029 ± 416 2.5 ± 1 16 ± 8 24 ± 4
HCPD 51 (30) 57 ± 9 0.68 ± 0.22
Total
PD patients 80 (51) 61 ± 9 0.62 ± 0.24
HCPD 84 (50) 59 ± 8 0.66 ± 0.25
NA sample DemTect BDI‐II
Jülich
Old 48 (26) 61 ± 5 1.58 ± 0.41* 16 ± 2 5 ± 5
Young 52 (26) 26 ± 3 1.24 ± 0.24 5 ± 4
1000BRAINS Jülich
Old 47 (25) 64 ± 4 1.79 ± 0.43* 15 ± 2 6 ± 5
Young 41 (23) 28 ± 4 1.28 ± 0.26 4 ± 4
Total
Old 95 (51) 63 ± 5 1.68 ± 0.43*
Young 93 (49) 27 ± 4 1.26 ± 0.25

SCZ, schizophrenia; HCSCZ, matched healthy controls (HCs) of SCZ sample; PD, Parkinson's disease; HCPD, matched HCs of PD sample; NA, normal aging; characteristic values in mean ± standard deviation; DVARS, derivative of root mean squared variance over voxels (head movement parameter) [Power et al., 2012]; significant difference in age (clinical samples), gender and movement are marked with * for P < 0.05; SCZ: OZP‐equivalent [Gardner et al., 2010], Olanzapine equivalent dose; PANSS, Positive and Negative Symptom Scale, (PS, Positive Symptoms Scale/NS, Negative Symptoms Scale/GEN, General Psychopathology Scale); PD: LEDD [Tomlinson et al., 2010], Levodopa equivalent daily dose; H & Y Scale, Hoehn and Yahr Scale; UPDRS‐III, Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale Part III; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; NA: DemTect, Mild Cognitive Impairment and Early Dementia Detection, BDI‐II, Beck Depression Inventory II.

RS fMRI Data Acquisition, Preprocessing, and Analysis

During image acquisition (see Supporting Information Table SI for fMRI parameters), participants were instructed to lie still, let their mind wander and not fall asleep (confirmed at debriefing). SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) was used for image realignment, spatial normalization to the MNI‐152 template using the unified segmentation approach [Ashburner and Friston, 2005], and smoothing “5‐mm full‐width at half‐maximum Gaussian kernel”.

We investigated 12 functional networks, robustly defined by previous quantitative meta‐analyses, to reflect neural correlates of a broad set of cognitive, social‐affective/motivational and motor functions (see Table 2 for an overview and Supporting Information Table SII for detailed network coordinates and corresponding brain regions). Only meta‐analytic networks with a minimum of 10 nodes were included, since a lower number of features are uninformative for robust classification. RS functional connectivity (RSFC) within each network was computed per subject by first extracting the time‐series for each node within 6 mm of the meta‐analytic peaks. To reduce spurious correlations, variance explained by the six movement parameters and their derivatives (modeled as first and second order effects) as well as the mean white‐matter and cerebrospinal fluid signal time‐courses was removed from the time series [Satterthwaite et al., 2013; Varikuti et al., 2016]. Subsequently, time series were high‐pass filtered retaining frequencies above 0.01 Hz. Connectivity was computed as the Fisher's Z‐transformed Pearson correlation between the time series of each network's nodes; connectivity values were adjusted for effects of acquisition site, gender, movement, total brain volume, and (only for the clinical samples) age [cf. Schilbach et al., 2016, 2014] to avoid classification based on spurious between‐subject effects.

Table 2.

Network overview

Network (Abbr.) Network Contrast Nodes Publications
EmoSF emotional scene and face processing emotional scene > neutral scene and emotional face>neutral face 24 Sabatinelli et al. [2011]
ER cognitive emotion regulation reappraise > naturalistic emotional responses 14 Buhle et al. [2014]
ToM theory‐of‐mind cognition ToM > non‐social baseline 15 Bzdok et al. [2012]
Empathy empathic processing “feel into” affect‐laden social situations > watched or listened passively 19 Bzdok et al. [2012]
Rew reward‐related decision making ME: reward valence and decision stages 25 Liu et al. [2011]
AM autobiographical memory autobiographical memory > non‐autobiographical baseline 22 Spreng et al. [2009]
SM semantic memory access to word meaning > processing word structure 23 Binder et al. [2009]
WM working memory ME: n‐back, sternberg, delayed matching to sample and delayed simple matching tasks 23 Rottschy et al. [2012]
CogAC cognitive action control ME: stroop‐task, spatial interference task, stop‐signal task and go/no‐go task 19 Cieslik et al. [2015]
VigAtt vigilant attention ME: detection task, discrimination task 16 Langner and Eickhoff [2013]
MNS mirror neuron system action observation ∩ action imitation 11 Caspers et al. [2010]
Motor motor execution finger tapping > baseline; excl. regions associated with visually paced finger‐tapping tasks 10 Witt et al. [2008]

ME, main effect.

