Abstract
Human motor cortex can activate pelvic floor muscles (PFM), but the motor cortical representation of the PFM is not well characterized. PFM representation is thought to be focused in the supplementary motor area (SMA). Here we examine the degree to which PFM representation is distributed between SMA and the primary motor cortex (M1), and how this representation is utilized to activate the PFM in different coordination patterns. We show that two types of coordination patterns involving PFM can be voluntarily accessed: one activates PFM independently of synergists and a second activates PFM prior to and in proportion with synergists (in this study, the gluteus maximus muscle – GMM). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) showed that both coordination patterns involve overlapping activation in SMA and M1, suggesting the presence of intermingled but independent neural populations that access the different patterns. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) confirmed SMA and M1 representation for the PFM. TMS also showed that, equally for SMA and M1, PFM can be activated during rest but GMM can only be activated after voluntary drive to GMM, suggesting that these populations are distinguished by activation threshold. We conclude that PFM representation is broadly distributed in SMA and M1 in humans.
Introduction
The central nervous system has several efferent centres that cause activity in pelvic floor muscles (PFM). For example, during urine storage, low-level afferent firing produced by the bladder causes the onset of events that involve control centres at different levels in the nervous system1–4. First, the onset of spinal guarding reflexes may facilitate activity in the bladder base, urethra, and external urethral sphincter, while inhibiting activity in the detrusor muscle1,2. Second, activity in the rostral pons may result in increased activity in the external urethral sphincter1,2. Third, activity in the motor cortex may modulate activity in descending reticulospinal and corticospinal tracts to pelvic floor motoneurons1–5. The involvement of the motor cortex in controlling PFM via corticospinal projections can also be observed in other behaviours such as voluntary switching between voiding and storage, both for the bladder and for the bowel1,2,6–8, as well as during voluntary movements that require activation of PFM2,9–12. However, the contribution of the motor cortex to the control of PFM is sparsely studied; and therefore, the organization and function of the motor cortical representation of the human pelvic floor are not well understood.
Interestingly, potential dysfunction within the motor cortical representation of the pelvic floor has been described in disorders associated with pelvic pain, making this representation an important area to study. Altered cortical control of PFM has been suggested in clinical conditions, such as chronic pelvic pain and incontinence7,13–18, and the extent of motor cortical involvement has been suggested to predict how the patient would respond to the treatment15,17,18. For example, in recent resting state neuroimaging studies – when compared to healthy controls – chronic pelvic pain patients exhibited altered frequency content of the fMRI signal obtained from a pelvic-specific motor region and altered connectivity between that motor region and other brain regions13,14. Furthermore, in recent brain morphology studies – when compared to healthy controls – chronic pelvic pain patients displayed significant differences in gray matter volume and diffusion anisotropy in motor cortical areas consistent with control of the pelvic floor19,20. In incontinence patients, interaction between the periaqueductal gray (PAG) and the motor cortical representation of the pelvic floor might be altered15. This altered brain-bladder control can be adjusted for by learning voluntary PFM activation and urge suppression strategies15,21. Within this patient group, the extent of motor cortical involvement during bladder filling predicted responses to PFM training as a treatment option for urinary incontinence15. In stroke patients, cortical lesions can result in various bladder and bowel dysfunctions, including urinary and faecal incontinence18. In a case study of two patients, it has been recently suggested that stimulation of the motor cortical representation of the pelvic floor constitutes a new treatment for patients with refractory pelvic and perineal pain17. Therefore, characterizing the pelvic floor motor cortical representation may enable new brain-based therapies to be designed for these conditions.
A first part of this characterization is to determine whether the pelvic floor motor cortical representation is localized or distributed. A long-standing belief is that the control and representation of PFM occur in a localized region in the medial wall of the precentral gyrus toward the supplementary motor area (SMA)1,2,9,10,12,15. Under this hypothesis, coordination between PFM and synergistic muscles would emerge from a cortical structuring scheme involving co-activation of SMA to activate the PFM and the primary motor cortex (M1) to activate the synergist. However, this is not necessarily true. Research in animal models suggests that the role of the motor cortex in controlling voluntary movements emerges from distributed networks rather than discrete and localized representations of individual muscles22–25. Research in humans also points to individual muscles being represented in both SMA and M126. Thus, in addition to the pelvic floor representation in SMA, the pelvic floor musculature may also have a representation in M1 that overlaps with the representation of muscles with which the PFM synergistically co-active. We have previously reported indirect evidence of this overlapping representation between pelvic floor and lower limb muscles in M19,11. Therefore, in the present study we used a more refined paradigm to map the PFM motor cortical representation in which we carefully scaled cortical activity to different levels using voluntary muscle activation, and determined the response in SMA and M1 using both functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS).
Results
Summary
Electromyography (EMG) results showed that two types of coordination patterns involving pelvic floor muscles (PFM) can be voluntarily accessed: an “isolated-pelvic” coordination pattern that activates PFM independently of synergists, in this study, the gluteus maximus muscle – GMM; and a “gluteal-pelvic” coordination pattern that activates PFM prior to and in proportion with the synergistic GMM. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) results showed that both coordination patterns involve overlapping activation in SMA and M1, suggesting the presence of intermingled but independent neural populations that access the different coordination patterns. TMS results confirmed a representation of the PFM distributed in both SMA and M1. Similarly, for each motor region (SMA and M1), TMS results showed that PFM potentials can be evoked during rest but GMM potentials can only be evoked after voluntary pre-activation of GMM, suggesting that these intermingled populations may be distinguished by activation threshold.
EMG characterization of coordination patterns
Our EMG recordings confirmed the existence of the isolated-pelvic and gluteal-pelvic coordination patterns we have described previously9. Cuing participants to voluntarily contract their PFM did not generate any associated activity in either the GMM or the first dorsal interosseous muscle (FDI, Fig. 1A). Cuing participants to voluntarily contract their GMM induced coupled activity in the PFM, and this activity led GMM activity by over 100 ms (Fig. 1A – inset). Voluntary activation of the FDI activated neither the PFM nor the GMM (Fig. 1A). In contrast to our prior work9, here we also varied the intensity of the primary muscle activation to look for evidence of coupled and coordinated neural drive to the PFM. We found that increasing GMM voluntary activation was associated with an increase in PFM co-activation and a decrease in the onset latency measure that was the difference between PFM onset and GMM onset, both in an example participant (Fig. 1B) and in group data (Fig. 1C, Table 1). Linear mixed effects regression models applied to the population of participants verified the following (see detailed statistical results in Table 1).
Table 1.
A. PFM co-activation magnitude during GMM scaled voluntary activation and during FDI scaled voluntary activation | ||
PFM during GMM Fig. 1C, left | PFM during FDI Fig. 1C, left | |
Population Mean Slope | 0.5320 [%-PFM-MVC per %-GMM-MVC] |
0.0306 [%-PFM-MVC per %-FDI-MVC] |
standard error (SE) | 0.0608 | 0.0100 |
p-value | p < 0.0001 | p = 0.0039 |
95% confidence interval | 0.4091, 0.6548 | 0.0100, 0.0508 |
Population Mean Intercept
[%-PFM-MVC] |
22.7975 | 1.5960 |
standard error (SE) | 2.8943 | 0.5070 |
p-value | p < 0.0001 | p = 0.0031 |
95% confidence interval | 16.9524, 28.6427 | 0.5714, 2.6206 |
GMM co-activation magnitude during PFM scaled voluntary activation | ||
GMM during PFM | ||
Population Mean Slope
[%-GMM-MVC per %-PFM-MVC] |
0.0251 | |
standard error (SE) | 0.0049 | |
p-value | p < 0.0001 | |
95% confidence interval | 0.0151, 0.0351 | |
Population Mean Intercept
[%-GMM-MVC] |
0.4364 | |
standard error (SE) | 0.1589 | |
p-value | p = 0.0089 | |
95% confidence interval | 0.1156, 0.7573 | |
B. PFM co-activation latency during GMM scaled voluntary activation | ||
PFM during GMM
Fig. 1C, right |
||
Population Mean Slope
[ms per %-GMM-MVC] |
−0.7444 | |
standard error (SE) | 0.3404 | |
p-value | p = 0.0345 | |
95% confidence interval | −0.05701, −1.4318 | |
Population Mean Intercept
[ms: milliseconds, PFM leading GMM] |
139.85 | |
standard error (SE) | 20.0510 | |
p-value | p < 0.0001 | |
95% confidence interval | 180.35, 99.3596 |
PFM/FDI relation may not be meaningful since the increase in PFM co-activation magnitude is greater during GMM voluntary activation compared to voluntary FDI activation. Fig. 1C, left: estimated slope difference = 0.5066, SE = 0.1621, p = 0.0033, 95%CI (0.1790, 0.8343). PFM co-activation magnitude is greater during low GMM voluntary activation compared to low voluntary FDI activation. Fig. 1C, left: estimated intercept difference = 21.1847 [%-PFM-MVC], SE = 2.5868, p < 0.0001, 95%CI (15.9564, 26.4129).
The increase in PFM co-activation magnitude during GMM voluntary activation is greater than the increase in GMM co-activation magnitude during PFM voluntary activation. Estimated slope difference = 0.4340, SE = 0.0115, p < 0.0001), 95%CI (0.4115, 0.4565). PFM co-activation magnitude during low GMM voluntary activation is greater than GMM co-activation magnitude during low PFM voluntary activation (estimated intercept difference = 24.0397, %-MVC, SE = 3.2886, p < 0.0001, 95%CI (17.3982, 30.6812).
Positive co-activation latency means that PFM co-activation happened before the associated GMM voluntary activation.
Statistics are based on linear mixed effects regression models; see METHODS section.
PFM (pelvic floor muscles); GMM (gluteus maximus muscle); FDI (first dorsal interosseous muscle).
First, PFM co-activation magnitude increases with increasing GMM voluntary activation magnitude (Fig. 1C, left; Table 1A). Second, PFM co-activation magnitude does slightly increase with increasing FDI voluntary activation magnitude (Fig. 1C, left; Table 1A). However, we do not consider PFM/FDI relation to be meaningful since the increase in PFM co-activation magnitude is greater during GMM voluntary activation compared to voluntary FDI activation (Fig. 1C, left; Table 1A), and, PFM co-activation magnitude is greater during low GMM voluntary activation compared to low voluntary FDI activation (Fig. 1C, left; Table 1A). Third, GMM co-activation magnitude does slightly increase with increasing PFM voluntary activation magnitude (Table 1A). However, the increase in PFM co-activation magnitude during GMM voluntary activation is greater than the increase in GMM co-activation magnitude during PFM voluntary activation (Table 1A), and, PFM co-activation magnitude during low GMM voluntary activation is greater than GMM co-activation magnitude during low PFM voluntary activation (Table 1A). Fourth, increasing GMM voluntary activation decreases the timing of PFM co-activation relative to the onset of GMM voluntary activation (Fig. 1C, right; Table 1B). As expected, population mean intercept was significant, as PFM led the GMM by ~140 milliseconds (Fig. 1C, right; Table 1B).
To summarize, human PFM can be activated independently of GMM across a wide range of activation intensities, but PFM co-activate in proportion to and in advance of GMM during voluntary GMM activation. PFM activity leads GMM activity by a shorter duration as voluntary GMM activity becomes more intense, but PFM activity does not become synchronous with or trail GMM activity for any contraction intensity. The asymmetry in coupling between the PFM and GMM points toward independent neural populations in the motor cortex that are used to control the isolated-pelvic and gluteal-pelvic coordination patterns, but does not localize these populations as we begin to in the next section.
fMRI localization of motor cortical control centres
We used two independent groups of participants in our fMRI analysis to enhance the validity of our findings. First, using fMRI data collected while a first group of participants (Group A) voluntarily activated FDI and GMM in blocks at a comfortable level, we observed significant brain activation in the contrast (GMM > FDI, Fig. 2A) in distinct brain areas (Z > 2.3, p < 0.05, cluster corrected), including three ROIs in the motor cortex: SMA, left M1 (M1-L) and right M1 (M1-R). In each ROI, all voxels achieved statistical significance in the contrast of voluntary GMM activation greater than voluntary FDI activation, and had at least 25% probability of belonging to a certain Brodmann area (BA: BA6 for SMA, BA4L for M1-L, BA4R for M1-R) based on the Jülich Histological Atlas. M1-L and M-1R were similar in size (453 voxels in M1-L and 441 voxels in M1-R). Second, using fMRI data collected while a different set of participants (Group B) voluntarily activated the GMM and PFM in blocks to three distinct levels: low (L), medium (M), and high (H), we observed similar changes in brain activity (as measured by BOLD signal strength) in the identified ROIs (SMA, M1-L, M1-R) during GMM scaled voluntary activation as well as during PFM scaled voluntary activation (Fig. 2B). PFM scaled voluntary activation in the primary motor cortex was symmetric: at the individual level, we observed similar number of active voxels in M1-L and M1-R (paired t-test: p = 0.1244). Activation in each ROI did not depend strongly on the gender (unpaired t-test: M1-L, p = 0.0591; M1-R, p = 0.0612).
We have hypothesized that scaling GMM voluntary activation magnitude, i.e., scaling the voluntary drive to elicit the gluteal-pelvic coordination pattern, would be associated with scaling of brain activity in motor regions related to the control of the GMM, and possibly to the control of the PFM – when assuming localized representation of the PFM in SMA and cortical structuring of the gluteal-pelvic coordination pattern. Our ANCOVA analysis (see METHODS) of the averaged BOLD signals, extracted from each ROI in each participant in Group B, showed scaling in BOLD signal strength in accordance to the activation level of GMM in the three identified motor ROIs. Specifically, on the population level, BOLD signal strength significantly increased in SMA, M1-L, and M1-R during increased voluntary GMM activation (Table 2A).
Table 2.
A. ANCOVA results in Group B, during scaled GMM voluntary activation
Testing for scaling in averaged BOLD signal extracted from three pre-defined ROIs from Group A | |||
β 1 comparing BOLD strength during L to strength during H | SMA | M1-L | M1-R |
Estimated Mean Difference (L-H) | −79.8394 | −61.0844 | −71.5869 |
standard error (SE) | 15.1499 | 8.8635 | 14.4091 |
p-value | p < 0.0001 | p < 0.0001 | p < 0.0001 |
95% confidence interval | −109.91, −49.711 | −78.6760, −43.4929 | −100.1800, −42.9889 |
β 2 comparing BOLD strength during M to strength during H | |||
Estimated Mean Difference (M-H) | −49.8340 | −42.1037 | −49.3905 |
standard error (SE) | 14.3108 | 8.1451 | 13.3617 |
p-value | p = 0.0007 | p < 0.0001 | p = 0.0004 |
95% confidence interval | −78.2371, −21.4310 | −58.2695, −25.9378 | −75.9097, −22.8713 |
B. ANCOVA results in Group B, during scaled PFM voluntary activation
Testing for scaling in averaged BOLD signal extracted from three pre-defined ROIs from Group A | |||
β 1 comparing BOLD strength during L to strength during H | SMA | M1-L | M1-R |
Estimated Mean Difference (L-H) | −29.4630 | −38.1871 | −39.1023 |
standard error (SE) | 4.8281 | 5.2601 | 5.8865 |
p-value | p < 0.0001 | p < 0.0001 | p < 0.0001 |
95% confidence interval | −39.0455, −19.8805 | −48.6270, −27.7473 | −50.7854, −27.4192 |
β 2 comparing BOLD strength during M to strength during H | |||
Estimated Mean Difference (M-H) | −28.9518 | −34.9262 | −33.9300 |
standard error (SE) | 4.7456 | 5.2520 | 5.9434 |
p-value | p < 0.0001 | p < 0.0001 | p < 0.0001 |
95% confidence interval | −38.3705, −19.5330 | −45.3499, −24.5024 | −45.7259, −22.1340 |
Scaling in BOLD signal strength during three distinct levels of voluntary activation, low (L), medium (M), and high (H), is investigated using an ANCOVA model. See MEHODS section. Aij = Intercept + β1 * L + β2 * M + 0 * (H) + β3 * Rij + Ui + Eij Aij is the 75 percentile of the pre-assumed Active period, while Rij is the 50 percentile of the preceding pre-assumed Rest period. L is identifier of low activation, M is identifier of medium activation, and H is identifier of high activation.
Each participant is assumed to have a unique intercept [Intercept + Ui].
SMA (supplementary motor area); M1-L (primary motor cortex, left hemisphere); M1-R (primary motor cortex, right hemisphere).
Based on previous studies9–12,27,28, we hypothesized that scaling BOLD signal strength in SMA would be to activate and scale PFM activity and that scaling BOLD signal strength in M1-L and M1-R would be to activate and scale GMM activity. However, the following fMRI results during scaling of the isolated-pelvic coordination pattern did not validate this initial hypothesis of localized control and representation of the PFM in SMA. Our ANCOVA analysis of BOLD signal strength showed scaling in BOLD signal strength in accordance to the activation level of PFM not only in SMA ROI but also bilaterally in the primary motor cortex in M1-L and M1-R ROIs. Specifically, on the population level, BOLD signal strength significantly increased in SMA, M1-L, and M1-R during increased voluntary PFM activation (Table 2B). In contrast to previous studies, we can conclude that cortical control of human PFM may extend to motor regions lateral to the medial wall of the motor cortex.
To summarize, the above fMRI results could not validate our initial hypotheses of localized control and representation of the PFM in SMA, and that during voluntary and scaled GMM activation, scaling of brain activity in SMA would be to activate and scale PFM activity and that scaling of brain activity in M1-L and M1-R would act solely to activate and scale GMM activity. Therefore, the TMS study was particularly required to determine whether the cortical representation of the PFM is localized in SMA or distributed in the motor cortex.
TMS evidence of non-localized control of human PFM
Confirming our fMRI findings, we found that hotspots for the PFM were not confined over a single cortical region (Fig. 3). Inspecting individual fMRI and TMS data from 7 participants who had both procedures revealed (1) distributed activation in the contrast PFM greater than rest extending posterior and lateral to medial SMA (consistent with M1), and (2) PFM motor evoked potentials (MEPs) did not appear confined to the medial wall and SMA (Fig. 3A). MEP size (normalized to SMA) revealed that PFM MEPs induced by stimulating M1 were significantly larger than PFM MEPs induced by stimulating a non-motor reference region (Fig. 3B; p < 0.001, paired t-test), but were not significantly different from PFM MEPs induced by stimulating SMA (Fig. 3B; p = 0.99, paired t-test). The TMS map of the PFM, averaged across participants, broadly mirrored the fMRI activation map of the PFM (Fig. 3C).
More detailed stimulation over the SMA hotspot for the PFM (Fig. 4, left) or the M1 hotspot for the PFM (Fig. 4, right) revealed very similar findings. First, we note that it was possible to induce MEPs in the PFM at rest, but not possible to induce MEPs in the GMM at rest or during PFM voluntary activation; however, as previously observed28, it was possible to induce MEPs in the GMM during GMM voluntary activation (Fig. 4A). This result immediately suggests that there are two neural populations in the motor cortex related to the activation of these muscles, and that these populations are distinguished by baseline excitability.
Additionally, stimulating either PFM hotspots during different levels of PFM voluntary pre-activation – then during different levels of GMM voluntary pre-activation – resulted in modulating MEPs induced in the PFM, and in the PFM and the GMM, respectively (Fig. 4B, Table 3A,B). Changes in MEP magnitude with changes in voluntary activation were significant. First, in activating the isolated-pelvic coordination pattern, we found that scaling PFM voluntary activation was associated with modulation in peak-to-peak values of MEPs induced in the PFM (Fig. 4B: columns 1 & 3, 1st row; Table 3A), but was not associated with induction of MEPs in the GMM (Fig. 4B: columns 1 & 3, 2nd row; Table 3B). Second, in activating the gluteal-pelvic coordination pattern, we found that scaling GMM voluntary activation was associated with modulation in peak-to-peak values of MEPs induced in the PFM (Fig. 4B: columns 2 & 4, 1st row; Table 3A) and in the GMM (Fig. 4B: columns 2 & 4, 2nd row; Table 3B).
Table 3.
A. Response in muscle group of interest (PFM) | SMA | M1-L | ||
PFM MEP (% max) | Vol. PFM | Vol. GMM | Vol. PFM | Vol. GMM |
Population Mean Slope
[%-PFM-MAX-MEP per %-PFM-MVC] |
0.5048 | 0.3951 | 0.5324 | 0.3864 |
standard error (SE) | 0.0574 | 0.1003 | 0.0992 | 0.0662 |
p-value | p = 0.0001 | p = 0.0076 | p = 0.0017 | p = 0.0011 |
95% confidence interval | 0.3642, 0.6453 | 0.1497, 0.6405 | 0.2897, 0.7751 | 0.2244 to 0.5484 |
Population Mean Intercept
[%-PFM-MAX-MEP] |
30.1635 | 35.7143 | 25.5859 | 28.0732 |
standard error (SE) | 2.4474 | 4.8129 | 5.5339 | 3.1934 |
p-value | p < 0.0001 | p = 0.0003 | p = 0.0036 | p = 0.0001 |
95% confidence interval | 24.1750, 36.1521 | 23.9377, 47.4910 | 12.0451, 39.1268 | 20.2591, 35.8873 |
B. Response in muscle group of interest (GMM) | SMA | M1-L | ||
GMM MEP (% max) | Vol. PFM | Vol. GMM | Vol. PFM | Vol. GMM |
Population Mean Slope
[%-GMM-MAX-MEP per %-GMM-MVC] |
−0.0230 | 0.6835 | 0.2369 | 0.5534 |
standard error (SE) | 0.0484 | 0.1599 | 0.0572 | 0.1458 |
p-value | p = 0.6506 | p = 0.0052 | p = 0.0061 | p = 0.0090 |
95% confidence interval | −0.1413, 0.0953 | 0.2924, 1.0747 | 0.0970, 0.3768 | 0.1966, 0.9101 |
Population Mean Intercept
[%-GMM-MAX-MEP] |
4.7665 | 34.5084 | 4.3409 | 39.9516 |
standard error (SE) | 2.1595 | 8.2745 | 1.5374 | 4.3683 |
p-value | p = 0.0694 | p = 0.0059 | p = 0.0302 | p < 0.0001 |
95% confidence interval | −0.5175, 10.0505 | 14.2615, 54.7553 | 0.5791, 8.1028 | 29.2628, 50.6405 |
C. Onset of MEP during GMM scaled voluntary activation | SMA | M1-L | ||
MEP in PFM | MEP in GMM | MEP in PFM | MEP in GMM | |
Population Mean Slope
[ms per %-GMM-MVC] |
0.0284 | 0.0240 | 0.0021 | 0.0144 |
standard error (SE) | 0.0177 | 0.0156 | 0.0174 | 0.0135 |
p-value | p = 0.1585 | p = 0.1754 | p = 0.9090 | p = 0.3275 |
95% confidence interval | −0.0148, 0.0716 | −0.0142, 0.0622 | −0.0406, 0.0447 | −0.0187, 0.0475 |
Population Mean Intercept
[ms: milliseconds] |
21.3103 | 18.3875 | 21.4185 | 18.7656 |
standard error (SE) | 0.8234 | 1.3216 | 0.6752 | 0.6972 |
p-value | p < 0.0001 | p < 0.0001 | p < 0.0001 | p < 0.0001 |
95% confidence interval | 19.2956, 23.3249 | 15.1536, 21.6213 | 19.7663, 23.0708 | 17.0597, 20.4715 |
Activating the isolated-pelvic coordination pattern, scaling PFM voluntary activation (columns 2 & 4) was not associated with induction of MEPs in the GMM, as expected from the EMG study results. Maximum co-activation of GMM [%-GMM-MVC] was small: 4.6501 while stimulating SMA and 3.9290 while stimulating M1-L.
The onset difference was significant, the onset of MEPs induced in the PFM happened after the onset of the MEPs induced in the GMM, regardless of the activation level of the GMM. While stimulating SMA, estimated intercept difference [onset of PFM MEP - onset of GMM MEP] = 2.7468 ms, SE = 0.7532, p = 0.0003, 95%CI (4.2259, 1.2678). While stimulating M1-L, estimated intercept difference [onset of PFM MEP - onset of GMM MEP] = 2.5997 ms, SE = 0.7994, p = 0.0012, 95%CI (4.1694, 1.0301).
Each PFM hotspot, SMA and M1-L, was stimulated under different levels of voluntary pre-activation.
Statistics are based on linear mixed effects regression models; see METHODS section.
PFM (pelvic floor muscles); GMM (gluteus maximus muscle).
Vol. PFM: Scaling PFM voluntary activation.
Vol. GMM: Scaling GMM voluntary activation.
Finally, during the activation of the gluteal-pelvic coordination pattern and regardless of activation level of the GMM, the onset of MEPs induced in the PFM was about 3 ms behind the onset of the MEPs induced in the GMM (Fig. 4C, Table 3C). The onset difference was significant: the onset of MEPs induced in the PFM happened after the onset of the MEPs induced in the GMM, regardless of the activation level of the GMM (Fig. 4C, Table 3C). Additionally, MEP onsets were not significantly modulated by voluntary drive: we found that increasing activation level of the GMM was not associated with change in the onset of MEPs induced in the GMM or in the PFM (Table 3C). We note that the intensity of the TMS stimulation we used did not produce supraphysiological EMG in the GMM: minimum MEP magnitudes were significantly less than maximum unperturbed background EMG activity in the 20 ms prior to the TMS pulse (p = 0.0141, paired t-test). Collectively, these results may indicate a lack of TMS evidence that there is a subcortical delay in excitation of gluteal activation relative to pelvic floor activation when activating the gluteal-pelvic coordination pattern.
To summarize, TMS results suggest that activating the isolated-pelvic coordination pattern modulates motor cortical excitability of pelvic-specific neurons in both SMA and M1, while activating the gluteal-pelvic coordination pattern modulates motor cortical excitability of the pelvic-specific neurons as well as the intermingled gluteal-specific neurons in both SMA and M1. Collectively, these results confirm that PFM representation is not just localized in SMA but extends to M1, and that the PFM and the synergistic GMM may be coordinated via the activation of intermingled cortical neurons in both SMA and M1.
Discussion
Here we find that the pelvic floor muscles (PFM) are represented broadly in the human motor cortex, both in SMA and M1. Our results suggest that the cortical representation controlling PFM overlaps with the cortical representation controlling synergistic muscles (such as the gluteals, GMM); however, neurons comprising the pelvic floor representation may be distinct from the synergist representation in activation threshold. Furthermore, our results suggest that the PFM motor cortical representation may be accessed independently of the synergist representation, yet PFM and their synergistic muscles may be cortically coordinated via the co-activation of intermingled neurons. We graphically summarize our findings (Fig. 5).
Traditionally, the motor cortical representation of the PFM has been thought to be dominated by a region in the medial wall of the precentral gyrus toward SMA1,2,11,12,29–32, and the contribution of M1 has not been fully recognized11,29–32. There are at least three reasons why the M1 representation of the PFM has not been apparent in previous studies. First, studies that use only fMRI and seek specificity in the pelvic floor representation by contrasting brain activity during pelvic floor contraction with brain activity during contraction of another muscle may not have adequately considered co-activation patterns of the two muscles. For example, pelvic floor contraction has been contrasted with toe muscle contraction12, but we have recently shown that pelvic floor and toe muscles co-activate during muscle contraction11. Therefore, assuming that M1 representations of the pelvic floor and the synergist overlap, the contrast may downplay the contribution of M1. Second, previous studies did not typically examine the PFM with TMS in addition to fMRI as we have done here; in a single study that did use TMS, separate M1 and SMA activation hotspots for the PFM appeared to emerge27. Third, and perhaps most interestingly, the choice of the experimental excitation of the pelvic floor could engage different brain networks that are differentially connected to SMA versus M1. We have recently confirmed in humans that trunk muscles have both an SMA and M1 representation, but using resting state functional connectivity, we found that these representations appear to be communicating with different brain networks26. We have previously published preliminary evidence that this dual representation also applies to the PFM, with the SMA component of the representation preferentially communicating with brain areas known to be involved in the sensory representation of bladder state (such as the posterior insula)11. Therefore, studies that excite the pelvic floor via bladder filling may preferentially activate SMA compared to M129,30,32. Our present study suggests that the M1 representation of the PFM is at least as strong as the SMA representation of the PFM, and enables new studies to decode how these regions may cooperate to achieve different types of tasks.
Our results, also, relate to a long-standing debate about the source of neural coupling that assembles muscles into functional groups, termed muscle synergies. Work in animal and human models suggest that circuits of sub-cortical interneurons may encode specific patterns of muscle activity that can be activated by cortical neurons33–38. Additionally, a different form of muscle coordination is that coordination of muscles can be structured at a cortical level22–24,39–44. In this view, motor cortical output can independently reach different muscles, but is automatically coordinated at the cortical level during task performance45. Our results are in line with previous suggestions of cortical structuring of muscle synergies by overlapping motor representations23–25. However, our work goes on to make the novel suggestion that temporal characteristics of muscle coordination may be encoded by differences in excitability of separate motor cortical populations. Our TMS results provide evidence that the two intermingled populations that control the PFM and GMM are distinct in their excitability thresholds – relatively low threshold for the PFM and relatively high threshold for the GMM. If a common input is distributed to both populations during voluntary activation of the gluteal-pelvic coordination pattern, recruitment of the motor cortical neurons for the PFM would potentially occur first, analogous to the size principle of spinal motor neuron recruitment46. Lower excitability threshold of the motor cortical neurons that control the PFM in SMA and M1 could be the result of differences in motor cortical neuron size for different muscles47, or the result of GMM-independent and PFM-specific excitatory pre-synaptic inputs to continuously regulate urine storage1,2,6,48,49.
Our work is limited in that we did not study other movements that co-activate the PFM to test generalizability of our findings; for example, finding representations of the PFM intermingled with other coordinated trunk and arm muscles (e.g. deltoid, see50). Also, it is possible that the representation of the PFM could be localized to SMA, and apparent activation of the PFM during M1 stimulation may results from stimulus spread. We do not find this explanation plausible for three reasons. First, this would not explain the scaling of BOLD signal in M1 during activation of the isolated-pelvic coordination pattern when the GMM muscle is inactive and activity in other non-PFM muscles (such as the abdominals) is likely quite modest during submaximal PFM contraction51. Second, responses in the PFM to stimulation of M1 were no smaller than responses to stimulation of SMA, which would be expected as the stimulation moved farther away from the true representation. Third, data in previous TMS studies suggest separate representations of the PFM in SMA and M127.
Our work has implications for localizing dysfunction in disorders of the human pelvic floor. In previous studies, we have focused on the medial wall of the motor cortex, in particular SMA, as the main location of PFM motor cortical control, as well as a possible contributor to urologic chronic pelvic pain syndrome (UCPPS)9,11,13,14. Our recent resting state fMRI research provides evidence of altered motor control in patients with UCPPS, based on SMA frequency content and SMA connectivity with other brain regions13,14. Furthermore, our novel findings extend the representation and control of the PFM to motor regions beyond the medial wall of the motor cortex; thus, expanding our discovery process to new areas that were not previously considered in research on UCPPS. Therefore, basing our analysis on PFM-specific M1, may provide additional evidence related to the contribution of M1 to UCPPS and/or the distinction between the representation of the PFM in SMA and M1. These findings may be useful in understanding underlying mechanisms visceral sensitivity in different disorders that are associated with chronic pelvic pain; for example, comparing patients with UCPPS to patients with functional gastrointestinal disorders. Furthermore, these findings may be useful to extend promising research on the analgesic effects of repetitive TMS in UCPPS17,52,53, and anorectal disorders8, by providing additional potential targets (either active or control).
Methods
Participant population
We performed three studies and recruited a total of 43 healthy participants with age (mean ± SD) of 30.0 ± 5.5 (range: 22–46 years). Participants included 25 women with age 29.2 ± 6.1 (range: 22–46 years) and 18 men with age 28.7 ± 4.6 (range: 24–43 years). There was no difference in age by sex (p = 0.7628, unpaired t-test). Participants could be included if they were older than 18 years of age, were able to participate in the informed consent process, were safe to be scanned by magnetic resonance imaging, were safe to be stimulated with transcranial magnetic stimulation, and reported no history of neurological conditions or chronic pain. Female participants were deferred if they were in the menstrual phase. In each study, participants were asked to void at the beginning of the experimental session so that they would have no bladder or bowel urgency during the session – no sessions were interrupted due to urgency. Each study was completed in a single day with each participant. We performed these studies at the University of Southern California (USC), and the USC Institutional Review Board approved all procedures. All aspects of the study conformed to the principles described in the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided informed consent.
Overview of methods
In order to determine whether the pelvic floor motor cortical representation is localized or distributed we performed three separate studies using different experimental modalities: electromyography (EMG), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). All studies focused on two main types of coordination patterns that involve the pelvic floor muscles (PFM): “isolated-pelvic” pattern that activates the PFM independently of the gluteus maximus muscle (GMM, a non-PFM synergistic muscle group; PFM is the primary muscle) and “gluteal-pelvic” pattern that co-activates the PFM with the GMM (GMM is the primary muscle). These coordination patterns have been described previously9,50,54, but have not been fully characterized at either the muscle or brain level. The isolated-pelvic coordination pattern is elicited by instructing participants to contract their pelvic floor as if to stop the flow of urine (note that it does not involve an instruction to “isolate” the PFM). The gluteal-pelvic coordination pattern is elicited by instructing participants to contract their gluteal muscles (note that it does not involve an instruction to co-activate the PFM). We hypothesized that motor cortex contains a location that primarily represents the PFM in the medial wall of the precentral gyrus located anteriorly toward the supplementary motor area (SMA)9,10,12,27 and a bilateral representation specific to the GMM located posteriorly toward the primary motor cortex (M1). We hypothesized that the isolated-pelvic coordination pattern is produced by activity focused in the localized PFM representation in SMA, and that the gluteal-pelvic coordination pattern is structured in the cortex by co-activation of the separate and localized PFM and GMM representations. In the first study, we used EMG to determine if isolated-pelvic and gluteal-pelvic coordination patterns were stable; i.e., they would be observed at different levels of voluntary drive. In the second study, we tested our hypotheses by first localizing the hypothesized PFM and GMM representations, and then examining how brain activity would change in these regions as the voluntary drive to elicit isolated-pelvic, or gluteal-pelvic, coordination pattern was varied. In the third study, we tested the specificity of the hypothesized PFM and GMM representational areas by determining how their motor cortical excitability would vary as the voluntary drive to elicit isolated-pelvic, or gluteal-pelvic, coordination pattern was varied. The following sections describe the acquisition and analysis methods for each modality (EMG, fMRI, and TMS).
EMG acquisition
In forty-three healthy participants (25 women + 18 men), we measured muscle activation using EMG to define characteristics of PFM activation patterns (isolated-pelvic and gluteal-pelvic); we did not analyse EMG data from one female participant because it was an incomplete dataset. We also analysed activation of a finger muscle to establish an appropriate control muscle group that is not associated with coordinated PFM activity9. We chose the right first dorsal interosseous muscle (FDI) as the control muscle because we have previously verified that the FDI does not co-activate with the PFM9. We did not choose a trunk or a lower limb muscle as the control because of the known synergistic coupling between PFM and surprisingly distal muscles, such as the toe and shoulder muscles11,50.
With the participant resting in a supine position, we recorded surface EMG data from the PFM, the right GMM, and the right (FDI). We recorded an EMG signal from the PFM using a medical-grade rectal EMG sensor (Pathway Rectal EMG Sensor, The Prometheus Group, Dover, NH), which provided a bipolar recording from two bar electrodes – 12 mm apart, with dimensions 30 mm by 7 mm – mounted longitudinally along a cylindrical plug-type applicator. The rectal EMG sensor is 50 mm long with a stopper that ensures that one end of the sensor rests stably at the anal verge. The rectal EMG sensor likely recorded an aggregate signal from the PFM that included the anal sphincter and levator ani, with possible small contributions from more distant muscles55. Activity of the PFM has been previously measured using a rectal sensor, and does not contain significant cross-talk from the GMM9,50,54. We recorded EMG signals from the GMM and FDI with miniature electrode/preamplifiers (DELSYS, Boston, MA) with two silver recording surfaces, 5 mm long and 10 mm apart. The EMG preamplifier filters had a bandwidth of 20–450 Hz, with gains of 10,000 for the PFM and 1000 for the GMM and FDI, and a sampling rate of 2000 Hz.
Participants performed separate trials, each of which involved voluntary activation of a different coordination pattern. For all trial types, we first acquired EMG data corresponding to maximal voluntary contraction (MVC) of the given coordination pattern, by adopting the same conditions of the following activation trials. In finger trials, we instructed participants to contract their FDI muscle to generate index finger abduction. In isolated-pelvic trials, we instructed participants to contract their PFM as if to stop the flow of urine. In gluteal-pelvic trials, we instructed participants to “squeeze” their GMM. For each coordination pattern, participants received visual EMG feedback from the primary muscle guiding them to produce muscle contractions to 15%, 35%, and 45% MVC (in random order across participants). For each coordination pattern, each participant produced 20 contractions per %MVC level. Each contraction was cued randomly in time. Once a given contraction was cued, an audio signal ramped up and down in pitch to guide the participant through a smooth activation/deactivation over a period of 2 seconds.
EMG analysis
We processed the EMG data to calculate (1) the onset of each voluntary activation for each primary muscle group, (2) the onset of associated activations for the non-primary muscle groups, and (3) the maximum amplitude of the activity profile for each muscle. EMG signals from all recorded muscles were first high-pass filtered at 100 Hz (fourth-order zero-lag Butterworth filter), rectified, low-pass filtered at 30 Hz9,50, and then normalized to identically processed EMG data from the maximum activation trial. EMG data were then smoothed with a 500 ms moving average. Activation onsets were defined to occur when the smoothed EMG exceeded 2 standard deviations (SD) of the EMG baseline noise with the muscle at rest. We have used these methods in our prior work9,11; however, in the present work we did not average EMG signals across repeated activations in each participant (Fig. 1B). Each activation was quantified and expressed as percentage MVC (expressed as %MVC). All participants performed repeated and scaled muscle contractions; therefore, each participant provided multiple EMG data points for each muscle group that were used in the group analyses.
To evaluate the statistical significance of changes in magnitude and timing of muscle activation associated with increases in voluntary activation of the primary muscle of the task (Fig. 1C), we used linear mixed effects regression models with participant-level random intercepts and random slopes (SAS version 9.4, SAS Institute). First, we report the statistical significance of the regression slopes relating changes in muscle activation magnitude to changes in the activation magnitude of the primary muscle – i.e., βo in Yij = αo + (βo + boi) * Xij + uoi + εij, where Yij was defined as the EMG co-activation in the muscle of interest (non-primary muscle) and Xij was defined as the EMG activation in the primary muscle, for the jth activation by the ith participant. Additionally, we report the statistical significance of the difference in regression slopes, based on the primary muscle group, relating changes in muscle activation magnitude to changes in primary muscle activation magnitude – i.e., β1 in Yij = (αo + uoi + (α1 * X2i)) + (βo + boi + (β1 * X2i)) * X1ij + εij, where Yij was defined as the EMG co-activation in the muscle of interest, X1ij was defined as the EMG activation in the primary muscle, X2i was defined as the primary muscle identifier (one of two primary muscles), and (X1ij * X2i) was the interaction term of interest, for the jth activation by the ith participant. Finally, we report the statistical significance of the regression slope relating changes in muscle activation latency to changes in primary muscle activation magnitude – i.e., βo in Yij = αo + (βo + boi) * Xij + uoi + εij, where Yij was defined as the onset difference between the muscle of interest and the primary muscle (ms) and Xij was defined the EMG activation in the primary muscle, for the jth activation by the ith participant.
fMRI acquisition
We measured brain activation associated with the voluntary muscle activation tasks (described above) using fMRI. To ensure the repeatability of our findings, we used two separate participant groups (A and B). In a first group of healthy participants (Group A: n = 23, 14 women + 9 men; Fig. 2A), we used fMRI recordings during blocks of comfortable GMM and FDI voluntary contraction to localize motor regions of interest (ROIs) related to activation of the gluteal-pelvic coordination pattern, since the gluteal-pelvic coordination pattern involves both the PFM and GMM. In another group of healthy participants (Group B: n = 20, 11 women + 9 men; Fig. 2B), we used fMRI recordings during blocks of scaled muscle contraction (three distinct levels) to examine changes in brain activity (as measured by BOLD signal strength) in the identified motor ROIs during voluntary activation of the isolated-pelvic or gluteal-pelvic coordination patterns. We used a 3-tesla scanner (GE Signa Excite) with an eight-channel head coil. We positioned participants supine while viewing a fixation crosshair, and placed foam pads to limit head motion. As in our previous fMRI studies of PFM activation9,11, we collected T2-weighted echo planar image volumes with blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast (echo time, 34.5 ms; flip angle, 90°; field of view, 220 mm; pixel size, 3.43 mm) continually every 2.5 s during two imaging runs. Each volume consisted of 37 axial slices (3 mm slice thickness, 0.5 mm interslice gaps) that covered the brain from vertex to cerebellum. We additionally acquired a T1-weighted high-resolution anatomical image from each participant, for the purpose of spatially registering functional images. For Group A (Fig. 2A), we cued participants to voluntarily activate each muscle group (to ~20% effort) in two separate runs, FDI activation run followed by GMM activation run. Participants performed 6 activation blocks in the scanner, during each block they contracted and relaxed every 2 seconds following a flashing crosshair. For Group B (Fig. 2B), we cued participants to voluntarily activate each muscle group (to three distinct levels: low (L), medium (M), high (H)) in two separate runs, PFM activation run followed by GMM activation run. Participants performed 6 activation blocks in the scanner (L, M, H, L, M, H), during each they contracted and relaxed following a flashing crosshair. The EMG sensors were not used during the fMRI acquisition. Therefore, before entering the actual MRI scanner, all participants were trained on producing muscle activation patterns in the FDI, PFM, and GMM similar to the activation patterns observed during the EMG study, while receiving the same instructions on how to contract each muscle group. This training was performed in a mock MRI scanner to ensure an identical environment and experimenters verified the EMG activation patterns during the training step. Participants in Group A were trained to produce one low level of muscle activation, while participants in Group B were trained to produce distinct L, M, and H activation of the isolated-pelvic and gluteal-pelvic coordination patterns.
fMRI preprocessing for both groups
For participants in both groups, we preprocessed each participant’s fMRI data using the FMRIB Expert Analysis Tool (FEAT, http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk)56, which included skull extraction using the brain extraction tool (BET) in FSL (FMRIB Software Library), slice timing correction, motion correction, and spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel with full-width half-maximum of 5 mm and nonlinear high-pass temporal filtering (100 s). We used a general linear model (GLM) to examine the changes in BOLD signal associated with muscle activation for the three tasks (FDI and GMM in Group A, PFM and GMM in Group B). We performed participant-level whole-brain GLM analyses of individual runs in each participant to determine the change in BOLD signal during the activation blocks compared with the rest blocks.
fMRI processing for Group A
We then performed a group-level mixed-effect (FLAME 1 in FSL) analysis, with unpaired two-sided t-tests of voxel-wise regression coefficients, to identify voxels in standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates with significant differences in response based on the muscle group being voluntarily contracted by the participant. The contrast of interest was GMM > FDI; i.e., (GMM > Rest) > (FDI > Rest). We thresholded the group-level image for this contrast with cluster-based correction for multiple comparisons (Z > 2.3 and p < 0.05). Finally, using the thresholded group-level image and the Jülich Histological Atlas within FSL57, we calculated motor cortical ROIs for specific Brodmann areas (BA: BA6 for supplementary motor cortex [SMA], BA4 for bilateral primary motor cortex [M1-L, M1-R]; Fig. 2A). Each ROI only included voxels that were statistically significant in the GMM > FDI contrast and had ≥25% probability of belonging to the BA of interest.
fMRI processing for Group B
We then projected each motor cortical ROI (SMA, M1-L, M1-R, identified by processing Group A data at the group level) into the native space of each participant in Group B to select voxels that were active (Z > 2.3, p < 0.05, cluster corrected) in the contrast of muscle activity compared to rest – we tested both GMM > Rest and PFM > Rest – regardless of the contraction level. For each participant, contracting muscle, and ROI, BOLD signals observed from the selected voxels were averaged; therefore, each participant provided six averaged BOLD signals (3 ROIs x 2 activation runs [PFM, GMM]; Fig. 2B). Finally, to evaluate the statistical significance of changes in the averaged BOLD signal in association with increases in voluntary activation of each primary muscle group, we used analysis of covariance (ANCOVA, SAS version 9.4, SAS Institute). ANCOVA was used to evaluate whether population means of the dependent variable (BOLD signal strength during muscle activation blocks) are equal across levels of the categorical independent variable (activation level), while statistically controlling for the effects of another variable that is not of primary interest (BOLD signal strength during rest blocks). To perform the analysis, we calculated a representative value of BOLD signal strength during an activation block (A) and a representative value of BOLD signal strength during the preceding rest block (R), for each activation level. A was defined as either the 50th or 75th percentile of a BOLD signal interval that corresponded to an activation block, while R was defined as the 50th percentile of the preceding BOLD signal interval that corresponded to the preceding rest block. Results for defining A were not affected by choosing the 50th or 75th percentile, and only the results from the 75th percentile are reported for simplicity. An ANCOVA model (Aij = Intercept + β1 * L + β2 * M + (0) * H + β3 * Rij + Ui + Eij; i was participant identifier [1 to 20] and j was block identifier [1 to 6]) was fit to all data from all 20 participants in Group B, per contracting muscle (PFM, GMM) and per ROI (SMA, M1-L, M1-R). The ANCOVA model assumed that each participant had a unique BOLD baseline (Intercept + Ui)58,59. BOLD variability during an activation block can be affected by BOLD variability during the preceding rest block due to brain-physiology-related low-frequency drifts in BOLD60; therefore, we used ANCOVA analysis to include rest blocks as baseline measurements of the outcome variable that is BOLD signal strength during muscle activation blocks. L, M, and H were the identifiers of low, medium, and high activation levels, respectively. Participants repeated each activation level twice; therefore, the extracted BOLD signal could be divided into 6 active intervals/blocks interleaved with 6 preceding rest intervals/blocks. For each contracting muscle and ROI, we report the statistical significance of changes in BOLD signal strength when increasing muscle activation magnitude, by comparing BOLD signal strength during low-level activation to BOLD signal strength during high-level activation (β1 slope) and by comparing BOLD signal strength during medium-level activation to BOLD signal strength during high-level activation (β2 slope).
TMS acquisition
In seven healthy participants (4 women + 3 men, a convenience sample from Group B in the fMRI data collection), we obtained motor evoked potentials (MEPs) from the PFM and right GMM, with participants resting supine, while using a single-pulse biphasic magnetic stimulator (Magstim Rapid2, Magstim) and a 110-mm double-cone coil (Magstim). The stimulation system was integrated with the EMG system of the Brainsight Frameless System (Rogue Research). Brainsight EMG system had an overall EMG amplification of 2500, an overall bandwidth of 16–470 Hz, and a sampling rate of 3000 Hz per channel. We recorded EMG signals from the PFM and GMM, using electrodes similar to the electrodes used in the EMG study described above. After acquiring a few visible MEPs, Brainsight MEP-selection window was adjusted and fixed throughout the entire TMS session to avoid including stimulation artefacts in calculating MEP peak-to-peak amplitude and onset. The MEP-selection window was 47 ms on average, extending from 13–60 ms after each TMS pulse.
fMRI results (Fig. 2B) did not clearly support the hypothesis of localized motor cortical representation of the PFM, and led us to hypothesize that the motor cortical representation of the PFM is not only localized to the medial wall of the precentral gyrus toward SMA and can be intermingled with the motor cortical representation of their synergists, i.e., the GMM. Therefore, the TMS procedure involved the following steps that were applied to each participant: (1) Mapping the medial motor cortex to identify PFM hotspot(s); (2) Calculating PFM resting motor threshold (rMT) to use as the TMS intensity during muscle contraction; and, (3) Calculating peak-to-peak amplitude and latency onset of MEPs induced during muscle contraction. After mapping the medial motor cortex, we focused on stimulating the identified hotspot in SMA and in M1 in the left hemisphere (corresponding to M1-L in fMRI), without stimulating the identified hotspot in M1 in the right hemisphere (corresponding to M1-R in fMRI), to reduce data collection time and stimulation risks.
PFM stimulation at rest for mapping, hotspots, and motor threshold
We constructed a grid of stimulation points with spatial resolution of 7 × 7 mm using Brainsight Frameless System (Rogue Research). In each participant, the stimulation grid covered the medial motor cortex, including SMA and bilateral M1 (extending ~21 mm lateral to the midline). We verified that the grid covered all activated motor regions during GMM voluntary activation by overlaying the grid on top of the first-level fMRI statistical image obtained by analysing the participant’s fMRI data from the GMM activation run (contrast: GMM > Rest). Prior to stimulation, we pre-selected a hypothetical PFM hotspot along the medial wall of the motor cortex, guided by the participant’s own fMRI GMM activation map. We then stimulated this hypothetical hotspot to select a stimulation intensity to use during the mapping procedure. We selected the smallest stimulation intensity that achieved 100% success rate and highest consistency of MEPs induced in the PFM. Stimulating at the selected intensity, each grid point was stimulated three times with a 6-second interstimulus interval (ISI). Grid points were stimulated in a randomized order selected by Brainsight (Brainsight 2.3.3). Combining reasonably short ISI and random walk between grid points has been validated to produce reliable brain maps, while lowering mapping time and number of stimuli without increasing chances of TMS-induced plasticity61. Hotspots were then selected for SMA and M1, using a motor mapping algorithm offered by Brainsight software and by inspecting the amplitude and consistency of the MEPs associated with prospective hotspots, as well as the underlying anatomical structures to identify SMA and M1. To visualize the group average motor map for the PFM (Fig. 3C), we first interpolated individual participants PFM maps into a 1-mm standard grid using natural neighbour interpolation (griddata in MATLAB), normalized these maps to their maximum MEP value, and finally averaged the standardized and normalized maps.
Muscle contraction is known to lower motor cortical excitability62–64; therefore, we aimed to stimulate identified PFM hotspots at their rMT – as a conservatively low stimulation intensity – while testing our hypotheses. At each PFM hotspot, we began by stimulating the hotspot at 4–5 intensities (above and under the mapping intensity, 5 stimuli per intensity at 6-second ISI). PFM rMT was selected as the percentage of maximum stimulator output (%MSO) with ≈ 50% success rate of eliciting consistent MEPs in the PFM. We then stimulated identified hotspots at rMT during muscle activation, as described in the next section.
PFM-hotspot stimulation during muscle activation
Participants performed separate trials (Fig. 4B), each of which involved voluntary pre-activation of a different primary muscle group (PFM followed by GMM) and single pulse TMS. For all trial types, we first acquired EMG data corresponding to MVC. For each primary muscle group, participants received visual feedback guiding them to sustain muscle contractions to 20%, 35%, and 50% MVC, while individual PFM hotspots were stimulated at the identified rMT. For each voluntarily contracting muscle, each participant produced 20 contractions per %MVC level with the potential of eliciting 40 MEPs due to simultaneous EMG data collection from the PFM and GMM.
TMS analysis
We processed the EMG data and TMS trigger signal to calculate the amplitude of the activity for each muscle at the time of applying TMS. Furthermore, we processed Brainsight EMG data to calculate peak-to-peak amplitude and onset of elicited MEPs. For each contraction, we collected from each Brainsight EMG channel (one for the PFM and one for the right GMM) a signal that extended for 150 ms, 50 ms before and 100 ms after the onset of the TMS pulse. Within each participant’s MEP-selection window, the onset of an MEP was assumed when the rectified EMG signal exceeded 2 standard deviations of a baseline signal that spanned the 20 ms leading to the onset of the TMS pulse. For each TMS pulse, Brainsight software (Brainsight 2.3.3) outputted the peak-to-peak absolute amplitude (in microvolts) of the recorded EMG signal within the MEP-selection window. For across-participant comparisons and group analyses, all MEP amplitude values were normalized. For each hotspot: PFM MEP amplitude values were normalized to the highest PFM MEP amplitude observed during both the PFM and GMM voluntary pre-activation trials. GMM MEP amplitude values were normalized to the highest GMM MEP amplitude observed during both the PFM and GMM voluntary pre-activation trials.
We have hypothesized changes in motor cortical excitability of pelvic- and gluteal-specific neural populations during the activation of the isolated-pelvic and gluteal-pelvic coordination patterns. Changes in motor cortical excitability can be quantified by assessing changes in MEPs’ peak-to-peak and onset values, while stimulating at a fixed intensity28,62–64. To evaluate the statistical significance of changes in amplitude (Fig. 4B) and timing (Fig. 4C) of MEPs associated with increases in voluntary pre-activation, we used linear mixed effects regression models with participant-level random intercepts and random slopes (SAS version 9.4, SAS Institute). First, we report the statistical significance of the regression slopes relating changes in amplitudes of MEPs (PFM and GMM) to changes in GMM pre-activation magnitude and to changes in PFM pre-activation magnitude – i.e., βo in Yij = αo + (βo + boi) * Xij + uoi + εij, where Yij was defined as the amplitude of MEP induced in one muscle (PFM or GMM) and Xij was defined the EMG pre-activation level in the same muscle at the time of the TMS pulse, for the jth activation by the ith participant. Finally, we report the statistical significance of the regression slope relating changes in MEP onset latency (onset of PFM relative to onset of GMM) to changes in GMM pre-activation magnitude, as well as the statistical significance of the difference in the intercepts – i.e., β1 and α1, respectively, in Yij = (αo + uoi + (α1 * X2i)) + (βo + boi + (β1 * X2i)) * X1ij + εij, where Yij was defined as the MEP onset (ms), X1ij was defined as the EMG pre-activation level in the GMM at the time of the TMS pulse, X2i was defined as the MEP identifier (MEP observed either in PFM or in GMM), and (X1ij * X2i) was the interaction term of interest regarding difference in the slopes, for the jth GMM activation by the ith participant.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Acknowledgements
The contents of this research paper were developed under support from the USC Division of Biokinesiology and Physical Therapy, support from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases (NIDDK) - National Institutes of Health (NIH) (R01DK110669) to JJK, and support from the Interstitial Cystitis Association to JJK.
Author Contributions
All authors contributed to study design. M.Y. and J.W. collected data. M.Y. and J.K. analysed data, wrote the main manuscript, and prepared all figures. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Fowler CJ, Griffiths D, de Groat WC. The neural control of micturition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:453–466. doi: 10.1038/nrn2401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Griffiths D. Neural control of micturition in humans: a working model. Nature reviews. Urology. 2015;12:695. doi: 10.1038/nrurol.2015.266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.DasGupta R, Kavia RB, Fowler CJ. Cerebral mechanisms and voiding function. BJU international. 2007;99:731–734. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-410X.2007.06749.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kavia RBC, Dasgupta R, Fowler CJ. Functional imaging and the central control of the bladder. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2005;493:27–32. doi: 10.1002/cne.20753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Vanderhorst VG, Terasawa E, Ralston HJ, Holstege G. Monosynaptic projections from the nucleus retroambiguus to motoneurons supplying the abdominal wall, axial, hindlimb, and pelvic floor muscles in the female rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2000;424:233–250. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20000821)424:2<233::AID-CNE4>3.0.CO;2-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Griffiths D, Fowler C. The micturition switch and its forebrain influences. Acta physiologica. 2013;207:93–109. doi: 10.1111/apha.12019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lefaucheur J-P. Excitability of the motor cortical representation of the external anal sphincter. Experimental brain research. 2005;160:268–272. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2170-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Remes-Troche JM, Rao SS. Neurophysiological testing in anorectal disorders. Expert review of gastroenterology & hepatology. 2008;2:323–335. doi: 10.1586/17474124.2.3.323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Asavasopon S, et al. Cortical Activation Associated with Muscle Synergies of the Human Male Pelvic Floor. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:13811–13818. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2073-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Leyton A, Sherrington CS. Observations on the excitable cortex of the chimpanzee, orang-utan, and gorilla. Experimental Physiology. 1917;11:135–222. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.1917.sp000240. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rana M, Yani MS, Asavasopon S, Fisher BE, Kutch JJ. Brain connectivity associated with muscle synergies in humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:14708–14716. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1971-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Schrum A, Wolff S, Van der Horst C, Kuhtz-Buschbeck J. Motor cortical representation of the pelvic floor muscles. The Journal of urology. 2011;186:185–190. doi: 10.1016/j.juro.2011.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kilpatrick LA, et al. Alterations in resting state oscillations and connectivity in sensory and motor networks in women with interstitial cystitis/painful bladder syndrome. The Journal of urology. 2014;192:947–955. doi: 10.1016/j.juro.2014.03.093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kutch JJ, et al. Altered resting state neuromotor connectivity in men with chronic prostatitis/chronic pelvic pain syndrome: A MAPP: Research Network Neuroimaging Study. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2015;8:493–502. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.05.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Griffiths D, Clarkson B, Tadic SD, Resnick NM. Brain mechanisms underlying urge incontinence and its response to pelvic floor muscle training. The Journal of urology. 2015;194:708–715. doi: 10.1016/j.juro.2015.03.102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kutch JJ, et al. Brain signature and functional impact of centralized pain: a multidisciplinary approach to the study of chronic pelvic pain (MAPP) network study. Pain. 2017;158:1979–1991. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Louppe J-M, et al. Motor cortex stimulation in refractory pelvic and perineal pain: Report of two successful cases. Neurourol Urodyn. 2013;32:53–57. doi: 10.1002/nau.22269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Brittain K, Peet S, Castleden C. Stroke and incontinence. Stroke. 1998;29:524–528. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.29.2.524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Woodworth D, et al. Unique microstructural changes in the brain associated with urological chronic pelvic pain syndrome (UCPPS) revealed by diffusion tensor MRI, super-resolution track density imaging, and statistical parameter mapping: a MAPP network neuroimaging study. PloS one. 2015;10:e0140250. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kairys AE, et al. Increased brain gray matter in the primary somatosensory cortex is associated with increased pain and mood disturbance in patients with interstitial cystitis/painful bladder syndrome. The Journal of urology. 2015;193:131–137. doi: 10.1016/j.juro.2014.08.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Abrams P, et al. Fourth International Consultation on Incontinence Recommendations of the International Scientific Committee: Evaluation and treatment of urinary incontinence, pelvic organ prolapse, and fecal incontinence. Neurourology and urodynamics. 2010;29:213–240. doi: 10.1002/nau.20870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Sanes JN, Donoghue JP. Plasticity and primary motor cortex. Annual review of neuroscience. 2000;23:393–415. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Rathelot J-A, Strick PL. Muscle representation in the macaque motor cortex: an anatomical perspective. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2006;103:8257–8262. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602933103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rathelot J-A, Strick PL. Subdivisions of primary motor cortex based on cortico-motoneuronal cells. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106:918–923. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0808362106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Overduin SA, d’Avella A, Carmena JM, Bizzi E. Microstimulation activates a handful of muscle synergies. Neuron. 2012;76:1071–1077. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Smith JA, et al. The motor cortical representation of a muscle is not homogeneous in brain connectivity. Experimental Brain Research. 2017;235:2767–2776. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-5011-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Turnbull GK, Hamdy S, Aziz Q, Singh KD, Thompson DG. The cortical topography of human anorectal musculature. Gastroenterology. 1999;117:32–39. doi: 10.1016/S0016-5085(99)70547-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Fisher BE, et al. Method for Assessing Brain Changes Associated With Gluteus Maximus Activation. journal of orthopaedic & sports physical therapy. 2013;43:214–221. doi: 10.2519/jospt.2013.4188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zhang H, et al. An fMRI study of the role of suprapontine brain structures in the voluntary voiding control induced by pelvic floor contraction. Neuroimage. 2005;24:174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kuhtz-Buschbeck J, et al. Activation of the supplementary motor area (SMA) during voluntary pelvic floor muscle contractions—an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2007;35:449–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.12.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Blok BF, Sturms LM, Holstege G. A PET study on cortical and subcortical control of pelvic floor musculature in women. The Journal of comparative neurology. 1997;389:535–544. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19971222)389:3<535::AID-CNE12>3.0.CO;2-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Seseke S, et al. Voluntary pelvic floor muscle control—an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2006;31:1399–1407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Cheung VC, et al. Stability of muscle synergies for voluntary actions after cortical stroke in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106:19563. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910114106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Giszter SF, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Bizzi E. Convergent force fields organized in the frog’s spinal cord. J Neurosci. 1993;13:467–491. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-02-00467.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Mussa-Ivaldi F, Giszter S, Bizzi E. Linear Combinations of Primitives in Vertebrate Motor Control. PNAS. 1994;91:7534–7538. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.16.7534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Saltiel P, Wyler-Duda K, D’Avella A, Tresch MC, Bizzi E. Muscle synergies encoded within the spinal cord: Evidence from focal intraspinal NMDA iontophoresis in the frog. J. Neurophysiol. 2001;85:605–619. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.2.605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Caggiano V, Cheung VC, Bizzi E. An Optogenetic Demonstration of Motor Modularity in the Mammalian Spinal Cord. Scientific reports. 2016;6:35185. doi: 10.1038/srep35185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Levine AJ, et al. Identification of a cellular node for motor control pathways. Nature neuroscience. 2014;17:586–593. doi: 10.1038/nn.3675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Nashner LM. Fixed patterns of rapid postural responses among leg muscles during stance. Experimental Brain Research. 1977;30:13–24. doi: 10.1007/BF00237855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Nashner LM. Adaptation of human movement to altered environments. Trends in Neurosciences. 1982;5:358–361. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(82)90204-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Petersen TH, Rosenberg K, Petersen NC, Nielsen JB. Cortical involvement in anticipatory postural reactions in man. Experimental Brain Research. 2009;193:161–171. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1603-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Massion J. Movement, posture and equilibrium: interaction and coordination. Progress in neurobiology. 1992;38:35–56. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(92)90034-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Gahéry Y, Massion J. Co-ordination between posture and movement. Trends in Neurosciences. 1981;4:199–202. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(81)90064-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Tanji J. The supplementary motor area in the cerebral cortex. Neuroscience research. 1994;19:251–268. doi: 10.1016/0168-0102(94)90038-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kutch JJ, Kuo AD, Bloch AM, Rymer WZ. Endpoint force fluctuations reveal flexible rather than synergistic patterns of muscle cooperation. J. Neurophysiol. 2008;100:2455–2471. doi: 10.1152/jn.90274.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Henneman E. Relation between size of neurons and their susceptibility to discharge. Science. 1957;126:1345–1347. doi: 10.1126/science.126.3287.1345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Krings T, Naujokat C, Keyserlingk DGV. Representation of cortical motor function as revealed by stereotactic transcranial magnetic stimulation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Electromyography and Motor Control. 1998;109:85–93. doi: 10.1016/S0924-980X(97)00078-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hetrick DC, et al. Pelvic floor electromyography in men with chronic pelvic pain syndrome: a case-control study. Neurourol Urodyn. 2006;25:46–49. doi: 10.1002/nau.20162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Tadic, S. D., Holstege, G. & Griffiths, D. J. The CNS and bladder dysfunction. F1000 medicine reports4 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 50.Hodges P, Sapsford R, Pengel L. Postural and respiratory functions of the pelvic floor muscles. Neurourology and urodynamics. 2007;26:362–371. doi: 10.1002/nau.20232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Thompson JA, O’Sullivan PB, Briffa NK, Neumann P. Differences in muscle activation patterns during pelvic floor muscle contraction and valsalva manouevre. Neurourology and urodynamics. 2006;25:148–155. doi: 10.1002/nau.20203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lefaucheur J-P, et al. Somatotopic organization of the analgesic effects of motor cortex rTMS in neuropathic pain. Neurology. 2006;67:1998–2004. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000247138.85330.88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Lefaucheur J-P, et al. Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) Clinical Neurophysiology. 2014;125:2150–2206. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Bø K, Stien R. Needle EMG registration of striated urethral wall and pelvic floor muscle activity patterns during cough, Valsalva, abdominal, hip adductor, and gluteal muscle contractions in nulliparous healthy females. Neurourology and Urodynamics. 1994;13:35–41. doi: 10.1002/nau.1930130106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Floyd W, Walls E. Electromyography of the sphincter ani externus in man. The Journal of Physiology. 1953;122:599–609. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1953.sp005024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. Fsl. Neuroimage. 2012;62:782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Eickhoff SB, et al. A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage. 2005;25:1325–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Handwerker DA, Ollinger JM, D’Esposito M. Variation of BOLD hemodynamic responses across subjects and brain regions and their effects on statistical analyses. Neuroimage. 2004;21:1639–1651. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Levin JM, et al. Influence of baseline hematocrit and hemodilution on BOLD fMRI activation. Magnetic resonance imaging. 2001;19:1055–1062. doi: 10.1016/S0730-725X(01)00460-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Yan L, et al. Physiological origin of low‐frequency drift in blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) Magnetic resonance in medicine. 2009;61:819–827. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Ruit VDM, Perenboom MJ, Grey MJ. TMS brain mapping in less than two minutes. Brain stimulation. 2015;8:231–239. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Hess CW, Mills K, Murray N. Responses in small hand muscles from magnetic stimulation of the human brain. The Journal of Physiology. 1987;388:397. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Cheng J, et al. Identification, Localization, and Modulation of Neural Networks for Walking in the Mudpuppy (Necturus Maculatus) Spinal Cord. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:4295–4304. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-11-04295.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Todd G, Taylor JL, Gandevia S. Measurement of voluntary activation of fresh and fatigued human muscles using transcranial magnetic stimulation. The Journal of physiology. 2003;551:661–671. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.044099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.