Abstract
Top-down modulation of sensory processing is a critical neural mechanism subserving numerous important cognitive roles, one of which may be to inform lower-order sensory systems of the current ‘task at hand’ by conveying behavioral context to these systems. Accumulating evidence indicates that top-down cortical influences are carried by directed interareal synchronization of oscillatory neuronal populations, with recent results pointing to beta-frequency oscillations as particularly important for top-down processing. However, it remains to be determined if top-down beta-frequency oscillations indeed convey behavioral context. We measured spectral Granger Causality (sGC) using local field potentials recorded from microelectrodes chronically implanted in visual areas V1/V2, V4, and TEO of two rhesus macaque monkeys, and applied multivariate pattern analysis to the spatial patterns of top-down sGC. We decoded behavioral context by discriminating patterns of top-down (V4/TEO-to-V1/V2) beta-peak sGC for two different task rules governing correct responses to identical visual stimuli. The results indicate that top-down directed influences are carried to visual cortex by beta oscillations, and differentiate task demands even before visual stimulus processing. They suggest that top-down beta-frequency oscillatory processes coordinate processing of sensory information by conveying global knowledge states to early levels of the sensory cortical hierarchy independently of bottom-up stimulus-driven processing.
Introduction
Perception is not driven solely by sensory stimulation. Rather, endogenous processing actively modulates and routes sensory input based on prior knowledge, adapts perception to satisfy task demands, and solves ambiguities in the sensory stream1–3. Mounting evidence indicates that oscillatory activity conveys information between visual cortical areas4–6. Anatomical studies show that cortical areas are linked via unique patterns of cortical projections and terminations between the cortical laminae that define the cortical hierarchy7–9. Recent studies have revealed that information transfer across the cortical hierarchy occurs in unique frequency regimes, with gamma frequency rhythms subserving bottom-up (feedforward) information transfer, while beta rhythms mediate transfer in the reverse (top-down) direction10–16. Furthermore, top-down beta frequency influences may directly affect stimulus-related processing, as indicated by recent studies demonstrating that the magnitude of top-down beta-frequency rhythms is increased to the hemisphere representing an attended stimulus, resulting in enhanced bottom-up processing of the attended stimulus12,14,15. Top-down beta-frequency synchronization may play a general role in behavior by conveying moment-to-moment task demands of the organism to lower level sensory systems in order to maintain global knowledge states5,17. Specifically, top-down beta rhythms may mediate the interaction between endogenously generated top-down information, variously described as hypotheses, prior knowledge, or attentional locus, and stimulus-generated bottom-up information5,15,18. Consequently, we hypothesize that behavioral context is encoded in the pattern of top-down beta synchronization in visual cortex.
We tested this hypothesis in two macaque monkeys performing a visual discrimination task (Fig. 1), in which the behavioral context was determined by the task rule governing the correct response to each visual stimulus, the rule being randomly varied across trial blocks. Local field potentials were chronically recorded from microelectrodes posterior to the lunate sulcus (areas V1/V2) and extrastriate areas V4 and TEO. Both top-down (V4/TEO-to-V1/V2) and bottom-up (V1/V2-to-V4/TEO) directed synchrony were quantified using spectral Granger Causality in a stationary time period when the animal could anticipate the visual stimulus, but before it was presented. In this way, top-down influences were isolated from any confounding effects of stimulus processing. We successfully decoded behavioral context at a level significantly exceeding chance (50%) in both monkeys (76% and 82%) by two-class multivariate pattern analysis, using the spatial pattern of prestimulus top-down beta synchrony directed from V4/TEO to V1/V2 as the classification feature.
The results of this study indicate that the endogenous pattern of top-down beta influence from V4/TEO carries task-specific information to early visual cortex (V1/V2). We conclude from this that beta oscillations adaptively coordinate the processing of sensory information by conveying behavioral context to early levels of the sensory cortical hierarchy. We infer that behavioral context is conveyed from higher to lower levels of visual cortex by beta-frequency synchronized oscillations because pattern classification analysis successfully discriminated different visuomotor contingencies using higher-to-lower-level beta-frequency visual cortical influences as classification features. Our results thus support the notion that top-down beta-frequency oscillations play a general role in mediating the interaction between high-level cognitive processing and stimulus-related activity.
Materials and Methods
Task
Two well-trained adult rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) monkeys (M1 and M2) performed a go/no-go visual pattern discrimination task (Fig. 1). The stimulus set contained four patterns (each belonging to one of two categories): two “lines” and two “diamonds”. The task rule determined the stimulus-response contingency that governed whether the correct behavioral response to a visual stimulus pattern type (line or diamond) was go or no-go. It was randomly reversed across trial blocks. Control of behavioral context was achieved by (randomly) changing the task rule.
The stimuli were displayed on a screen at a distance of 57 centimeters from the eyes of the subject. Each of the four stimuli consisted of four solid white dots (0.9 degrees visual angle per side), with two of the dots arranged diagonally on opposite corners of an outer square (six degrees visual angle), and the other two dots arranged diagonally on the opposite corners of an inner square (two degrees visual angle) (Fig. 1). Line stimuli were patterns where the dots on the outer and inner squares were slanted in the same direction, while diamond stimuli had outer and inner dots slanted in opposing directions. Although the V1 recording sites were expected to have a precise retinotopic relation with the stimulus dots, V1 retinotopic mapping was not available for these monkeys. However, the design of the stimulus ensured that categorization could not be accomplished by observing any single dot, and that the total area, contrast, edge length, and brightness were constant across all stimulus types.
The mean level of correct performance was 92.4 +/− 3.9% for M1 (18 sessions, minimum 84%, maximum 97%) and 95.7 +/− 3.3% for M2 (19 sessions, minimum 89%, maximum 99%). Each trial was initiated when the monkey engaged a lever with the dominant hand. When the lever was depressed and maintained in the depressed position, the trial commenced. After initiation of the trial by the lever press, there was a random period of 200–1215 ms before the appearance of the visual stimulus. The visual stimulus was displayed for 100 ms followed by a 400 ms window during which the monkey was required to release the lever on go trials, or maintain lever pressure on no-go trials. Correct go responses were rewarded.
Electrophysiological recordings
In both M1 and M2, local field potentials (LFPs), which index the local synaptic activity of the neuronal population at a recording site19, were differentially recorded from bipolar Teflon-coated platinum-iridium microelectrodes (more advanced tip near the boundary between the gray and white matter, less advanced tip at the pial surface) chronically implanted at up to 16 cortical sites in the hemisphere contralateral to the dominant hand (for further details see20). The bipolar microelectrodes were composed of 0.125 mm diameter wires having 2.5 mm tip separation. Electrode positions were verified in both monkeys by postmortem visual inspection and magnetic resonance imaging of M1. New to the present study, recordings were from areas in the ventral visual stream corresponding to V1/V2, V4, and the temporal occipital area (TEO). Recording sites posterior to the lunate sulcus corresponded to V1/V2, while sites within the prelunate gyrus (V4) and posterior inferotemporal cortex (TEO) were designated as extrastriate cortex. Recordings from M1 were from three V1/V2 recording sites, and three extrastriate recording sites. Of these three extrastriate sites, two were in area V4 and one was in TEO. All recording sites were posterior to the posterior middle temporal sulcus. Recordings from M2 were from three V1/V2 recording sites, and one extrastriate site lying in area V4. The LFP from each bipolar recording electrode was differentially recorded, amplified, and band-passed filtered (−6 dB at 1 and 100 Hz, 6 dB per octave falloff) using a Grass model P511J amplifier, and digitized (12 bits/sample at 200 samples/sec). Differential recording reduced the common contributions to the two electrode tips by more than 10000 times, thus excluding propagated fields from more than a few millimeters away and localizing activity to the tissue between the tips of the bipolar electrode. All experiments were performed by Dr. Richard Nakamura at the Laboratory of Neuropsychology of the National Institute for Mental Health. Animal care and recordings were approved and in accordance with National Institute of Mental Health guidelines at the time. Surgical methods were as previously described20.
The data used in this report were recorded during multiple daily sessions spanning several months, and have not previously been studied. One session was recorded from each monkey per day with a typical recording session composed of 1000 trials. The study employed 18 and 19 sessions from M1 and M2, respectively. Visual and automated artifact rejection reduced the number of correct trials (including both go and no-go responses) available for spectral analysis to 10178 and 8267 for M1 and M2, respectively. An analysis of interactions between other cortical areas in the same monkeys was previously published21. However, the current report represents a new analysis of interactions between visual cortical areas. Since interactions between extrastriate cortex and V1/V2 are known to be hierarchical, this study used these regions to investigate cortical top-down and bottom-up influences.
Data acquisition began 85 msec before stimulus onset. Data were not recorded immediately following the lever press due to data storage limitations, and so it was not possible to analyze the temporal evolution of changes taking place between the lever press and the stimulus. Neural activity evoked by the stimulus was absent during the prestimulus period20.
Experimental design and statistical analysis
Analyses were conducted on two adult rhesus macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta). The task design ensured each monkey was unaware of the identity of the impending stimulus, such that cortical processing related exclusively to the task stimulus-response contingency of a given block could be analyzed during the pre-stimulus period. Statistical analysis consisted of non-parametric randomization tests or when necessary, estimates of the standard error were derived via bootstrap methods22, which formed the basis for the statistical inference.
Spectral analysis
Short-window autoregressive (AR) spectral analysis was performed on all available prestimulus (85 ms) LFP time series data. AR spectral analysis involves application of Fourier-based techniques to an AR model rather than directly to the LFP data. These techniques were utilized instead of direct-data Fourier-based techniques since the spectral resolution of the latter is not sufficient for the short time window analyzed23, whereas the spectral resolution and minimal frequency that may be resolved by the AR approach are not limited by the data period that is analyzed24,25. Supplementary Figure S1 provides a detailed simulation demonstrating this advantage of AR modeling. Despite this advantage, AR models might be unstable at low frequencies and near the Nyquist frequency, and so we limited analysis to frequencies between 5 and 90 Hz, based on the 200 Hz sampling frequency of the data. To ensure that each trial of local field potential data could be considered a realization of a zero-mean stochastic process, as required by the AR modeling procedure, the ensemble average was subtracted from each trial for each recording site included in the model26. A model order of 10 was used based on the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and previous determination that this value is optimal for this type of data21. Extensive testing revealed that the spectral results of this study were not sensitive to the choice of 10 as the model order. In fact, recomputing AR models, with model order varying from 5 to 15, produced spectral peaks having the same peak frequency but differing in peak width (increasing width with decreasing model order).
Subtracting the mean value of the trial ensemble from each trial of the prestimulus V4/TEO and V1/V2 local field potential time series allowed the LFPs to be treated as stochastic processes with stationary mean and variance27. We constructed two AR models (restricted and unrestricted) each for each pair of LFPs, represented in the following as variables X and Y. (Definitions are given for AR models of X. Similar definitions may be given for Y).
First, the restricted AR model of X is defined as:
1 |
where Xt represents the present value of X, Xt−i represents past values of X, α1i are the model coefficients, m is the model order, and ε1t is the restricted residual error. Second, X may also be represented by the unrestricted AR model:
2 |
where Yt−i represents past values of Y, α2i and β2i are model coefficients, and ε2t is the unrestricted residual error. Spectral power, coherence, and Granger Causality were computed by well-established methods26,28. For a value of spectral Granger Causality (sGC) to be significant requires that the unrestricted residual error variance be significantly less than the restricted residual error variance. This requirement also controls for the fact that the number of parameters is greater in the unrestricted than the restricted model.
The expression for sGC has a natural interpretation as the fraction of the total power of X that is predicted by Y. It is expressed as a ratio, where the numerator represents the total power of X at a given frequency ω, and the denominator represents the intrinsic power, i.e. the power not predicted by Y. If the intrinsic power equals the total power, it means that Y provides no additional predictability of X (in the unrestricted model) above that provided solely by the past of X alone (in the restricted model). In the restricted AR model (Equation 1), the causal influence from Y to X must be zero. The causal power is the amount of additional predictability provided by Y in the unrestricted AR model (Equation 2).
sGC can also be viewed as a directional decomposition of neuronal synchronization via its relationship with coherence. The total interdependence is the sum of: (1) the sGC from stochastic process X to stochastic process Y; (2) the sGC from Y to X; and (3) the instantaneous causality, which accounts for instantaneous correlation between X and Y, as would be caused by a mutual simultaneous input to X and Y (see26).
To facilitate statistical analysis, site pair identification, and pattern classification, AR spectral estimates were computed on 1000 bootstrap resamples of the data22, and then averaged over sites (for power spectra) or site pairs (for coherence and sGC spectra). The resulting mean of the resampled spectra is equivalent to the spectrum that would result from an AR model fit over all trials.
To determine frequencies where the top-down and bottom-up spectra significantly differed, a directional asymmetry analysis was performed according to the following procedure15: 1000 bootstrap spectra gave rise to 1000 difference spectra computed (over the entire spectrum from 5 to 90 Hz) as the top-down (V4/TEO to V1/V2) GC spectrum minus the bottom-up (V1/V2 to V4/TEO) GC spectrum for each bootstrap resample. The standard error of the directional asymmetry was then computed from the bootstraps via the following equation22:
3 |
where B is the number of bootstraps, in this case 1000, is the statistic of interest computed on each bootstrap b, and is the mean of all .
To correct for multiple comparisons across frequencies the maximal absolute deviation of each bootstrap difference spectrum from the mean of all bootstrap difference spectra was calculated for each frequency15. This resulted in the following modification to Equation 3:
4 |
where ω indexes each frequency of the spectrum. The resulting standard error was then the maximal standard error that could have been generated across all frequencies and was thus the omnibus standard error29–31. Using this standard error, a confidence interval of the mean corresponding to a two-tailed test of p < 0.001 was derived as:
5 |
where n is the number of trials, and the standard error is multiplied by the t-value with df = n − 1, at the 99.95th percentile of student’s t-distribution. Statistical significance was then assessed at those frequencies where the confidence interval did not contain zero. The frequencies where top-down and bottom-up sGC spectra were significantly different (p < 0.001) are indicated by the shaded grey region in Fig. 2d, indicating that significant directional asymmetry (top-down sGC > bottom-up sGC) only exists in a limited portion of the spectrum around 16 Hz.
Removal of 1/f background component
The V1/V2 and V4/TEO power spectra had a large 1/f background component that masked the beta oscillatory component32. To observe the oscillatory component, it was necessary to remove this background component. Power in microvolts was first converted to dB, and then the data were linearized with a logarithmic transform and a line was fit to the data via robust regression between 5 and 30 Hz, using the Welsh weighting function. The resulting regression coefficients were used to specify an exponential function, which was subtracted from the non-log transformed data. The residual is plotted in Fig. 2a,b as the de-noised power spectra. The fitting region of 5–30 Hz was roughly centered on the dominant top-down beta peak frequency of 16 Hz, such that the robust fit would maximally expose deviations from linearity at this frequency after the log-transform. A wider fitting region would result in greater error due to the contribution of deviations from other possible spectral concentrations, such as in the gamma or delta bands. This also explains why the resulting residual spectrum appears clipped at zero, which is due to the power after the subtraction of the estimated 1/f background component becoming less than zero as a result of error at the left and right extrema of the fit.
Identification of top-down spectral peaks
A total of twelve V4/TEO-V1/V2 pairs of the recording sites in M1 and M2 were possible. Our goal was to identify the site pairs which had a significant top-down (V4/TEO to V1/V2) spectral peak in the range of frequencies showing significant directional asymmetry (as depicted by the grey region on the mean (across-pair) spectra of Fig. 2d). To accomplish this aim, top-down sGC spectra were first computed over the 1000 bootstrap resamples (taken from all trials). Spectral peaks between 5 and 90 Hz were then identified in each bootstrap resample (using the findpeaksG.m Matlab function written by T.C. O’Haver) for all site pairs in the study. Each peak was then tested for significance at the p < 0.05 level against a null distribution.
The null distribution was created as follows: (1) for one null resample, trial order was randomized so that for each AR model the order of trials for site 1 was random with respect to site 2; (2) AR models were fit, and sGC spectra derived; (3) the significance threshold was defined as the maximum value across all frequencies of all top-down sGC spectra; and (4) steps 1–3 were repeated 1000 times to create the null distribution. This procedure controlled for the possibility of spurious results due to multiple comparisons across frequencies and pairs.
The empirical probability density for significant top-down sGC spectral peaks, computed as a function of frequency between 5 and 90 Hz from all site pairs and bootstrap resamples, and fit via a smoothing spline (with a smoothing parameter of 0.075), is presented in Fig. 3a. The largest concentration of significant top-down peaks was observed at approximately 16 Hz. To focus analysis on this largest concentration, we computed the empirical probability density by selecting the top-down spectral peak closest to the ~16 Hz peak for all site pairs and bootstrap resamples (shown in Fig. 3b). To isolate site pairs with a consistent top-down spectral peak concentration sufficiently near the average (~16 Hz), we computed the mean peak frequency over pairs for each bootstrap (solid vertical line in Fig. 3b) and derived its standard error via Equation 3. This allowed us to compute the 95% confidence interval via Equation 5 (vertical dashed lines in Fig. 3b). The site pairs having their mean peak frequency inside this 95% confidence interval were considered to have a top-down spectral peak sufficiently close to the peak significant directional asymmetry, indicating that they may be members of a synchronized oscillatory network.
To ensure that the top-down sGC peaks were not driven by differences in signal to noise ratio, we computed the spectral asymmetry for each pair as the difference between top-down sGC and bottom-up sGC, and compared this difference to the same quantity computed on the time reversed data. As proposed by33, if the difference between the net flow and reverse-net flow is significantly different from zero, then the causal relation is not rejected as spurious, since spurious contributions will not reverse. The test involved computing a single-sample t-test (df = 10) for each pair between the net flow difference (net flow – reverse-net flow), and zero, with the two-tailed p-value for all pairs being significant at p < 0.05 after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.
Pattern classification
A linear Support-Vector-Machine (SVM) was implemented via libSVM34 to attempt to classify the spatial patterns of prestimulus top-down sGC according to which stimulus-response contingency (task rule) was in effect35. The go response was the correct response to a line stimulus for contingency 1 trials (no-go response to diamond stimuli), whereas the go response was the correct response to a diamond stimulus for contingency 2 trials (no-go response to line stimuli). The spatial patterns used to train the SVM corresponded to the set of significant magnitudes of the top-down sGC pairs shown by arrows in Fig. 4. The machine learning process progressed, individually for M1 and M2, as follows:
The trial data for each contingency were randomly split in half into testing and training sets.
A bootstrap resample was drawn for the training and testing sets of each contingency, AR models were fit, and top-down sGC spectra were derived. This procedure was repeated 200 times, resulting in 200 exemplars of the top-down sGC pattern, for both training and testing sets of each contingency.
The SVM pattern classification feature for each site pair in each of the 200 bootstrap resamples was taken as the magnitude of the closest top-down sGC peak to 16 Hz. If all pairs did not exhibit a peak for a particular bootstrap resample, that resample was deleted. Deletion was a rare event that occurred for less than ½ a percent of all bootstrap resamples (M1: 0.32%, M2: 0.041%). The resulting top-down sGC values were normalized (converted to z-scores) so that each feature (top-down sGC magnitude) had a mean of zero, and unit variance across training and testing sets of both contingencies. Thus the data were not disturbed relative to each condition (training and testing set, and contingency), but the SVM features were balanced36.
The classification model was created by fitting the linear SVM, with a cost parameter of 1, to the data of the testing set.
The classification model from 4) was validated by application to the training data, resulting in a scalar classification accuracy.
These steps together, comprising a delete-d jackknife cross-validation procedure, were repeated 5000 times, producing a mean classification accuracy, and an estimate of its standard error.
Statistical evaluation of the SVM results was performed via application of delete-d jackknife cross-validation22. Delete-d jackknife cross-validation entails training (model fitting) on a portion of the total data (n − d), and then testing the model accuracy on the remaining d samples. This is an attractive approach for the current problem since sGC is biased as a function of the number of trials. Thus, by selecting d to be half the data, the bootstrap sGC will have equal trial numbers yielding equivalent bias across training and testing groups and between contingencies. Since the number of contingency 1 and contingency 2 trials was not exactly equal, each bootstrap resample used selections with n equal to the contingency with the lowest number of trials. The delete-d jackknife standard error is computed according to the following formula:
6 |
where n is the number of trials, is the total number of possible selections of d elements that can be made from n, is the statistic computed on each of the possible subsets, and is the mean of that statistic over subsets. In the current case of approximately 10 000 trials per monkey, the number of possible subsets is effectively infinite. However, a Monte Carlo approach known as the jackknife-sampling variance estimator (JSVE) well approximates the variance of the estimator37. The standard error based on the JSVE method is estimated as:
7 |
where M is a random number of subsets of d selected from the total possible. Even when M is dramatically smaller than the total number of possible selections, the JSVE still outperforms a number of estimators, such as the bootstrap37. Figure 5 shows that in the current data the standard error estimate quickly reaches an asymptote as M exceeds 250 subsamples, and remains very stable as it approaches the 5 000 subsamples used to estimate the standard error. In addition, the estimation error falls to negligible levels as M increases beyond. Using the standard error derived via the JSVE procedure, a single-sample t-test was conducted to determine if the mean classification accuracy differed from the chance level of 50%.
Data Availability
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Results
Prestimulus Beta-Frequency Oscillatory Synchrony in Visual Cortex
Beta-frequency oscillations in V1/V2 and V4/TEO were detected as spectral peaks near 16 Hz in the de-noised LFP power spectra (Fig. 2a,b) computed during a brief prestimulus window. Generally, LFP time series are well described as stochastic processes, and spectral power peaks indicate the frequency and magnitude of narrow-band oscillatory activity in those processes. During the prestimulus period, the monkey had already pressed a lever (and maintained it in the depressed position) to begin the trial, and was awaiting well known visual stimuli. Prestimulus synchronization of the V1/V2 and V4/TEO beta-frequency oscillations was observed as a peak in the average V1/V2-extrastriate coherence spectrum at 17 Hz (Fig. 2c). A peak in the coherence spectrum indicates narrow-band synchronization between the two processes used to derive it.
Prestimulus Oscillatory Synchrony Supports Top-down Signaling to V1
Based on this strong tendency for prestimulus V1/V2 and V4/TEO LFPs to oscillate in the beta frequency range, we hypothesized that beta-frequency synchrony supports top-down signaling from extrastriate cortex to V1/V2. To test this hypothesis, we next computed bottom-up and top-down spectral Granger Causality (sGC) between LFPs in V1/V2 and V4/TEO. The sGC measures statistical causality at spectral frequencies in the Nyquist range, quantifying the degree to which the prediction of a value of one time series can be improved by knowledge of the past values of another time series as a function of frequency.
The mean top-down sGC spectrum showed a peak at 16 Hz, closely matching the frequency of the power peaks and coherence peak, while the mean bottom-up sGC spectrum did not show a beta peak (Fig. 2d). A peak in the sGC spectrum indicates narrow-band directed synchrony between the two processes used to derive it. This result supports the hypothesis that beta-frequency synchrony underlies top-down signaling from extrastriate cortex to V1/V2. According to the known relation between coherence and sGC, we inferred that synchronization between V1/V2 and extrastriate areas is dominated by a top-down transfer of information from extrastriate cortex to V1/V2.
To more precisely determine the difference between top-down and bottom-up sGC spectra shown in Fig. 2d, we tested for an sGC directional asymmetry at all frequencies between 5 and 90 Hz using a bootstrap resampling approach. We found top-down sGC to be significantly greater than bottom-up sGC only for frequencies between 8 and 23 Hz (grey region in Fig. 2d); this range was centered very close to the 16 Hz mean top-down peak (p < 0.001, two-tailed corrected bootstrap resampling test, n = 12). In fact, the probability density of top-down sGC peaks was most prominent in the low-beta-frequency range, with the peak probability density at ~16 Hz being at least 2.5 times larger than at any other frequency examined (Fig. 3a). These results point to the presence of top-down physiological signaling from extrastriate low-beta-frequency oscillatory generators to V1/V2 low-beta-frequency generators in subjects awaiting stimulus presentation as they performed the visual pattern discrimination task.
Prestimulus Top-down Directed Beta-Frequency Synchrony Predicts Behavioral Context
The evidence for top-down V4/TEO to V1/V2 physiological signaling suggested that the transmission of top-down influences from V4/TEO to V1/V2 may carry task-related behavioral information. We therefore tested the hypothesis that the spatial pattern of top-down beta-frequency sGC contains task-specific behavioral information. To perform this test, we applied a linear Support-Vector-Machine (SVM) classifier, using the top-down beta-frequency peak sGC magnitudes as classification features, in order to predict the task rule (behavioral context) that was in effect. The top-down beta-frequency peak sGC magnitudes used for classification were from the 8 site pairs (out of the 12 possible) that exhibited two important properties: (1) the mean peak frequency was inside the 95% confidence interval of the overall mean of 16 Hz; and (2) the standard deviation of peak frequency was low (Fig. 3b, red bars). These 8 site pairs thus showed a consistent top-down directed synchrony in a narrow frequency band around 16 Hz. By contrast, the other 4 site pairs had their mean peak frequency outside the 95% confidence interval and had a large variability of that peak frequency (Fig. 3b, blue bars). They thus did not show a consistent top-down directed synchrony in a narrow frequency band. The subsequent analysis focused on these 8 site pairs showing consistent top-down directed synchrony in a narrow frequency band around 16 Hz.
The spatial patterns of these consistent top-down narrow-band beta-frequency sGC peaks, and of their corresponding coherence peaks, are depicted in the maps of Fig. 4, where peak coherence is represented by lines connecting V4/TEO and V1/V2 electrode sites, peak sGC is represented by arrows from a V4/TEO site to a V1/V2 site, and line or arrow thickness represents the magnitude of peak coherence or sGC.
We found that top-down, but not bottom-up, sGC was highly correlated with coherence (Fig. 6). At 16 Hz there was a significant linear correlation (R(6) = 0.90, p < 0.005, Bonferroni corrected) between top-down sGC and coherence values, and top-down sGC explained 81% of the variance in coherence (Fig. 6a). By contrast, bottom-up sGC and coherence were not significantly correlated (R(6) = 0.55, p = 0.315, Bonferroni corrected). We also computed the correlation between sGC and coherence at 16 Hz after first aligning the mean sGC and coherence magnitudes and standardizing the variance of each pair over bootstraps (Fig. 6b). Even after this normalization, the fraction of the coherence variance explained by the top-down sGC of the 8 site pairs having consistent top-down narrow-beta-band directed synchrony (48%) greatly exceeded that explained by the bottom-up sGC of these site pairs (16%), and even more greatly exceeded that explained by the top-down sGC of the other (inconsistent) 4 site pairs (4%).
Support-Vector-Machine (SVM) pattern classification, with the set of top-down narrow-beta-band peak sGC values (taken from the 8 consistent site pairs) as classification features, provided evidence that top-down beta-frequency sGC encodes the task rule (Fig. 7). The classification accuracy was 76% for M1, which was significantly greater than the 50% chance classification level (t(10177) = 2.05, p = 0.020), and 82% for M2, which was also significantly greater than the chance level (t(8266) = 2.70, p = 0.004). The results were verified by applying a linear discriminant analysis pattern classifier to the same classification data.
To ensure that these results were specifically due to a difference in beta-frequency spectral peak magnitudes between task contingencies, we performed an additional statistical contrast where the classification was based on the top-down sGC magnitude at frequencies randomly selected between 5 and 50 Hz. This resulted in classification accuracies that were near the 50% chance level (Fig. 7: M1: 51%, t(10177) = 0.21, p = 0.42); M2: 56%, t(8266) = 1.12, p = 0.132)), thus demonstrating that significant classification performance critically depends on the peak frequencies in a narrow beta-frequency band.
To summarize, these results demonstrate that a top-down narrow-beta-band synchrony directed to V1/V2 from V4/TEO exists in a brief prestimulus window in monkeys highly trained to perform a visuomotor pattern discrimination task, and that this top-down narrow-beta-band directed synchrony predicts the task rule that is in effect. Since task rule determines the behavioral context under which the monkey was performing, the results indicate that behavioral context is conveyed to V1 from extrastriate cortex by top-down beta oscillatory signaling.
Discussion
We report that prestimulus top-down beta-frequency directed synchrony in visual cortex discriminates the task rule (behavioral context) that governs correct behavioral performance. Our results indicate that task-related behavioral context is conveyed by endogenous top-down neural influences from extrastriate visual cortex (V4/TEO) to V1/V2. Extrastriate visual cortex itself likely receives contextual information via influences from higher level areas located in frontal and parietal cortex, via known anatomical projections38,39. Overall, our findings support a cortical model in which contextual information about the behavioral significance of expected stimuli is propagated to V1 by a cascade of top-down influences flowing down a cortical hierarchy5.
This main finding is based on the observation that before stimulus presentation in a visuomotor pattern discrimination task, top-down directed synchrony is found in a narrow frequency band around 16 Hz for a majority of the site pairs examined. This result suggests the existence of an anticipatory network of visual cortical neuronal populations that are phase-synchronized in a narrow beta-frequency band. The findings of this report are thus consistent with the concept of phase-synchronized large-scale cortical networks that have previously been proposed to operate in cognition10,38,40–44.
The sGC technique28 that we employed to measure directed synchrony is based on a well-principled and long-established methodology27. Although several studies (e.g.45,46) have attempted to infer cortical information transfer from the sign of oscillatory phase difference, it has previously been demonstrated that phase difference may not accurately reflect the direction of influence in cortical circuits21,47,48.
The linear SVM pattern classification technique was used to discriminate task rule based on top-down beta-frequency directed synchrony entirely from within visual cortex. The classification results were validated by linear discriminant analysis. The classification findings may be considered surprising given that visual cortex is not traditionally considered to process task rules. However, they are consistent with an expanding body of evidence showing that visual processing is contextual2, and that V1 can be “pre-tuned” in preparation for visual perception49. Only correct trials were considered in this study. Too few incorrect trials were left for analysis after artifact rejection, and so comparison of correct and incorrect performance was not possible.
The top-down extrastriate-to-V1/V2 directed synchrony that we report is in the beta frequency range (~16 Hz), consistent with a growing number of reports relating interareal beta-frequency interactions to endogenous cognitive processing (see reviews)3,50. Although beta oscillations have previously been most associated with somatosensory-motor function51, accumulating evidence supports the idea that they occur throughout the brain during wait periods when subjects are prepared for a sensory event or for motor behavior17. We now report that in visual cortex they mediate the expectancy of visual processing. Moreover, our finding that top-down beta-frequency synchrony directed to V1/V2 predicts behavioral contingency suggests that beta oscillations may also actively convey endogenous, task-related contextual information to lower-order areas in other (non-visual) sensory systems.
The precise neuronal mechanism by which top-down influence operates in visual cortex is unknown. However, our results suggest that it is modulatory. A likely mechanism is that top-down influences from extrastriate cortex modulate V1/V2 inhibitory interneurons to increase their synchrony and their response gain52–55. V1/V2 inhibitory interneurons may in turn control the output of the infragranular tall pyramidal neuron, thought to be the principle projection neuron from V1 to non-sensory thalamic and cortical areas56. Since descending fiber pathways to V1 terminate in infragranular layers, where prominent beta activity occurs57, it is likely that top-down modulatory influences target the infragranular layers, as suggested by modeling studies52.
The fact that no stimulus was present during the analysis period of our study suggests the reason why we did not observe prominent gamma-frequency influences. It may be that descending beta and ascending gamma influences can be decoupled in time. If so, top-down beta influences to V1/V2 prior to stimulus onset may modify subsequent V1/V2 stimulus responses. At times, top-down beta influences may also interact directly with stimulus-driven input. Both mechanisms dictate that feedforward stimulus-driven input carried by gamma oscillations is constrained by behavioral context, via descending beta frequency modulation12,14,15,58.
Taken together, our results argue for the idea that extrastriate cortex transmits top-down influences to V1/V2 when well-trained monkeys expect a familiar visual input. We find that these influences (1) depend on synchronous activity between extrastriate and V1/V2 neuron populations, and (2) carry behaviorally relevant task information. The evidence provided here supports the hypothesis that top-down influences from higher areas within the visual cortical hierarchy dynamically constrain lower-level activity in an adaptive, task-specific manner.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
The authors gratefully acknowledge Richard Nakamura for his role in designing the experimental paradigm and performing the electrophysiological recordings. This work was supported by the U.S. National Institute of Mental Health under grant MH062404 and the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, through the “Severo Ochoa” Programme for Centres/Units of Excellence in R&D” (SEV-2015-490).
Author Contributions
R.C. designed the experiment and collected the data. C.G.R. and S.L.B. designed and conducted the analysis and wrote the paper.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-018-25267-1.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Craig G. Richter, Email: craiggrichter@gmail.com
Steven L. Bressler, Email: bressler@fau.edu
References
- 1.Engel AK, Fries P, Singer W. Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:704–716. doi: 10.1038/35094565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gilbert CD, Sigman M. Brain states: top-down influences in sensory processing. Neuron. 2007;54:677–696. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wang X-J. Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol Rev. 2010;90:1195–1268. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00035.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Bressler SL, Richter CG. Interareal oscillatory synchronization in top-down neocortical processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2015;31:62–66. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Fries P. Rhythms for Cognition: Communication through Coherence. Neuron. 2015;88:220–235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Felleman D, Van Essen DC. Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 1991;1:1–47. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hilgetag CC, O’Neill MA, Young MP. Indeterminate organization of the visual system. Science. 1996;271:776–777. doi: 10.1126/science.271.5250.776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Markov NT, et al. Anatomy of hierarchy: feedforward and feedback pathways in macaque visual cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 2014;522:225–259. doi: 10.1002/cne.23458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bressler SL, Richter CG, Chen Y, Ding M. Cortical functional network organization from autoregressive modeling of local field potential oscillations. Statistics in Medicine. 2007;26:3875–3885. doi: 10.1002/sim.2935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Buschman TJ, Miller EK. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science. 2007;315:1860–1862. doi: 10.1126/science.1138071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bosman CA, et al. Attentional Stimulus Selection through Selective Synchronization between Monkey Visual Areas. Neuron. 2012;75:875–888. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.van Kerkoerle T, et al. Alpha and gamma oscillations characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111:14332–14341. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1402773111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bastos AM, et al. Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels. Neuron. 2015;85:390–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Richter CG, Thompson WH, Bosman CA, Fries P. Top-Down Beta Enhances Bottom-Up Gamma. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:6698–6711. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3771-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Michalareas G, et al. Alpha-Beta and Gamma Rhythms Subserve Feedback and Feedforward Influences among Human Visual Cortical Areas. Neuron. 2016;89:384–397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Engel, A. K. & Fries, P. Beta-band oscillations-signalling the status quo? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 18.Bastos AM, et al. Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron. 2012;76:695–711. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lopes da Silva F. EEG and MEG: relevance to neuroscience. Neuron. 2013;80:1112–1128. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ledberg A, Bressler SL, Ding M, Coppola R, Nakamura R. Large-scale visuomotor integration in the cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2007;17:44–62. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Brovelli A, et al. Beta oscillations in a large-scale sensorimotor cortical network: Directional influences revealed by Granger causality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2004;101:9849–9854. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308538101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Efron, B. & Tibshirani, R. J. An Introduction to the Bootstrap. (CRC Press, 1994).
- 23.Nalatore H, Rangarajan G. Short-window spectral analysis using AMVAR and multitaper methods: a comparison. Biol. Cybern. 2009;101:71–80. doi: 10.1007/s00422-009-0318-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Cohen, M. X. Analyzing Neural Time Series Data. (MIT Press, 2014).
- 25.Ozaki, T. Time Series Modeling of Neuroscience Data. 20120549, (CRC Press, 2012).
- 26.Ding, M., Chen, Y. & Bressler, S. L. Granger Causality: Basic Theory and Application toNeuroscience. Handbook of Time Series Analysis: Recent Theoretical Developments and Applications, 10.1002/9783527609970.ch17 (2006).
- 27.Bressler SL, Seth AK. Wiener-Granger causality: a well established methodology. Neuroimage. 2011;58:323–329. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Geweke, J. Measurement of linear dependence and feedback between multiple time. Journal of the American Statistical Association (1982).
- 29.Westfall, P. H. & Young, S. S. Resampling-Based Multiple Testing. (John Wiley & Sons, 1993).
- 30.Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;15:1–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Holmes AP, Blair RC, Watson NG, Ford I. Nonparametric Analysis of Statistic Images from Functional Mapping Experiments. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1996;16:7–22. doi: 10.1097/00004647-199601000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.He, B. J. Scale-free brain activity: past, present, and future. Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed)18, 480–487 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 33.Haufe, S., Nikulin, V. V. & Nolte, G. Latent Variable Analysis and Signal Separation. (Springer Science & Business Media, 2012).
- 34.Chang C-C, Lin C-J. LIBSVM: A library for support vector machines. ACM Transactions on Intelligent Systems and Technology (TIST) 2011;2:27–27. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cortes C, Vapnik V. Support-vector networks. Mach Learn. 1995;20:273–297. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Juszczak, P., Tax, D. M. J. & Duin, R. P. W. Feature scaling in support vector data description. Proc. ASCI 2002, 8th Annual Conf. of the Advanced School for Computing and Imaging 95–102 (2002).
- 37.Shao J. The Efficiency and Consistency of Approximations to the Jackknife Variance Estimators. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1989;84:114–119. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1989.10478745. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bressler, S. L., Tang, W., Sylvester, C. M., Shulman, G. L. & Corbetta, M. Top-down control of human visual cortex by frontal and parietal cortex in anticipatory visual spatial attention. 28, 10056–10061 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 39.Markov NT, et al. Cortical high-density counterstream architectures. Science. 2013;342:1238406. doi: 10.1126/science.1238406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bressler SL. Large-scale cortical networks and cognition. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1995;20:288–304. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(94)00016-I. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Bressler SL. Inferential constraint sets in the organization of visual expectation. Neuroinformatics. 2004;2:227–238. doi: 10.1385/NI:2:2:227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bressler S, Kelso J. Cortical coordination dynamics and cognition. Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed) 2001;5:26–36. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01564-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bressler SL, Tognoli E. Operational principles of neurocognitive networks. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006;60:139–148. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Meehan TP, Bressler SL. Neurocognitive networks: findings, models, and theory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:2232–2247. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Stein von A, Chiang C, König P. Top-down processing mediated by interareal synchronization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2000;97:14748–14753. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.26.14748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Saalmann YB, Pigarev IN, Vidyasagar TR. Neural mechanisms of visual attention: how top-down feedback highlights relevant locations. Science. 2007;316:1612–1615. doi: 10.1126/science.1139140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Salazar RF, Dotson NM, Bressler SL, Gray CM. Content-specific fronto-parietal synchronization during visual working memory. Science. 2012;338:1097–1100. doi: 10.1126/science.1224000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Matias FS, et al. Modeling positive Granger causality and negative phase lag between cortical areas. Neuroimage. 2014;99:411–418. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Farber DA, Machinskaya RI, Kurganskii AV, Petrenko NE. Functional Organization of the Brain during Preparation for Recognition of Image Fragments. Neurosci Behav Physi. 2015;45:1055–1062. doi: 10.1007/s11055-015-0185-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Siegel M, Donner TH, Engel AK. Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13:121–134. doi: 10.1038/nrn3137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Jenkinson N, Brown P. New insights into the relationship between dopamine, beta oscillations and motor function. Trends Neurosci. 2011;34:611–618. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lee JH, Whittington MA, Kopell NJ. Top-down beta rhythms support selective attention via interlaminar interaction: a model. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2013;9:e1003164. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cannon J, et al. Neurosystems: brain rhythms and cognitive processing. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2014;39:705–719. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Mitchell JF, Sundberg KA, Reynolds JH. Differential Attention-Dependent Response Modulation across Cell Classes in Macaque Visual Area V4. Neuron. 2007;55:131–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Tiesinga PH, Fellous J-M, Salinas E, José JV, Sejnowski TJ. Inhibitory synchrony as a mechanism for attentional gain modulation. J. Physiol. Paris. 2004;98:296–314. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2005.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Briggs, F. Organizing principles of cortical layer 6. Front. Neural Circuits4 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 57.Buffalo EA, Fries P, Landman R, Buschman TJ, Desimone R. Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108:11262–11267. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011284108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Roberts MJ, et al. Robust gamma coherence between macaque V1 and V2 by dynamic frequency matching. Neuron. 2013;78:523–536. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.