SVM Features and Classification

To examine whether the RSFC pattern of a network contains predictive information on the respective groups (SCZ vs. HCSCZ, PD vs. HCPD, old vs. young) non‐sparse linear two‐class SVMs were computed using LibSVM [Chang and Lin, 2011] (https://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/~cjlin/libsvm). SVMs' were trained separately for each of all three analyses (PD, SCZ, NA) and each of the functional networks. Of note, we did not attempt between‐patient classification (i.e., PD vs. SCZ), as the different groups were closely matched to their respective controls but substantially different from each other with respect to age, gender, and movement. The input variables (features) to the SVM consisted of edge‐wise RSFC between all nodes of a given network. Each SVM was trained and tested by a nested 10‐fold cross‐validation scheme for each individual group (see e.g., Fig. 1 [Xia et al., 2013]) [cf. Lemm et al., 2011]. The inner loop used a 10‐fold cross‐validation within the training group to optimize the soft‐margin slack parameter. For each fold of the outer loop, the left‐out (unseen) 10% were then classified using the SVM trained on the (entire) training‐set using the optimized parameter. This nested scheme ensured that classifier optimization and evaluation was performed independent of each other [Kriegeskorte et al., 2009]. Classification performance was evaluated based on accuracy (Acc.) balanced accuracy (bAcc.), sensitivity (Sens.), and specificity (Spec.) as well as two measures derived from signal‐detection theory: the area under the receiver operating characteristics (ROC) curve (AUC) [Fawcett, 2004] and d'. Acc. denotes the overall proportion of subjects correctly classified as patients (PD, SCZ) or advanced age versus healthy or younger age, respectively. The bAcc. is calculated as the average proportion of subjects correctly classified as patients (PD, SCZ) or advanced age versus healthy or younger age, respectively. Sens. indicates the percentage of patients (SCZ or PD) correctly classified as ill or subjects correctly classified as old in the aging sample (true positives). Spec. in turn represents the fraction of HCs correctly classified as healthy or subjects correctly identified as young in the aging sample (true negatives). AUC refers to the area under the ROC curve. An ROC curve depicts the relationship between true positive rate and false positive rate, and its AUC value indicates the sensitivity of the diagnostic process independent of any specific decision criterion. Finally, we assessed d', an alternative index of diagnostic sensitivity independent of the decision criterion, calculated as z(true positive rate) – z(false positive rate). To increase robustness, the entire procedure was repeated 25 times, and each performance measures was averaged across repetitions. To examine significant differences in classification performance between networks within each group, pairwise t‐tests were performed for each of the 12 networks based on the accuracies obtained from the 25 cross‐validation outer loop replications of the separate SVMs (significance threshold of P < 0.05, Bonferroni‐corrected for the number of pairwise network comparisons).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Linear two‐class SVM nested 10‐fold cross‐validation scheme. Illustration of a SVM example for classification of the SCZ sample based on the EmoSF network. As input variables (DATA) (= features) served the subjects' RSFCs of all edges of every network. The inner loop was performed in a 10‐fold manner with 10 repetitions conducted as parameter setting optimization on a training sample. The outer loop was performed in a 10‐fold manner with 25 repetitions conducted as classification accuracy testing on an unseen test set. Classification performance measures are computed based on the confusion matrix. Acc., accuracy; Sens., sensitivity; Spec., specificity; AUC, area under the ROC curve and d' (see “Materials and Methods” section for explanation). [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

To compare the separately conducted classifications for SCZ versus HCSCZ and PD versus HCPD subgroups, accuracies obtained for each individual analysis for every network were converted to standardized z‐scores by reference to the binomial distribution reflecting chance level and corrected for multiple comparisons by the amount of networks‐based classifications. Log‐likelihood ratios were estimated to identify networks showing better classification performance for one patient group than the other. To investigate significant differences in classification performance between the groups, t‐tests were calculated based on the 25 accuracies obtained from the cross‐validation outer loop replications of the separate SVMs performed in each group (SCZ, PD, NA) for each of the 12 networks (significance threshold of P < 0.05, Bonferroni‐corrected for the number of groups and networks).

RESULTS

As expected, SCZ patients could be distinguished above chance from matched HCs based on RSFC in the Rew network (Acc. = 68%; AUC =0.73). In turn, PD patients were distinguished above chance from their matched HCs based on RSFC in the motor network (Motor; Acc. = 70%; AUC = 0.77). Finally, old and young subjects were differentiated very well from each other based on RSFC in the WM network (Acc. = 79%; AUC = 0.84). Results are summarized as follows: Figure 2A for polar plot of group classification accuracies, Table 3 for Acc., Sens., Spec. and AUC, Supporting Information Table SIII for bAcc., Supporting Information Table SIV for d', Supporting Information Figure S1 for z‐standardized accuracies of all groups and Supporting Information Figure S2 for variance of accuracies.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Group classification results of the SVM. (A) Polar plot of group classification accuracies based on all 12 networks for SCZ (in green), PD (in blue) and NA (in yellow). Accuracy refers to the proportion of subjects correctly classified as patients (PD, SCZ) or older age and subjects correctly classified as being HCs or younger age. (B) Polar plot of z‐standardized accuracies (corrected for multiple comparisons) of patients classification for SCZ (in green) and PD (in blue). (C) Log‐likelihood ratios of classification performance for networks showing higher classification for one patient group vs. the other. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

Table 3.

Classification results of the SVM of all groups based on specific networks

Network (Abbr.) SCZ vs. HCSCZ Acc. (Sens./Spec.) AUC PD vs. HCPD Acc. (Sens./Spec.) AUC Old vs. Young Acc. (Sens./Spec.) AUC
EmoSF 72% (77%/68%) 0.79 63% (64%/63%) 0.68 88% (89%/86%) 0.93
ER 71% (77%/65%) 0.76 69% (74%/64%) 0.74 78% (79%/76%) 0.86
ToM 61% (74%/46%) 0.62a 67% (70%/64%) 0.71 78% (77%/80%) 0.84
Empathy 71% (73%/69%) 0.78 63% (61%/65%) 0.69 78% (80%/75%) 0.83
Rew 68% (73%/62%) 0.73 66% (70%/63%) 0.71 87% (85%/88%) 0.93
AM 62% (67%/57%) 0.71 75% (78%/73%) 0.76 80% (80%/80%) 0.89
SM 61% (67%/54%) 0.68a 69% (65%/73%) 0.75 84% (85%/83%) 0.90
WM 62% (65%/60%) 0.66 65% (68%/63%) 0.71 79% (80%/77%) 0.84
CogAC 68% (73%/63%) 0.69 62% (66%/57%) 0.67 73% (73%/74%) 0.83
VigAtt 68% (72%/63%) 0.72 65% (68%/63%) 0.67 80% (78%/83%) 0.89
MNS 64% (65%/63%) 0.73 57% (64%/51%) 0.53a 84% (83%/84%) 0.91
Motor 61% (72%/50%) 0.61 70% (68%/73%) 0.77 80% (79%/81%) 0.90

Abbreviations: Acc., Accuracy (in %)/Sens., sensitivity (in %)/Spec., specificity (in %)/AUC, area under the ROC curve.

a

Network with no significant classification result.

Acc. refers to the proportion of subjects correctly classified as patients (PD, SCZ) or older age and subjects correctly classified as being healthy or younger age (mean of sensitivity and specificity). Sensitivity relates to the percentage of patients (SCZ or PD) correctly classified as being ill or else subjects correctly identified as old in the aging sample (true positives). Specificity relates to the percentage of healthy subjects correctly classified as being healthy or else subjects correctly identified as young in the aging sample (true negatives). AUC refers to the area under the ROCs curve. The ROC curve depicts the relationship between true positive rate and false positive rate.

Considering the performance of all functional networks in distinguishing SCZ and PD patients from their respective HCs, a clear differentiation between networks becomes evident, even though only 2 (SCZ) and 1 (PD) out of 12 networks, respectively, did not significantly exceed chance accuracy (Fig. 2B). The following results and discussion are focused on networks with superior classification performance for the respective disorders. In this context, we would like to re‐iterate that we did not attempt to train any classifier to distinguish SCZ from PD patients, since the two samples differed substantially from each other in various confounding factors such as age, gender distribution, and within‐scanner movement.

For SCZ, the emotional scene and face processing (EmoSF) network (Acc. = 72%; AUC = 0.79) as well as the networks for empathic processing (Empathy; Acc. = 71%; AUC = 0.78) and cognitive action control (CogAC; Acc. = 68%; AUC = 0.69) distinguished patients most accurately from their HCs. Hence these networks' connectivity patterns may be considered to contain the highest level of information with respect to SCZ. The EmoSF network was significantly better in the SCZ classification compared with all other networks (P < 0.001). For PD, the networks subserving autobiographical memory (AM; Acc. = 75%; AUC = 0.76), motor execution (Motor; Acc. = 70%; AUC = 0.77), semantic memory (SM; Acc. = 69%; AUC = 0.75), and theory‐of‐mind cognition (ToM; Acc. = 67%; AUC = 0.71) yielded the highest classification accuracies, that is, contained the most informative PD‐related differences in RSFC. The AM network was significantly better in the PD classification compared with all other networks (P < 0.001). All network comparison results within the patient groups are summarized in Supporting Information Tables SV and SVI.

The between‐network comparison of classification performance with respect to SCZ and PD revealed that the networks discriminating either disorder from their respective controls were highly specific (Fig. 2B,C), indicating that these networks carry differential amounts of information regarding SCZ and PD, respectively. In particular, both EmoSF and Empathy networks showed the best performance at distinguishing SCZ patients from HCs (EmoSF: z = 5.9; Empathy: z = 5.5) but were notably worse at discriminating PD patients from their HCs (EmoSF: z = 3.2; Empathy: z = 3.2). Similarly, the CogAC network exhibited high accuracy at classifying SCZ patients and their respective HCs (z = 4.7) but inferior performance at distinguishing PD patients from their HCs (z = 2.7).

In turn, the motor network very well classified PD patients and their HCs (z = 5) but was remarkably ineffective at classifying SCZ patients and their HCs (z = 2.9). Likewise, the AM and SM networks achieved high accuracies in classifying PD patients and controls (AM: z = 6.3; SM: z = 4.5) but performed much less well when classifying SCZ patients and controls (AM: z = 3.2; SM: z = 2.5). Networks which were most accurate in distinguishing SCZ from HCs (EmoSF, Empathy, and CogAC) exhibited significant better classification performance in the SCZ group compared to the PD group (EmoSF: P < 0.001; Empathy: P < 0.001; CogAC: P < 0.001; Supporting Information Table SVII). Likewise, networks which performed best at discriminating PD patients from HCs (AM, Motor, SM, and ToM) showed significant better classification performance in the PD group compared with the SCZ group (AM: P < 0.001; Motor: P < 0.001; SM: P < 0.001; ToM: P < 0.001; Supporting Information Table SVII).

This differential picture markedly contrasted with the results obtained for the classification between old and young subjects. In the aging sample, each network yielded accuracies ≥ 73% (see Supporting Information Table SVIII for network comparison results within NA), significantly outperforming every classification obtained in the SCZ or PD samples (P < 0.001; see Fig. 2A, Supporting Information Figure S1, Table 3, Supporting Information Tables SIX and SX).

In particular, for each network the accuracy for classifying a previously unseen participant as young or old was about 10% higher than any clinical classification based on the same network. Additionally, the comparison of all three separate group classifications revealed that the variance of the classification accuracies over the 25 replications of the outer loop was distinctively lower for the classification of age, as compared with classifying the clinical status (Supporting Information Fig. S2).

DISCUSSION

We assessed whether RSFC patterns in a diverse set of functionally defined brain networks allowed for a classification of patients with SCZ or PD or healthy older adults on the one hand, and their respective healthy or young controls on the other. Thereby, we evaluated which functional system was most informative for a given condition (i.e., SCZ, PD, or higher age). Conversely, our analysis also assessed the amount of information on each condition found in a given network. Our results show in a proof‐of‐principle manner that networks pertaining to functions known to be affected by SCZ, PD, or aging indeed exhibited good classification performance for the respective condition. Furthermore, each network's young–old classification outperformed any disease‐related classification. This indicates that specific networks are affected by and associated with the diseases, whereas for healthy older adults RSFC appears to be altered rather globally.

Conceptual Considerations

Our study demonstrates that machine‐learning techniques can be successfully used to assess whether RSFC in functional systems known to be affected in SCZ, PD, or advanced age exhibits high classification capacity for the respective condition. Further, our approach compared the classification capacity of RSFC patterns between different functional networks and between several clinical and physiological states. Of note, for each classification, target and control groups (i.e., SCZ vs. HCscz, PD vs. HCPD, old vs. young) were well matched with respect to gender and (for the clinical samples) age. In addition, RSFC variance attributable to these confounding factors or within‐scanner movement was regressed out of the data before the SVM analyses. Therefore, these confounds were evidently heterogeneous across the three groups (SCZ, PD, NA) but should not have influenced classification accuracy within each condition. In spite of proper matching and state‐of‐the‐art removal of variance related to motion [cf. Power et al., 2012; Satterthwaite et al., 2013], residual effects that only manifest in the multivariate pattern cannot be fully ruled out. However, one factor worth noting is that, for example, we observed differential classification performance across networks in the SCZ sample, largely ruling out a dominant general effect of head motion.

Given that both groups were assessed under their regular medication, differences in classification performance may be influenced by pharmacological treatment. In particular, we cannot exclude that classification results of networks modulated via dopaminergic transmission (e.g., reward or motor system) might originate from interactions between disease condition and medication. Unfortunately, however, we could not perform a more detailed assessment of the influence of medication, as the compounds, duration of treatment and doses varied considerably between subjects, with many receiving a combination of drugs.

When comparing classification performance to previous work based on whole‐brain functional connectomes [cf. Chen et al., 2015; Long et al., 2012; Meier et al., 2012; Su et al., 2013; Tang et al., 2012; Vergun et al., 2013; Yu et al., 2013], we note that our approach yielded higher functional specificity, allowing inference on the amount of disease‐specific information in well‐defined functional systems. We acknowledge that even though most of the classifications well exceeded chance level, the achieved network‐based classification accuracies are not strong enough for successful connectivity‐based single‐subject diagnosis. Still, our “sparse” approach achieved classification accuracies comparable to those reported in previous whole‐brain studies, whose feature space obviously was substantially larger than ours. This is particularly noteworthy given that two further aspects besides feature space could be expected to decrease classifier performance in our study [Arbabshirani et al., 2016; Haller et al., 2014; Kambeitz et al., 2015; Schnack and Kahn, 2016; Varoquaux et al., 2016]: First, all of our three groups were based on relatively large samples that were combined from two different measurement sites and hence should be more heterogeneous than usual. Second, we used replicated 10‐fold cross‐validation, rather than the more optimistic leave‐one‐out approach [Varoquaux et al., 2016]. We thus argue that the chosen combination of examining robustly defined functional networks and optimized analysis through replicated and nested 10‐fold cross‐validation may provide valuable new insights into the pathophysiology of brain disorders that is not attainable through global analyses of the entire functional connectome.

Classification of SCZ Patients and Controls

We found that the networks subserving EmoSF, empathic processing as well as CogAC yielded the best performance. Aberrant processing of emotional stimuli [Takahashi et al., 2004] and impaired abilities to relate to others' affective states [Benedetti et al., 2009; Derntl et al., 2012; Harvey et al., 2012] are features of SCZ and mirrored in the degree of SCZ‐related information that is contained in the EmoSF (AUC = 0.79) and Empathy (AUC = 0.78) networks. Further, the good classification performance of the CogAC network resonates well with alterations in cognitive control processes as a core deficit in SCZ [cf. Lesh et al., 2011].

Somewhat surprisingly, the Rew network did not differentiate SCZ from HCs with high accuracy, given the prominent role of the dopaminergic system [Toda and Abi‐Dargham, 2007] and aberrant salience processing in psychosis [Heinz and Schlagenhauf, 2010; Radua et al., 2015] and the association with the reward system in this disorder. We conjecture that this lack of predictive information could arise from the fact that in contrast to task‐activation data, RSFC analyses primarily capture the tonic rather than phasic state of these networks [Schultz et al., 1997].

Classification of PD Patients and Controls

The superior classification performance observed for the motor execution network (AUC = 0.77) is hardly surprising, since motor impairments represent a key clinical feature of PD, and differences in action‐related brain circuitry are well established in this disorder [Herz et al., 2014; Rowe and Siebner, 2012; Tessitore et al., 2014]. The finding that the AM (AUC = 0.76) and SM (AUC = 0.75) networks also achieved a very good differentiation of PD patients from HCs was rather surprising, though. Although PD is a neurodegenerative disorder and dementia is common in PD patients [Aarsland et al., 2001, 2003], several patients showed evidence for mild cognitive impairment, using the MoCA for screening. We can hence only speculate that the RSFC differences in AM and SM networks may pick up these deficits as revealed by standard behavioral screening instruments.

