Skip to main content
BioMed Research International logoLink to BioMed Research International
. 2018 Apr 22;2018:7231818. doi: 10.1155/2018/7231818

Tracking of Physical Fitness of Primary School Children in Trier: A 4-Year Longitudinal Study

Andreas Roth 1,, Steffen C E Schmidt 2, Ilka Seidel 1, Alexander Woll 2, Klaus Bös 2
PMCID: PMC5937626  PMID: 29850555

Abstract

Objective

The aim of this study is to measure the motor development and tracking of physical fitness (PF) components of primary school children of Trier in Germany.

Methods

Two longitudinal cohorts, of 1768 children (915 f, 853 m) aged 5–11, were measured. In longitudinal cohort 1, a total of 116 female and 137 male participants aged 6.80 ± 0.42 years at baseline were measured four times from grade 1 to grade 4 (response: 40.4%). Participants of longitudinal cohort 2 (166 f, 149 m; 6.70 ± 0.36 years at baseline, response: 42.6%) were examined three times from grade 1 to grade 3 with the German Motor Test 6–18 (DMT 6–18).

Results

Physical fitness increased significantly over time in all test tasks except flexibility. Gender-specific differences were found in 20 m sprint, 6-minute run, balancing backwards, jumping sideways, and stand and reach. 74.4% of PF stability coefficients were moderate (r = 0.30 to 0.60). Stability of PF declined with increased time frames. Tracking was lower in girls than in boys. Flexibility showed the highest stability among PF variables (r > 0.50). BMI showed the overall highest stability coefficient with r > 0.7.

Conclusions

Gender-specific differences of PF were obvious but cannot always be secured statistically in primary school. Tracking was only moderate. Variability in the timing and speed of the adolescent growth spurt and sexual maturation influence stability of PF. Results from longitudinal cohort 2 largely confirm those from longitudinal cohort 1.

1. Introduction

Regular physical activity (PA) and physical fitness (PF) are important factors for the health and social development of children and adolescents. PF is considered one of the most important health markers [1]. It is defined as a set of personal properties (i.e., cardiorespiratory endurance, skeletal muscle endurance, skeletal muscle power, flexibility, agility, balance, reaction time, and body composition) which people inherit or advance to perform PA. Cardiorespiratory endurance, muscular strength, endurance, body composition, and flexibility are referred to as health-related fitness. Balance, coordination, speed, agility, and power are often characterized as performance-related fitness [2].

There is consensus that high PF positively influences the physical and psychological health of children and adolescents. Studies show that cardiorespiratory fitness as well as muscular fitness reduce overall and abdominal obesity and decrease cardiovascular risk factors [1, 36]. Moreover, cardiorespiratory fitness and muscular fitness can have a positive influence on mental health [1, 3] and are also positively associated with academic achievement [7]. An increase in PF is positively associated with bone health and higher quality of life after cancers and chemotherapy-induced treatments [1].

Motor performance, measured by PF components in childhood, is a process of change which is determined by development-related changes and by training-dependent performance improvements. In the early school years pupils are subject to extensive physical changes. Internal organs develop their full functionality, the body undergoes widespread sequential changes, and the central nervous system matures [8].

During physical development of childhood speed, endurance and strength increase in both genders [810]. However, especially at primary school age, the time of motor ability development varies due to different maturation levels [11]. The development of speed and endurance is quicker than the development of muscular strength. Coordination is based on a large inter- and intraindividual range of performance because of the interplay of complex external and internal factors [8]. At primary school age, girls have better flexibility than boys [8, 9].

In adolescence, motor performance continues to improve and sex differences become more considerable. In speed, endurance, and strength, boys often achieve higher results than girls. These differences become significant at the age of 12 to 13 years [11]. Due to various developments of endocrinological processes in the context of puberty, the effects of training in males increase enormously, especially in strength [11, 12]. PF usually reaches its peak in late adolescence or early adulthood. Girls reach the plateau of conditionally based testing tasks earlier than boys [13].

In general, numerous internal and environmental factors can lead to several instabilities in individual development. Reported childhood development is heavily dependent on the sample and the test battery used. Particularly with coordination tasks, there are often low correlations between the results of different tasks [8].

Due to individual differences in development, future motor performance is hard to predict. Measuring a sample at different points in time is called tracking and provides a coefficient for the longitudinal stability of a certain variable [14]. These coefficients of motor performance stability demonstrate if fit children are also fit at a later point in life. This is an important question in talent promotion, but it also allows the early detection of clumsy and motor deficit children.

It has been confirmed that tracking coefficients depend on the time frame between different measures. In general, the correlation increases with a shorter time frame. The height of the coefficient also depends on the age of the sample at baseline [14]. At young school age the coefficients are mostly lower than after puberty [15]. Coefficients also depend on sample size, the claimed motor ability, and motor testing tasks used [15]. Studies show that boys reach slightly higher coefficients than girls [10]. There are also differences in the stability of anthropometric and motor skills, and Body Mass Index is usually more stable than PF [16].

PF components were tracked from childhood to adolescence [1619], during adolescence [20], or from childhood or adolescence to adulthood [2124], and during childhood [10, 25, 26].

Falk et al. [10] examined the tracking of field-assessed fitness components for 319 pupils (116 f 203 m) from the second grade, which corresponds to the age of 6-7 years, to the sixth grade. The correlations of fitness components over the 4-year period in both sexes varied between r = .36 and .66. Stability was generally lower in girls than in boys.

The study of McMillan and Erdmann [25] tracked health-related fitness components for 409 boys and 409 girls from kindergarten (6.1 ± 0.3 years) to the fifth grade. The correlations from kindergarten to the second grade lie in the range of r = .39 to r = .82, and from kindergarten to the third grade from r = .37 to r = .84. Vandorpe and colleagues [26] examined 371 children between six and nine years of age at baseline regarding motor coordination and sports club participation in three consecutive years. Correlation coefficients ranging from .66 (6–8 years) to .87 (7–9 years) revealed that coordination of children is a highly stable factor in this study.

The main objective of the TrieKis project was to carry out a screening of all primary school children of Trier, in order to identify the students' strengths and weaknesses at different measurements points.

In the following paper the motor development and stability of motor performance in primary school age are the focus.

The main research questions are as follows:

  1. What is the course of motor performance in primary school?

  2. How stable is motor performance in primary school?

2. Research Methods

2.1. Study Sample and Design

The data was collected during a community-based, longitudinal study in Trier (population approximately 115,000) with four measurements, during the school years 2008/2009, 2009/2010, 2010/2011, and 2011/2012. Participants were recruited from 23 out of 24 in Trier available primary schools in Trier and were tested mostly in physical education lessons. The study was a joint project of the sports association in Rhineland-Pfalz, the Supervision and Services Directorate in Trier, the Physical Education College of Trier, and the Research Center for School Sports and Physical Education of Children and Young Adults (FoSS).

Participation was mandatory for the pupils, and parents were informed early and could refuse participation for their children. The tests were organized and carried out by trained instructors of the Physical Education College of Trier in cooperation with FoSS.

A total of 1768 different subjects (915 f, 853 m) aged 5–11 were tested 4266 times over the course of the study. The total numbers of participants for each of the four measurement points were 2008/2009: 623, 2009/2010: 1404, 2010/2011: 1335, and 2011/2012: 1335. In the early stages of the study 623 pupils (313 f, 310 m) were measured in grade 1. That led to a total number of 252 (116 f. 137 m.; 6.80 ± 0.42 years at baseline) pupils who were tested at each of the four measurement points (longitudinal cohort 2008; LC 1). This represents 40.4% (37.1% girls; 44.2% boys) of the initial sample. In 2009/2010, 739 (381 f, 358 m) new pupils participated. From those, 315 (166 f, 149 m; 6.70 ± 0.36 years at baseline) were tested three times (longitudinal cohort 2009; LC 2). This represents 42.6% (43.6% girls; 41.6% boys) of the sample which started in 2009/2010. Descriptive statistics of the sample are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics ofparticipants of the longitudinal study in Trier, Germany.

Grade Sex 2008/2009 2009/2010 2010/2011 2011/2012
N Age N Age N Age N Age
1 Boys 310 6.86 ± 0.44 358 6.80 ± 0.42
1 Girls 313 6.80 ± 0.45 381 6.76 ± 0.43

2 Boys 321 7.84 ± 0.66 337 7.93 ± 0.46
2 Girls 344 7.81 ± 0.47 360 7.91 ± 0.46

3 Boys 309 8.95 ± 0.52 234 9.20 ± 0.46
3 Girls 312 8.98 ± 0.50 239 9.09 ± 0.75

4 Boys 204 10.16 ± 0.51
4 Girls 222 10.17 ± 0.52

Total 623 6.83 ± 0.45 1404 7.27 ± 0.72 1318 8.41 ± 0.71 899 9.63 ± 0.77

The sample shows representative characteristics regarding age, sex, anthropometrics, and PF of primary school children for a moderate-sized city in Germany. Since the sample was recruited from 23 out of 24 available primary schools in Trier, we assume that the sample is also representative in terms of social status and migration background.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Physical Fitness

The German Motor Test (DMT 6–18) [13] was used to assess PF. One-week reliability of test tasks performed by a comparable team of trained instructors is on average r = .82. Test battery was successfully checked for validity [13].

Cardiorespiratory fitness was measured by the 6-minute run. Strength endurance of upper extremities was evaluated by number of push-ups in 40 seconds. Strength endurance of the torso muscles was evaluated by number of sit-ups in 40 seconds. Speed strength of lower extremities was checked by standing long jump. Action speed was evaluated by the 20 m sprint using a stop watch. Cross-motor coordination under time constraint was measured by jumping sideways. Backward balancing allowed the assessment of gross motor coordination during dynamic precision tasks. The number of steps on each beam was added. The stand and reach test was used for the assessment of trunk flexibility and the flexibility of the sciatic-crural muscle group [13].

In a 45-minute time frame, ten to twelve pupils were tested by a group of eight test instructors from the Physical Education College of Trier.

2.2.2. Body Mass Index

Height was measured, without shoes, to the nearest 0.1 cm using a tape measure. Weight was measured standardized to the nearest 0.1 kg using a Korona Alva digital metric scale (Sundern, North Rhine Westfalia, Germany). Body Mass Index (BMI) has been accepted as a viable diagnostic tool for estimating fat mass in children and adolescents and was also measured in the study [27]. BMI can be calculated as body weight (kg)/(body size (m))2.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS Statistics 24.0. Significance level was set to p < .05. To quantify stability, Pearson's correlation coefficients were calculated between grades. Tracking analyses included only subjects that participated in every measurement point. Significant PF changes over time and timesex interactions were analysed via repeated measurement ANOVA (rmANOVA) and F-value and partial eta2 effect sizes are reported.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive statistics of LC 1 are shown in Table 2, and descriptive statistics of LC 2 are shown in Table 3.

Table 2.

Mean and standard deviation for longitudinal cohort 1 (LC1) in test tasks and BMI, N = 253.

Task Sex Grade 1 Grade 2 Grade 3 Grade 4
(6.8 ± 0.4 years) (7.8 ± 0.4) (8.9 ± 0.4) (10.1 ± 0.4)
Mean ± s Mean ± s Mean ± s Mean ± s
20 m sprint
(sec)
m 4.97 ± 0.45 4.50 ± 0.34 4.32 ± 0.34 4.16 ± 0.35
f 5.07 ± 0.38 4.58 ± 0.34 4.41 ± 0.26 4.22 ± 0.28
5.02 ± 0.42 4.53 ± 0.34 4.36 ± 0.31 4.19 ± 0.32

Standing long jump
(cm)
m 112.5 ± 18.0 119.0 ± 19.2 130.4 ± 19.0 137.3 ± 21.8
f 109.3 ± 15.8 118.3 ± 16.8 126.5 ± 14.1 134.0 ± 18.2
111.1 ± 17.1 118.7 ± 18.1 128.7 ± 17.0 135.8 ± 20.3

Balancing backwards m 23.8 ± 8.7 26.7 ± 9.3 31.5 ± 10.6 36.3 ± 9.3
f 24.8 ± 8.1 31.2 ± 8.8 34.5 ± 9.8 38.4 ± 8.0
24.3 ± 8.4 28.8 ± 9.3 32.8 ± 10.3 37.2 ± 8.8

Sit-ups m 12.5 ± 5.6 16.5 ± 6.1 19.4 ± 6.6 21.7 ± 5.9
f 13.4 ± 4.9 16.2 ± 5.1 18.1 ± 4.8 20.6 ± 5.1
12.9 ± 5.3 16.4 ± 5.6 18.8 ± 5.8 21.2 ± 5.6

Push-ups m 9.4 ± 3.3 12.2 ± 3.8 14.5 ± 3.9 16.6 ± 4.0
f 9.9 ± 3.5 12.3 ± 3.6 14.6 ± 3.0 16.4 ± 3.3
9.6 ± 3.4 12.2 ± 3.7 14.5 ± 3.5 16.5 ± 3.7

Jumping sideways m 17.2 ± 4.8 21.8 ± 6.1 28.6 ± 7.2 34.3 ± 8.6
f 18.7 ± 5.2 23.5 ± 5.7 30.6 ± 6.9 36.1 ± 7.2
17.9 ± 5.0 22.6 ± 6.0 29.5 ± 7.1 35.1 ± 8.0

Stand and reach
(cm)
m 0.0 ± 5.8 −2.1 ± 6.3 −0.5 ± 6.4 −2.5 ± 7.4
f 2.0 ± 5.9 1.6 ± 6.1 2.6 ± 6.6 2.1 ± 7.1
0.9 ± 5.9 −0.4 ± 6.5 0.9 ± 6.7 0.9 ± 6.7

6-minute run
(m)
m 921.4 ± 97.3 944.4 ± 112.7 994.1 ± 105.5 1021.6 ± 148.9
f 902.4 ± 74.4 895.0 ± 97.0 944.3 ± 82.5 959.0 ± 106.6
912.8 ± 88.1 922.1 ± 108.6 971.6 ± 98.8 993.3 ± 134.9

BMI m 15.9 ± 2.2 16.6 ± 2.3 16.9 ± 3.0 17.9 ± 3.0
f 15.5 ± 1.7 16.1 ± 1.9 16.3 ± 2.1 17.5 ± 2.5
15.7 ± 2.0 16.4 ± 2.2 16.6 ± 2.6 17.7 ± 2.8

Table 3.

Mean and standard deviation for longitudinal cohort 2 (LC 2) in test tasks and BMI, N = 315.

Task Sex Grade 1 Grade 2 Grade 3
(6.7 ± 0.4 years) (7.8 ± 0.4) (9.1 ± 0.4)
Mean ± s Mean ± s Mean ± s
20 m sprint
(sec)
m 4.78 ± 0.40 4.54 ± 0.37 4.29 ± 0.37
f 4.87 ± 0.35 4.63 ± 0.35 4.37 ± 0.36
4.83 ± 0.37 4.59 ± 0.36 4.33 ± 0.37

Standing long jump
(cm)
m 111.2 ± 18.5 123.7 ± 17.9 130.0 ± 22.8
f 108.7 ± 15.6 119.2 ± 17.0 127.4 ± 19.6
109.9 ± 17.0 121.3 ± 17.5 128.6 ± 21.2

Balancing backwards m 18.6 ± 8.7 25.7 ± 9.7 33.1 ± 8.8
f 23.0 ± 9.0 29.2 ± 9.4 35.9 ± 8.6
20.9 ± 9.1 27.5 ± 9.7 34.5 ± 8.8

Sit-ups m 13.2 ± 6.1 17.1 ± 6.0 18.5 ± 5.3
f 13.7 ± 5.7 16.4 ± 5.0 17.8 ± 5.2
13.5 ± 5.9 16.7 ± 5.5 18.1 ± 5.3

Push-ups m 8.8 ± 3.6 12.4 ± 3.2 14.5 ± 3.8
f 9.3 ± 3.6 12.4 ± 3.5 14.5 ± 3.8
9.1 ± 3.6 12.4 ± 3.4 14.6 ± 3.6

Jumping sideways m 18.0 ± 5.9 24.4 ± 6.2 30.7 ± 7.4
f 18.5 ± 4.9 25.9 ± 6.0 32.0 ± 6.7
18.3 ± 5.4 25.2 ± 6.1 31.4 ± 7.1

Stand and reach
(cm)
m −1.2 ± 5.7 −1.6 ± 6.3 −2.3 ± 7.2
f 1.3 ± 5.9 2.3 ± 6.8 1.7 ± 7.3
0.1 ± 5.9 0.5 ± 6.8 −0.2 ± 7.5

6-minute run
(m)
m 890.0 ± 108.0 950.1 ± 97.3 1000.0 ± 141.6
f 842.3 ± 109.5 905.1 ± 87.5 937.0 ± 126.2
865.0 ± 111.2 926.5 ± 94.8 967.0 ± 137.2

BMI m 16.3 ± 2.6 16.2 ± 2.5 17.1 ± 2.7
f 15.9 ± 1.8 16.0 ± 2.2 17.0 ± 2.6
16.1 ± 2.2 16.1 ± 2.3 17.0 ± 2.7

PF increased significantly over time in all test tasks for both longitudinal cohorts except for stand and reach. Means and confidence intervals of physical fitness for LC 1 and LC 2 are shown in Figures 1(a)1(h).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Means and confidence intervals of physical fitness for longitudinal cohort 1 (LC 1) and longitudinal cohort 2 (LC 2).

In LC 1 the stand and reach performance decreased significantly and significant timesex interaction shows that the decrease is mainly due to boys (time: F = 8.74; p < .01; eta2 = .034; sex: F = 22.82; p < .01; timesex: F = 7.95; p = <.01; eta2 = .017).

Girls achieved better results than boys in balancing backwards (LC 1: F = 184.28; p =< .01; eta2 = .033/LC 2: F = 319.87; p < .01; eta2 = .937), stand and reach (LC 1: F = 22.82; p =< .01; eta2 = .084/LC 2: F = 29.07; p < .01; eta2 = .086), and jumping sideways (LC 1: F = 7.19; p =< .01; eta2 = .028/LC 2: F = 3.79; p = .05; eta2 = .012). Boys achieved better results in the 20 m sprint (LC 1: F = 5.69; p = .02; eta2 = .022/LC 2: F = 6.38; p = .02; eta2 = .020) and in the 6-minute run (LC 1: F = 17.90; p =< .01; eta2 = .029/LC 2: F = 115.31; p =< .01; eta2 = .078).

Additional significant interactions between time and sex were found for LC 1 in sit-ups (F = 9.52; p < .01; eta2 = .037) and 6-minute run (F = 7.25; p < .01; eta2 = .029). In both test tasks, boys showed higher gains than girls. For LC 2, girls showed higher gains than boys in stand and reach (F = 5.23; p = .02; eta2 = .029).

F-values (F), significance (p), and partial eta squared (eta2) from rmANOVAS for both longitudinal cohorts (LC 1+ LC 2) are shown in Table 4.

Table 4.

rmANOVA results for longitudinal cohort 1 (LC 1) and longitudinal cohort 2 (LC 2).

Task LC (start year) Effects F = p = Eta2 =
20 m sprint (sec) LC 1 (2008) Time 552.18 <.01 .688
Sex 5.69 .02 .022
Timesex 0.66 .42 .003
LC 2 (2009) Time 336.93 <.01 .518
Sex 6.38 .02 .020
Timesex 0.25 .62 .001

Standing long jump (cm) LC 1 (2008) Time 207.79 <.01 .467
Sex 2.05 .15 .009
Timesex 0.19 .66 .001
LC 2 (2009) Time 173.29 <.01 .369
Sex 3.14 .08 .011
Timesex 0.002 .968 .000

Balancing backwards LC 1 (2008) Time 184.28 <.01 .423
Sex 8.48 <.01 .033
Timesex 0.20 .66 .001
LC 2 (2009) Time 319.87 <.01 .505
Sex 19.11 <.01 .937
Timesex 2.64 .11 .008

Sit-ups LC 1 (2008) Time 218.54 <.01 .469
Sex 0.598 .44 .002
Timesex 9.52 <.01 .037
LC 2 (2009) Time 109.63 <.01 .261
Sex 0.29 .59 .001
Timesex 3.48 .06 .011

Push-ups LC 1 (2008) Time 270.53 <.01 .523
Sex 0.09 .77 .000
Timesex 1.84 .18 .007
LC 2 (2009) Time 276.31 <.01 .472
Sex 0.35 .55 .001
Timesex 0.54 .46 .002

Jumping sideways LC 1 (2008) Time 630.16 <.01 .717
Sex 7.19 <.01 .028
Timesex 0.12 .95 .000
LC 2 (2009) Time 1291.64 <.01 .807
Sex 3.79 .05 .012
Timesex 1.34 .25 .004

Stand and reach (cm) LC 1 (2008) Time 8.74 <.01 .034
Sex 22.82 <.01 .084
Timesex 7.95 <.01 .031
LC 2 (2009) Time 2.08 .13 .007
Sex 29.07 <.01 .086
Timesex 5.23 .02 .017

6-minute run (m) LC 1 (2008) Time 61.52 <.01 .200
Sex 17.90 <.01 .068
Timesex 7.25 <.01 .029
LC 2 (2009) Time 115.31 <.01 .273
Sex 25.90 <.01 .078
Timesex 1.04 .36 .003

BMI LC 1 (2008) Time 183.79 <.01 .423
Sex 13707.97 .09 .011
Timesex 0.04 .83 .000
LC 2 (2009) Time 83.0 <.01 .211
Sex 1.12 .29 .004
Timesex 2.99 .09 .010

3.2. Stability of Motor Performance

A measurement tracked if there was a positive relationship in subjects between measurement points [28]. The magnitude of the correlation coefficient can be estimated according to r < 0.30, low stability; r = 0.30 to 0.60, moderate; r > 0.60, moderately high [14].

Table 5 shows the correlations of the two longitudinal cohorts separated by gender.

Table 5.

Pearson correlation coefficients (r) of test tasks and BMI.

Longitudinal cohort 1 Boys (N = 137)
Test task Grades 1-2 Grades 1–3 Grades 1–4

20 m sprint .552 .633 .569
Standing long jump .635 .592 .597
Balancing backwards .589 .499 .444
Push-ups .376 .328 .311
Sit-ups .679 .534 .548
Jumping sideways .489 .469 .317
Stand and reach .593 .590 .531
6-minute run .476 .470 .499
BMI .845 .774 .804

Girls (N = 115)

Test task Grades 1-2 Grades 1–3 Grades 1–4

20 m sprint .494 .456 .340
Standing long jump .624 .500 .392
Balancing backwards .453 .438 .376
Push-ups .414 .276 .279
Sit-ups .585 .438 .377
Jumping sideways .501 .428 .268
Stand and reach .616 .655 .640
6-minute run .429 .517 .339
BMI .880 .726 .743

Longitudinal cohort 2 Boys (N = 149)

Test task Grades 1-2 Grades 1–3

20-meter sprint .604 .632
Standing long jump .535 .568
Balancing backwards .323 .467
Push-ups .297 .331
Sit-ups .523 .480
Jumping sideways .583 .512
Stand and reach .576 .512
6-minute run .625 .565
BMI .727 .707

Girls (N = 166)

Test task Grades 1-2 Grades 1–3

20 m sprint .522 .550
Standing long jump .599 .503
Balancing backwards .548 .433
Push-ups .235 .356
Sit-ups .414 .411
Jumping sideways .456 .481
Stand and reach .624 .615
6-minute run .342 .261
BMI .878 .897

p = <.01.

All correlations are significant at the level of p < .01. On average, boys showed slightly higher correlation coefficients than girls in both cohorts. Moreover, the coefficients for one period are, on average, of moderate stability. The highest correlation coefficients were found from grade 1 to grade 2, with each additional year decreasing.

The results from LC 2 largely confirm those from LC 1. Boys also showed higher coefficients than girls; however the reduction of r from grades 1-2 to grades 1–3 was less pronounced.

For both longitudinal cohorts, stand and reach and standing long jump showed the highest stability. The coefficients were constantly above r = .50. On the other hand, the correlations for push-ups were poor. Four correlations were below r = 0.30 and represented “low stability” according to Malina [14]. Correlations of BMI were considerably higher than correlations of motor tests. The values were higher than r = 0.7.

4. Discussion

The first goal of the TrieKis study was to present the motor development process of primary schoolchildren for a medium-sized city (Trier; population approximately 115,000) in Germany. The study showed that PF increased significantly over time in all test tasks for both longitudinal cohorts, except for stand and reach.

In the second part of the study, stability of children's physical fitness during primary school period was assessed. PF usually generates higher correlations in comparison to PA [23]. In the course of the study the correlation coefficients decreased slightly, indicating an expected loss of stability when the observed time frame gets larger. Concerning gender differences, boys showed higher correlation coefficients than girls in nearly all test tasks.

4.1. Development of PF

PF increased significantly over time in all test tasks and BMI for both longitudinal cohorts, except in stand and reach. The girls in LC 1 and LC 2 achieved better results than boys in balancing backwards, jumping sideways, and stand and reach. The boys of both longitudinal cohorts had better results in the 20 m sprint and in the 6-minute run. These results confirm previous research and show that girls have an advantage in coordination-based tasks and flexibility [8, 9, 29] and boys perform better in conditioning-based test tasks [10, 15, 29]. However, in our study not every conditioning-based test task revealed significantly better performance for boys. For strength-based tasks, sit-ups and push-ups, results were inconsistent. The observed range of age in this study did not include puberty, where boys especially show increased effects from strength-based training [11, 12].

As a result of physical development there was also a recognizable increase of BMI over time in both cohorts (LC 1; F = 183.79; p < .01; LC 2: F = 83.0; p < .01).

The results for both longitudinal cohorts are comparable and the second cohort confirms the findings from cohort 1.

Besides the expected overall increase in motor performance during primary school, differentiated gender effects were observed in this study. Although the observed age span did not include puberty, differences in the development of PF between genders were significant (20 m sprint; 6-minute run; stand and reach; jumping sideways; balancing backwards). Assuming that hormonal differences were not yet very pronounced in the observed age span, an explanation could be the fact that boys prefer different sports than girls, mainly due to social pressure. Additionally, even in comparable sports, training content differs between boys and girls [30].

4.2. Stability

Regarding the stability of children's PF during primary school, we found decreasing Pearson's correlation coefficients when the observed time span increased. The fact that boys showed higher correlation coefficients than girls has also been found in other studies [10].Overall, the lowest correlations were found for push-ups. This finding may be due to the fact that during the push-up task instructors had to judge correct execution, and since instructors changed every year, this may have led to measurement errors.

Correlations of stand and reach were high and constantly above r = 0.50. Correlations of BMI were considerably higher than the correlations of motor tests. This confirmed other study results [16]. The values were always above r = 0.7 and “moderately high” [14]. BMI and flexibility are probably quite stable in childhood and adolescence [15]. Falk et al. [10], McMillan and Erdmann [25], and Vandorpe et al. [26] tracked quite similar test groups and also used field-tests in their studies.

Falk et al. [10] examined 319 children (297 boys and 116 girls) from the second to the sixth grade. He found gender-specific differences as in this study. The stability of BMI (boys: r = .52; girls: r = .47) was clearly lower than in this study (LC1; boys: r = .804; girls: r = .743). Regarding standing long jump, the correlations (boys: r = .43; girls: r = .40) in this study were higher for boys (LC 1: boys: r = .597). The correlations among the girls were roughly same (LC 1: girls: r = .392).

McMillan and Erdmann [25] applied sit-ups and a sit and reach test to check strength and flexibility. Over three years of tracking (Kindergarten-Grade 2; 6.1 ± 0.3 years), they found correlations in sit-ups of r = .44 (m.) and r = .39 (f.) and in sit and reach r = .48 (m.) and .52 (f.). This study found similar and even slightly higher correlations: LC 1: sit-ups: r = .548 (m.), r = .377 (f.); LC 1: stand and reach: r = .531 (m.), r = .640 (f.). Flexibility tests seem to be more stable than strength-based tasks. This is also confirmed when the results of LC 2 are compared with McMillan and Erdmann [25].

Concerning the correlations of the coordination tests for a 2-year interval (8–10 years) at primary school age, Vandorpe et al. [26] found better stabilities (r > 0.8) than in the TrieKis study (r = .428–.512).

Summarizing the results from our study, we have to state that PF in primary school is only of moderate stability. Besides form on the day, rather unstable PA behavior of primary school children could explain this finding. Other studies have shown that PF usually generates higher correlations in comparison to PA [22]. During primary school, many children change their PA from gymnastics or unorganized PA to organized team sports with different training forms and demands [9].

4.3. Strength and Limitations of the Study

The DMT 6–18 is a quality-proven test battery that allowed standardized assessment of the PF of the Trier primary school children at 23 different schools. The sample was representative for primary schools of Trier. It was possible to examine motor development and track health-related fitness test data from a large number of boys and girls in two longitudinal cohorts for a 1-, 2-, and 3-year period.

A limitation of the study is that 20 m sprint was measured manually and not by a light-barrier system; therefore measurement inaccuracies may have occurred. Test instructors also changed several times during the course of the study, and hence differences in test execution might have occurred.

A further limitation is that only 40% of the base line took part at all three following measurements, and therefore a bias in measured PF development may have occurred. Furthermore, the second longitudinal cohort only participated from grades 1 to 3 and not from grades 1 to 4.

How the variations in maturation and physical activity levels of participants influenced test results and tracking is unknown. For further investigation, level of activity should be collected annually. In this study, data existed only for the first study year.

Moreover gender differences in performance and development were discussed. These differences change substantial during puberty and we could not rule out the possibility that at least some of the measured children had already reached puberty. In future studies, biological age needs to be assessed in order to interpret gender differences more precisely.

Finally we have to state that the results are not unrestricted generalizable beyond Germany. The age groups in grades of primary schools differ in different countries. Moreover physical education and PA of pupils differ between countries; however the PA of German children and adolescents lies within the average of European countries [31].

4.4. Conclusion

Physical fitness increased over the course of the study. Gender-specific differences were obvious, but this could not always be observed (e.g., standing long jump). Consistent with literature, girls are undoubtedly superior in flexibility and boys are better in cardiorespiratory fitness.

The results also demonstrate significant tracking for young school children. Correlations decline with reduced time frames. It is possible that tracking was lower in girls than in boys because of their earlier maturation. In total, moderate stabilities were found. Six out of 90 correlations were only “low” and 17 out of 90 correlations were “moderately high” [14]. Stabilities of BMI were continuously higher than PF.

Studies are difficult to compare due to the variety of fitness tests used, different ages of the subjects at baseline, span of the longitudinal follow-up, and different sample sizes [10, 15]. Variability in the timing and speed of the adolescent growth spurt and sexual maturation will also influence tracking.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge support by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and Open Access Publishing Fund of Karlsruhe Institute of Technology.

Abbreviations

BMI:

Body mass index

DMT 6–18:

German Motor Test 6–18

PA:

Physical activity

PF:

Physical fitness.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.Ortega F. B., Ruiz J. R., Castillo M. J., Sjöström M. Physical fitness in childhood and adolescence: a powerful marker of health. International Journal of Obesity. 2008;32(1):1–11. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Howley E. T. Type of activity: resistance, aerobic and leisure versus occupational physical activity. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2001;33(6):364–369. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200106001-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Smith J. J., Eather N., Morgan P. J., Plotnikoff R. C., Faigenbaum A. D., Lubans D. R. The health benefits of muscular fitness for children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Medicine. 2014;44(9):1209–1223. doi: 10.1007/s40279-014-0196-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rodrigues L. P., Leitão R., Lopes V. P. Physical fitness predicts adiposity longitudinal changes over childhood and adolescence. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2013;16(2):118–123. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2012.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.He Q.-Q., Wong T.-W., Du L., et al. Physical activity, cardiorespiratory fitness, and obesity among Chinese children. Preventive Medicine. 2011;52(2):109–113. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lopes V. P., Maia J. A. R., Rodrigues L. P., Malina R. Motor coordination, physical activity and fitness as predictors of longitudinal change in adiposity during childhood. European Journal of Sport Science. 2012;12(4):384–391. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2011.566368. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.London R. A., Castrechini S. A longitudinal examination of the link between youth physical fitness and academic achievement. Journal of School Health. 2011;81(7):400–408. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2011.00608.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Bös K., Ulmer J. Motor development. Monatsschrift fur Kinderheilkunde. 2003;151(1):14–21. doi: 10.1007/s00112-002-0623-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.B K., Worth A., Opper E., Oberger J., Woll A. Final Report on the Research Project. Baden-Baden, Germany: Nomos; 2009. Motorik-Modul: A study on the motor performance and physical activity of children and adolescents in Germany. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Falk B., Cohen Y., Lustig G., et al. Tracking of physical fitness components in boys and girls from the second to sixth grades. American Journal of Human Biology. 2001;13(1):65–70. doi: 10.1002/1520-6300(200101/02)13:1&#x0003c;65::AID-AJHB1008&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Beunen G., Malina R. Growth and physical performance relative to the timming of the adolsescent spurt. Exercise & Sport Sciences Reviews. 1988;16(1):503–540. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fröhlich M., Pieter A., Gießing J., et al. Strength and endurance training for children and adolescents in Germany. Current status. Leistungssport. 2009;39(2):1–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bös K., Schlenker L., Büsch D., et al. German Motor Test. Hamburg, Germany: Feldhaus; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Malina R. M. Tracking of physical activity and physical fitness across the lifespan. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 1996;67(3):48–57. doi: 10.1080/02701367.1996.10608853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ahnert J., Schneider W. Development and stability of motor abilities from preschool age to early adulthood. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie. 2007;39(1):12–24. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pate R. R., Trost S. G., Dowda M., et al. Tracking of physical activity, physical inactivity, and health-related physical fitness in rural youth. Pediatric exercise science. 1999;11(4):364–376. doi: 10.1123/pes.11.4.364. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lima R. A., Bugge A., Pfeiffer K. A., Andersen L. B. Tracking of Gross Motor Coordination From Childhood Into Adolescence. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 2017;88(1):52–59. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2016.1264566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Beunen G., Ostyn M., Simons J., et al. Development and tracking in fitness components: leuven longitudinal study on lifestyle, fitness and health. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 1997;18(3):S171–S178. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-972710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Janz K. F., Dawson J. D., Mahoney L. T. Tracking physical fitness and physical activity from childhood to adolescence: The Muscatine study. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2000;32(7):1250–1257. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200007000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Maia J. A. R., Lefevre J., Claessens A., Renson R., Vanreusel B., Beunen G. Tracking of physical fitness during adolescence: A panel study in boys. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2001;33(5):765–771. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200105000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lefevre J., Philippaerts R. M., Delvaux K., et al. Daily physical activity and physical fitness from adolescence to adulthood: A longitudinal study. American Journal of Human Biology. 2000;12(4):487–497. doi: 10.1002/1520-6300(200007/08)12:4&#x0003c;487::AID-AJHB8&#x0003e;3.3.CO;2-N. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Matton L., Thomis M., Wijndaele K., et al. Tracking of physical fitness and physical activity from youth to adulthood in females. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2006;38(6):1114–1120. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000222840.58767.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Fortier M. D., Katzmarzyk P. T., Malina R. M., Bouchard C. Seven-year stability of physical activity and musculoskeletal fitness in the Canadian population. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2001;33(11):1905–1911. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200111000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Trudeau F., Shephard R. J., Arsenault F., Laurencelle L. Tracking of physical fitness from childhood to adulthood. Canadian Journal of Applied Physiology. 2003;28(2):257–271. doi: 10.1139/h03-020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.McMillan C. S., Erdmann L. D. Tracking adiposity and health-related physical fitness test performances from early childhood through elementary school. Pediatric exercise science. 2010;22(2):231–244. doi: 10.1123/pes.22.2.231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Vandorpe B., Vandendriessche J., Vaeyens R., et al. Relationship between sports participation and the level of motor coordination in childhood: A longitudinal approach. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2012;15(3):220–225. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kromeyer-Hauschild K., Wabitsch M., Kunze D., et al. Percentiles of body mass index in children and adolescents evaluated from different regional German studies. Monatsschrift fur Kinderheilkunde. 2001;149(8):807–818. doi: 10.1007/s001120170107. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Twisk J. W. R., Kemper H. C. G., Mellenbergh G. J. Mathematical and analytical aspects of tracking. Epidemiologic Reviews. 1994;16(2):165–183. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.epirev.a036149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Baquet G., Twisk J. W. R., Kemper H. C. G., Van Praagh E., Berthoin S. Longitudinal follow-up of fitness during childhood: Interaction with physical activity. American Journal of Human Biology. 2006;18(1):51–58. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.20466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Weimann E. Gender-related differences in elite gymnasts: The female athlete triad. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2002;92(5):2146–2152. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00572.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Schmidt S. C. E., Henn A., Albrecht C., Woll A. Physical activity of German children and adolescents 2003-2012: The MoMo-study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2017;14(11, article no. 1375) doi: 10.3390/ijerph14111375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from BioMed Research International are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES