Abstract
Background
Ethnic minority individuals have an increased risk of developing a psychotic disorder, particularly if they live in areas of ethnic segregation, or low own group ethnic density. The neurobiological mechanisms underlying this ethnic minority associated risk are unknown. We used functional MRI to investigate neural responses to faces of different ethnicity, in individuals of black ethnicity, and a control group of white British ethnicity individuals.
Methods
In total 20 individuals of black ethnicity, and 22 individuals of white British ethnicity underwent a 3T MRI scan while viewing faces of black and white ethnicity. Own group ethnic density was calculated from the 2011 census. Neighbourhood segregation was quantified using the Index of Dissimilarity method.
Results
At the within-group level, both groups showed greater right amygdala activation to outgroup faces. Between groups, the black ethnicity group showed greater right amygdala activation to white faces, compared to the white ethnicity group. Within the black ethnicity group, individuals living in areas of lower own group ethnic density showed greater right amygdala reactivity to white faces (r = −0.61, p = 0.01).
Conclusions
This is the first time an increased amygdala response to white faces has been demonstrated in individuals of black ethnicity. In the black ethnicity group, correlations were observed between amygdala response and neighbourhood variables associated with increased psychosis risk. These results may have relevance for our understanding of the increased rates of paranoia and psychotic disorders in ethnic minority individuals.
Key words: Amygdala, ethnic density, ethnicity, ethnic minority, fMRI, paranoia, psychosis, risk factors, schizophrenia
Background
Being a member of an ethnic minority group is one of the most well established environmental risk factors for schizophrenia (Cantor-graae et al. 2005; Bourque et al. 2011). Early research focused on the risk associated with migration (Ødegaard, 1932). It has subsequently become clear that this increased risk is not secondary to selective migration (Selten et al. 2002; van der Ven et al. 2015), and the effect persists among second-generation migrants, indicating that the risk is associated with being a member of a minority group, rather than the act of migration itself (Hutchinson et al. 1996; Bresnahan et al. 2007; Bourque et al. 2011). In various settings, the increased risk appears greatest for individuals of black ethnicity (Cantor-graae et al. 2005; Bresnahan et al. 2007). In the UK, individuals of black ethnicity were found to have incidence rate ratios compared with the white British population of between six and nine (Fearon et al. 2006). This risk also appears to be somewhat specific for psychosis. Although there is some evidence that affective disorders may have slightly increased incidence rates in first-generation immigrants, this appears to be driven by manic presentations, and there is no evidence of increased rates of depression or anxiety in the UK black ethnicity population (Shaw et al. 1999; Brugha et al. 2004; Weich et al. 2004; Swinnen & Selten, 2007; Mindlis & Boffetta, 2017). Similarly, in the USA a number of studies have reported reduced rates of depression and anxiety in African Americans (Kessler et al. 2003; Riolo et al. 2005; Breslau et al. 2006; Himle et al. 2009; Asnaani et al. 2010; Gibbs et al. 2013).
Interestingly, despite the fact that communities with high proportions of ethnic minority individuals are frequently the most deprived, living in an area where one's own ethnic group forms a larger proportion of the population is relatively protective against schizophrenia (Shaw et al. 2012; Kirkbride et al. 2014). One hypothesis is that higher own group ethnic density buffers the individual from the adverse psychiatric consequences of social isolation and racist experiences (Bécares et al. 2009). Epidemiological work has also suggested that greater levels of segregation between members of ethnic minority groups and the rest of the community may be linked to a greater incidence of psychotic disorders (Kirkbride et al. 2014).
Cognitive models propose that minority status is associated with greater levels of social threat (Combs et al. 2002; Morgan et al. 2010). However, the neurobiological mechanisms underlying both the increased risk of psychosis in ethnic minority groups, and the protective effects of increased own group ethnic density, are not known. One previous fMRI study investigated the neural correlates of the stress response in Turkish migrants living in Germany, and identified differences in anterior cingulate activation (Akdeniz et al. 2014). The amygdala is also an area of potential interest given it is a key component of the neural circuit processing threatening experiences (Chekroud et al. 2014; Fox et al. 2015; Underwood et al. 2016).
A greater right amygdala neural response to pictures of faces of people with an outgroup ethnicity, compared with those with in-group ethnicity, has been interpreted as representing threat appraisal (Chekroud et al. 2014). This outgroup effect has been consistently demonstrated in multiple studies of individuals of white ethnicity, but has not been thoroughly investigated in individuals of black ethnicity (Cunningham et al. 2004; Lieberman et al. 2005; Wheeler & Fiske, 2005; Ronquillo et al. 2007; Chekroud et al. 2014; Fox et al. 2015). In psychosis, meanwhile, amygdala hyperactivity has been linked to paranoid symptoms (Goghari et al. 2010; Pinkham et al. 2015; Underwood et al. 2016). Individuals from ethnic minority populations typically have much greater exposure to outgroup individuals than the native population. Amygdala hyperactivity to frequent outgroup exposure could therefore partially underlie the greater levels of paranoia seen in both ethnic minority patient and population samples (Bhugra et al. 2000; Combs et al. 2002; Cohen et al. 2004; Veling et al. 2007; Wickham et al. 2014), and potentially have relevance as regards the increased rates of psychotic illnesses in these groups.
In view of this evidence, we hypothesised that individuals who identified as being of black ethnicity would display a greater right amygdala response to white faces, than individuals who identified as being of white ethnicity; who would, in turn, show a greater response to black faces. Within the black ethnic minority group, we further hypothesised that increased amygdala reactivity to white faces would correlate with increased ethnic segregation, and decreased own group ethnic density, as these ethnicity related neighbourhood variables are associated with increased psychosis risk.
Methods
Participants
Healthy volunteers were recruited throughout the UK via newspaper, online and leaflet advertising, and in person recruitment, as part of a study examining environmental risk factors for psychosis. Participants were aged 18–45, with no history of mental illness, in good physical health, and with the capacity to give informed consent.
The black ethnic minority group needed to be a first or second generation migrant, to self-identify as being of black ethnicity, and were classified as of either black Caribbean or black African ethnicity based on either their own (first generation migrants) or their parents’ (second generation migrants) country of origin. The white British ethnicity group needed to self-identify as being of white British ethnicity and have at least one parent of UK nationality. The white British ethnicity group was age-matched within 5 years to the black ethnic minority group. All subjects provided written informed consent after the study had been fully explained.
Sociodemographic assessment
Population density, and own group ethnic density as a fraction of the total population were obtained from the 2011 census; Indices of Multiple Deprivation (where a lower ranking equates to a more deprived area) were obtained from 2015 data (English Indices of Deprivation, 2015). Values were obtained for each participant's current statistical lower layer super output area (LSOA, geographical areas with populations of roughly 1500 individuals).
In order to quantify ethnic segregation, the Index of Dissimilarity method was used to calculate an index of segregation at the ward-level, using census output areas as the smaller geographic unit. A score of 0 is equivalent to no segregation, while a score of 1 is equivalent to complete segregation (see online Supplementary Methods for further information) (Yalonetzky, 2015). Own group ethnic density and ethnic segregation were both calculated with participants assigned to either black African, black Caribbean or white British ethnicity.
fMRI task
An event-related, implicit face perception task was employed based on a previously reported experimental design (see Fig. 1) (Cunningham et al. 2004). During the task, participants indicated whether a visual stimulus appeared to the left or right of a fixation cross. The stimuli consisted of either black or white male faces presented for 30 ms (short trials) or 525 ms (long trials) or a white square also presented for 30 or 525 ms. Faces were taken from the Stanford University faces database (https://stanforduniversity.qualtrics.com/SE/?SID=SV_aX0ovSkASZR9Py4), 18 faces of each ethnicity were chosen and matched on ratings of attractiveness, stereotypicality and age. In order to mask the short duration stimuli, all stimuli were preceded by an abstract picture for 30 ms and followed by the same picture for either 525 ms (short trials) or 30 ms (long trials). As a result, all trials lasted a total of 585 ms. During the short trials, the participant's experience was of seeing an abstract picture and they were not aware of the implicitly presented face. We checked this after the experiment and no participant reported seeing a face during the presentation of these stimuli. During the long trials, they saw either a white square or a face. A fixation cross appeared for 2055 ms between trials. Each face trial was followed by four white square trials giving an interstimulus interval between faces ranging from 10.62 to 11.12 s. Participants completed three runs. During each run participants would be presented with six short trials of black faces, six long trials of black faces, six short trials of white faces and six long trials of white faces, shown in a random order without replacement.
Data acquisition
Imaging data were acquired using a Philips 3T Intera magnetic resonance imaging system. Functional imaging consisted of T2* weighted transverse echo planar image (EPI) slices. Each run consisted of 161 volumes, collected in an ascending direction, with 2.19 mm × 2.19 mm voxel dimensions in the plane, repetition time (TR) – 2000 ms, and echo time (TE) –30 ms.
The EPI scans were followed by a gradient-echo scan (TR = 9.7 ms, TE = 4.6 ms, flip angle –9°, slice thickness 1.20 mm, 0.94 × 0.94 mm2 in-plane resolution, 150 slices).
fMRI data analysis
Analysis of fMRI data was undertaken using SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) in Matlab 7.9. The first two volumes for each participant were discarded so as to avoid T1 equilibration effects. Slice timing correction was applied to each volume. Spatial realignment to the third volume was followed by coregistration of each participant's functional and anatomical data. Images were then normalised into standard MNI space using the normalization parameters estimated by T1 structural image unified segmentation. Smoothing of the resampled images (3 mm × 3 mm × 3 mm) was with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full-width-half-maximum.
First level analysis using the general linear model included six conditions (short and long presentations of the white square, white faces, and black faces), and individual events were convolved with the canonical haemodynamic response function. The model included the six movement parameters from realignment, and six-volume to volume movement parameters, as regressors of no interest, additionally any volumes where between volume movement was greater than 0.5 mm were removed. We excluded any runs with extended shifts of over 2 mm. Participants were included in the second level analysis if they had at least two usable runs. First level contrasts were constructed between faces and white squares.
The second level within-group analyses were performed using paired t tests. The second level between groups analyses were performed using independent samples t tests, and a 2 × 2 flexible factorial ANOVA implemented in SPM8. Following from a priori hypothesis, region of interest (ROI) analyses were conducted for the amygdala using Marsbar (http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/) and the included Automated Anatomical Labelling ROI library. As per SPM conventions t test p values are reported as one-tailed.
Statistical analysis
Analyses were carried out using SPSS for Macintosh version 23.0. After exclusion of outliers of more than 1.5 interquartile ranges below or above the 1st or 3rd quartiles, continuous variables were assessed for normality using the Shapiro–Wilks test. Differences between group means were assessed using an independent samples t test for normally distributed variables, after using Levene's test to check for equality of variances. Mann–Whitney U tests were used for non-normally distributed variables. Between-group differences in terms of the magnitude of the amygdala outgroup effect, was tested with a two-tailed independent samples t test. Correlations between normally distributed variables were assessed using Pearson's product moment coefficient, and were only reported if they remained significant after removal of outliers defined as a Cook's d of >n/4. Correlations involving non-normally distributed variables were assessed using Spearman's rank correlation coefficient. All correlations were two-tailed with p < 0.05 defined as significant. Neighbourhood variables found on bivariate testing to show a significant correlation with right amygdala response were subsequently entered into a stepwise regression with right amygdala response as the dependent variable.
Data availability
Imaging and demographic data are available from the corresponding author on request.
Results
Sociodemographic and neighbourhood variables
A total of 42 individuals were included in the study (black ethnic minority group n = 20, white British ethnicity group n = 22). There were no significant differences between groups in terms of sex or age. In terms of neighbourhood variables the black ethnic minority group had significantly lower own group ethnic density percentages and lived in significantly more deprived, less segregated, and more densely populated areas (demographics of the entire sample are reported in online Supplementary Table S1, while details of those included in the imaging analysis are described in Table 1). In the black ethnicity group, participant characteristics in terms of age, gender, and deprivation were similar to UK averages. In the white ethnicity group, the study population was younger compared with the UK median age but otherwise similar (see online Supplementary Material).
Table 1.
Black ethnic minority (n = 17) | White British ethnicity (n = 19) | P | |
---|---|---|---|
Age (years), median (IQR) | 24.5 (20.8–32.2) | 24.0 (22.5–28.1) | 0.98a |
Sex n (%) | |||
Male | 7 (41) | 9 (50) | 0.71b |
Female | 10 (59) | 10 (53) | |
Migration n (%) | |||
1st generation | 7 (41) | ||
2nd generation | 10 (59) | ||
Black Caribbean, n (%) | 3 (18) | ||
Black African, n (%) | 14 (82) | ||
% own ethnicity, median (IQR) | 9.1 (6.0–24.6) | 86.3 (77.2–95.4) | <0.001a |
Index of multiple deprivation rank/1000, median (IQR) | 7.2 (5.0–13.5) | 22.8 (19.0–31.4) | <0.001a |
Index of segregation, median (IQR) | 0.28 (0.18–0.31) | 0.32 (0.28–0.42) | 0.008a |
Population density: persons per hectare, median (IQR) | 91.7 (59.1–126.7) | 38.6 (1.9–96.1) | 0.005a |
Mann–Whitney.
χ2 test.
Within the black ethnic minority group, own group ethnic density negatively correlated with the degree of segregation (rs = −0.809, p < 0.001) (correlations between other neighbourhood variables are described in the online Supplementary Data).
Functional imaging results
Three participants from each group were excluded due to excessive movement or failed scans. For the 30ms presentations neither black nor white faces elicited significant right amygdala activation in either the white British or black ethnic minority groups (all contrasts against the white square baseline p > 0.05). In addition, there were no significant differences between groups, or within groups between stimuli, for the 30 ms presentations. All results reported below are for 525 ms presentations.
At the within-group level, both groups individually showed greater right amygdala activation to outgroup faces (white British ethnicity group t = 1.90, p = 0.036; black ethnic minority group t = 2.38, p = 0.015) (see Fig. 2). This was reflected in a significant group × task interaction (F = 7.85, p = 0.008) (see Fig. 2). The magnitude of the outgroup effect was not significantly different between groups (t = 1.1, p = 0.3).
Between groups, compared with the white British ethnicity group, the black ethnic minority group showed significantly greater right amygdala activation to white faces (t = 1.84, p = 0.038), and significantly lower right amygdala activation to black faces (t = 1.80, p = 0.040) (see Fig. 2).
Whole brain exploratory analyses were conducted to investigate the potential involvement of other regions in the effects and to enable comparisons with studies investigating other regions (peak threshold p < 0.001, cluster threshold p < 0.05 FWE corrected). No significant results were observed for the between groups, or group × task interaction analyses. For the black ethnicity group, white faces > black faces contrast one small cluster was observed in the right cerebellum (p = 0.007, cluster size −76 voxels, coordinates x = 33, y = −58, z = −26).
Relationships between imaging results and neighbourhood variables
Within the black ethnic minority group right amygdala response to white faces significantly correlated with living in an area of greater ethnic segregation (rp = 0.831, p < 0.001) and lower own group ethnic density (rs = −0.611, p = 0.009) (see Fig. 3 and online Supplementary Material for results using alternative measures of ethnic density). The right amygdala response also positively correlated with greater population density (rp = 0.627, p = 0.002) and lower levels of deprivation (rs = 0.601, p = 0.011). These findings survive Bonferroni correction accounting for the four separate tests (p < 0.0125). All four neighbourhood variables were entered into a stepwise regression with right amygdala response as the dependent variable; from this only own group ethnic density remained a significant predictor (r = 0.689, p = 0.002, see Table 2). In the white ethnicity group, no significant correlations were observed between neighbourhood variables and right amygdala response to black faces.
Table 2.
Variable | B | SE | Beta | T | P | R2 | P |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model A | |||||||
Constant | 3.44 | 6.41 | 0.537 | 0.601 | 0.571 | 0.027 | |
Own group ethnic density | −0.172 | 0.164 | −0.399 | −1.05 | 0.315 | ||
Index of segregation | −12.5 | 12.5 | −0.283 | −0.996 | 0.339 | ||
Own group ethnic density | 0.032 | 0.029 | 0.345 | 1.11 | 0.288 | ||
Index of multiple deprivation | <0.001 | <0.001 | 0.363 | 1.36 | 0.199 | ||
Model B | |||||||
Constant | 7.52 | 1.37 | 5.51 | <0.001 | 0.475 | 0.002 | |
Own group ethnic density | −0.297 | 0.081 | −0.689 | −3.68 | 0.002 |
Model A includes all neighbourhood variables. Model B includes only ‘own group ethnicity’ as determined by a stepwise regression.
Discussion
We demonstrated increased amygdala activation to white faces in a group of individuals of black ethnicity – both within group compared with black faces and between groups when compared with white British individuals. The white British ethnicity group showed a greater amygdala response to black faces both within, and between the group. In addition, within the black ethnic minority group, a stepwise regression including neighbourhood variables that correlated with amygdala reactivity, found that lower own group ethnic density was significantly associated with greater amygdala reactivity to white faces.
Our finding of increased right amygdala reactivity to black faces in the white British ethnicity group has been repeatedly demonstrated in white ethnicity participants (Cunningham et al. 2004; Lieberman et al. 2005; Wheeler & Fiske, 2005; Ronquillo et al. 2007; Chekroud et al. 2014). This is, however, to our knowledge the first time that increased reactivity to white faces has been unambiguously demonstrated in individuals of black ethnicity. One previous study demonstrating greater amygdala activity to outgroup faces included four participants of black ethnicity but did not comment on this subgroup specifically (Hart et al. 2000). A study by Lieberman et al. found that African-American ethnic minority individuals showed an increased amygdala response while viewing black faces (Lieberman et al. 2005). This study, however, included only nine African-American subjects and used a different task that focused on differences in perceptual and verbal encoding, which may explain the inconsistency with our findings. While our findings suggest a general out-group effect, this finding may potentially have a greater psychological impact for ethnic minority groups given their generally greater exposure to outgroups.
This is also the first time that amygdala reactivity has been linked to neighbourhood variables related to ethnic minority associated risk for psychosis. Living in areas of low own group ethnic density has been shown to increase the risk of psychosis in ethnic minorities (Bosqui et al. 2014). Our results extend these findings by indicating that lower own group ethnic density is also linked to greater amygdala response to outgroup faces in black individuals. Living in an environment where one is more isolated from one's own ethnic group (i.e. an area of low own group ethnic density), could conceivably contribute to outgroup individuals being perceived as a greater threat. Taken with evidence that experiences of racism are correlated with amygdala activation to white faces in black individuals (Greer et al. 2012), this suggests that environmental exposures are associated with functional alterations in the brain circuits involved in threat processing. While a number of our results were significant when examining the bilateral amygdala (see online Supplementary Material), our findings were driven by the right amygdala. The right amygdala appears to be particularly implicated in the processing of race, although the underlying reasons for this remain unclear (Chekroud et al. 2014).
Methodological considerations
We investigated a group of black ethnic minority individuals who were first or second generation migrants relative to the white British ethnic majority. This group appears to display the greatest increase in the incidence of psychosis, compared with white British individuals (Fearon et al. 2006; Coid et al. 2008). However, it should be recognised that other ethnic groups, including non-British white individuals, and Asian individuals, have increased rates of psychosis as well, and migrants may be exposed to other adverse experiences in addition to those associated with being a member of a minority group (Fearon et al. 2006; Coid et al. 2008). Ethnic minority status itself is likely a crude proxy for multiple complex, interacting factors that cumulatively act to increase the risk of psychosis. Our study used a convenience sampling procedure and future work should determine how representative the sample is of the population in general.
Our finding of an association between ethnicity related neighbourhood variables and amygdala reactivity suggests that this mechanism may have some relevance for our understanding of the neurobiological basis of ethnic minority associated psychosis risk. This, however, is a speculative interpretation, and the current study does not establish causality. Capturing the relationship between amygdala functioning and real-world experiences of being in a minority group could potentially be accomplished using experience sampling methodologies (Gevonden et al. 2016; Reininghaus et al. 2016).
Study participants were intentionally unaffected by mental illness, which precluded investigating direct associations with psychopathology. As our participants did not have a mental illness it is important to recognise the possibility that our findings may be interpreted as physiologically appropriate or neurobiological correlates of psychological resilience. However, the fact that in patients with schizophrenia greater amygdala responses are associated with paranoid symptoms suggests this may not be the case (Goghari et al. 2010; Pinkham et al. 2015; Underwood et al. 2016). Amygdala dysfunction is implicated in a wide range of disorders, so even if aberrations identified in the current study have pathoaetiological relevance, it may not be specific to psychotic disorders. Furthermore, drawing parallels with patient groups is limited by the fact that our task specifically measured differences in amygdala reactivity to faces of differing ethnicities, whereas most probes used in patient populations tend to involve emotion processing. Investigating amygdala response to faces of differing ethnicity in patient populations would assist with determining if this mechanism is linked to psychopathology.
When relating neighbourhood variables to amygdala response we were primarily interested in the influence of own group ethnic density, and ethnic segregation. We additionally measured population density and deprivation ranking. The stepwise regression resulted in a model only including own group ethnic density. Nearly all these variables, however, are strongly correlated with one another and thus determining which play mediating as opposed to confounding roles is not possible in the current study. Larger sample sizes would allow for the investigation of interactions between variables, while replication in other settings would establish whether the degree to which the effect is generalizable. Additionally, while the link between psychosis risk and own group ethnic density is well established, the evidence for the influence of ethnic segregation is less clear (Kirkbride et al. 2007, 2014).
We did not observe significant between or within group differences, for the 30 ms stimuli presentations. This is surprising given that this subliminal presentation has previously been found to evoke greater amygdala responses to outgroup faces than the 525 ms presentation (Cunningham et al. 2004). Potential reasons underlying this discrepancy may include differences in masking stimuli between the current and previous study, or differences as regards image characteristics such as levels of brightness and contrast which subliminal stimuli may be particularly sensitive to.
Implications for understanding the mechanisms underlying ethnic minority associated risk for psychosis
Cognitive models of psychosis propose an increased level of social threat anticipation as part of a pathway to the development of persecutory beliefs (Bentall & Fernyhough, 2008). Of note, a higher degree of paranoid symptomatology has been described in ethnic minority groups, both in patient (Bhugra et al. 2000; Veling et al. 2007), and healthy populations (Combs et al. 2002; Cohen et al. 2004; Wickham et al. 2014). In the current study, both the black ethnic minority group and the white British ethnicity group displayed increased amygdala reactivity to outgroup faces. The consequence for each group, however, may be different given that ethnic minorities typically have much greater outgroup contact in day-to-day life. Following from this, if the increased amygdala response to outgroup faces is interpreted as a marker of threat, this suggests that ethnic minority individuals have more frequent threat experience.
Although amygdala hyperactivity has been linked to paranoid symptoms in psychosis (Goghari et al. 2010; Pinkham et al. 2015; Underwood et al. 2016), reactivity during facial processing is not consistently raised in schizophrenia (Taylor et al. 2012). The black ethnic minority group in the current study did not show overall increased amygdala reactivity, only to specific stimuli. A differential sensitivity to various stimuli may partially explain the variability of findings observed in studies of amygdala function in schizophrenia.
Conclusions
Amygdala reactivity to white faces is increased in black ethnic minority individuals, and correlated with measures of own group ethnic density and segregation. These findings indicate that black ethnic minority individuals show the same response to outgroup faces seen in white ethnic majority groups, and also suggest that in this population ethnic segregation and lower own group ethnic density is associated with greater amygdala reactivity to outgroup faces.
Acknowledgements
O.H. has received investigator-initiated research funding from and/or participated in advisory/speaker meetings organised by Astra-Zeneca, Autifony, BMS, Eli Lilly, Heptares, Jansenn, Lundbeck, Lyden-Delta, Otsuka, Servier, Sunovion, Rand and Roche. Neither Dr Howes or his family have been employed by or have holdings/ a financial stake in any biomedical company. M.M. has consulted for Cambridge Cognition, Lundbeck and Forum Pharmaceuticals in the past 3 years. He has also received research funding from Takeda, Eli Lilly and Roche. Medical Research Council and Wellcome trust. This study was funded by Medical Research Council-UK (no. MC-A656-5QD30), Maudsley Charity (no. 666), Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, and Wellcome Trust (no. 094849/Z/10/Z) grants to O.H. and the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London. R.M.’s work is supported by the Wellcome Trust (no. 200102/Z/15/Z). T.D.’s work is supported by a EU-FP7 MC-ITN IN-SENS grant (no. 607616). The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.
Declaration of interest
The other authors declare no competing financial interests.
Author contributions
R.M. designed and performed experiments, analysed data, and wrote the paper. M.B designed and performed experiments, and reviewed the manuscript. T.D. and M.Q. performed experiments and reviewed the paper. S.T. designed experiments and reviewed the manuscript. M.M. analysed data and reviewed the paper. O.H. obtained funding, designed the study and wrote the paper.
Supplementary material
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003506.
References
- Akdeniz C, Tost H, Streit F, Haddad L, Wüst S, Schäfer A et al. (2014) Neuroimaging evidence for a role of neural social stress processing in ethnic minority–associated environmental risk. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asnaani A, Richey JA, Dimaite R, Hinton DE and Hofmann SG (2010) A cross-ethnic comparison of lifetime prevalence rates of anxiety disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 198, 551–555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bécares L, Nazroo J and Stafford M (2009) The buffering effects of ethnic density on experienced racism and health. Health and Place 15, 670–678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bentall RP and Fernyhough C (2008) Social predictors of psychotic experiences: specificity and psychological mechanisms. Schizophrenia Bulletin 34, 1012–1020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bhugra D, Hilwig M, Corridon B, Neehall J, Rudge S, Mallett R et al. (2000) A comparison of symptoms in cases with first onset of schizophrenia across four groups. European Journal of Psychiatry 14, 241–250. [Google Scholar]
- Bosqui TJ, Hoy K and Shannon C (2014) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the ethnic density effect in psychotic disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 49, 519–529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourque F, van der Ven E and Malla A (2011) A meta-analysis of the risk for psychotic disorders among first- and second-generation immigrants. Psychological Medicine 41, 897–910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breslau J, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Kendler KS, Su M, Williams D and Kessler RC (2006) Specifying race-ethnic differences in risk for psychiatric disorder in a USA national sample. Psychological Medicine 36, 57–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bresnahan M, Begg MD, Brown A, Schaefer C, Sohler N, Insel B et al. (2007) Race and risk of schizophrenia in a US birth cohort: another example of health disparity? International Journal of Epidemiology 36, 751–758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brugha T, Jenkins R, Bebbington P, Meltzer H, Lewis G, Farrell M et al. (2004) Risk factors and the prevalence of neurosis and psychosis in ethnic groups in Great Britain. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 39, 939–946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cantor-graae E Ph D and Selten J (2005) Schizophrenia and migration: a meta-analysis and review. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 12–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chekroud AM, Everett JAC, Bridge H and Hewstone M (2014) A review of neuroimaging studies of race-related prejudice: does amygdala response reflect threat? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8, 179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen CI, Magai C, Yaffee R and Walcott-Brown L (2004) Racial differences in paranoid ideation and psychoses in an older urban population. American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 864–871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coid JW, Kirkbride JB, Barker D, Cowden F, Stamps R, Yang M et al. (2008) Raised incidence rates of all psychoses among migrant groups. Archives of General Psychiatry 65, 1250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Combs DR, Penn DL and Fenigstein A (2002) Ethnic differences in subclinical paranoia: an expansion of norms of the paranoia scale. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology 8, 248–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham WA, Johnson MK, Raye CL, Chris Gatenby J, Gore JC and Banaji MR (2004) Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science 15, 806–813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- English Indices of Deprivation (2015) (http://www.gov.uk/government/statistics/english-indices-of-deprivation-2015) (n.d.).
- Fearon P, Kirkbride JB, Morgan C, Dazzan P, Morgan K, Lloyd T et al. (2006) Incidence of schizophrenia and other psychoses in ethnic minority groups: results from the MRC AESOP study. Psychological Medicine 36, 1541–1550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox AS, Oler JA, Tromp DPM, Fudge JL and Kalin NH (2015) Extending the amygdala in theories of threat processing. Trends in Neurosciences 38, 319–329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gevonden M, Myin-Germeys I, Wichers M, Booij J, van den Brink W, van Winkel R et al. (2016) Reactivity to social stress in ethnic minority men. Psychiatry Research 246, 629–636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs TA, Okuda M, Oquendo MA, Lawson WB, Wang S, Thomas YF et al. (2013) Mental health of African Americans and Caribbean blacks in the United States: results from the national epidemiological survey on alcohol and related conditions. American Journal of Public Health 103, 330–338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goghari VM, Sponheim SR and MacDonald AW (2010) The functional neuroanatomy of symptom dimensions in schizophrenia: a qualitative and quantitative review of a persistent question. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34, 468–486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greer TM, Vendemia JMC and Stancil M (2012) Neural correlates of race-related social evaluations for African Americans and white Americans. Neuropsychology 26, 704–712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart AJ, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, Mcinerney SC, Fischer H and Rauch SL (2000) Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs ingroup face stimuli. Neuroreport 11, 2351–2355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Himle JA, Baser RE, Joseph R, Denise R and Jackson JS (2009) Anxiety disorders among African Americans, blacks of Caribbean descent, and non-hispanic whites in the United States. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23, 578–590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutchinson G, Takei N, Fahy TA, Bhugra D, Gilvarry C, Moran P et al. (1996) Morbid risk of schizophrenia in first-degree relatives of white and African-Caribbean patients with psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry 169, 776–780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Koretz D, Merikangas KR et al. (2003) The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the national comorbidity survey replication (NCS-R). JAMA 289, 3095–3105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkbride JB, Jones PB, Ullrich S and Coid JW (2014) Social deprivation, inequality, and the neighborhood-level incidence of psychotic syndromes in East London. Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, 169–180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkbride JB, Morgan C, Fearon P, Dazzan P, Murray RM and Jones PB (2007) Neighbourhood-level effects on psychoses: re-examining the role of context. Psychological Medicine 37, 1413–1425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman MD, Hariri A, Jarcho JM, Eisenberger NI and Bookheimer SY (2005) An fMRI investigation of race-related amygdala activity in African-American and Caucasian-American individuals. Nature Neuroscience 8, 720–722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mindlis I and Boffetta P (2017) Mood disorders in first- and second-generation immigrants: systematic review and meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry 210, 182–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan C, Charalambides M, Hutchinson G and Murray RM (2010) Migration, ethnicity, and psychosis: toward a sociodevelopmental model. Schizophrenia Bulletin 36, 655–664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ødegaard Ø (1932) Emigration and Insanity: A Study of Mental Disease among the Norwegian born Population of Minnesota. Acta Psychiatr Neurol Scand Suppl 4, 1–206. [Google Scholar]
- Pinkham AE, Liu P, Lu H, Kriegsman M, Simpson C and Tamminga C (2015) Amygdala hyperactivity at rest in paranoid individuals with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 172, 784–792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reininghaus U, Kempton MJ, Valmaggia L, Craig TKJ, Garety P, Onyejiaka A et al. (2016) Stress sensitivity, aberrant salience, and threat anticipation in early psychosis: an experience sampling study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 712–722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riolo SA, Nguyen TA, Greden JF and King CA (2005) Prevalence of depression by race/ethnicity: findings from the national health and nutrition examination survey III. American Journal of Public Health 95, 998–1000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ronquillo J, Denson TF, Lickel B, Lu ZL, Nandy A and Maddox KB (2007) The effects of skin tone on race-related amygdala activity: an fMRI investigation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2, 39–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Selten J-P, Cantor-Graae E, Slaets J and Kahn RS (2002) Ødegaard's selection hypothesis revisited: schizophrenia in Surinamese immigrants to The Netherlands. The American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 669–671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw CM, Creed F, Tomenson B, Riste L and Cruickshank JK (1999) Prevalence of anxiety and depressive illness and help seeking behaviour in African Caribbeans and white Europeans: two phase general population survey. British Medical Journal 318, 302–306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw RJ, Atkin K, Becares L, Albor CB, Stafford M, Kiernan KE et al. (2012) Impact of ethnic density on adult mental disorders: narrative review. British Journal of Psychiatry 201, 11–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swinnen SGHA and Selten J (2007) Mood disorders and migration. British Journal of Psychiatry 190, 6–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SF, Kang J, Brege IS, Tso IF, Hosanagar A and Johnson TD (2012) Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of emotion perception and experience in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 71, 136–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Underwood R, Kumari V and Peters E (2016) Cognitive and neural models of threat appraisal in psychosis: a theoretical integration. Psychiatry Research 239, 131–138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Ven E, Dalman C, Wicks S, Allebeck P, Magnusson C, van Os J et al. (2015) Testing Ødegaard's selective migration hypothesis: a longitudinal cohort study of risk factors for non-affective psychotic disorders among prospective emigrants. Psychological Medicine 45, 727–734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veling W, Selten JP, Mackenbach JP and Hoek HW (2007) Symptoms at first contact for psychotic disorder: comparison between native Dutch and ethnic minorities. Schizophrenia Research 95, 30–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weich S, Nazroo J, Sproston K, McManus S, Blanchard M, Erens B et al. (2004) Common mental disorders and ethnicity in England: the EMPIRIC study. Psychological Medicine 34, 1543–1551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wheeler ME and Fiske ST (2005) Controlling racial prejudice: social-cognitive goals affect amygdala and stereotype activation. Psychological Science 16, 56–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wickham S, Taylor P, Shevlin M and Bentall RP (2014) The impact of social deprivation on paranoia, hallucinations, mania and depression: the role of discrimination social support, stress and trust. PLoS ONE 9, e105140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yalonetzky G (2015) Index of dissimilarity In The Wiley Blackwell Encyclopedia of Race, Ethnicity, and Nationalism (ed. Stone John), Oxford, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, pp. 1–3. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003506.
Data Availability Statement
Imaging and demographic data are available from the corresponding author on request.