Finally, the good classification performance achieved by the ToM network (AUC = 0.71) was unexpected but matches a growing literature of impaired social cognition in PD patients [Bora et al., 2015; Poletti et al., 2011; Díez‐Cirarda et al., 2015].

Age Group Classification

One of the most striking observations from this study was that every single network achieved a better classification with respect to age group than with respect to SCZ or PD. While we hypothesized that the broad spectrum of age‐related changes in various mental functions [Craik and Salthouse, 2011; Glisky, 2007; Seidler et al., 2010] would be reflected by changes in several networks [Craik and Salthouse, 2011; Hedden, 2007; Mather, 2016; Seidler et al., 2010], the consistency (across both networks and replications) of high classification accuracies is intriguing. It stands to reason that the mechanisms underlying the discriminative changes in functional connectivity patterns may be diverse. In particular, they should include neurodegeneration (cognitive networks [Hedden, 2007]), neurochemical changes (Rew networks [Bäckman et al., 2006]), altered affective processing (social‐affective networks [Mather, 2016]) and use‐dependent plasticity (motor networks [Demirakca et al., 2016]). In addition, it may be argued that in spite of all inter‐individual variability age‐related changes represent a more homogeneous change of the neuro‐functional architecture [Ferreira et al., 2016; Meier et al., 2012] relative to the inevitable heterogeneity among clinical populations.

Given that connectivity patterns of all systems differentiated very well between young and old participants, we acknowledge the possibility that the relevant drivers may be of non‐neural origin. In particular, despite of our optimized confound removal [Power et al., 2012; Satterthwaite et al., 2013; Varikuti et al., 2016], we cannot exclude that residual effects related to motion or brain atrophy as well as physiological effects such as macro‐ and microvascular changes and their cumulative impact on hemodynamic signals [D'Esposito et al., 2003] may have contributed to our findings.

Although the contributions of neural and non‐neural effects outlined in this section certainly warrant further investigation, one of the most critical conclusions that should be taken from the high classification accuracy between younger and older participants is the danger of obtaining spuriously high accuracies in clinical classification studies if patients and HCs are not carefully matched for age.

Conclusions and Outlook

We investigated the potential of RS connectivity patterns in a wide variety of functional networks to distinguish SCZ and PD patients from matched HCs as well as old from young adults. We showed that networks defined by robust activation due to mental operations known to be affected in the respective condition indeed contained information on the respective condition that is captured by our pattern‐classification approach and translates into good classification accuracies. Classification accuracies obtained through replicated, nested 10‐fold cross‐validation were not only generally comparable to those obtained from whole‐brain analyses but also revealed a differentiated picture for both disorders in comparisons. Both SCZ and PD were specifically well predicted by distinct networks that resonate well with known clinical and pathophysiological features. The presented approach thus opens an avenue toward robust and more specific assessments of clinical and developmental differences in functional systems than previous whole‐brain analyses. One of the most striking findings of this work was the fact that integrity in all networks was much better at identifying participants with advanced age than with any of the two disorders. While the most likely heterogeneous mechanisms behind this phenomenon certainly need to be addressed in more detail, the current findings highlight the importance of considering age‐related effects as a potential source of bias in clinical classification studies.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURES

These authors declare following conflicts of interest: K.R. was partly funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research [BMBF 01GQ1402] and the Alzheimer Forschung Initiative e.V. [AFI 13812] and the position of J.H. was subsidized by the START‐Program of the Faculty of Medicine at the RWTH Aachen University, Germany [23/12 to K.R.]. K.R. has received honoraria for presentations from Lilly and research grants from Pfizer and Merck.

Supporting information

Supporting Information.

REFERENCES

  1. Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP, Lolk A, Nielsen H, Kragh‐Sørensen P (2001): Risk of dementia in Parkinson's disease: A community‐based, prospective study. Neurology 56:730–736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP, Lolk A, Kragh‐Sørensen P (2003): Prevalence and characteristics of dementia in Parkinson disease: An 8‐year prospective study. Arch Neurol 60:387–392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andlin‐Sobocki P, Jönsson B, Wittchen H‐U, Olesen J (2005): Cost of disorders of the brain in Europe. Eur J Neurol 12(Suppl 1):1–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arbabshirani MR, Plis S, Sui J, Calhoun VD (2016): Single subject prediction of brain disorders in neuroimaging: Promises and pitfalls. NeuroImage 145:137–165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ashburner J, Friston KJ (2005): Unified segmentation. NeuroImage 26:839–851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bäckman L, Nyberg L, Lindenberger U, Li S‐C, Farde L (2006): The correlative triad among aging, dopamine, and cognition: Current status and future prospects. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30:791–807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Barch DM (2005): The cognitive neuroscience of schizophrenia. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 1:321–353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown G (1996): Beck Depression Inventory‐Second Edition (BDI‐II). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  9. Benedetti F, Bernasconi A, Bosia M, Cavallaro R, Dallaspezia S, Falini A, Poletti S, Radaelli D, Riccaboni R, Scotti G, Smeraldi E (2009): Functional and structural brain correlates of theory of mind and empathy deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 114:154–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL (2009): Where Is the Semantic System? A Critical Review and Meta‐Analysis of 120 Functional Neuroimaging Studies. Cereb Cortex 19:2767–2796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bora E, Walterfang M, Velakoulis D (2015): Theory of mind in Parkinson's disease: A meta‐analysis. Behav Brain Res 292:515–520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Braver TS, West R (2008): Working memory, executive control, and aging In: Craik FIM, Salthouse TA, editors. The handbook of aging and cognition, 3rd ed New York, NY, US: Psychology Press; pp 311–372. [Google Scholar]
  13. Brunet‐Gouet E, Decety J (2006): Social brain dysfunctions in schizophrenia: A review of neuroimaging studies. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 148:75–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, Weber J, Ochsner KN (2014): Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta‐analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex 24:2981–2990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bzdok D, Schilbach L, Vogeley K, Schneider K, Laird AR, Langner R, Eickhoff SB (2012): Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta‐analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Struct Funct 217:783–796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Caspers S, Moebus S, Lux S, Pundt N, Schütz H, Mühleisen TW, Gras V, Eickhoff SB, Romanzetti S, Stöcker T, Stirnberg R, Kirlangic ME, Minnerop M, Pieperhoff P, Mödder U, Das S, Evans AC, Jöckel K‐H, Erbel R, Cichon S, Nöthen MM, Sturma D, Bauer A, Jon Shah N, Zilles K, Amunts K (2014): Studying variability in human brain aging in a population‐based German cohort—rationale and design of 1000BRAINS. Front Aging Neurosci 6:149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Caspers S, Zilles K, Laird AR, Eickhoff SB (2010): ALE meta‐analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. NeuroImage 50:1148–1167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chang C, Lin C‐J (2011): LIBSVM. A library for support vector machines. ACM Trans Intell Syst Technol 2:1–27:27. [Google Scholar]
  19. Chen Y, Yang W, Long J, Zhang Y, Feng J, Li Y, Huang B (2015): Discriminative analysis of Parkinson's disease based on whole‐brain functional connectivity. PloS One 10:e0124153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cieslik EC, Mueller VI, Eickhoff CR, Langner R, Eickhoff SB (2015): Three key regions for supervisory attentional control: evidence from neuroimaging meta‐analyses. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 48:22–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cole DM, Beckmann CF, Oei NYL, Both S, van Gerven JMA, Rombouts SARB (2013): Differential and distributed effects of dopamine neuromodulations on resting‐state network connectivity. NeuroImage 78:59–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cole DM, Smith SM, Beckmann CF (2010): Advances and pitfalls in the analysis and interpretation of resting‐state FMRI data. Front Syst Neurosci 4:8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Craik FIM, Salthouse TA (2011): The Handbook of Aging and Cognition, 3rd ed New York, NY: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  24. Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SARB, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, Smith SM, Beckmann CF (2006): Consistent resting‐state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:13848–13853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Demirakca T, Cardinale V, Dehn S, Ruf M, Ende G (2016): The exercising brain: Changes in functional connectivity induced by an integrated multimodal cognitive and whole‐body coordination training. Neural Plast 2016:8240894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Dennis NA, Cabeza R (2008): Neuroimaging of healthy cognitive aging In: Craik FIM, Salthouse TA, editors. The Handbook of Aging and Cognition, 3rd ed New York, NY: Psychology Press; pp 1–54. [Google Scholar]
  27. Derntl B, Finkelmeyer A, Voss B, Eickhoff SB, Kellermann T, Schneider F, Habel U (2012): Neural correlates of the core facets of empathy in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 136:70–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Deserno L, Boehme R, Heinz A, Schlagenhauf F (2013): Reinforcement learning and dopamine in schizophrenia: Dimensions of symptoms or specific features of a disease group?. Front Psychiatry 4:172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. D'Esposito M, Deouell LY, Gazzaley A (2003): Alterations in the BOLD fMRI signal with ageing and disease: a challenge for neuroimaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:863–872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Díez‐Cirarda M, Ojeda N, Peña J, Cabrera‐Zubizarreta A, Gómez‐Beldarrain MÁ, Gómez‐Esteban JC, Ibarretxe‐Bilbao N (2015): Neuroanatomical correlates of theory of mind deficit in Parkinson's disease: A Multimodal Imaging Study. PLoS One 10:e0142234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Duncan GW, Firbank MJ, O'Brien JT, Burn DJ (2013): Magnetic resonance imaging: A biomarker for cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease?. Mov Disord off J Mov Disord Soc 28:425–438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Eaton WW, Thara R, Federman B, Melton B, Liang KY (1995): Structure and course of positive and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52:127–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Elgh E, Domellöf M, Linder J, Edström M, Stenlund H, Forsgren L (2009): Cognitive function in early Parkinson's disease: A population‐based study. Eur J Neurol 16:1278–1284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Fawcett T (2004): ROC Graphs: Notes and Practical Considerations for Researchers. Machine Learning 31:1–38. [Google Scholar]
  35. Ferreira LK, Regina ACB, Kovacevic N, Martin M. D GM, Santos PP, Carneiro C, de G, Kerr DS, Amaro E, McIntosh AR, Busatto GF (2016): Aging effects on whole‐brain functional connectivity in adults free of cognitive and psychiatric disorders. Cereb Cortex 26:3851–3865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Gardner DM, Murphy AL, O'Donnell H, Centorrino F, Baldessarini RJ (2010): International consensus study of antipsychotic dosing. Am J Psychiatry 167:686–693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Glisky EL (2007): Changes in Cognitive Function in Human Aging In: Riddle DR, editor. Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Häfner H, Maurer K, an der Heiden W (2013): ABC Schizophrenia study: An overview of results since 1996. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 48:1021–1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Haller S, Lovblad K‐O, Giannakopoulos P, Van De Ville D (2014): Multivariate pattern recognition for diagnosis and prognosis in clinical neuroimaging: State of the art, current challenges and future trends. Brain Topogr 27:329–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Harvey P‐O, Zaki J, Lee J, Ochsner K, Green MF (2012): Neural substrates of empathic accuracy in people with Schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull sbs042 39:617–628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hedden T (2007): Imaging cognition in the aging human brain In: Riddle DR, editor. Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Heinz A, Schlagenhauf F (2010): Dopaminergic dysfunction in schizophrenia: Salience attribution revisited. Schizophr Bull 36:472–485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Herz DM, Eickhoff SB, Løkkegaard A, Siebner HR (2014): Functional neuroimaging of motor control in Parkinson's disease: A meta‐analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 35:3227–3237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hoehn MM, Yahr MD (1967): Parkinsonism: Onset, progression and mortality. Neurology 17:427–442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hoops S, Nazem S, Siderowf AD, Duda JE, Xie SX, Stern MB, Weintraub D (2009): Validity of the MoCA and MMSE in the detection of MCI and dementia in Parkinson disease. Neurology 73:1738–1745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ (1992): Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: A clinico‐pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 55:181–184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Jankovic J (2008): Parkinson's disease: Clinical features and diagnosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 79:368–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kalbe E, Kessler J, Calabrese P, Smith R, Passmore AP, Brand M, Bullock R (2004): DemTect: A new, sensitive cognitive screening test to support the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment and early dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 19:136–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kalia LV, Lang AE (2015): Parkinson's disease. Lancet Lond Engl 386:896–912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kambeitz J, Kambeitz‐Ilankovic L, Leucht S, Wood S, Davatzikos C, Malchow B, Falkai P, Koutsouleris N (2015): Detecting neuroimaging biomarkers for schizophrenia: A meta‐analysis of multivariate pattern recognition studies. Neuropsychopharmacol off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol 40:1742–1751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kay SR, Flszbein A, Opfer LA (1987): The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull 13:261–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Keller JB, Hedden T, Thompson TW, Anteraper SA, Gabrieli JDE, Whitfield‐Gabrieli S (2015): Resting‐state anticorrelations between medial and lateral prefrontal cortex: Association with working memory, aging, and individual differences. Cortex J Devoted Study Nerv Syst Behav 64:271–280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kelly C, de Zubicaray G, Di Martino A, Copland DA, Reiss PT, Klein DF, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, McMahon K (2009): L‐dopa modulates functional connectivity in striatal cognitive and motor networks: A double‐blind placebo‐controlled study. J Neurosci 29:7364–7378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kriegeskorte N, Simmons WK, Bellgowan PSF, Baker CI (2009): Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: The dangers of double dipping. Nat Neurosci 12:535–540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kring AM, Elis O (2013): Emotion deficits in people with Schizophrenia. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 9:409–433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Langner R, Eickhoff SB (2013): Sustaining attention to simple tasks: a meta‐analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychol Bull 139:870–900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Laruelle M, Kegeles LS, Abi‐Dargham A (2003): Glutamate, dopamine, and schizophrenia: From pathophysiology to treatment. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1003:138–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Lemm S, Blankertz B, Dickhaus T, Müller K‐R (2011): Introduction to machine learning for brain imaging. NeuroImage 56:387–399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Lesh TA, Niendam TA, Minzenberg MJ, Carter CS (2011): Cognitive control deficits in schizophrenia: Mechanisms and meaning. Neuropsychopharmacology 36:316–338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Liu X, Hairston J, Schrier M, Fan J (2011): Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: a meta‐analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35:1219–1236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Long D, Wang J, Xuan M, Gu Q, Xu X, Kong D, Zhang M (2012): Automatic classification of early Parkinson's disease with multi‐modal MR imaging. PLoS One 7:e47714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Mather M (2016): The affective neuroscience of Aging. Annu Rev Psychol 67:213–238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Meier TB, Desphande AS, Vergun S, Nair VA, Song J, Biswal BB, Meyerand ME, Birn RM, Prabhakaran V (2012): Support vector machine classification and characterization of age‐related reorganization of functional brain networks. NeuroImage 60:601–613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Movement Disorder Society Task Force on Rating Scales for Parkinson's Disease (2003): The Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS): Status and recommendations. Mov Disord 18:738–750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Narr KL, Leaver AM (2015): Connectome and schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry 28:229–235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, Cummings JL, Chertkow H (2005): The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 53:695–699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. New AB, Robin DA, Parkinson AL, Eickhoff CR, Reetz K, Hoffstaedter F, Mathys C, Sudmeyer M, Grefkes C, Larson CR, Ramig LO, Fox PT, Eickhoff SB (2015): The intrinsic resting state voice network in Parkinson's disease. Hum Brain Mapp 36:1951–1962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Nieoullon A (2002): Dopamine and the regulation of cognition and attention. Prog Neurobiol 67:53–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Obeso JA, Rodríguez‐Oroz MC, Benitez‐Temino B, Blesa FJ, Guridi J, Marin C, Rodriguez M (2008): Functional organization of the basal ganglia: Therapeutic implications for Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord 23(Suppl 3):S548–S559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Poewe W, Seppi K, Tanner CM, Halliday GM, Brundin P, Volkmann J, Schrag A‐E, Lang AE (2017): Parkinson disease. Nat Rev Dis Primer 3:17013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Poldrack RA (2011): Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data: From reverse inference to large‐scale decoding. Neuron 72:692–697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Poletti M, Enrici I, Bonuccelli U, Adenzato M (2011): Theory of Mind in Parkinson's disease. Behav Brain Res 219:342–350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2012): Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59:2142–2154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Prodoehl J, Burciu RG, Vaillancourt DE (2014): Resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging in Parkinson's disease. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 14:448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Radua J, Schmidt A, Borgwardt S, Heinz A, Schlagenhauf F, McGuire P, Fusar‐Poli P (2015): Ventral striatal activation during reward processing in psychosis: A neurofunctional meta‐analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 72:1243–1251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Rajah MN, D'Esposito M (2005): Region‐specific changes in prefrontal function with age: A review of PET and fMRI studies on working and episodic memory. Brain 128:1964–1983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Rodriguez M, Rodriguez‐Sabate C, Morales I, Sanchez A, Sabate M (2015): Parkinson's disease as a result of aging. Aging Cell 14:293–308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Rottschy C, Langner R, Dogan I, Reetz K, Laird AR, Schulz JB, Fox PT, Eickhoff SB (2012): Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate‐based meta‐analysis. NeuroImage 60:830–846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Rowe JB, Siebner HR (2012): The motor system and its disorders. NeuroImage 61:464–477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Sabatinelli D, Fortune EE, Li Q, Siddiqui A, Krafft C, Oliver WT, Beck S, Jeffries J (2011): Emotional perception: Meta‐analyses of face and natural scene processing. NeuroImage 54:2524–2533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Sala‐Llonch R, Bartrés‐Faz D, Junqué C (2015): Reorganization of brain networks in aging: A review of functional connectivity studies. Front Psychol 6:663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Gerraty RT, Ruparel K, Loughead J, Calkins ME, Eickhoff SB, Hakonarson H, Gur RC, Gur RE, Wolf DH (2013): An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting‐state functional connectivity data. NeuroImage 64:240–256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Schilbach L, Derntl B, Aleman A, Caspers S, Clos M, Diederen KMJ, Gruber O, Kogler L, Liemburg EJ, Sommer IE, Müller VI, Cieslik EC, Eickhoff SB (2016): Differential patterns of dysconnectivity in mirror neuron and mentalizing networks in Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull 1135–1148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Schilbach L, Müller VI, Hoffstaedter F, Clos M, Goya‐Maldonado R, Gruber O, Eickhoff SB (2014): Meta‐analytically informed network analysis of resting state fMRI reveals hyperconnectivity in an introspective socio‐affective network in Depression. PLoS One 9: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Schnack HG, Kahn RS (2016): Detecting neuroimaging biomarkers for psychiatric disorders: Sample size matters. Front Psychiatry 7:50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague R (1997): A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275:1593–1599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Seidler RD, Bernard JA, Burutolu TB, Fling BW, Gordon MT, Gwin JT, Kwak Y, Lipps DB (2010): Motor control and aging: Links to age‐related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:721–733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, Filippini N, Watkins KE, Toro R, Laird AR, Beckmann CF (2009): Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:13040–13045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim ASN (2009): The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta‐analysis. J Cogn Neurosci 21:489–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Su L, Wang L, Shen H, Feng G, Hu D (2013): Discriminative analysis of non‐linear brain connectivity in Schizophrenia: An fMRI study. Front Hum Neurosci 7: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Takahashi H, Koeda M, Oda K, Matsuda T, Matsushima E, Matsuura M, Asai K, Okubo Y (2004): An fMRI study of differential neural response to affective pictures in Schizophrenia. NeuroImage 22:1247–1254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Tang Y, Wang L, Cao F, Tan L (2012): Identify Schizophrenia using resting‐state functional connectivity: An exploratory research and analysis. Biomed Eng Online 11:50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Tessitore A, Giordano A, De Micco R, Russo A, Tedeschi G (2014): Sensorimotor connectivity in Parkinson's disease: The role of functional neuroimaging. Front Neurol 5: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Toda M, Abi‐Dargham A (2007): Dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: Making sense of it all. Curr Psychiatry Rep 9:329–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Tomlinson CL, Stowe R, Patel S, Rick C, Gray R, Clarke CE (2010): Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord off J Mov Disord Soc 25:2649–2653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. van Os J, Kapur S (2009): Schizophrenia. Lancet Lond Engl 374:635–645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Varikuti DP, Hoffstaedter F, Genon S, Schwender H, Reid AT, Eickhoff SB (2016): Resting‐state test‐retest reliability of a priori defined canonical networks over different preprocessing steps. Brain Struct Funct 222:1447–1468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Varoquaux G, Raamana PR, Engemann DA, Hoyos‐Idrobo A, Schwartz Y, Thirion B (2016): Assessing and tuning brain decoders: Cross‐validation, caveats, and guidelines. NeuroImage 145:166–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Vergun S, Deshpande AS, Meier TB, Song J, Tudorascu DL, Nair VA, Singh V, Biswal BB, Meyerand ME, Birn RM, Prabhakaran V (2013): Characterizing functional connectivity differences in aging adults using machine learning on resting state fMRI data. Front Comput Neurosci 7:38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Witt ST, Meyerand ME, Laird AR (2008): Functional neuroimaging correlates of finger tapping task variations: An ALE meta‐analysis. NeuroImage 42:343–356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Wolfers T, Buitelaar JK, Beckmann CF, Franke B, Marquand AF (2015): From estimating activation locality to predicting disorder: A review of pattern recognition for neuroimaging‐based psychiatric diagnostics. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 57:328–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Xia M, Wang J, He Y (2013): BrainNet Viewer: A network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS One 8:e68910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Yu Y, Shen H, Zhang H, Zeng L‐L, Xue Z, Hu D (2013): Functional connectivity‐based signatures of Schizophrenia revealed by multiclass pattern analysis of resting‐state fMRI from schizophrenic patients and their healthy siblings. Biomed Eng Online 12:10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supporting Information.


Articles from Human Brain Mapping are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES